Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Journal of Business Research 53 (2001) 103 ± 109

Age differences in memory for radio advertisements:


the role of mnemonics
Malcolm C. Smitha,*, Mark R. Phillips Jrb
a
Department of Marketing, Faculty of Management, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, MB, Canada R3T 5V4
b
University of Oregon, Eugene, OR 97403, USA
Accepted 1 June 1999

Abstract

This study examines age differences in memory for radio advertisements. Younger and older adults listened to a bogus radio program.
Half of each age category listened to a version of the program embedded with two ads utilizing a simple rhyming mnemonic device. The
other half of each age category listened to the same program, but with ads without any rhyming mnemonics. Various measures of recall and
recognition of the target ads were taken both immediately after exposure and after a 7-day delay. While no age differences in memory
emerged, mnemonic versions of the ads resulted in better scores for overall unaided memory, prompted brand recall, and overall memory for
both age groups. There were no differences in memory scores over the 7-day delay, nor were there any interaction effects. D 2001 Elsevier
Science Inc. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Memory; Age differences; Radio advertisements; Mnemonics

Although our society is rapidly aging, the research Research has also been conducted on learning aids which
literature concerning issues on marketing to older adults is might help older consumers remember the content of text
surprisingly sparse. It has been estimated that the ``mature'' and advertisements. Mnemonics, a learning aid that utilizes
market growth rate will be anywhere from 11% (Moschis, such things as rhymes, is used here as a learning tool to see
1992, p. 26) to 18% (Ostroff, 1989) during the last decade of if they will help consumers, and older consumers in parti-
this century. Subsequently, it will be growing at a faster rate cular, to better remember the content of ads.
than the population as a whole. Recently, however, market- Finally, this study examined not only immediate recall of
ing academics have taken notice of this emerging market ad content, but also delayed recall. This is an important
(e.g., Cole and Gaeth, 1990; Moschis, 1992; Tepper, 1994; factor for advertisers and delayed recall has rarely been used
Law and Craik, 1995 ; Smith, 1995; Sorce, 1995). Much of in the literature.
this research has dealt with the cognitive functioning of
older consumers, especially memory for advertising content
with television and print used almost exclusively as the 1. Aging and memory
media for these studies. Radio has been scarcely used, with
the notable exception of Cole et al. (1995, p. 617), who note The traditional view of aging and cognition has been
that ``because radio is an audio medium, theories developed that, with age, cognitive deficits emerge. One topic that has
to understand how television and print advertising work been heavily researched in the field of gerontology is that of
may not generalize to radio advertising.'' Therefore, one memory and aging. Findings have indicated that memory
objective of this study was to examine if any age differences for text decreases with age (see Light, 1991). This has often
in memory for advertising emerge when radio is utilized. been explained by means of a shrinkage of working memory
capacity and resources (e.g., Salthouse, 1992).
In the realm of marketing, much of the research inves-
tigating aging and memory has dealt with memory for
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +1-204-474-9484. advertising. Again, most of these studies have found that

0148-2963/01/$ ± see front matter D 2001 Elsevier Science Inc. All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 1 4 8 - 2 9 6 3 ( 9 9 ) 0 0 0 7 9 - X
104 M.C. Smith, M.R. Phillips / Journal of Business Research 53 (2001) 103±109

with age, memory for the content of ads declines (Stephens, (rhyming) without benefit of any prior training; (2) utilizing
1982; Cole and Houston, 1987; Law and Craik, 1995). an auditory rather than a visual medium; (3) conducting the
An audio medium such as radio provides no chance to test in an environment comfortable to subjects, thereby
self-pace the speed of input stimuli, nor repeat the ex- reducing anxiety; and (4) testing both immediate and
posure to the stimuli as processing is taking place. In delayed recall.
addition, radio avoids any interaction of pictorial and text
processing that may occur with television or print ads with
pictures (see Leigh, 1991). Thus, cognitive processing of 3. Timing of memory measures
radio ads must be carried out in a ``one-way,'' linear
fashion. As Cole et al. (1995) point out, a medium such Most often memory tasks are performed immediately
as speech (as in radio) requires continuous working mem- after the stimuli were seen/heard in a learning and memory
ory processing. Given the findings that older adults may study; however, the timing of the memory task has been
have a decrease in working memory capacity, one would shown to have an effect on memory performance. Findings
expect that memory for the content of radio advertising have been reported which demonstrated that age differences
would be superior for younger adults when compared to in recall for text diminish over time (Dixon et al., 1982;
older adults. Sinnot, 1986), while others have found that such age
differences are magnified over time (e.g., Cohen and Faulk-
ener, 1984).
2. Mnemonics

The goal of using mnemonics is to improve encoding 4. Hypotheses


efficiency. While this subject has received scant attention
in the marketing/advertising literature, it has been an area In order to assess whether older adults suffer memory
of focus in other disciplines. A recent review of a psychol- deficits relative to younger adults for the content of radio
ogy data base revealed 469 separate studies dealing with advertisements and what effect mnemonics will have on
mnemonics and memory. Various mnemonic strategies this possible differential, the following hypotheses were
including peg word, first letter, key word, method of loci, formulated.
link/chain, figure alphabet, Yodai, and rhymes have been
used with various subjects and disparate environmental H1. With radio ads, older adults will have lower scores
situations (see Malhotra, 1991 for a general overview of than younger adults for:
these mnemonic techniques). In addition, music, imagery,
H1a. unaided recall, aided recall, and recognition of the
and story telling have been investigated as mnemonics
advertising message;
(Hill et al., 1991; Scruggs and Brigham, 1991; Yalch,
1991; Richardson and Rossan, 1994). Consistent across H1b. unaided recall, aided recall, and recognition of the
all such subject groups and test situations is the implicit brand/company name;
assumption that improving encoding efficiency will im-
H1c. unaided recall, aided recall, and recognition of the
prove memory.
product category.
Yesavage et al. (1989) offer the following five limita-
tions of mnemonics research involving older adults: (1) H2. The use of mnemonics will result in increased levels
mnemonic techniques are too complicated for the older of memory in terms of unaided recall, aided recall and
adults to effectively utilize; (2) many studies rely on visual recognition for both younger and older adults.
images and visual image associations whereas older people H3. Unaided recall, aided recall, and recognition scores
have difficulty producing and remembering visual images; will be higher for both age groups immediately after
(3) high anxiety levels tend to depress test performances exposure than after a 7-day delay.
and older adults are often resistant and/or anxious about
using new techniques and participation in (new) test Encoding efficiency should increase with the use of
situations; (4) some findings suggest that encoding proces- mnemonics. This, coupled with the fact that older adults
sing may be more superficial in older adults vis-a-vis their may suffer encoding deficiencies, is the basis for the fourth
younger counterparts, which suggests that any improve- hypothesis.
ment in memory due to mnemonics may be restricted to
the short term; and (5) any treatment effects may be H4. There will be a significant interaction between age
negligible, which begs the question, ``Is mnemonic training and mnemonic treatment on memory for the brand/company
worth the time and resources expended to realize minus- names in the target ads (i.e., the older adults will more
cule improvements?'' greatly benefit from the use of mnemonics vis-a-vis the
This study attempts to address the aforementioned younger adults in terms of memory for the target brand/
limitations by: (1) using a simple mnemonic technique company names).
M.C. Smith, M.R. Phillips / Journal of Business Research 53 (2001) 103±109 105

5. Methods except for the target ads under investigation. These target
ads were for a fictitious travel agency (``Blue Sky Travel''),
The effects of age and type of advertisement were and a fictitious oral rinse (``Cavoloss'') and were embedded
examined in a 2 (age group)  2 (message type) between in the middle section of the program to avoid primacy and
subjects factorial design. Age was divided into younger and recency effects. One version's target ads were embedded
older adults and participants from each age group were with a rhyming mnemonic device (mnemonic ads) which
randomly assigned to one of two types of advertising was associated with the brand/company name. These
treatment (mnemonic and non-mnemonic) using radio as rhymes were ``Fly high with Blue Sky,'' and ``Toss the
the medium. Memory for advertising was tested both floss, use Cavoloss.'' The other version contained the same
immediately after exposure to the stimulus material and ad devoid of any rhyming mnemonic (non-mnemonic ads).
after a 7-day delay. In total, four variations of the tape were made: two with
mnemonic target ads and two with non-mnemonic target
5.1. Participants ads. In each of these cases, the order of target ads was
reversed to avoid ordering effects. Care was also taken to
One-hundred fourteen young adults in introductory busi- use music and a generic format that was deemed as non-
ness and communications classes from a large western offensive as possible to both age groups.
university in the USA volunteered to participate in the
study. Ninety-eight older adults were recruited over the 5.3. Procedures
telephone from the same city from the membership list of
Senior Health Services Class supplied by a local hospital. Of The older participants were divided into four groups and
the older adults recruited, 58 kept their appointments for the studied over a 2-week period. In order to provide a setting
designated day and time of the study. that would be familiar and comfortable to these participants,
their study site was the area where they usually held their
5.2. Stimuli Senior Health Services Class seminars. Each group came to
the study site on a designated day of the week and, after
Participants were exposed to one of two variations of a being exposed to the radio program stimulus, a self-paced
professionally prepared cassette tape of a 10-min bogus questionnaire was administered which was designed to
radio program. They were told that the excerpt they would measure recall and recognition of the target ads. Specific
hear was a test tape of a new format for an unnamed radio questions pertaining to this information were disguised
station. Their opinions were being solicited to help station among other questions that asked about the participants'
management determine if this format was viable. The two impressions of the program. Upon completion of the ques-
variations of the radio program were virtually identical tionnaire, the participants were excused and asked to return

Table 1
Means for memory categories by age, version, and time
Factors
Younger Older
Mnemonic Non-mnemonic Mnemonic Non-mnemonic
Dependent variables Immediate Delayed Immediate Delayed Immediate Delayed Immediate Delayed
Unaided product recall 1.80 1.47 1.22 1.38 1.48 1.68 1.04 1.14
Unaided brand recall 0.68 0.98 0.40 0.35 0.48 0.55 0.70 0.59
Unaided message recall 0.88 0.71 0.89 0.53 0.65 0.55 0.44 0.23

Aided product recall 1.46 1.76 1.35 1.65 1.23 1.18 0.85 0.91
Aided brand recall 0.69 1.10 0.56 0.62 0.77 0.86 0.52 0.82
Aided message recall 1.22 0.88 1.22 1.00 1.03 0.82 0.78 0.73

Prompted brand recall 0.93 1.08 0.65 0.47 0.81 0.73 0.74 0.55

Product recognition 3.22 3.43 3.13 3.06 3.16 3.09 2.89 3.05
Brand recognition 2.63 2.78 2.47 2.59 2.77 2.91 2.44 2.36

Overall unaided recall 3.36 3.16 2.51 2.26 2.61 2.77 2.19 1.95
Overall aided recall 3.37 3.73 3.13 3.26 3.03 2.86 2.15 2.45
Overall recognition 5.85 6.20 5.60 5.65 5.94 6.00 5.33 5.41
Overall memory 12.58 13.10 11.24 11.18 11.58 11.64 9.67 9.82
106 M.C. Smith, M.R. Phillips / Journal of Business Research 53 (2001) 103±109

Table 2
ANOVA results for major composite memory scores
Dependent variables
Overall unaided recall Overall aided recall Prompted brand recall Overall recognition Overall memory
Factors F p F p F p F p F p
Age 2.034 0.155 3.461 0.064 0.343 0.559 0.245 0.621 2.667 0.104
Version 6.097 0.014 1.531 0.217 4.981 0.026 2.686 0.102 4.667 0.032
Timing 0.213 0.645 0.246 0.621 0.069 0.793 0.340 0.560 0.067 0.795

Age  version 0.175 0.676 0.189 0.664 1.042 0.308 0.139 0.710 0.030 0.863
Age  timing 0.085 0.771 0.065 0.799 0.177 0.674 0.057 0.811 0.008 0.928
Version  timing 0.071 0.790 0.001 0.981 0.808 0.369 0.124 0.725 0.049 0.825

Age  version  timing 0.065 0.799 0.195 0.659 0.130 0.719 0.073 7.87 0.042 0.838

1 week later. The first session took approximately 40 min to participant was calculated by summing the composite un-
complete. Of the 58 older adults that were initially tested, 44 aided recall, aided recall, recognition, and prompted brand/
returned the following week. They were not told what the company name recall scores which gave a total of five major
second session would entail. During the session in week 2, dependent memory scores.
an identical questionnaire was administered to measure
delayed recall and recognition. In addition, demographic
data were collected. The session also took approximately 40 6. Results
min to complete.
The younger adults were studied in much the same The younger adults had an average age of 18.88 years
manner. Each class was divided approximately in half and (S.D. = 1.04 years) and the older adults had an average age
one group was exposed to the mnemonic version of the of 71.46 years (S.D. = 6.42 years). The older group had
program and the other group was exposed to the non- significantly more years of education (mean = 13.9) than the
mnemonic version. One week later, the questionnaire was younger adults (mean = 12.6) (t = 6.09, p < 0.001). In
re-administered to the younger adults (83 in number) to addition, there was no difference in how well each age
measure delayed recall and recognition. As with the older group liked the radio program. Using a seven-point scale
adults, they did not know what the second session would (where 1 = strongly dislike and 7 = strongly like) the mean
entail. Demographic data were also collected at this time. ratings were 3.51 for the older adults and 3.68 for the
Each session took the younger adults approximately 30 min students (t = 0.77, p > 0.05).
to complete. As seen in Table 2, the results of the ANOVA indicated
that there were no age differences in memory for any of the
5.4. Dependent measures five major composite memory scores. In all cases, older
adults performed as well as the younger adults in recalling
Memory (immediate and delayed) was assessed in the and recognizing content of the ads used. When education
following categories for each ad: (1) unaided recall of the was entered into the same ANOVA analysis as a covariate,
product category, brand/company name, and advertising no age difference emerged.
message; (2) aided recall of the product category, brand/ The mnemonic versions of the ads produced better
company name, and the advertising message; (3) recogni- memory scores in three of the five major composite depen-
tion of the product category and the brand/company name; dent variables. Overall unaided recall (F(1,292) = 6.10, p <
and (4) prompted recall of the brand/company name. 0.05), prompted brand recall (F(1,291) = 4.98, p < 0.05),
Participants received two points for a correct answer, one and overall memory (F(1,291) = 4.67, p < 0.05). All were
for a partially correct answer, and zero for an incorrect or no significantly higher for the mnemonic versions of the ads
answer. The scores for each ad were added together. (The than for the non-mnemonic versions.
means for each of these memory scores are presented in There were no differences in memory scores due to
Table 1.) A composite score was then calculated for unaided timing. In addition, there were no significant two- or
recall, aided recall, and recognition by summing the appro- three-way interactions.
priate product category, brand/company name, and advertis-
ing message scores. For example, an overall unaided recall
composite score was calculated for each participant by 7. Discussion
summing the recall scores for unaided product category,
unaided brand/company name, and unaided adverting mes- Contrary to Hypothesis 1, our older participants per-
sage for both ads. Finally, an overall memory score for each formed as well on the memory tasks as their younger
M.C. Smith, M.R. Phillips / Journal of Business Research 53 (2001) 103±109 107

counterparts which, in and of itself, is an interesting older subjects. To understand why this may have happened,
finding. Given that past research has found that age it is important to remember the five limitations identified by
differences often exist, the question arises, ``Why were Yesavage et al. (1989) in light of the methods utilized in
there no age differences found in this study?'' One possible this study.
explanation for this can be found in the gerontology First, a mnemonic strategy (rhyming) was chosen that
literature which claims that the social context of the was simple to administer and understand and was non-
memory task may play a roll in memory performance threatening in that it required no prior training and was
(Adams et al., 1995, 1997; Blanchard-Fields and Abeles, familiar to participants of all ages. One problem asso-
1996). That is, when the environment or the social setting ciated with mnemonic strategies, particularly in regard to
of the memory task is relevant to the participant, they may older adults, is that more sophisticated techniques require
be motivated to process the information and encode it for a level of learning and memory in order to apply the
later retrieval. It may be that when the setting and the task artifice to memory tasks. In other words, participants are
are conducive to motivate the subjects to process the required to remember how to remember, adding another
information presented, age differences previously reported level of difficulty to the task. This learning barrier is not
in the literature may disappear. In this study, both the only problematic for older adults but also unrealistic for
younger and older adults undertook the memory task in advertisers: how can such training be implemented?
an environment that was familiar to them and was non- Rhyming is both a ``natural'' mnemonic requiring no
threatening. The older adults may have been motivated to training and is applicable across media and, therefore,
process the information in the ads due to the fact that they attractive to advertisers.
were recruited, not for a university study, but for research Second, various studies have suggested that older people
being conducted by the group to which they belonged. The find it difficult to utilize visual imagery for memory tasks
fact that management personnel from the group were (e.g., Winograd and Simon, 1980). Consequently, a verbal
present and introduced the experimenters as conducting mnemonic (auditory rhyming) within a verbal medium
co-operative research with them, may have further still put (radio) was chosen for this study.
them at ease. This finding is in agreement with gerontolo- Third, it has been well documented that raised anxiety
gical research which has manipulated the social context of levels can depress test scores (e.g., Spielberger et al., 1979;
the memory task (e.g., Adams et al., 1995). Yesavage and Jacob, 1984). It is commonly believed that
Another explanation is that the older adults possessed a this anxiety effect tends to be exacerbated among older
relatively high level of education. While this variable is a adults. That is, older adults tend toward high anxiety when
known correlate of memory for text material, it remains to asked to utilize techniques in unfamiliar (and unnatural)
be seen if the same effect is observed for materials which are testing situations (Yesavage et al., 1989). As discussed
learned solely via an auditory means. above, care was taken to ameliorate any anxiety among
It may also be that the memory task was an easy one the senior participants.
for both age groups in that it may not have taxed their In addition, a radio format was also chosen that would
cognitive resources to the point that deficits would occur. be, if not wildly admired, less likely to offend subjects. This
It is important to note, however, that the ``learning'' of was not an easy task as the format and musical selection
the material was via a passive means. That is, the had to be identical for both elderly and young subjects,
participants in this study did not know that they would disparate groups by definition with obvious taste differ-
have to recall the content of the target ads. Consequently, ences. Such a result was seemingly achieved, as the two
even though the task may not have been ``easy'' per se, it groups' overall ratings of the program were equally neutral.
was still real-to-life. In fact, memory of such forms of In sum, it is believed that the test was conducted in a
advertising may not be naturally complex and demanding manner and in surroundings conducive to low anxiety levels
and, therefore, not push the limits of cognitive resources among older participants.
of any age group. Fourth, Yesavage et al. (1989) claim that older adults
Although no age-related differences in memory for the may only realize short term benefits from mnemonic stra-
content of the target ads were found, this finding supports tegies. This problem was addressed by taking a 7-day delay
the notion put forth by Sorce (1995) that cognitive decline is memory measurement (Hypothesis 3). Interestingly,
not necessarily an inevitable result of aging per se. Further although Hypothesis 3 was not supported, certain measure-
research is needed, however, to investigate to what extent ment scores were actually higher after 7 days than the initial
the social context in which the memory task is undertaken immediate scores. This suggests that contrary to previous
can affect memory scores as well as the effect of education findings, which indicated that older adults tend to encode
level on memory for auditory materials. more ``superficially'' than young people, the rhyming mne-
While Hypothesis 2 was supported in that the mnemonic monic used in this study may have provided an adequate
versions resulted in better memory scores for both ages, level of processing ``depth.'' If this were not the case we
Hypothesis 4 was not supported. That is, mnemonics served would have expected younger participants' delayed recall to
to increase memory scores equally for both the younger and both be higher than that of the older adults and to have
108 M.C. Smith, M.R. Phillips / Journal of Business Research 53 (2001) 103±109

eroded to a lesser extent. Instead, younger and older adults' intuitive to assume that multiple exposures will decrease
scores were equivalent and any anticipated erosion in memory scores.
memory failed to occur.
Certainly, no one would claim that an erosion of memory 7.1. Managerial implications
for advertising will not occur over the long term, mnemo-
nics or not. However, this preliminary finding does suggest To suggest that older adults comprise a target segment
that a mnemonic that is easy to use and does not require is, in a sense, specious. Obviously, this group is comprised
training, which is particularly applicable to advertising of a broad cross-section both in terms of demographic and
endeavors, and which is applied in a manner appropriate psychographic variables. However, target segments that
for older adults' usage can assist them in encoding and, contain an older adult component may benefit from
consequently, in recall of advertising messages. utilizing mnemonic devices. This effect may be particu-
Fifth, Yesavage et al. (1989) state that any increases in larly important for products or companies that have low
memory realized by the use of mnemonics may be so name recognition among target subjects, for the introduc-
negligible as to discourage the effort necessary to attain tion of new products, and/or for modifications or usage
the gain. We leave it to the readers to determine exactly innovations to existing product offerings. In these cases,
what constitutes a negligible gain, but assert that the effort increasing memory for the product/company/usage is a
required to realize this gain is likewise negligible. In short, primary goal and this research indicates that, at least for
the rhyme technique used herein is, by its very simplicity, radio advertising, a simple mnemonic will assist such
appropriate for the everyday use of seniors. The increased memory retention.
memory scores for the mnemonic versus non-mnemonic This research does not deal with buying behavior or other
version suggest that the rhymes had an effect on encoding. issues tangentially related to advertising; it specifically
The stability of memory scores after a 7-day delay suggests focuses on memory for advertising messages and suggests
that the encoding occurred at a sufficient depth so as to that where remembering simple concepts such as company/
enhance more than just short-term recall. The ability of product name and appropriate usage are preeminent, such as
advertisers to re-enhance processing through additional the examples above, that managers would be well advised to
exposures argues in favor of this technique. Certainly include simple mnemonic devices within their advertising
further research is needed in this area, but this is a promising messages, particularly when all or part of their target
first step. segment consists of older adults.
In addition to addressing the five points above, care was
taken to make the experiment as external and ecologically
valid as possible. Certainly the experimental situation itself 8. Conclusions
was artificial. But within the situational condition, other
factors were manipulated to approach realism. The radio Previous research has indicated that in general, older
program itself was prepared by a professional disk jockey adults remember less of the content of advertising than
and was consistent in terms of format and content with local younger adults. Another body of research suggests that
radio broadcasts. Participants of both ages were asked to mnemonic techniques that are too complicated and/or utilize
listen carefully to the program but were not discouraged visual imagery may disproportionately assist younger
from talking during its broadcast and judging from the adults. This study suggests that memory decline for radio
number of comments and interactions that took place during ads may not be inevitable and that mnemonics that are
the experiment, it seems likely participants were not inhib- ``older adult friendly'' will enhance memory for both
ited or intimidated by the process. This is consistent with younger and older adults. Given the aging of the baby
actual radio usage where the radio is often background, or boom generation, such findings should be of interest not
tertiary, to other activities. only to advertisers but also to government, public service,
The delayed recall condition was perhaps more ecologi- and senior-oriented service entities of all kinds. Certainly
cally valid than the initial exposure in that it was conducted simple mnemonic techniques warrant further research vis-a-
with much less pretense or manipulation. Simply, with no vis older consumers.
warning, respondents were asked to demonstrate what they
remembered from the week before. Certainly, the older
participants knew they were meeting for a reason, but had References
no idea as to the purpose; the young adults were not even
informed that they would be asked to respond after 7 days Adams C, Smith MC, Gadden CP, Perlmutter M. Qualitative age differ-
Ð those that attended class and had been involved in the ences in memory: a story retold by younger and older adults. Unpub-
initial measure were asked to participate. lished Manuscript. Eugene, OR, 97403: University of Oregon, 1995.
Adams C, Smith MC, Nyquist L, Perlmutter M. Adult age-group differ-
An obvious failing of the research design is that it did not ences in recall for the literal and interpretive meanings of narrative text.
incorporate multiple exposures into the experiment. While J Gerontol: Psychol Sci 1997;52B:P187 ± P95.
impossible to assert based on this study, it seems counter- Blanchard-Fields F, Abeles RP. Social cognition and aging. In: Birren JE &
M.C. Smith, M.R. Phillips / Journal of Business Research 53 (2001) 103±109 109

Wilson BA, editors. Handbook of the Psychology of Aging (4th edn.). Salthouse TA. Mechanisms of Age-Cognition Relations in Adulthood.
San Diego, CA: Academic Press, 1996, pp. 150 ± 61. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1992.
Cohen G, Faulkener D. Memory for text: some age differences in the nature Scruggs TE, Brigham FJ. Utility of musical mnemonics. Percept Mot Skills
of information that is retained after listening to text. In: Bouman H & 1991;72:881 ± 2.
Bouwhuis D, editors. Attention and Performance. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawr- Sinnot J. Prospective/intentional everyday memory: effect of age and pas-
ence Erlbaum Associates, 1984, pp. 501 ± 14. sage of time. Psychol Aging 1986;1(2):110 ± 6.
Cole CA, Castellano NM, Schum D. Quantitative and qualitative differ- Smith MC. Age differences in memory for narrative and expository print
ences in older and younger consumers' recall of radio advertising. In: advertisements. In: Hansen F, editor. European Advances in Consu-
Kardes FR & Sujan M, editors. Advances in Consumer Research. Pro- mer Research. Provo, UT: Association of Consumer Research, 1995,
vo, UT: Association of Consumer Research, 1995, pp. 617 ± 21. pp. 109 ± 112.
Cole CA, Gaeth GJ. Cognitive and age-related differences in the ability to Sorce P. Cognitive competence of older consumers. Psychol Mark
use nutritional information in a complex environment. J Mark Res 1995;12(6):467 ± 80.
(May) 1990;27:175 ± 84. Spielberger CD, Gonzales HP, Fletcher T. Test anxiety reduction, learning
Cole CA, Houston MJ. Encoding and media effects on consumer learning strategies, and academic performance. In: O'Neill HF & Spielberger
deficiencies in the elderly. J Mark Res (February) 1987;24:55 ± 63. CD, editors. Cognitive and Affective Learning Strategies. New York:
Dixon RA, Simon EW, Nowak CA, Hultsch DF. Text recall in adulthood as Academic Press, 1979, pp. 111 ± 131.
a function of level of information, input modality, and delay interval. J Stephens N. The effectiveness of time-compressed television advertise-
Gerontol 1982;37:358 ± 64. ments with older adults. J Advertising 1982;8(4):48 ± 55,76.
Hill RD, Allen C, McWhorter C. Stories as a mnemonic aid for older Tepper K. The role of labeling processes in elderly consumers' response to
learners. Psychol Aging 1991;6(3):484 ± 6. age segmentation cues. J Consum Res (March) 1994;20:503 ± 19.
Law S, Craik FIM. Are aging consumers vulnerable to the belief enhancing Winograd E, Simon EW. Visual memory and imagery in the aged. In: LW
effects of repetition? Paper presented at the 1995 Association for Con- Fozard, Fozard JL, Cermak LS, Arenberg D & Thompson LW, editors.
sumer Research Conference, Minneapolis, MN, October 1995. New Directions in Memory and Aging: Proceedings of the George A.
Leigh JH. Information processing differences among broadcast media: re- Talland Memorial Conference. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum As-
view and suggestions for research. J Advertising (June) 1991;20:71 ± 5. sociates, 1980, pp. 485 ± 506.
Light LL. Memory and aging: four hypotheses in search of data. Annu Rev Yalch RF. Memory in a jingle: music as a mnemonic device in commu-
Psychol 1991;42:333 ± 76. nicating advertising slogans. J Appl Psychol 1991;76:268 ± 75.
Malhotra NK. Mnemonics in marketing: a pedagogical tool. J Acad Mark Yesavage J, Jacob R. Effects of relaxation and mnemonics on memory,
Sci (Spring) 1991;19:141 ± 9. attention, and anxiety in the elderly. Exp Aging Res 1984;10:
Moschis GP. Marketing to Older Consumers: A Handbook of Information 211 ± 4.
for Strategy Development. Westport, CT: Quorum Books, 1992. Yesavage J, Lapp D, Sheikh JI. Mnemonics as modified for use by the
Ostroff J. An aging market: how businesses can prosper. Am Demogr elderly. In: Poon LW, Rubin DC & Wilson BA, editors. Everyday
1989;11:26 ± 8,33,58 ± 9. Cognition in Adulthood and Late Life. New York: Cambridge Univ.
Richardson JT, Rossan S. Age limitations on the efficacy of imagery mne- Press, 1989, pp. 598 ± 611.
monic instructions. J Ment Imagery 1994;18(3/4):151 ± 64.

You might also like