Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

Journal of Applied Psychology

1977, Vol. 62, No. 4, 493-498

Effects of Interactive Imagery on Learning:


Application to Advertising
Kathy A. Lutz and Richard J. Lutz
University of California, Los Angeles

The effect of type of picture accompanying word paired associates on recall


was investigated. Forty-eight brand and product pairs with their accompanying
pictures were drawn from the Yellow Pages of a local telephone directory.
Seventy-two subjects were placed into a four-group design consisting of two
control and two experimental groups. The two experimental groups were shown
either interactive or noninteractive pictures with each word pair. The two con-
trol groups were shown only the word pairs. The group receiving the interactive
imagery remembered more brand names than the noninteractive imagery group
(£<.01). Comparison of each experimental group with its control revealed
that only the interactive imagery group differed significantly from its control
(p < .01), suggesting that the interactive feature of a mediating image is a
necessary condition for a facilitative effect.

There has been a great deal of research and to Wittrock, 197S, for a review of right-
lately on the effects of imagery on learning, hemisphere brain processing and its implica-
indicating that concreteness as a stimulus tions for learning.) The present study in-
attribute is positively correlated with the vestigates the effects of external imagery on
learnability of material and that mental learning and also includes a simple self-report
imagery as a mnemonic device aids recall technique so that the functional stimulus of
(Bower, 1972; Paivio, 1969). An important mental imagery may be better understood.
distinction is that which is made between The effects of external imagery on learning
external imagery—pictures and other graphic have been investigated primarily within the
modes of stimulus presentation—and internal paired-associate learning paradigm. Experi-
or mental imagery, which external imagery mental participants shown an interactive
presumably elicits. Research in the area of image relating two items in a paired-associate
brain lateralization suggests that mental learning task have significantly higher recall
imagery may be operationally denned as scores than those not presented with an
right-hemisphere brain processing and can be interactive picture and given only rote repeti-
elicited by external imagery. The evidence tion instructions (Davidson, 1964; Kerst &
that external imagery elicits right-hemisphere Levin, 1973; Lippman & Shanahan, 1973;
processing has come from studies of brain Milgram, 1967; Reese, 196S). One necessary
electroencephalograms during the processing condition for a facilitative effect is the inter-
of different kinds of external stimuli such as active feature of the mediating image. An
pictures and printed text (Galloway & Harris, interactive image integrates the two items in
1974; Morgan, McDonald, & MacDonald, some mutual or reciprocal action. When pic-
1971). (The reader is referred to Palmer, torial interaction of the items is not achieved
197S, for a theoretical treatment of mental so that they are shown in a noninteractive
imagery and visual information processing and separate imaginal space (side by side),
the benefit of imagery as an associative aid
is greatly reduced or lost altogether (Bower,
Requests for reprints should be sent to Kathy A. 1970; Bernbach & Stalonas, 1973; Neisser &
Lutz, Behavioral Technology Laboratories, Depart-
ment of Psychology, University of Southern Cali- Kerr, 1973).
fornia, University Park, Los Angeles, California There is some evidence that the more pic-
90007. torial the interaction, the more facilitative
493
494 KATHY A. LUTZ AND RICHARD J. LUTZ

INTERACTIVE IMAGERY

Picture Interaction Letter Accentuation

Rocket Messenger Service Dixon Crane Co.

NON-INTERACTIVE IMAGERY

Name Product or Service

OBear Abrasive Saws


Figure 1. Examples of types of imagery mediating the brand-product pairs.

the mediating image. One study (Lippman & directory Yellow Pages. The present study
Shanahan, 1973) denned several types of investigated the memorableness of brand-
interactive imagery and found that the most product pairs and their accompanying images
facilitative image is that which achieves taken from the Yellow Pages of a telephone
maximum figural unity of the two elements. directory for a major metropolitan area. Two
In the Lippman and Shanahan study, con- basic types of accompanying images were
cerning vocabulary acquisition by children, identified: (a) an interactive image that inte-
word pairs that were also shown as pictures grates at least parts of both the brand and
in interaction were better learned than word product together in one, integrative picture
pairs for which the interaction stimulus con- and (b) a noninteractive image that depicts
sisted of a picture and a word rather than the brand or product separately from the
two pictures. written form of the other member of the pair.
Promoting a new or unfamiliar product This second type of imagery is by far the
can be considered a type of paired-associate most common, and the typical form includes
learning task, in that the consumer is to learn a picture of the product with the brand
to associate a particular brand or company written next to it. Based on evidence that
name with a product or service. One appplica- visual memory is better than verbal memory
tion of imagery is advertising presentations (Erdelyi & Becker, 1974; Haber, 1970;
in print media such as newspapers and phone Paivio & Csapo, 1969; Shepard, 1967), the
INTERACTIVE IMAGERY IN ADVERTISING 495

brand rather than the product should be pre- in the study if the product itself served as a cue
sented in pictorial form so that memory for or prompt for the brand name. Rhyming and al-
literation, for example, were not permitted because
the brand may be enhanced although not phonetic similarity could affect recall for such pairs
necessarily associated with the corresponding as Bob's Bail Bonds and Mauer's Flower Shop.
product or service. Both types of pictures— Generic brand or company names, such as Stork
those depicting the brand name and those Diaper Service and Minuteman Timeclock Corpora-
depicting the product or service—were in- tion, were excluded to avoid the confluence of
meaningfulness and imagery. Familiar local geo-
cluded in the noninteractive imagery treat- graphical locations such as street names and cities
ment so that any differential effects could be were also not allowed. Finally, names that served
investigated. Figure 1 shows examples of both as type or class cues for the product were not used
types of noninteractive imagery. Interactive in the study. Pairs such as Fu Ling Chinese
Restaurant and Nordic Danish Furniture were
imagery also includes picture interaction and excluded for this reason.
letter accentuation. In the latter category, Pictures accompanying the pairs were examined
some letter or letters in the brand name are and classified into two main categories, interactive
made to look like the product or one of its imagery and noninteractive imagery. Because all
characteristics. Picture interaction occurs brand-product pairs and pictures were taken from a
phone directory, it was not possible to obtain
when both items of the pair are depicted examples of both types of imagery for each item.
pictorially and are shown together in inter- Therefore, a four-group design was used that in-
action. Findings discussed earlier suggest that cluded two control groups. The noninteractive
items accompanied by picture interaction imagery group saw 24 pairs, consisting of 12 with
brand pictures and 12 with pictures of the product;
images should be better remembered than
its control group saw only the 24 printed items
those accompanied by letter accentuation with no pictures. The interactive imagery group
pictures. Examples of both types of inter- received a different set of 24 item pairs for which
active imagery are presented in Figure 1. interactive images were available, whereas its control
group received the same 24 pairs but without the
accompanying pictures.
Method
Subjects
Procedure
Seventy-two undergraduate college students in
four sections of an introductory marketing class Four versions of the stimulus booklet were pre-
participated in the study during a regular class pared with instructions on the front page and each
meeting at the request of the instructor. Of the 40 of 24 brand-product pairs presented on separate
juniors and 32 seniors, the majority were males, pages. Booklets for the two control groups con-
with the seven females distributed among the lour tained only printed stimuli on each page, whereas
groups. booklets for the two imagery groups included both
printed and pictorial stimuli. Twenty copies of each
version were prepared, and booklets were randomly
Materials and Design distributed to each class. The students were in-
structed to look at the contents of each page care-
Companies listed in the Yellow Pages of a metro- fully during the allotted time, since they would
politan telephone directory were used in this study. need to remember the items for the second part of
The students' task was to learn to associate the the experiment. The order of the pages was ran-
company's proper name or brand name with its domized so that all class members were not simul-
product or service. Forty-eight name-product pairs taneously viewing the same item. Viewing time for
were selected from the directory following these each page was 10 seconds. Following the acquisition
criteria: (a) The pair was deemed likely to be phase, the booklets were collected and answer sheets
unfamiliar to college students, (b) no artifact were distributed.
rendered the pair easily remembered, and (c) an On the answer sheet, the student indicated his or
appropriate picture accompanied the ad. Previous her grade level, sex, handedness, and type of mental
familiarity was subjectively determined by the processing experienced while viewing the stimulus
authors, who eliminated well-known brands. materials. Type of processing was assessed by a
To isolate the effect of imagery from that of self-report nominal scale consisting of the following
other factors such as associative meaningfulness, question: "What best describes how you spent the
pairs were excluded if the items of the pair were time looking at each page of the booklet [of stim-
phonetically similar, conceptually or semantical!)' uli! ?" Subjects responded to this question on a
related, or if they possessed any other mnemonic forced-choice basis by checking one of three cate-
relationship. In other words, pairs were not used gories, which described visual, verbal, and task-
496 KATHY A. LUTZ AND RICHARD J. LUTZ

Table 1 ences were found on individual difference


Means and Standard Deviations For Brand variables, except that students in the control
Names Recalled groups reported experiencing a form of
mental processing during the acquisition
Condition M SD
phase different from that experienced by the
Group experimental groups.
Interactive imagery"
Experimental 8.82 4.25 Recall Tests
Control 5.39 2.89 Scores for all four groups on the recall tests
Noninteractive imagery1"
Experimental 4.95 3.41 are given in Table 1. The interactive imag-
Control 5.22 4.78 ery group performed better than its control,
^(1,33) = 7.90, p < ,01. This result is not
Subgroup too surprising, considering the evidence that
Picture interaction visual memory is superior to verbal memory.
Experimental 6.53 2.94 The comparison of the noninteractive imag-
Control 3.67 2.11
Letter accentuation ery group, however, with its control revealed
Experimental 2.29 1.93 no difference, / ? (1,3S) — .04, ns. Apparently,
Control 1.72 1.45 the nature of the mediating image is of
Brand imagery critical importance in producing the facilita-
Experimental 3.21 2.49
Control 2.78 2.49
tive effect of imagery. This suggests that
Product imagery imagery effects are not attributable solely to
Experimental 1.74 1.48 the superiority of visual memory.
Control 2.44 2.20 The interactive imagery group recalled
more brand names than the noninteractive
Note, n = 18 for each control group.
« « = 17. imagery group, F ( l , 34) = 9.21, p < .01.
b
n = 19. Omega squared for this analysis is .19, indi-
cating that about one fifth of the variance
unrelated processing, respectively. To measure brand in recall scores is attributable to the type
recall, the answer sheet contained a series of 24 of imagery treatment.
blanks followed by the product classes for all of the In the noninteractive imagery treatment,
brand-product pairs. Students were instructed to half of the images depicted the brand name
fill in the blanks with brand names of those pairs
that they could remember. This recall task seems and half were pictures of the product or
most representative of the natural environment in service. A comparison of each of these sub-
which a consumer must think of a brand or com- groups with its corresponding subgroup in
pany for a desired product. An item was scored as the control condition revealed no differences
correct if brand name was correctly recalled (no
more than a two-letter discrepancy from correct
between recall of those two types. However,
spelling) and matched with its corresponding product a correlated or within-subjects test revealed
or service. that students in the noninteractive imagery
group recalled more items accompanied by
Results name images than items for which product
images were presented, £(18) = 2.83, p < .OS.
Differences in recall of brand names were This finding suggests that it is more effective
found between the two experimental groups, to present the brand rather than the product
with the interactive imagery group remem- in pictorial form.
bering more brands than the noninteractive Two types of images were presented to the
imagery group. Comparison of each experi- interactive imagery group during acquisi-
mental group with its control revealed no tion—letter accentuation and picture inter-
difference between the noninteractive imag- action. Table 1 presents the means for the
ery group and its control, whereas the inter- subgroups. Comparison of the experimental
active imagery group recalled significantly and control group on recall of picture inter-
more brands than did its control. No differ- action items revealed a significant difference,
INTERACTIVE IMAGERY IN ADVERTISING 497

with the experimental group recalling more Table 2


names of pairs accompanied by picture inter- Contingency Table of Reported Mental
action images, F(l, 33) = 11.04, p < .01. Processing and Treatment Groups
Surprisingly, however, the comparison of
letter accentuation items did not reveal a Mental processing
difference in recall between the experimental Group Verbal Visual
and control group. Of the 24 items, 14 were
presented using picture interaction and 10 Interactive imagery
using letter accentuation. The means for each No. 4.0 13.0
are presented in Table 1. The interactive % 5.6 18.1
imagery group recalled 47% of the items pre- Noninteractive imagery
sented with picture interaction and only 23% No. 3.0 16.0
of the letter accentuation items. A within- % 4.2 22.2
group test revealed a significant difference Combined control
between these proportions, £(16) = 4.84, p < No. 28.0 8.0
.01. Thus, superiority of the interactive imag- % 38.9 11.1
ery group seems to be due to the greater
recall of picture interaction items. indicates that students may not automatically
generate mental imagery for verbal stimuli, in
Individual Differences spite of the evidence that imagery is a potent
mnemonic aid. A comparison of the two
Comparison of each experimental group experimental groups on type of processing
with its control and also comparison of the reported no difference, x 2 U) — -03, ns, which
two experimental groups revealed no sta- suggests that mental imagery per se is not
tistical differences on the variables of grade necessarily facilitative.
level, sex, and handedness. The groups appear
to be equivalent on these dimensions, indi-
Discussion
cating that the randomization procedure was
successful. Research findings concerning imagery have
implications for the design of information
Mental Processing presentations. The use of pictures in ads
would seem justified by the evidence that
The two control groups were combined for visual memory is better than verbal memory.
the analysis of differences between treatments The present study indicated, however, that
on reported mental processing. The fre- merely presenting a picture in an ad will not
quency distribution of reported verbal or necessarily be an improvement over a purely
visual processing for the three groups—inter- verbal presentation. A distinction must be
active imagery, noninteractive imagery, and made between the types of external imagery
combined control—was analyzed using the and their differential effectiveness. An inter-
chi-square test. Table 2 indicates that the active image facilitates recall better than a
groups differed in their distribution of mental noninteractive image, presumably by in-
imagery report, with x s (2) = 24.73, p < .01. creasing the concreteness of the material to
Visual inspection indicates that both experi- be learned, that is, the association of the two
mental groups reported experiencing more items. The more concrete the association
mental imagery than did the control groups. becomes, the more memorable it is.
Collapsing the two imagery groups into one Previous studies on the effects of inter-
group in a 2 X 2 contingency table confirms active images suffer from a lack of ecological
the effect that experimental subjects gave representativeness (Snow, 1973) because the
greater frequency of mental imagery re- experimenters constructed the pairs to be
ports than did the control groups, with x 2 (0 learned as well as the mediating pictures
= 22.24, p<.0l. This result, as expected, rather than drawing them from naturally
498 KATHY A. LUTZ AND RICHARD J. LUTZ

occurring stimuli. Such use of contrived Davidson, R. E. Mediation and ability in paired-
stimuli limits the external validity of the associate learning. Journal of Educational Psychol-
ogy, 1964, 55, 352-356.
experimental results. Lippman and Shanahan Erdelyi, M. H., & Becker, J. Hyermnesia for pic-
(1973), for example, made up word-paralog tures: Incremental memory for pictures but not
(pseudoword) pairs and various types of words in multiple recall trials. Cognitive Psychol-
accompanying images. Not only were the pic- ogy, 1974, 6, 159-171.
tures contrived, but the paralogs were not Haber, R. N. How we remember what we see.
Scientific American, 1970, 222, 104-112.
naturally occurring elements of any vocabu- Kerst, S., & Levin, J. R. A comparison of experi-
lary. The present study used real-world stim- menter-provided and subject-generated strategies in
uli to increase the implications for advertising children's paired-associate learning. Journal of
effectiveness. Educational Psychology, 1973, 65, 300-303.
Lippman, M. Z., & Shanahan, M. W. Pictorial
The use of interactive imagery is clearly facilitation of paired-associate learning: Implica-
implied for effectively promoting the associa- tions for vocabulary training. Journal of Educa-
tion of a brand name with its product or tional Psychology, 1973, 64, 216-222.
service. Picture interaction is the most desir- Milgram, N. A. Verbal context versus visual com-
able type of imagery to use, but it requires pound in paired-associate learning by children.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1967,
that both items of the pair be portrayable 5, 597-603.
in pictorial form. Although products can usu- Morgan, A. H., McDonald, P. J., & Macdonald, H.
ally be pictured quite easily, it may require Differences in bilateral alpha activity as a function
some ingenuity to pictorially depict a service, of experimental task, with a note on lateral eye
movements and hypnotizability. Neuropsychologia,
and it may be impossible to create an image 1971, o, 459-469.
equivalent of some brand names. Letter Neisser, V., & Kerr, N. Spatial and mnemonic
accentuation images can be used for such properties of visual images. Cognitive Psychology,
brands, but an item analysis revealed large 1973, 5, 138-150.
variation in recall of these items, suggesting Paivio, A. Mental imagery and associative learning
and memory. Psychological Review, 1969, 76, 241-
that other variables are also effecting recall. 263.
The better remembered items had images that Paivio, A., & Csapo, K. Concrete-image and verbal
seemed humorous or unusual. Future research memory codes. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
should identify the other variables that con- ogy, 1969, 80, 279-285.
tribute to the effectiveness of letter accen- Palmer, S. E. Visual perception and world knowl-
edge: Notes on a model of sensory-cognitive
tuation images, since any advertiser can interaction. In D. A. Norman, E. E. Rumelhart,
potentially use this type of imagery. and the LNR Research Group (Eds.), Explora-
tions in cognition, San Francisco: Freeman, 1975.
References Reese, H. W. Imagery in paired-associate learning
Bernbach, H. A., & Stolonas, P. M. The effect of in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psy-
isolated imagery on free recall. Bulletin of Psycho- chology, 1965, 2, 290-296.
nomic Science, 1973, 1, 227-228. Shepard, R. N. Recognition memory for words, sen-
Bower, G. H. Imagery as a relational organizer in tences and pictures. Journal of Verbal Learning
associative learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1967, 6, 156-163.
and Verbal Behavior, 1970, 9, 529-533. Snow, R. E. Representativeness and quasi-repre-
Bower, G. H. Mental imagery and associative learn- sentativeness designs for research on teaching. Re-
ing. In L. Gregg (Ed.), Cognition in learning and view of Educational Research, 1973, 44, 265-291.
memory. New York: Wiley, 1972. Wittrock, M. C. The generative processes of memory.
Galloway, E., & Harris, P. R. Coupling between UCLA Educator, 1975, 17, 33-43.
cortical potentials from different areas. Science,
1974, 183, 873-875. Received April 26, 1976 •

You might also like