Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

SPECIAL TOPIC SERIES

Fear of Movement/(Re)injury in Chronic Low Back Pain


Education or Exposure In Vivo as Mediator to Fear Reduction?
Jeroen R. de Jong, MSc,*† Johan W. S. Vlaeyen, PhD,‡§㛳 Patrick Onghena, PhD,¶
Mariëlle E. J. B. Goossens, PhD,‡§ Mario Geilen, PT,# and Herman Mulder, OT#

improvements remained at half-year follow-up only in patients re-


Abstract: Clinical research of graded exposure in vivo with behav- ceiving the exposure in vivo. These patients also reported a significant
ioral experiments in patients with chronic low back pain who reported decrease in pain intensity at follow-up.
fear of movement/(re)injury shows abrupt changes in self-reported
pain-related fears and cognitions. The abrupt changes are more char- Key Words: pain-related fear, education, exposure in vivo, cognitive
acteristics of insight learning rather than the usual gradual progres- behavioral therapy, low back pain
sion of trial and error learning. The educational session at the start of (Clin J Pain 2005;21:9–17)
the exposure might have contributed to this insight. The current study
examines the contribution of education and graded exposure versus
graded activity in the reduction of pain-related fear and associated
disability and physical activity. Six consecutive patients with chronic
low back pain who reported substantial fear of movement/(re)injury
O ne of the recent developments in behavioral pain research
concerns the mechanisms by which patients with acute
pain become chronically disabled. A steadily increasing num-
were included in the study. After a no-treatment baseline measure-
ment period, all the patients received a single educational session, ber of studies are showing that observable physical perfor-
followed again by a no-treatment period. Patients were then randomly mance and self-reported disability levels in patients with sub-
assigned to either a graded exposure with behavioral experiments or acute and chronic pain are associated with cognitive and be-
an operant graded activity program. A diary was used to assess daily havioral, rather than sensory and biomedical, aspects of
changes in pain intensity, pain-related fear, pain catastrophizing, and pain.1–3 Fear-avoidance models have been proposed, and their
activity goal achievement. Standardized questionnaires of pain- major assumptions are successfully tested.3–7 A number of
related fear, pain vigilance, pain intensity, and pain disability were studies have reported that pain-related fear is one of the strong-
administered before and after each intervention and at the 6-month est predictors of variation in physical performance.5,8–11 Pro-
follow-up. An activity monitor was carried at baseline, during the
spective studies have revealed that, in patients with acute back
interventions, and 1 week at 6-month follow-up. Randomization tests
of the daily measures showed that improvements in pain-related fear
pain, pain-related fear is predictive of future disability and
and catastrophizing occurred after the education was introduced. The work status.12–14 If pain-related fear is indeed one of important
results also showed a further improvement when exposure in vivo mechanisms responsible for the development and maintenance
followed the no-treatment period after the education and not during of chronic pain disability, fear reduction should enhance the
the operant graded activity program. Performance of relevant daily prevention of further pain-related disability and distress.
activities, however, were not affected by the educational session and Current treatments of excessive fears and anxiety are
improved significantly only in the exposure in vivo condition. All based on the experimental psychologic work of Wolpe15 on
desensitization. Later studies revealed that the exposure to the
Received and accepted for publication December 31, 2003.
feared stimuli appeared to be the most essential component of
From the *Department of Rehabilitation, University Hospital Maastricht, the systematic desensitization. Craske and Rowe16 suggested
Maastricht, The Netherlands; †Department of Medical Psychology, Uni- that for fearful patients, first-hand evidence of actually expe-
versity Hospital Maastricht, Maastricht, The Netherlands; ‡Institute for riencing themselves behaving differently is far more convinc-
Rehabilitation Research, Hoensbroek, The Netherlands; §Department of ing than rational argument, and that the most essential step
Medical, Clinical, and Experimental Psychology, Maastricht University,
Maastricht, The Netherlands; 㛳Pain Management and Research Center,
consists of graded exposure to the situations the patient has
University Hospital Maastricht, Maastricht, The Netherlands; ¶Depart- identified as dangerous or threatening. The bioinformational
ment of Educational Sciences, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Leuven, theory of fear put forward by Lang et al17 predicts that there are
Belgium; and #Rehabilitation Center “Hoensbroeck,” Hoensbroek, The 2 main conditions to reduce fear. First, the fear network needs
Netherlands. to be activated, and second, new information needs to be avail-
Reprints: Jeroen R. de Jong, MSc, Department of Rehabilitation, University
Hospital Maastricht, PO Box 5800, Maastricht, NL 6202 AZ, The Neth-
able that does not confirm the fear expectations that are inher-
erlands (e-mail: jdjo@frev.azm.nl). ent to the fear memory. Following from cognitive models such
Copyright © 2004 by Lippincott Williams & Wilkins as the cognitive model of panic and the cognitive model of

Clin J Pain • Volume 21, Number 1, January/February 2005 9


de Jong et al Clin J Pain • Volume 21, Number 1, January/February 2005

obsessions,18,19 which assumes that cognitive errors can be pain intensity, pain-related fear, pain catastrophizing, and ac-
corrected through conscious reasoning, behavioral experi- tivity goal achievement, a diary was used. Standardized ques-
ments have been developed in which a collaborative empiri- tionnaires of pain-related fear, pain vigilance, pain intensity,
cism is the bottom line. Exposure (situational and interocep- and pain disability were administered before and after each
tive) is introduced as a behavioral experiment in which cata- treatment, and at 6-month follow-up. To measure actual activ-
strophic or irrational thoughts are being challenged. ity levels in the home situation, an activity monitor was carried
Clinical support of exposure in vivo with behavioral ex- at baseline, just after each intervention, and 1 week at 6-month
periments in chronic low back pain patients reporting substan- follow-up.
tial fear of movement/(re)injury has been reported by Vlaeyen
et al.20–22 Using a replicated single-case design, they provided Participants
preliminary evidence showing that compared with a no- Six consecutive patients who were referred for outpa-
treatment baseline period and a Graded Activity program tient behavioral rehabilitation, and who reported substantial
(based on operant-behavioral principles), the individually tai- fear of movement/(re)injury (Tampa Scale for Kinesiophobia
lored exposure in vivo was superior in decreasing the self- [TSK]24 score >39), were included in the study. The cutoff is
report measures of pain-related cognitions and fears, pain con- based on the median of the TSK distribution of chronic low
trol, and pain disability. Not only were improvements found on back pain samples reported earlier. Other inclusion criteria
the self-report measures, but also generalizing to increases in were nonspecific low back pain for at least 6 months and age
physical activity in the home situation as measured with am- between 18 and 65. Exclusion criteria were illiteracy, preg-
bulatory activity monitors.21 Although the exposure was pro- nancy, alcohol or drug abuse, and serious psychopathology
vided during a period of 3 or 4 weeks, the reduction of pain- based on the Symptom Checklist of which Dutch norms are
related fear was achieved within fewer than 3 exposure ses- available. Due to a major depressive disorder, a posttraumatic
sions of 1 hour. Such abrupt changes are more characteristic of stress disorder, pregnancy, and insufficient fluency in the
insight learning rather than the usual gradual progression of Dutch language, 4 of 10 consecutive patients with pain-related
trial and error learning.23 In these studies, the presentation of fear were excluded.
the rationale at the start of the exposure might have contributed
to this insight. Many patients reported that, for the first time, Procedure and Program Overview
they received a credible rationale for their current level of dis- Patients who met the selection criteria received a physi-
ability. cal examination and were subsequently invited for a semistruc-
If it seems that education has indeed a powerful treat- tured interview with the psychologist, during which a cogni-
ment effect at the start of an exposure in vivo, there is the pos- tive and behavioral analysis of the pain problem was made.
sibility of introducing treatment in a stepped care approach. To The Photograph series of Daily activities25 (PHODA)
tease out the differential effects of the educational part and the was used to develop a graded hierarchy of feared activities.
actual exposure component found in the studies of Vlaeyen et The patient is requested to place each of the 98 photographs
al,20–22 we decided to: 1) examine whether an educational ses- along a vertical line with 11 anchor points (ranging from 0 to
sion alone reduces fear levels; and (2) to compare the effec- 100) printed on a 100 cm × 40 cm hardboard. This fear ther-
tiveness of education followed by graded exposure in vivo mometer is placed on a table in front of the patient with the
with behavioral experiments as compared with education with following instruction: “Please watch each photograph care-
a operant graded activity program. By means of a replicated fully, and try to imagine you performing the same movement.
single-case experimental design, education (EDU) is con- Place the photograph on the thermometer according to the ex-
trasted with graded exposure in vivo and behavioral experi- tent in which you feel that this movement is harmful to your
ments (EXP) or an operant graded activity program (OPE). back.”
After this assessment, all patients started with the base-
MATERIALS AND METHODS line period (A), during which they completed daily measure-
ments at home. This period was followed by EDU, followed
Study Design again by a 3-week nontreatment period (B). Then, random as-
A replicated single-case experimental design was used signment either to 24 hours of EXP (C) spread over 6 weeks or
with multiple measurements. Patients were randomly assigned to 32 hours of OPE (D) spread over 8 weeks occurred.
to treatment group ABC or treatment group ABD. After a
3-week no-treatment baseline measurement period (A), all the Education
patients received EDU, followed again by a 3-week no- During the first part of the EDU, diagnostics tests are
treatment period (B). Then, random assignment to either being reviewed together with a physician. The physician ex-
6-week EXP (C) or 8-week OPE (D) occurred. Three kinds of plained to the patients that their pain must be viewed as a com-
outcome measures were included. To assess daily changes in mon condition that can be self-managed, rather than as a seri-

10 © 2004 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins


Clin J Pain • Volume 21, Number 1, January/February 2005 Fear of Movement/(Re)injury in Chronic Low Back Pain

ous disease or a condition that needs careful protection. In all” at one extreme and “impossible” at the other. This measure
cases where the pain-related fear was also fueled by the visual was administered on a daily basis during the whole duration of
confrontation with diagnostics tests, patients are explained that the study and the follow-up period of 1 week. The patients
they probably have overestimated the value of these tests, and were instructed to complete the scales each evening and to
that in symptom-free people, similar abnormalities can be send the package the next day to the researchers. This measure
found.26 One of the major goals of the second part of the EDU has been shown to be sensitive to graded exposure in vivo in
was to increase the willingness of the patient to finally engage previous studies.20–22
in activities they have been avoiding for a long time. The aim
was to correct the misinterpretations and misconceptions that Questionnaires
have occurred during the development of the pain-related fear. The Dutch version of the TSK was used. Vigilance for
The patient is given careful explanation of the fear-avoidance pain sensations was measured with the Dutch version of the
model. By using the patient’s individual symptoms, beliefs, Pain Vigilance and Awareness Questionnaire (PVAQ).32,33
and behaviors, how vicious circles (pain–catastrophic Patients also completed the Dutch version of the Roland Dis-
thought–fear–avoidance–disability–pain) maintain the pain ability Questionnaire (RDQ).35
problem is illustrated.
Activity Monitor
Graded Exposure In Vivo With Behavioral Experiments
Patients were requested to carry an ambulatory activity
Based on the graded hierarchy of fear-eliciting situa- monitor (uniaxial accelerometer)36 attached to a belt dorsally,
tions, individually tailored practice tasks were developed. close to lumbar discs L4 and L5. Movement counts were reg-
There was also 1 exposure session that actually took place in istered for an entire week during the moment of get up and
the work or home situation. A detailed description of the ex- bedtime, except for activities involving contact with water.
posure in vivo treatment can be found in Vlaeyen.27 The patients kept track of carrying times and the kind of ac-
Operant Graded Activity Program tivities performed by the help of a diary. At termination of the
This program was based on the operant principles de- registration period, the patient returned the device, and the data
scribed by Fordyce28 and Lindström et al.29 It was aimed at were downloaded to a personal computer. Movement counts
increasing healthy behaviors and decreasing pain behaviors. were added and subsequently divided by the time the acceler-
To avoid contamination between the EXP and the OPE, activi- ometer was carried. The activity monitor was carried 4 times
ties that were placed above 50 on the thermometer of the for an entire week.
PHODA were excluded from the OPE. The team members
monitored this without notifying the patient about this rule. Statistical Analyses
The rationale provided to the patients was that inactivity may For analyzing the resulting data of the daily measures, a
lead to disuse, which often promotes pain, and that increasing randomization test for single case experimental designs using
of the activity level and muscle strength consequently is likely the rationale of Edgington37 was carried out. Because interven-
to prevent future disability. tion B is expected to be superior to baseline A, and intervention
C is expected to be superior to A and intervention B, the null
Measures hypothesis that there is no differential effect for any of the
Daily Measures measurement times is tested using a randomization test on the
To check whether the EXP indeed modified the fear ap- differences between B and A, C, and A, C and B, D, and A, D
praisals, pain intensity, and activity goal achievement, a short and B. Although the follow-up (F) is expected to be superior to
questionnaire was used consisting of 11 visual analog scales A and will not change in relation to C and D, differences be-
(VAS; from 0 to 10) with items representing main factors of tween F and A, F and C, F and D, are also tested using random-
existing questionnaires for pain-related fear and catastrophiz- ization tests. The analysis is performed using the SCRT soft-
ing. These items were meant as manipulation check. Three ware (Single-Case Randomization Tests, version 1.1).38 The
program also allows the calculation of a combined P value of
main scores of part 1 were derived, consisting of the mean
each design when the cases are considered simultaneously in a
scores (range 0–10) of the items from the TSK, The Pain Anxi-
meta-analysis according to Edgington’s additive method.
ety Symptoms Scale,30 and the Pain Catastrophizing Scale.31
Daily pain intensity was measured with an additional VAS an-
chored with “no pain at all” at one extreme and “worst pain Preset Criteria for Nondaily Measures
experienced” at the other. The last 3 VAS referred to the per- To conclude whether the treatment could be considered
formance of personally relevant activities. Each scale consists successful, the authors decided to formulate preset criteria
of the same question: “How difficult was it to perform this partly based on existing norms. For the TSK and the PVAQ, a
activity today?” The scale was anchored with “no problem at reduction of more than 30 percentile points was considered

© 2004 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins 11


de Jong et al Clin J Pain • Volume 21, Number 1, January/February 2005

relevant. In line with Stratford et al,39 we decided that a change group, there is 1 patient were a significant change occurred
score of 5 can be clinically relevant for the RDQ. For the after EDU.
PHODA, no norms are available. Therefore, we estimated that
a 50% decrease would give enough support that the treat value Questionnaires
of the activities used in both treatment programs had de- Pain-Related Fear
creased. For the ambulatory activity monitor, standardized z- Table 2 shows that the mean TSK scores in both treat-
scores were calculated for each patient individually by sub- ment groups decrease after B and even decrease to a mean
tracting the mean number of baseline counts and dividing these score of 21 (<10th percentile) when the EXP is delivered and
values by the baseline standard deviation for that individual. not the OPE. For PHODA, a drastic reduction is observed at
This is done for mean counts in the week after EDU and the the end of EXP as compared with the A and B and remained at
treatment module. We estimated that an increase of 5 z-scores follow-up.
could be clinically relevant.
Pain Vigilance
As compared with baseline A, Table 2 shows that the
RESULTS decrease of the mean score of the PVAQ at the end of B was not
Daily Measures relevant in both treatment groups. However, under the influ-
Figure 1 shows the patterns of change of the mean ence of both EXP and OPE, the decrease of the mean score in
daily VAS ratings for fear of movement/(re)injury, pain both treatment groups is clinical relevant. Hereby, it is notable
catastrophizing, and fear of pain for the different treatment that in treatment group ABD, the reduction of 30% is, com-
groups. pared to A and B, just managed by OPE and that in group ABC,
Visual inspection of the data suggest that in both treat- an reduction of 70% is observed after EXP.
ment groups, the EDU produced substantial short-term de- Pain Disability
creases in fear of movement/(re)injury, pain catastrophizing, For the RDQ, Table 2 shows that relevant changes are
and fear of pain. Visual inspection also reveals that a further observed when both EXP and OPE are introduced. Overall,
decrease of these dependent variables occurs when EXP RDQ scores decrease from a mean of 15 to a mean of 2 in group
follows the no-treatment period after EDU. On the other hand, ABC and from 16 to 6 in group ABD. These differences
when OPE follows B, there is no further decrease. The results largely exceed the preset criterion of 5.
of the randomization test procedure are displayed in Table 1
and appear to confirm the conclusions of EDU. Compared Activity Monitor
with baseline A, significant changes are found in fear of In accordance with the self-reported difficulties in per-
movement/(re)injury, pain catastrophizing, and fear of formance of personal relevant daily activities at home, Figure
pain when EDU takes place. In addition to this, significant 3 shows that the mean standardized scores of the activity moni-
changes are also found in pain catastrophizing and fear of pain tor data did not change after EDU and only increased clinically
when EXP (C) is compared with B and not when OPE (D) is relevant in group ABC during EXP.
compared with B. Notable is that particularly in treatment
group ABC, all improvements remained during the follow-up DISCUSSION
period. The aim of this study was to examine the effects of a
Of interest, and quite unexpectedly, is that in treatment single educational session followed by actual exposure in vivo
group ABC, a significant reduction in pain intensity occurred or operant graded activity. Six consecutive patients with low
during follow-up as compared with baseline A (Fig. 2). Visual back pain who were referred for outpatient behavioral rehabili-
inspection of the self-reported difficulties in performance of tation and reported substantial fear of movement/(re)injury
daily activities at home in Figure 3 shows that they remained were included. A single-case experimental design was applied
unchanged during EDU and only decreased in the patients who in which chronically disabled patients were randomized over 2
received EXP. cognitive-behavioral treatments. Both the randomization tests
According to the graphical display, the randomization on the daily measures and pre–post assessments showed that
test on the raw data shows that the change in performance oc- during the education and the no-treatment period, subjective
curred in treatment group ABC for all the patients during EXP, ratings of pain-related fear and catastrophizing decreased sub-
and that the improvements remain during follow-up (a table stantially in all patients. This suggests that a highly individu-
that displays the P values is available). Compared with OPE in alized and tailored response to a personal formulation of a pa-
treatment group ABD, a significant change in performance oc- tient’s problem within a substantive model has good empirical
curred only in patient 2. However, contrary to patient 2, patient support in changing patient’s perceptions about the harmful-
3 and 4 show a significant change in performance during fol- ness of physical activity and threat value of pain and is sup-
low-up as compared with baseline. Finally, in each treatment posed to be part of CBT. Another point to make here is that our

12 © 2004 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins


Clin J Pain • Volume 21, Number 1, January/February 2005 Fear of Movement/(Re)injury in Chronic Low Back Pain

FIGURE 1. Mean daily measures of fear of movement/(re)injury, pain catastrophizing, and fear of pain across baseline (BAS: 21
days), psychoeducation (EDU: 21 days), exposure in vivo with behavioral tests (EXP: 12.5 hours over 35 days) in treatment group
ABC, operant exercise program (OPE: 35 hours over 49 days) in treatment group ABD, and 6-month follow-up period (FU: 7 days).

educational session does not appear to be a simple reassurance or so. In reducing pain-related fear, the results of the current
approach. On the whole, reassurance seems to have a transient study suggest that our education works in 1 session. This im-
impact with an immediate reduction of worry and concern fol- plicates that it would be a really effective intervention to use in
lowed by a rebound to preintervention levels within 24 hours primary care if one could identify at-risk patients.

© 2004 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins 13


de Jong et al Clin J Pain • Volume 21, Number 1, January/February 2005

TABLE 1. P Values for the ABC and ABD Treatment Group Randomization t
Tests on the KV-e Data for the Dependent Variables Fear of
Movement/(Re)injury, Pain Catastrophizing, Fear of Pain and Current
Pain Intensity

Treatment Treatment
Variables Sequence† Group ABC Sequence† Group ABD

Fear of movement/(re)injury BAS-EDU 0.024* BAS-EDU 0.024*


BAS-EXP 0.016* BAS-OPE 0.013*
EDU-EXP 0.097 EDU-OPE 0.776
BAS-FU 0.037* BAS-FU 0.037*
EXP-FU 0.813 OPE-FU 1.000
Pain catastrophizing BAS-EDU 0.024* BAS-EDU 0.024*
BAS-EXP 0.016* BAS-OPE 0.013*
EDU-EXP 0.016* EDU-OPE 0.882
BAS-FU 0.037* BAS-FU 0.037*
EXP-FU 0.292 OPE-FU 1.000
Fear of pain BAS-EDU 0.024* BAS-EDU 0.024*
BAS-EXP 0.016* BAS-OPE 0.013*
EDU-EXP 0.016* EDU-OPE 0.184
BAS-FU 0.037* BAS-FU 0.037*
EXP-FU 0.500 OPE-FU 0.968
Current pain intensity BAS-EDU 0.184 BAS-EDU 0.390
BAS-EXP 0.677 BAS-OPE 0.197
EDU-EXP 0.667 EDU-OPE 0.803
BAS-FU 0.037* BAS-FU 0.074
EXP-FU 0.271 OPE-FU 0.161
*P < 0.05.
†The intervention point was determined randomly with a minimum phase length of 1.
BAS, baseline; FU, follow-up.

However, despite the effect of education on pain-related same attitude object, one of which is an automatic, implicit
fear, the self-reported difficulties in performance of daily ac- attitude and the other being an explicit one. The following ci-
tivities at home and the subjective ratings of the PHODA re- tation from one patient in the current study is a nice illustration:
mained unchanged during that period. These variables only de- “After the educational session, I realized that my concerns
creased in the patients who received the graded exposure in about (re)injury were irrational, and that I had avoided a lot of
vivo. This suggests that education is not powerful enough to activities the last few years for no reason. However, when my
change actual escape and avoidance behavior. Why education wife asked me to go out for a ride on the bike yesterday, I
only influences beliefs about pain and is not actual escape and immediately said no.” Consequently, it might be expected that
avoidance behavior is an intriguing question, which merits fur- the dimensionality and thus the complexity in attitudes have an
ther examination. There is growing evidence that attitudes to- impact on (speed of) the change of avoidance and escape be-
ward physical activity, and the role of implicit versus explicit havior.
affective evaluations, play an important role in the treatment Another possible reason for the lack of association be-
outcomes of chronic low back pain. Studies have shown that tween beliefs and behavior is the use of self-report ques-
especially highly accessible attitudes such as the deeply in- tionnaires. It has been suggested that self-reports, as ex-
grained fear of movement/(re)injury will evoke automatic ac- plicit measures, assess what are called “self-attributed
tivation from memory when the object is encountered.40–42 If motives” and that they may be clouded with demand charac-
patients have labeled attitude objects as positive, approach be- teristics, self-presentational bias, or attributional a priori
havior will occur. Conversely, avoidance behavior is elicited theories.40,48–51 As a consequence, it could be that self-
when attitude objects have acquired a negative valence. Fur- attributed motives are akin to explicit attitudes that are ex-
thermore, it is possible that individuals can hold “dual atti- pressed when people are asked directly how they feel, and that
tudes,”43–47 which result from different evaluations of the implicit motives, assessed by the activity monitor, are similar

14 © 2004 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins


Clin J Pain • Volume 21, Number 1, January/February 2005 Fear of Movement/(Re)injury in Chronic Low Back Pain

FIGURE 2. Mean daily measures of self-reported difficulties in performance of activities at home, across baseline (BAS: 21 days),
psychoeducation (EDU: 21 days), exposure in vivo with behavioral tests (EXP: 12.5 hours over 35 days) in treatment group ABC,
operant exercise program (OPE: 35 hours over 49 days) in treatment group ABD, and 6-month follow-up period (FU: 7 days).

to habitual implicit attitudes, in that they automatically influ- modify escape and avoidance behavior due to pain-related
ence behavior. fear. It also suggests that the confrontation of fear-eliciting ac-
The finding that the activity monitor data follow the tivities in the treatment setting is analogue for how patients
same picture as the daily self-report measures on activity respond in daily life situations. Moreover, treatment gains pro-
achievement supports the assumption that actual exposure to duced during EXP seem to generalize to the home setting and
fear-eliciting activities and/or movements is necessary to in the absence of therapists.

© 2004 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins 15


de Jong et al Clin J Pain • Volume 21, Number 1, January/February 2005

Although the results show that exposure in vivo with be-


havioral tests are superior to an operant-behavioral graded ac-
tivity treatment in this subgroup of patients expressing sub-
stantial fear of movement/(re)injury, there are a number of ca-
veats to be considered. First, this study is limited in that it
included only 6 patients. On the other hand, a single-case ex-
perimental design was chosen with appropriate randomization
tests. Second, during the exposure in vivo, behavioral tests
have been developed. Theoretically, it is possible that the ef-
fects of the exposure in vivo treatment are due to this compo-
nent. However, in practice, behavioral tests are difficult to
separate from mere exposure, and they can best be used simul-
taneously. In any case, the findings of this study contribute to
the idea that interventions for patients with chronic low back
pain with substantial pain-related fear have to be tailored to the
specific attitudes of the patient in question.

REFERENCES
FIGURE 3. Activity monitor data. Mean standardized z-scores 1. Asmundson GJ, Norton PJ, Norton GR. Beyond pain: the role of fear and
(and standard error bars) for patients who received interven- avoidance in chronicity. Clin Psychol Rev. 1999;19:97–119.
tion ABC (baseline-education-exposure) and intervention ABD 2. Linton SJ. A review of psychological risk factors in back and neck pain.
(baseline-education-operant graded activity program). *Z- Spine. 2000;25:1148–1156.
score is based on intraperson baseline mean and SD. 3. Vlaeyen JWS, Linton SJ. Fear-avoidance and its consequences in chronic
musculosketal pain: a state of the art. Pain. 2000;85:317–332.
4. Lethem J, Slade PD, Troup JD, et al. Outline of a fear-avoidance model of
exaggerated pain perception—I. Behav Res Ther. 1983;21:401–408.
TABLE 2. Mean Scores of Fear of Movement/(Re)njury (TSK), 5. Vlaeyen JWS, Kole-Snijders AM, Boeren RG, et al. Fear of move-
Pain Vigilance (PVAQ), Self-reported Disability (RDQ), and ment/(re)injury in chronic low back pain and its relation to behavioral
performance. Pain. 1995;62:363–372.
Fearfulness of Movements (PHODA), Taken at Baseline,
6. Vlaeyen JWS, Kole-Snijders AMJ, Rotteveel AM, et al. The role of fear of
Before and After Each First Treatment Module, and at the movement/(re)injury in pain disability. J Occup Rehab. 1995;5:235–252.
6-Month Follow-up for Treatment Groups ABC (N = 3) and 7. Waddell G, Newton M, Henderson I, et al. A Fear-Avoidance Beliefs
ABD (N = 3) Questionnaire (FABQ) and the role of fear-avoidance beliefs in chronic
low back pain and disability. Pain. 1993;52:157–168.
Group Group 8. Al-Obaidi SM, Nelson RM, Al-Awadhi S, et al. The role of anticipation
Questionnaires Treatment ABC Treatment ABD and fear of pain in the persistence of avoidance behavior in patients with
chronic low back pain. Spine. 2000;25:1126–1131.
Tampa Scale for Baseline 41 Baseline 46 9. Burns JW, Mullen JT, Higdon LJ, et al. Validity of the Pain Anxiety
Kinesiophobia Education 29.33 Education 30 Symptoms Scale (PASS): prediction of physical capacity variables. Pain.
(TSK) 2000;84:247–252.
Start EXP 33.33 Start OPE 31.3 10. Crombez G, Vervaet L, Baeyens F, et al. Do pain expectancies cause pain
End 21.33 End 31.3 in chronic low back pain patients? A clinical investigation. Behav Res
Follow-up 20.33 Follow-up 33 Ther. 1996;34:919–925.
11. Crombez G, Vlaeyen JWS, Heuts PH, et al. Pain-related fear is more dis-
Pain Vigilance and Baseline 49.33 Baseline 50.3 abling than pain itself : evidence on the role of pain-related fear in chronic
Awareness Education 44.67 Education 45 back pain disability. Pain. 1999;80:329–339.
Questionnaire Start EXP 41 Start OPE 44.67 12. Fritz JM, George SZ, Delitto A. The role of fear-avoidance beliefs in acute
(PVAQ) low back pain: relationships with current and future disability and work
End 18.33 End 35.67 status. Pain. 2001;94:7–15.
Follow-up 19.67 Follow-up 37.33 13. Klenerman L, Slade PD, Stanley IM, et al. The prediction of chronicity in
Roland Disability Baseline 15 Baseline 16.33 patients with an acute attack of low back pain in a general practice setting.
Questionnaire Spine. 1995;20:478–484.
Education 12.67 Education 11.67 14. Sieben JM, Vlaeyen JW, Tuerlinckx S, et al. Pain-related fear in acute low
(RDQ) Start EXP 14.67 Start OPE 5.67 back pain: the first two weeks of a new episode. Eur J Pain. 2002;6:229–
End 2.67 End 5 237.
15. Wolpe J. Psychotherapy by Reciprocal Inhibition. Stanford, CA: Stanford
Follow-up 2 Follow-up 5.67 University Press; 1958.
The Photograph Baseline 68.33 Baseline 71.33 16. Craske MG, Rowe MK. A comparison of behavioral and cognitive treat-
Series of Daily Education 66 Education 72.33 ments of phobias. In: Davey GCL, ed. Phobias. A Handbook of Theory,
Activities Research and Treatment. Chichester: Wiley & Sons; 1997:247–280.
Start EXP 68.67 Start OPE 72 17. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN. Emotion and motivation: measuring
(PHODA)
End 11.67 End 59 affective perception. J Clin Neurophysiol. 1998;15:397–408.
Follow-up 12 Follow-up 58.67 18. Clark DM. A cognitive approach to panic. Behav Res and Ther.
1986;42:461–470.

16 © 2004 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins


Clin J Pain • Volume 21, Number 1, January/February 2005 Fear of Movement/(Re)injury in Chronic Low Back Pain

19. Rachman S. A cognitive theory of obsessions: elaborations. Behav Res 35. Deyo RA, Battie M, Beurskens AJ, et al. Outcome measures for low back
Ther. 1998;36:385–401. pain research: a proposal for standardized use [erratum in Spine 1999;24:
20. Vlaeyen JW, de Jong J, Geilen M, et al. Graded exposure in vivo in the 418]. Spine. 1998;23:2003–2013.
treatment of pain-related fear: a replicated single-case experimental de- 36. Bussman JBJ, van de Laar YM, Neleman MP, et al. Ambulatory acceler-
sign in four patients with chronic low back pain. Behav Res Ther. 2001; ometer to quantify motor behaviour in patients after failed back surgery: a
39:151–166. validation study. Pain. 1998;74:153–161.
21. Vlaeyen JW, De Jong J, Geilen M, et al. The treatment of fear of 37. Edgington ES. Randomization Tests. 3rd ed. New York, NY: Marcel
movement/(re)injury in chronic low back pain: further evidence on the Dekker; 1995.
effectiveness of exposure in vivo. Clin J Pain. 2002;18:251–261. 38. Onghena P, Van Damme G. SCRT 1.1: Single-case randomization tests.
22. Vlaeyen JW, De Jong JR, Onghena P, et al. Can pain-related fear be re- Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 1994;26:369.
duced? The application of cognitive-behavioural exposure in vivo. Pain 39. Stratford PW, Binkley JM, Riddle DL, Guyatt GH. Sensitivity to change
Res Manage. 2002;7:144–153. of the Roland-Morris Back Pain Questionnaire: part 1. Phys Ther.
23. Rachman S, Whittal M. Fast, slow and sudden reductions in fear. Behav 1998;78:1186–1196.
Res Ther. 1989;27:613–620. 40. Fazio RH. How do attitudes guide behavior? In: Sorrentino MR, Higgings
24. Kori SH, Miller RP, Todd DD. Kinesiophobia: a new view of chronic pain ET, eds. The Handbook of Motivation and Cognition: Foundations of So-
behavior. Pain Manage. 1990;35–43. cial Behavior. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1986:204–243.
25. Kugler K, Wijn J, Geilen M, et al. The Photograph series of Daily Activi- 41. Sanbonmatsu DM, Fazio RH. The role of attitudes in memory-based de-
ties (PHODA). CD-rom version 1.0. The Netherlands: Institute for Reha- cision making. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1990;59:614–622.
bilitation Research and School for Physiotherapy Heerlen; 1999. 42. Sarafino EP. Health Psychology. Biopsychosocial Interactions. New
26. Jensen MC, Brant-Zawadzki MN, Obuchowski N, et al. Magnetic reso- York, NY: John Wiley; 1998.
nance imaging of the lumbar spine in people without back pain [see com- 43. Wilson TD, Lindsey S, Schooler TY. A model of dual attitudes. Psychol
memts]. N Engl J Med. 1994;33:69–73. Rev. 2000;107:101–126.
27. Vlaeyen JWS, de Jong JR, Sieben J, Combez G. Graded Exposure in vivo 44. Ajzen I. Nature and operation of attitudes. Annu Rev Psychol. 2001;52:
for Pain-Related Fear. In: Gatchel R, Turk DC, eds. Psychological Ap- 27–58.
proaches to Pain Management. Guilford Press; 2002. 45. Bohner G, Wänke M. Attitudes and Attitude Change. East Sussex: Psy-
28. Fordyce WE. Behavioral Methods for Chronic Pain and Illness. St Louis, chology Press; 2002.
MO: Mosby; 1976:291–293. 46. Hermans D, Vansteenwegen D, Crombez G, et al. Expectancy-learning
29. Lindström I, Ohlund C, Eek C, et al. The effect of graded activity on and evaluative learning in human classical conditioning: affective prim-
patients with subacute low back pain: a randomized prospective clinical ing as an indirect and unobtrusive measure of conditioned stimulus va-
study with an operant conditioning behavioral approach. Phys Ther. 1992; lence. Behav Res and Ther. 2002:40.
72:279–290. 47. de Jong PJ, van den Hout MA, Rietbroek H, et al. Dissociations between
30. McCracken LM, Zayfert C, Gross RT. The Pain Anxiety Symptoms implicit and explicit attitudes toward phobic stimuli. Cognition and Emo-
Scale: development and validation of a scale to measure fear of pain. Pain. tion. 2004. In press.
1992;50:67–73. 48. Sherman SL, Presson CC, Chassin L, et al. Implicit and explicit attitudes
31. Sullivan MJL, Bishop SR, Pivik J. The Pain Catastrophizing Scale: de- toward cigarette smoking: the effects of context and motivation. J Soc
velopment and validation. Psychol Assess. 1995;7:524–532. Clin Psychol. 2004. In press.
32. McCracken LM. Attention to pain in persons with chronic pain: a behav- 49. de Jong PJ. Implicit self-esteem and social anxiety: differential self-
ioral approach. Behav Ther. 1997;28:271–281. favouring effects in high and low anxious individuals. Behav Res Ther.
33. Roelofs J, Peters ML, Vlaeyen JWS. Dutch version of the Pain Vigilance 2002;40:501–508.
and Awareness Questionnaire: validity and reliability in a pain-free popu- 50. Fazio RH, Jackson JR, Dunton BC, et al. Variability in automatic activa-
lation. Behav Res Ther. 2002;40:1081–1091. tion as an unobstrusive measure of racial attitudes: a bona fide pipeline? J
34. Roland M, Morris R. A study of the natural history of back pain: I. De- Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;69:1013–1027.
velopment of a reliable and sensitive measure of disability in low back 51. Nisbett RE, Wilson TD. Telling more than we can know: verbal reports on
pain. Spine. 1983;8:141–144. mental processes. Psychol Rev. 1977;84:231–259.

© 2004 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins 17

You might also like