The Role of Social Relationships in Bipo

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Psychiatry Research 219 (2014) 248–254

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Psychiatry Research
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psychres

Review article

The role of social relationships in bipolar disorder: A review


Sarah Greenberg, Katherine L. Rosenblum, Melvin G. McInnis, Maria Muzik n
Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, 4250 Plymouth Road, Ann Arbor, MI 48105, USA

art ic l e i nf o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Social relationships and attachment are core developmental elements of human existence and survival
Received 15 November 2013 that evolve over the lifetime of an individual. The internal and external factors that influence them
Received in revised form include the presence of illness in the individual or in their immediate environment. The developmental
9 May 2014
aspects of attachment and social relationships have become increasingly of interest and relevance in
Accepted 22 May 2014
Available online 4 June 2014
light of early developmental epigenetic modification of gene expression patterns that may influence
subsequent behavioral patterns and outcomes. This review examines extant literature on attachment
Keywords: and social relationships in bipolar cohorts. Despite many methodological challenges, the findings
Mood disorder indicate that social relationships and capacity for attachment are significantly compromised in
Attachment
individuals with bipolar disorder compared to other mood disorders and normal controls. Though
Social support
extant research is limited, research clearly points toward the importance of social relationships on the
Parent–child relationship
etiology, course, and consequences of bipolar disorder. We highlight a number of key considerations for
future research.
& 2014 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

Contents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 248
2. Methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 250
3. Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 250
3.1. Child attachment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 250
3.2. Adult attachment. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 251
3.3. Social support . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 252
4. Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 252
5. Conclusion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 253
Acknowledgments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 253
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 253

1. Introduction reviews the literature on the role of social relationships in


emotional wellness across the life cycle in persons with bipolar
Studies examining social relations among individuals with disorder. The data presented are organized in the framework of
bipolar disorder diagnoses suggest that these individuals are more Social Convoy Theory.
likely to report insecure attachment relationships and poor social Bipolar disorder is characterized by a lifelong pattern of
support. This association may be highly relevant for the indivi- pathological mood swings ranging from the energized states with
duals’ course of illness and life trajectory, as studies in the general misguided volition and intoxicating euphoria (or irritability) of the
population have shown that supportive relationships may buffer manic phases, to the spectrum of depression with compromised
against life's stressful experiences. This paper comprehensively energy, volition, and slowed cognition, concentration, and physical
activity. There are overt and covert debilitating consequences,
socially, personally, and vocationally, placing bipolar disorder
n
Corresponding author. Tel.: þ 1 734 232 0206; fax: þ1 734 764 4031. among the top ten causes of years lost to disability (Murray and
E-mail address: muzik@med.umich.edu (M. Muzik). Lopez, 1997). Even euthymic and remitted bipolar individuals

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2014.05.047
0165-1781/& 2014 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
S. Greenberg et al. / Psychiatry Research 219 (2014) 248–254 249

report lower functioning and well-being compared to patients environments (i.e., low maternal licking/grooming) increased
with chronic medical illness, major depressive disorder (Cooke methylation of the glucocorticoid receptor (GR) gene, which is
et al., 1996), or the general population (Arnold et al., 2000). Two associated with decreased expression of this GR gene in the
years following the first onset of bipolar mania, only a third of hippocampus area, leading to an increased and dysregulated stress
individuals return to their former level of functioning (Tohen et al., response in the offspring. Conversely, being reared in high quality
2000; Duffy et al., 2007). caregiving (i.e., high maternal licking/grooming) was associated
Individuals with bipolar disorder generally have trouble with with a decreased GR gene methylation, increased GR expression in
relationships across the lifespan. Disrupted life domains in those the hippocampal area and decreased stress response. This research
with bipolar disorder include self-expression/self-improvement, illustrates the effect of maternal care from one generation to next
family relationships, other social relationships, and work relations; – maternal care given to offspring shapes offspring biological and
these disruptions are observed even when the individuals with behavioral outcomes. DNA methylation, along with other environ-
bipolar disorder are euthymic at the time of assessment (Robb mental factors, has similarly been proposed as epigenetic mechan-
et al., 1997). The social cost of bipolar disorder is thought to be isms that contribute to intergenerational influences in humans.
considerable, but there are relatively few studies that address the Current research and clinical indicators are consistent with a
social burden of this illness. Angst (1998) reported that the divorce complex interplay between environmental and biological factors
rate is three- to six-folds higher in bipolar II versus controls. in the developmental trajectory of bipolar disorder (McGowan and
Family members and caregivers are reported to be considerably Kato, 2008). It is postulated that environmental factors may evoke
burdened, and their burden is frequently associated with the level or protect against biological vulnerabilities in bipolar disorder, a
of depression in the affected person (Ostacher et al., 2008). Thus, combination of a genetic diathesis and stress (environment) (Post,
not surprisingly, individuals with bipolar disorder are more likely 1992). The genetic diathesis–stress model may be helpful in
to be living alone, yet they are also noted to be more likely poor, understanding why close relationships may be so severely
less educated, or unemployed (or if employed more likely to have impacted by bipolar disorder; children of affected parents are
missed days), compared to those with major depression or no not only genetically predisposed, but parental disorder may also
affective disorder (Shippee et al., 2011). The social limitations of increase the risk though an unpredictable caregiving environment,
this illness are compounded in part by the chaotic episodic nature which in turn may interfere with the offspring's development of a
of the disorder, but also based on the diluted capacity of affected secure attachment. Disrupted early caregiving may be an environ-
individuals to establish healthy attachments that serve as tem- mental risk that may potentiate genetic and epigenetic vulner-
plates for enduring relationships. abilities. These factors may, in turn, create a deteriorating cycle –
The causes of bipolar disorder are not yet fully elucidated, but it early close relationships may be affected by parental disorder, and
is widely accepted within the field that an interaction between if the children develop the disorder, their disorder may further
biological susceptibility and environmental influences contribute interfere with and complicate their future relationships.
to its etiology (McInnis et al., 2007). Environmental factors, Few studies have focused on the role of interpersonal relation-
including social context, may shape biological mechanisms that ships on the presentation or course of bipolar disorder, or
are genetically determined, or influence genetic function (expres- conversely, the impact of bipolar disorder on close relationships.
sion) within the individual over the course of development. There There are three central domains of close relationships, including
is evidence of at least three associated genetic polymorphisms in (a) early childhood child–parent attachment, (b) adult attachment
those diagnosed with bipolar disorder (Clayton-Smith et al., 2010; (including romantic partnership), and (c) social support. Associa-
Cohen-Woods et al., 2010; Cannon et al., 2011). The implications of tions have been described between these domains and other
these genetic variations, including abnormalities in cell structure, psychiatric disorders including psychotic (Breier and Strauss,
and dysregulation of cellular transmitters or receptors, are among 1984) and depressive disorders (for a review see Wai Wan and
the biological consequences related to these genetic polymorph- Green (2009)), borderline personality disorder (for a review of the
isms that may explain some, but in reality very little, of the attachment literature, see Agrawal et al. (2004)), social anxiety
phenotypic aspects of bipolar disorder. The field that studies the disorder (Eng et al., 2001), and general psychiatric symptomatol-
non-genetic biology of gene expression is epigenetics, investigat- ogy (Hipwell et al., 2000; Shorey and Snyder, 2006).
ing how environment influences gene functionality. Epigenetics Attachment Theory is a conceptual framework initially pro-
has been proposed responsible for some of the phenotypic varia- posed by Bowlby (1969), a psychiatrist and ethologist who aimed
tions in bipolar disorder course and illness severity that genetics to elucidate the evolutionary purpose underlying the formation of
or environmental effects alone are unlikely to explain (Petronis, early social bonds between parents and offspring. Bowlby pro-
2003). Epigenetic studies suggest that early social context, most posed that offspring are neurologically hardwired to form long-
importantly early caregiving experiences, is a potentially crucial lasting emotional attachments to their caregivers for the purpose
modifier of the associations between genetic vulnerability and of proximity seeking in times of perceived danger, thus promoting
development of mental health risks, and underscores the critical survival of the species. Bowlby postulated this as a universal
importance of secure child–parent attachment relationships as principle for all mammalian species. His follower, Mary Ainsworth,
foundation for lifelong health (Miller et al., 1997; Francis et al., later specified that while attachments are universal for mamma-
1999). The stress that an infant experiences when exposed to lian species, in humans, the quality of the attachment bond is
inadequate parenting, leading to the establishment of an insecure unique for each dyad and is shaped by the cumulative experience
attachment relationship pattern, has been found to hold mean- of day-to-day interactions over the first year between the baby and
ingful biological and social implications for gene expression and is the caregiver. She demonstrated that the quality of attachment can
related to poor behavioral adaptation later in life (for a review, see be measured in an experimental paradigm, the Strange Situation
Champagne and Curley (2005)) Paradigm (SSP) (Ainsworth and Wittig, 1969) The SSP classifies
DNA methylation exemplifies an epigenetic mechanism that attachment styles as secure, insecure–avoidant, insecure–ambiva-
involves modulating gene expression as a consequence of early lent, and disorganized through coding of behavioral indicators of
caregiving that may hinder the offspring's responsiveness (i.e. approach and comfort seeking in a reunion episode following
social responsivity) later in life. Most of this work has been separation from the caregiver. The attachment style is thought to
demonstrated in animal studies. For example, Champagne et al. develop over the first year of life as consequence to the parents’
(2003) showed that rodent offspring reared in poor quality caregiving style. Later in life, during adolescence and adulthood,
250 S. Greenberg et al. / Psychiatry Research 219 (2014) 248–254

the measurement of attachment is through a person's narratives of security, in turn, provides children with subsequent increased
childhood caregiving experiences during times that would elicit sense of self-esteem and efficacy, and more trusting, well-
attachment behaviors, such as separations from caregivers, illness, regulated friendships with peers (Rosenblum et al., 2009). As the
or emotional and physical hurt (Obegi and Berant, 2010). Such infant matures, the social convoy grows and changes as he or she
narratives of attachment relevant memories are also called “inter- forms close relationships with others, such as friends or romantic
nal working models of relationships,” and are thought of as partners. As the child develops into an adult, he or she continues
memory templates that guide relational and social experiences to exist within a social convoy. Paralleling secure attachment
across the lifespan (Main et al., 1985). Most interviews that tap patterns in infancy and early childhood when interacting with
into adult attachment working models tend to measure an adult's caregivers, secure adult attachment reflects a balance between inti-
attachment to his or her family of origin instead of adult romantic macy and independence, whereby the adult is able to establish and
relations (Jacobvitz et al., 2002). Finally, early attachment experi- maintain closeness and intimacy, and yet does not unduly worry about
ences can also be tapped into through retrospective self-report acceptance by others. High levels of perceived social support provide
measures that ask an adult to report on their relationships with the “secure base” and “safe haven” for an adult, allowing for optimal
one or both parents when growing up (Parker et al., 1979). There functioning. The link between attachment and social support is that
are several self-report attachment measures available and vali- perceptions of attachment figures are associated with perceptions of
dated (Armsden and Greenberg, 1987; Collins and Read, 1990; support from those same figures (Asendorpf and Wilpers, 2000).
Bartholomew and Horowitz, 1991; Brennan et al., 1998). In Research supports that for adults, the presence of at least one
summary, attachment is an established construct in developmen- supportive close relationship is more important than who the specific
tal psychology known to contribute to a person's adjustment and relationship is with (e.g., parent, romantic partner etc.) (Levitt, 1991).
developmental trajectory across the life span. Thus, the Social Convoy Model suggests that both social support and
Most studies of social support seem to use semi-structured attachment theories reflect an underlying, evidence-based assumption
interviews or self report measures to elicit information about that “crisesþ support/coping¼increased wellbeing” (Antonucci and
perceived social support and functional support, such as instru- Akiyama, 1994, p. 40).
mental support, emotional support, or companionship. For exam- The Social Convoy Model facilitates understanding how indivi-
ple, the Interpersonal Support Evaluation List (Cohen and Wills, duals, regardless of age or partnership status, experience support
1985) is a commonly used self-report scale that measures func- from others, and the impact that this support has on their
tional or perceived social support. The use of semi-structured functioning and wellbeing. In the current review we use a Social
interviews to elicit social support information is seemingly less Convoy framework to organize the extant literature on bipolar
common. One concern in measuring social support information, disorder and close relationships from a lifespan perspective,
whether through interview or self-report, is the potential for drawing from research on both attachment and social support.
reporting bias; in particular, the risk that current mood states
may shape the respondent's evaluation of their social support.
2. Methods
While reporting biases is a concern for any study with any
population that solely uses self-report measures, this is a parti-
This literature review reports on the results of a systematic review conducted
cular concern for studies involving psychiatric populations, given
with the PubMed, Medline, and PsycINFO search engines. We used the following
that one's perspective of social support or attachment could be key words: “bipolar” (used in all searches), “attachment,” “parenting”, “marriage”,
affected by their current mood. In spite of this issue, however, self- “relationship”, and/or “social support.” Titles and abstracts of the articles listed
report measures do give valuable insight into participants’ percep- through this search were individually reviewed to determine if they met the
following additional methodological criteria:
tions of their social relationships, and have been found to be valid
and reliable.
1. original empirical study published in a peer-reviewed journal;
Several prescient studies on bipolar disorder, summarized 2. sample includes adults with bipolar disorder;
briefly in subsequent sections of this paper, hint at the important 3. analysis focused on bipolar sample (i.e., analyses did not simply reflect group-
role that may be played by close relationships. The paucity of ing bipolar with other psychiatric illness); and
research on close attachment relationships and bipolar disorder 4. measures of aspects of the social convoy (i.e. adult or child attachment, social
support).
represents a significant gap in the literature, in particular in view
of the emerging interest in epigenetic influence on human devel-
This search yielded 23 studies, 15 regarding social support and bipolar disorder,
opment (Szyf, 2011). and eight regarding attachment and bipolar disorder (three children, five adults).
The Social Convoy Model characterizes the role of close Results of this search are listed in Table 1 and are summarized in the following
relationships across the lifespan (Kahn and Antonucci, 1980), and sections.
is useful for considering the importance of close relationships over
the lifetime of individuals affected with bipolar disorder. This
model provides a conceptual framework to integrate early attach- 3. Results
ment relationships with primary caregivers and social support
provided by friends and romantic partners later in life, under- In the following sections we provide a summary of the extant
scoring the lifelong importance of attachment and social support literature on close social relationships and bipolar disorder,
needs. According to this model, relationships across the lifespan reporting findings separately for bipolar disorder and child attach-
are continuations of relationships that were formed in childhood ment, adult attachment, and social support.
based on formative experiences with early attachment caregivers.
From this perspective, support provided by parents in early child- 3.1. Child attachment
hood plays a critical role as an infant begins life within a social
convoy and his or her parents are typically the primary available Studies of psychosocial functioning in the offspring of parents
social relationships. Sensitive support is likely to yield higher with bipolar disorder have focused primarily on assessments of
levels of attachment security in children. Secure children trust behavior and psychopathology of the child (Chang et al., 2003a,
their caregivers, and are able to use them both as a “secure base” 2003b; Duffy et al., 2007; Bella et al., 2011; Diler et al., 2011), while
as they explore the world, and as a “safe haven” for comfort and relatively few have addressed specific elements of attachment
regulation in times of distress (Bowlby, 1988). Early attachment between children and their parents. Radke-Yarrow et al. (1985)
S. Greenberg et al. / Psychiatry Research 219 (2014) 248–254 251

Table 1
Methodological characteristics among studies.

Domain Study Relationship(s) Total Bipolar n Controls? Other Study Measure of Measure
measured N psych designa relationshipb typec
dx?

Child attachment Radke-Yarrow et al. Child 99 BP offspring Yes Yes CS SS O


(1985) n ¼14
Zahn-Waxler et al. Child 27 BP offspring Yes No CS SS O
(1984) n ¼7
Gilbert et al. (2007) General 40 BP n¼ 40 No No CS AAS SR
Joyce (1984) Parents 158 BP n¼ 58 Yes No CS PBI SR

Adult attachment Kökçü and Kesebir General 163 BP n¼ 44 Yes No CS AAS SR


(2010)
Marazziti et al. (2007) Romantic partner 126 BPI n¼ 31; BPII No Yes CS ECR SR
n ¼31
Morriss et al. (2009) Friends, family 148 BPI n¼ 107 Yes No CS BHRQ SR
Rosenfarb et al. (1994) Parents, peers 156 BP n¼ 25 Yes Yes CS IPPA, PCRQ SR
Beyer et al. (2003) General 141 BP n¼ 85 Yes No CS DSSI SR
Cohen et al. (2004) Best friend, parent, romantic 52 BPI n¼ 52 No No L SI SR
partner
Dorz et al. (2003) General 163 BP n¼ 31 No Yes CS SSQ SR
Gutiérrez-Rojas et al. General 108 BP n¼ 108 No No CS SASS SR
(2011)
Johnson et al. (1999) General 59 BPI n¼ 59 No No L ISEL, ISSI CI, SR

Social support Johnson et al. (2000) General 31 BPI n¼ 31 No No L ISEL SR


Johnson et al. (2003) General 94 BPI n¼ 71; BPII No No L ISEL, ISSI CI, SR
n ¼23
Kulhara et al. (1999) General 118 BP n¼ 118 No No L SSQ SR
O'Connell et al. (1985) General 60 BPI n ¼ 60 No No L PRI CI
Romans and McPherson General 284 BPI n¼ 52 Yes Yes CS ISSI CI
(1992)
Sanchez-Moreno et al. General 88 BP n¼ 88 No No CS MSPSS SR
(2010)
Staner et al. (1997) Social network 77 BP n¼ 27 Yes Yes L SSNI SR
Stefos et al. (1996) General 21 BP n¼ 21 No No L SSNI SR
Strauss and Johnson Treatment provider, general 58 BPI n¼ 58 No No L WAI, ISEL SR
(2006)
Weinstock and Miller General 92 BPI n¼ 92 No No L ISEL SR
(2010)

a
CS ¼ Cross-Sectional, L¼ Longitudinal.
b
SS ¼Strange Situation, AAS ¼ Adult Attachment Scale, PBI ¼ Parental Bonding Instrument, ECR ¼Experiences in Close Relationships, BHRQ ¼Bartholomew–Horowitz
Relationship Questionnaire, IPPA ¼Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment, PCRQ ¼Parent-Child Relations Questionnaire, DSSI ¼ Duke Social Support Index, SI ¼Five-item
Scale of Supportive Interactions, SSQ ¼Social Support Questionnaire, SASS¼ Social Adaptation Self-Evaluation Scale, ISEL ¼ Interpersonal Support Evaluation List, ISSI ¼Inter-
view Schedule for Social Interaction, PRI¼Personal Resources Inventory, MSPSS ¼Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support, SSNI ¼ Social Support Network
Inventory, WAI ¼Working Alliance Inventory.
c
SR ¼Self-Report Questionnaire, O ¼ Observational, CI ¼ Clinician-Rated Interview.

and Zahn-Waxler et al. (1984) studied attachment in children of challenges to the caregiving environment and childhood attach-
adults with bipolar disorder using the standard paradigm, the SSP ment security may both directly contribute to difficulties in child
(Ainsworth and Wittig, 1969). Both studies were limited by small behavioral and emotional self regulation, as parents struggle to
samples, such that the two studies combined only included 21 provide effective co-regulation, but also may interfere with effec-
children of mothers with bipolar disorder. Nonetheless, the results tive childhood learning of social sills including capacity for secure
were remarkable – 19 of 21 children at two- or three-years old had emotional attachment (Bowlby, 1988), which have implications for
an insecure attachment style. Though the sample sizes were relationships later in life.
extremely small, the results of these two studies strongly suggest
that parent–child attachment should be examined further in 3.2. Adult attachment
parents with bipolar disorder. A more recent study of parent–
child interactions that included 60 families (30 bipolar and 30 Patterns of attachment in bipolar disorder may be considered
controls) found that parent–child relationships in the bipolar in several dimensions, the most common contemporal context
group were characterized by significantly less warmth, affection, being either a focus on adult attachment to other adults (i.e.
and intimacy, and there are more quarreling and forceful punish- romantic partners and friends), or with a focus on adult attach-
ment (Schenkel et al., 2008). ment that reflects their current state of mind about their own
Close examination of the qualities of early caregiving environ- family of origin (i.e., current mental representations of early
ments provided by symptomatic parents with bipolar disorder is experiences with one's own mother or father in early childhood).
likely to be of considerable importance. Parents with bipolar Our search identified five studies published on adult attachment in
disorder may be hindered in their ability to provide an appropriate bipolar cohorts; however, the type of attachment relationship that
and predictable response to a child, not only due to the extreme each study measured differed from one other. The different
“highs” of mania and the “lows” of depression, but also because of constructs of attachment reduces comparative analysis to a qua-
the vicissitudes and turmoil of the sub-threshold symptoms that litative perspective with a challenge to the field for a systematic
characterize the chronicity of bipolar disorder. These types of approach in the future. In addition, all five studies used different
252 S. Greenberg et al. / Psychiatry Research 219 (2014) 248–254

self-report measures to assess attachment. Four studies engaged a support found that a strong treatment alliance is related to strong
healthy control group to compare attachment among individuals social support and that treatment alliance is negatively correlated
with bipolar disorder to healthy controls; one study (Joyce, 1984) with depressive symptoms (Strauss and Johnson, 2006). These
examining adults’ retrospective accounts of their relationship to studies may have been limited not only by a small sample size, but
their parents using the Parental Bonding Instrument (Parker et al., a small period of follow-up time (seven out of 10 longitudinal
1979) found no difference in attachment styles between the two studies had a follow-up period of 1 year or less) which may be too
groups. The remaining three studies found more insecure attach- short of a time to measure episode recurrence, especially manic
ment styles in individuals with bipolar disorder compared to the episode recurrence.
non-affected controls (Rosenfarb et al., 1994; Morriss et al., 2009;
Kökçü and Kesebir, 2010). For example, Morriss et al. (2009), who
examined attachment to family and friends using the Bartholo- 4. Discussion
mew–Horowitz Relationship Questionnaire (Bartholomew and
Horowitz, 1991) found that most (79%) of bipolar individuals Social relationships and attachments are core to human exis-
reported an insecure attachment, as opposed to only 32% of tence and survival. From birth until death, humans exist within a
controls. However, this difference may be explained by the social convoy that has implications for well-being. The relevance
presence of a psychiatric diagnosis in general and not be specific to bipolar disorder is established in the association of current
to bipolar disorder; the two studies that compared other psychia- metrics of recurrence and severity with evidence of poor social
tric diagnoses with bipolar disorder found a preponderance of support and relationships patterns. This leads to the conclusion
insecure attachment relationships independent of diagnosis that social convoys play a significant role in the outcome of the
(Rosenfarb et al., 1994; Marazziti et al., 2007). To control for illness; however, there are a number of inconsistencies in the
reporting biases from symptomatic subjects, three studies findings primarily due to the low overlap in the specific methods
included only participants with bipolar disorder (Joyce, 1984; that each study operationalized the relevant constructs (e.g.,
Gilbert et al., 2007; Kökçü and Kesebir, 2010) and one separated illness severity could be defined by age of onset, duration of
subjects into groups based on current mood state (Morriss et al., illness, number of hospitalizations, etc for bipolar disorder). Most
2009). The latter study found that attachment was affected by studies do not control for reporting biases based on the current
mood; for example, scores for secure attachment were no different mood state of the individual. It is highly probable that one's
between manic patients and healthy controls but depressed and perspective of perceived social support or attachment could be
euthymic individuals with history of bipolar disorder were more affected based on current mood state, and this variation, when left
likely to report insecure attachment. unaccounted, may attenuate effects observed for social support on
bipolar symptomatology. Several studies do not distinguish
3.3. Social support between depressive and manic episodes. Studies also differed on
how they coded non-traditional relationships. For example, Cohen
Five cross-sectional and 10 prospective studies focused on et al. (2004) coded the lowest level of support for missing
social support and bipolar disorder. Several concluded that lower relationships (e.g., no romantic partner, deceased parents) and
levels of perceived social support were related to unfavorable Morriss et al. (2009) excluded for non-traditional parenting (i.e. if
outcomes in bipolar disorder. In this section we summarize the a subject was raised by more or less than two parents). This
primary findings of these studies with regards to social support eliminates the possibility that a missing or non-traditional rela-
and bipolar disorder. tionship has a different effect on close relationships than a mere
The five published cross-sectional studies indicate that, as a low level of support or an insecure attachment. Additionally, no
group, individuals with bipolar disorder experience a lower level studies currently take into account the high level of heritability of
of social support than controls (Romans and McPherson, 1992; bipolar disorder. This is relevant because family members who
Beyer et al., 2003), but that the level of social support is similar to may (or may not) provide social support may also suffer from a
that of patients with other psychiatric diagnoses (Romans and diagnosis of bipolar disorder, and in this regard, it would be
McPherson, 1992; Dorz et al., 2003). This finding is similar to helpful to know more about the characteristics of study partici-
studies of adult attachment, where any psychiatric diagnosis pants’ ‘social convoy’. Finally, sampling issues pose significant
predicted insecure attachment. Sanchez-Moreno et al. (2010) and methodological constraints on the conclusions that can be drawn
Gutiérrez-Rojas et al. (2011) found that social support is negatively from these studies. Most have small sample sizes, some recruit
correlated with current depressive symptoms and previous num- only severely ill patients (e.g., recruiting from an acute psychiatric
ber of depressive episodes. Conversely, Romans and McPherson ward), some only analyze bipolar I patients, and some group
(1992) found that manic episodes were more detrimental to social bipolar I and II patients together, and many lack a control group.
support. It has been well established that adults with bipolar disorder
In contrast to cross-sectional studies, longitudinal studies often have trouble with social cognition or emotion perception,
provide insight on temporal relationships, including recurrence especially nonverbal emotion processing (Van Rheenen and
of mood episodes, symptoms severity, impairment, and levels of Rossell, 2013). This is important given that emotion perception
social support. In the extant literature, findings are mixed: social may be a skill that is first learned and practiced in early childhood
support does not influence episode relapse (Staner et al., 1997), and affects an individual throughout the lifespan (Cole et al., 1994),
social support influences manic or depressive episode relapse and that deficits in emotion perception have implications for
(O'Connell et al., 1985; Stefos et al., 1996; Kulhara et al., 1999, quality of life (Fulford et al., 2014). Early caregiving experiences
Johnson et al., 2003), social support only influences depressive in early childhood underlie the development of relationship
episode relapse (Johnson et al., 1999, 2000; Cohen et al., 2004; templates (or attachment-relevant working models), which in
Weinstock and Miller, 2010), and social support influences manic turn serve as emotion regulators in the context of social interac-
episode relapse (Strauss and Johnson, 2006). Two studies identi- tions and communications across the life span (Rosenblum et al.,
fied by our approach examined social support and lithium out- 2006). Thus, early caregiving experiences in individuals with
come, and found that strong social support significantly predicted genetic vulnerability for bipolar disorder could modify, possibly
good lithium outcome (O'Connell et al., 1985; Kulhara et al., 1999). through epigenetic mechanisms, the expression of genes underlying
The sole study that measured treatment alliance as a form of social neuro-circuitry relevant for emotion processing, and either ameliorate
S. Greenberg et al. / Psychiatry Research 219 (2014) 248–254 253

or heighten emotion understanding or processing deficits. Such Arnold, L.M., Witzeman, K.A., Swank, M.L., McElroy, S.L., Keck Jr., P.E., 2000. Health-
epigenetic shaping of emotion regulation patterns early in life could related quality of life using the SF-36 in patients with bipolar disorder
compared with patients with chronic back pain and the general population.
have implications throughout the lifespan for individuals with bipolar Journal of Affective Disorders 57 (1–3), 235–239, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
disorder, and while speculative at this point, investigation of such s0165-0327(99)00042-7 (doi:).
processes would be innovative and relevant. Asendorpf, J.B., Wilpers, S., 2000. Attachment security and available support:
closely linked relationship qualities. Journal of Social and Personal Relation-
Although limited, the extant literature regarding the frame- ships 17 (1), 115–138, http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0265407500171006.
work of social convoys and how early attachment shapes subse- Bartholomew, K., Horowitz, L.M., 1991. Attachment styles among young adults: a
quent relational security seems to have clinical implications test of a four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 61
(2), 226–244.
regarding the establishment of a secure working alliance. In Bella, T., Goldstein, T., Axelson, D., Obreja, M., Monk, K., Hickey, M.B., Goldstein, B.,
general, people seeking psychiatric services have better outcomes Brent, D., Diler, R.S., Kupfer, D., Sakolsky, D., Birmaher, B., 2011. Psychosocial
when they have a good relationship with providers (Martin et al., functioning in offspring of parents with bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective
Disorders 133 (1–2), 204–211, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2011.03.022.
2000). This holds true for people with bipolar disorder (Berk et al.,
Berk, M., Berk, L., Castle, D., 2004. A collaborative approach to the treatment
2004; Zeber et al., 2008). When patients come for treatment, it is alliance in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders 6 (6), 504–518, http://dx.doi.org/
likely that their attitudes they hold towards treatment and their 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2004.00154.x.
providers are shaped by previous relationship experiences, more Beyer, J.L., Kuchibhatla, M., Looney, C., Engstrom, E., Cassidy, F., Krishnan, K.R.R.,
2003. Social support in elderly patients with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders
specifically, their attachment relationships and social support 5 (1), 22–27, http://dx.doi.org/10.1034/j.1399-5618.2003.00016.x.
expectations. Their previous relationships may have influenced Bowlby, J., 1969. Attachment and Loss, Volume I: Attachment. Hogarth Press.
how trusting they are; this could have implications for the quality Bowlby, J., 1988. A Secure Base. Basic Books, New York.
Breier, A., Strauss, J., 1984. The role of social relationships in the recovery from
of the working alliance they establish with a treatment provider. It psychotic disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry 141 (8), 949–955.
is also important for clinicians to consider relational factors when Brennan, K.A., Clark, C.L., Shaver, P.R., 1998. Attachment Theory and Close Relation-
assessing their patients’ quality of relationships, whether roman- ships. Guilford Press, New York, NY.
Cannon, D.M., Klaver, J.K., Gandhi, S.K., Solorio, G., Peck, S.A., Erickson, K., N Akula, J.
tic, with their children, or with others. This is in line with current S., Eckelman, W.C., Furey, M.L., Sahakian, B.J., McMahon, F.J., Drevets, W.C., 2011.
evidence-based therapeutic approaches that emphasize the whole Genetic variation in cholinergic muscarinic-2 receptor gene modulates M2
family perspective (Eisner and Johnson, 2008; Luciano et al., 2012; receptor binding in vivo and accounts for reduced binding in bipolar disorder.
Molecular Psychiatry 16 (4), 407–418.
Miklowitz et al., 2013). The clinical implication of how social Champagne, F.A., Curley, J.P., 2005. How social experiences influence the brain.
convoys affect treatment with individuals suffering bipolar dis- Current Opinion in Neurobiology 15 (6), 704–709, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
order is an area that needs further research. conb.2005.10.001.
Champagne, F.A., Francis, D.D., Mar, A., Meaney, M.J., 2003. Variations in maternal
care in the rat as a mediating influence for the effects of environment on
development. Physiology and Behavior 79 (3), 359–371, http://dx.doi.org/
5. Conclusion 10.1016/s0031-9384(03)00149-5.
Chang, K., Steiner, H., Dienes, K., Adleman, N., Ketter, T., 2003a. Bipolar offspring: a
window into bipolar disorder evolution. Biological Psychiatry 53 (11), 945–951.
Social relationships are fundamental to human nature, and Chang, K., Steiner, H., Ketter, T., 2003b. Studies of offspring of parents with bipolar
decades of research support that early caregiving experiences disorder. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part C: Seminars in Medical
shape how individuals understand and process cognitions and Genetics 123C (1), 26–35, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.c.20011.
Clayton-Smith, J., Giblin, C., Smith, R.A., Dunn, C., Willatt, L., 2010. Familial 3q29
emotions relevant for social interactions, which subsequently may microdeletion syndrome providing further evidence of involvement of the
impact the quality of all social relationships across the life span. 3q29 region in bipolar disorder. Clinical Dysmorphology 19 (3), 128–132, http:
Individuals with bipolar disorder have impairments in social //dx.doi.org/10.1097/MCD.0b013e32833a1e3c.
Cohen-Woods, S., Craig, I., Gaysina, D., Gray, J., Gunasinghe, C., Craddock, N., Elkin,
relationships, as described in parenting contexts, romantic and
A., Jones, L., Kennedy, J., King, N., Korszun, A., Knight, J., Owen, M., Parikh, S.,
family relationships and broader societal functioning. Epigenetic Strauss, J., Sterne, A., Tozzi, F., Perry, J., Muglia, P., Vincent, J., McGuffin, P.,
processes where own early caregiving experiences interact with Farmer, A., 2010. The Bipolar Association Case-Control Study (BACCS) and meta-
genetic sensitivity could contribute to the establishment of such analysis: no association with the 5,10-Methylenetetrahydrofolate reductase
gene and bipolar disorder. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B:
social processing and communication impairments. The limited Neuropsychiatric Genetics 153B (7), 1298–1304, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
current data on attachment quality and social convoys in indivi- ajmg.b.31101.
duals with bipolar disorder supports the notion that more atten- Cohen, A.N., Hammen, C., Henry, R.M., Daley, S.E., 2004. Effects of stress and social
support on recurrence in bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders 82,
tion to this area of research is needed. 143–147.
Cohen, S., Wills, T.A., 1985. Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis.
Psychological Bulletin 98, 310–357.
Cole, P.M., Michel, M.K., Teti, L.O.D., 1994. The development of emotion regulation
Acknowledgments
and dysregulation: a clinical perspective. Monographs of the Society for
Research in Child Development 59 (2–3), 73–102, http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
This research was supported by the Heinz C. Prechter Bipolar j.1540-5834.1994.tb01278.x.
Collins, N.L., Read, S.J., 1990. Adult attachment, working models, and relationship
Research Fund at the University of Michigan Depression Center. quality in dating couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 58 (4),
644–663.
Cooke, R.G., Robb, J.C., Young, L.T., Joffe, R.T., 1996. Well-being and functioning in
References patients with bipolar disorder assessed using the MOS 20-ITEM short form (SF-
20). Journal of Affective Disorders 39 (2), 93–97, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
Agrawal, H.R., Gunderson, J., Holmes, B.M., Lyons-Ruth, K., 2004. Attachment 0165-0327(96)00016-x.
studies with borderline patients: a review. Harvard Review of Psychiatry 12 Diler, R.S., Birmaher, B., Axelson, D., Obreja, M., Monk, K., Hickey, M.B., Goldstein, B.,
(2), 94–104, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10673220490447218 (doi:). Goldstein, T., Sakolsky, D., Iyengar, S., Brent, D., Kupfer, D., 2011. Dimensional
Ainsworth, M.D.S., Wittig, B.A., 1969. Attachment and exploratory behavior of one- psychopathology in offspring of parents with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Dis-
year-olds in a strange situation. In: Foss, B.M. (Ed.), Determinants of Infant orders 13 (7–8), 670–678, http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-5618.2011.00966.x.
Behavior. Methuen, London, pp. 111–136. Dorz, S., Borgherini, G., Conforti, D., Scarso, C., Magni, G., 2003. Depression in
Angst, J., 1998. The emerging epidemiology of hypomania and bipolar II disorder. inpatients: bipolar vs unipolar. Psychological Reports 92 (3), 1031–1039, http:
Journal of Affective Disorders 50, 143–151. //dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.92.3.1031-1039.
Antonucci, T.C., Akiyama, H., 1994. Convoys of attachment and social relations in Duffy, A., Alda, M., Crawford, L., Milin, R., Grof, P., 2007. The early manifestations of
children, adolescents, and adults. In: Nestman, F., Hurrelmann, K. (Eds.), Social bipolar disorder: a longitudinal prospective study of the offspring of bipolar
Networks and Social Support in Childhood and Adolescence. Aldine de Gruyter, parents. Bipolar Disorders 9 (8), 828–838, http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-
Berlin and New York, pp. 37–52. 5618.2007.00421.x.
Armsden, G.G., Greenberg, M.T., 1987. The Inventory of Parent and Peer Attach- Eisner, L.R., Johnson, S.L., 2008. An acceptance-based psychoeducation intervention
ment: individual differences and their relationship to psychological well-being to reduce expressed emotion in relatives of bipolar patients. Behavior Therapy
in adolescense. Journal of Youth and Adolescence 16, 427–454. 39 (4), 375–385.
254 S. Greenberg et al. / Psychiatry Research 219 (2014) 248–254

Eng, W., Heimberg, R.G., Hart, T.A., Schneier, F.R., Liebowitz, M.R., 2001. Attachment Correlates of subjective and objective burden among caregivers of patients with
in individuals with social anxiety disorder: the relationship among adult bipolar disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 118 (1), 49–56, http://dx.doi.
attachment styles, social anxiety, and depression. Emotion 1 (4), 365–380. org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2008.01201.x.
Francis, D., Diorio, J., Liu, D., Meaney, M.J., 1999. Nongenomic transmission across Parker, G., Tupling, H., Brown, L.B., 1979. A parental bonding instrument. British
generations of maternal behavior and stress responses in the rat. Science 286 Journal of Medical Psychology 52, 1–10.
(5442), 1155–1158, http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.286.5442.1155. Petronis, A., 2003. Epigenetics and bipolar disorder: new opportunities and
Fulford, D., Peckham, A.D., Johnson, K., Johnson, S.L., 2014. Emotion perception and challenges. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part C: Seminars in Medical
quality of life in bipolar I disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders 152, 491–497. Genetics 123C (1), 65–75, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.c.20015.
Gilbert, P., McEwan, K., Hay, J., Irons, C., Cheung, M., 2007. Social rank and Post, R., 1992. Transduction of psychosocial stress into the neurobiology of
attachment in people with a bipolar disorder. Clinical Psychology and Psy- recurrent affective disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 149 (8), 999–1010.
chotherapy 14, 48–53. Radke-Yarrow, M., Cummings, E.M., Leon, K., Chapman, M., 1985. Patterns of
Gutiérrez-Rojas, L., Jurado, D., Gurpegui, M., 2011. Factors associated with work, attachment in two- and three-year-olds in normal families and families with
social life and family life disability in bipolar disorder patients. Psychiatry parental depression. Child Development 56 (4), 884–893.
Research 186 (2), 254–260. Robb, J.C., Cooke, R.G., Devins, G.M., Young, L.T., Joffe, R.T., 1997. Quality of life and
Hipwell, A.E., Goossens, F.A., Melhuish, E.C., Kumar, R., 2000. Severe maternal lifestyle disruption in euthymic bipolar disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research
psychopathology and infant–mother attachment. Development and Psycho- 31 (5), 509–517.
pathology 12, 157–175. Romans, S.E., McPherson, H.M., 1992. The social networks of bipolar affective
Jacobvitz, D., Curran, M., Moller, N., 2002. Measurement of adult attachment: the disorder patients. Journal of Affective Disorders 25, 221–228.
place of self-report and interview methodologies. Attachment and Human Rosenblum, K.L., Dayton, C., McDonough, S.C., 2006. Communicating feelings: links
Development 4 (2), 207–215, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14616730210154225. between mothers’ representations of their infants, parenting, and infant
Johnson, L., Lundstrom, O., Aberg-Wistedt, A., Mathe, A.A., 2003. Social support in bipolar emotional development. In: Mayseless, O. (Ed.), Parenting Representations:
disorder: its relevance to remission and relapse. Bipolar Disorders 5, 129–137. Theory, Research and Clinical Implications. Cambridge University Press, New
Johnson, S.L., Meyer, B., Winett, C., Small, J., 2000. Social support and self-esteem York, pp. 109–148.
predict changes in bipolar depression but not mania. Journal of Affective Rosenblum, K.L., Dayton, C., Muzik, M., 2009. Infant social and emotional develop-
Disorders 58 (1), 79–86. ment: emergent competence in a relational context. In: Zeanah, C. (Ed.),
Johnson, S.L., Winett, C.A., Meyer, B., Greenhouse, W.J., Miller, I., 1999. Social Handbook of Infant Mental Health, 3rd ed. Guilford Press, New York,
support and the course of bipolar disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 108 pp. 80–103.
(4), 558–566. Rosenfarb, I.S., Becker, J., Khan, A., 1994. Perceptions of parental and peer
Joyce, P.R., 1984. Parental bonding in bipolar affective disorder. Journal of Affective attachments by women with mood disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology
Disorders 7, 319–324. 103 (4), 637–644.
Kahn, R.S., Antonucci, T.C., 1980. Convoys over the life course: attachment, roles, Sanchez-Moreno, J., Martinez-Aran, A., Gadelrab, H.F., Cabello, M., Torrent, C., del
and social support. In: Baltes, P.B., Brim, O. (Eds.), Life-Span Development and Mar Bonnin, C., Ferrer, M., Leonardi, M., Ayuso-Mateos, J.L., Vieta, E., 2010. The
Behavior. Academic Press, New York. role and impact of contextual factors on functioning in patients with bipolar
Kökçü, F., Kesebir, S., 2010. The relationship between attachment style, and
disorder. Disability and Rehabilitation 32 (S1), S94–S104, http://dx.doi.org/
temperament, personality, and bipolar symptoms: a controlled study on bipolar
10.3109/09638288.2010.520810.
patients and their children. Turkish Journal of Psychiatry 21 (4), 309–318.
Schenkel, L.S., West, A.E., Harral, E.M., Patel, N.B., Pavuluri, M.N., 2008. Parent–child
Kulhara, P., Basu, D., Mattoo, S.K., Sharan, P., Chopra, R., 1999. Lithium prophylaxis of
interactions in pediatric bipolar disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology 64 (4),
recurrent bipolar affective disorder: long-term outcome and its psychosocial
422–437, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20470.
correlates. Journal of Affective Disorders 54 (1), 87–96.
Shippee, N.D., Shah, N.D., Williams, M.D., Moriarty, J.P., Frye, M.A., Ziegenfuss, J.Y.,
Levitt, M.J., 1991. Attachment and close relationships: a life-span perspective. In:
2011. Differences in demographic composition and in work, social, and
Gerwitz, J.L., Kurtines, W.M. (Eds.), Intersections With Attachment. Lawrence
functional limitations among the populations with unipolar depression and
Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 183–205.
bipolar disorder: results from a nationally representative sample. Health and
Luciano, M., Del Vecchio, V., Giacco, D., De Rosa, C., Malangone, C., Fiorillo, A., 2012.
Quality of Life Outcomes 9, 1–9.
A ‘family affair’? The impact of family psychoeducational interventions on
Shorey, H.S., Snyder, C.R., 2006. The role of adult attachment styles in psycho-
depression. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics 12 (1), 83–91.
pathology and psychotherapy outcomes. Review of General Psychology 10 (1),
Main, M., Kaplan, N., Cassidy, J., 1985. Security in infancy, childhood, and adult-
1–20, http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.10.1.1.
hood: a move to the level of representation. Monographs of the Society for
Staner, L., Tracy, A., Dramaix, M., Genevrois, C., Vanderelst, M., Vilane, A., Bauwens,
Research in Child Development 50 (1–2), 66–104, http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/
F., Pardoen, D., Mendlewicz, J., 1997. Clinical and psychosocial predictors of
3333827.
Marazziti, D., Dell'Osso, B., Catena Dell'Osso, M., Consoni, G., Del Debbio, A., Mungai, recurrence in recovered bipolar and unipolar depressives: a one-year controlled
F., Vivarelli, L., Albanese, F., Piccinni, A., Rucci, P., Dell'Osso, L., 2007. Romantic prospective study. Psychiatry Research 69 (1), 39–51, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
attachment in patients with mood and anxiety disorders. CNS Spectrums 12 s0165-1781(96)03021-1.
(10), 751–756. Stefos, G., Bauwens, F., Staner, L., Pardoen, D., Mendlewicz, J., 1996. Psychosocial
Martin, D.J., Garske, J.P., Davis, M.K., 2000. Relation of the therapeutic alliance with predictors of major affective recurrences in bipolar disorder: a 4-year long-
outcome and other variables: a meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting and itudinal study of patients on prophylactic treatment. Acta Psychiatrica Scandi-
Clinical Psychology 68 (3), 438–450, http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006x.68.3.438. navica 93 (6), 420–426, http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1996.tb10672.x.
McGowan, P., Kato, T., 2008. Epigenetics in mood disorders. Environmental Health Strauss, J.L., Johnson, S.L., 2006. Role of treatment alliance in the clinical manage-
and Preventive Medicine 13 (1), 16–24, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12199-007- ment of bipolar disorder: stronger alliances prospectively predict fewer manic
0002-0. symptoms. Psychiatry Research 145 (2), 215–223.
McInnis, M.G., Burmeister, M., DePaulo, J.R., 2007. Major mood disorders. In: Szyf, M., 2011. The early life social environment and DNA methylation: DNA
Rimoin, D.L., Connor, J.L., Emery, A.E.H., Pyeritz, R.E. (Eds.), Emery and Rimoin's methylation mediating the long-term impact of social environments early in
Principles and Practice of Medical Genetics, 5th ed. Elsevier, Philadelphia, life. Epigenetics 6 (8), 971–978.
pp. 2615–2628. Tohen, M., Hennen, J., Zarate, C.M., Baldessarini, R.J., Strakowski, S.M., Stoll, A.L.,
Miklowitz, D.J., Schneck, C.D., Singh, M.K., Taylor, D.O., George, E.L., Cosgrove, V.E., Faedda, G.L., Suppes, T., Gebre-Medhin, P., Cohen, B.M., 2000. Two-year
Howe, M.E., Dickinson, L.M., Garber, J., Chang, K.D., 2013. Early intervention for syndromal and functional recovery in 219 cases of first-episode major affective
symptomatic youth at risk for bipolar disorder: a randomized trial of family- disorder with psychotic features. American Journal of Psychiatry 157, 220–228.
focused therapy. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Van Rheenen, T.E., Rossell, S.L., 2013. Is the non-verbal behavioural emotion-
Psychiatry 52 (2), 121–131. processing profile of bipolar disorder impaired? A critical review. Acta
Miller, L., Kramer, R., Warner, V., Wickramaratne, P., Weissman, M., 1997. Inter- Psychiatrica Scandinavica 128 (3), 163–178, http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
generational transmission of parental bonding among women. Journal of the acps.12125.
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 36 (8), 1134–1135. Wai Wan, M., Green, J., 2009. The impact of maternal psychopathology on child–
Morriss, R.K., van der Gucht, E., Lancaster, G., Bentall, R.P., 2009. Adult attachment mother attachment. Archives of Women's Mental Health 12, 123–134.
in bipolar I disorder. The British Psychological Society 82, 267–277. Weinstock, L.M., Miller, I.W., 2010. Psychosocial predictors of mood symptoms
Murray, C., Lopez, A., 1997. Global mortality, disability, and the contribution of risk 1 year after acute phase treatment of bipolar I disorder. Comprehensive
factors: global burden of disease study. The Lancet 349 (9063), 1436–1442. Psychiatry 51 (5), 497–503, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2010.02.001.
O'Connell, R., Mayo, J., Eng, L., Jones, J., Gabel, R., 1985. Social support and long-term Zahn-Waxler, C., Chapman, M., Cummings, E.M., 1984. Cognitive and social
lithium outcome. The British Journal of Psychiatry 147, 272–275. development in infants and toddlers with a bipolar parent. Child Psychiatry
Obegi, J.H., Berant, E., 2010. Attachment Theory and Research in Clinical Work with and Human Development 15 (2), 75–85, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf00706165.
Adults. Guilford Press, New York. Zeber, J.E., Copeland, L.A., Good, C.B., Fine, M.J., Bauer, M.S., Kilbourne, A.M., 2008.
Ostacher, M.J., Nierenberg, A.A., Iosifescu, D.V., Eidelman, P., Lund, H.G., Ametrano, Therapeutic alliance perceptions and medication adherence in patients with
R.M., Kaczynski, R., Calabrese, J., Miklowitz, D.J., Sachs, G.S., Perlick, D.A., 2008. bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders 107 (1), 53–62.

You might also like