Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/232442073

Locus of Control and Social Support: Interactive


Moderators of Stress

Article  in  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology · August 1984


DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.47.2.378

CITATIONS READS

186 2,239

3 authors, including:

Rod A. Martin
The University of Western Ontario
96 PUBLICATIONS   10,185 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Enjoying Retirement View project

Myndplan app development View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Rod A. Martin on 01 August 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal oTPtnonality and Social Piychc Copyright 1984 by the
1984. Vol. 47. No. 2. 378-389 American Piydtolofical Aanoauoo, Inc.

Locus of Control and Social Support:


Interactive Moderators of Stress
Herbert M. Lefcourt, Rod A. Martin, and Wendy E. Saleh
University of Waterloo, Ontario

Social support has been regarded as a strong moderator of stress. Recent research
has suggested, however, that this effect is not universal, that some supports are
more effective than others, and that some persons make better use of supports than
others. In the present studies, our results corroborated previous findings that persons
with an internal locus of control derive greater benefits from social support than
do those who have a more external orientation. Furthermore, we found that the
moderating effect of social supports largely occurs among those who are less generally
affiliative and are more highly autonomous. As previous investigators have suggested,
social supports may be most beneficial to those who are more instrumental and
sparing in their approach to social interactions.

Research concerned with responses to stress Miller, Ware, & Sherk, 1981), a sense of com-
has been prominent in the psychological lit- mitment (Kobasa, 1982), the response to chal-
erature for a considerable period of time. After lenge (Johnson & Sarason, 1979), a sense of
the publication of Holmes and Rahe's (1967) humor (Martin & Lefcourt, 1983, 1984), and
scale assessing the impact of life changes, this social support (Silver & Wortman, 1981; Hu-
research area expanded even further. Although saini, 1982).
the effects of stressors on individuals' states of This latter factor, social support, is one of
well-being have been thoroughly documented, those self-evident variables that laypersons as-
it has been equally apparent that situations sume have an ameliorative effect on stress. The
alone can account for only a small portion of stress-buffering effects of social support have
the variance in predicting distress. been well demonstrated in a number of studies
In more recent years, investigators have ex- (Brown, Bhrolchain, & Harris, 1975; Eaton,
plored a number of individual difference vari- 1978; Sarason, 1980). Nevertheless, in the
ables in the hope of explaining why some peo- same way that investigators were stimulated
ple readily succumb to the effects of stressors to search for moderators of life stress by the
while others appear to be more resilient. Ko- findings that people varied in their responses
basa (1979, 1982) has denned what she refers to given stressors, a number of writers have
to as "hardiness," Garmezy (1981) speaks of been questioning whether people do not differ
"invulnerability," and Murphy and Moriarity in the likelihood of deriving benefits from dif-
(1976) write of "resilience" as terms describ- ferent sources of social support and whether
ing the resistance to stress. different sources of support may not offer more
These and other investigators have exam- or less protection against stress. For example,
ined the role of a number of variables that Phillips and Fischer (1981) have found that
constitute these larger constructs. Among such the extent of one's social network and the
variables have been locus of control (Johnson presence of family-based social support do not
& Sarason, 1978; Kobasa, 1979; Lefcourt, necessarily help to moderate stress effects,
whereas instrumental supports and the num-
ber of non-kin confidants can have a substan-
This research was supported by a SSHRC grant #410- tial effect on how stress is experienced. Schae-
81-0276 to the first author and a SSHRC doctoral fellow- fer, Coyne, and Lazarus (1981) compared the
ship to the second author. We express appreciation to Carol effects of social network size and three types
Fide, who helped to conduct this investigation.
Requests for reprints should be sent to to Herbert M.
of social support (tangible, emotional, and in-
Lefcourt, Department of Psychology, University of Wa- formational) on psychological symptoms, mo-
terloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada N2L 3G1. rale, and ill health resulting from stressful
378
MODERATORS OF STRESS 379

events. Network proved to be weaker than sample across a variety of reinforcement areas.
other support variables and informational Second, we have used two scales from Jack-
support was associated primarily with positive son's Personality Research Form (Jackson,
morale. On the other hand, low tangible and 1967), one that focuses on persons' tendencies,
emotional support were more related to to immerse themselves in social interactions
depression and negative morale. In conclusion, (need affiliation) and one that focuses on the
these authors state that social support research desire to function autonomously (need auton-
would benefit from attention to the multidi- omy). If Sandier and Lakey are correct, a more
mensionality of support and to greater spec- discriminating and strategic use of social sup-
ificity in hypotheses concerning the relation ports will have a more moderating effect on
between types of support and adaptational stressors. Therefore, persons who are not ex-
outcomes. ceptionally gregarious and can function au-
Most pertinent to the present study is an tonomously, and are thus presumably more
article by Sandier and Lakey (1982). They discriminating and instrumental in their social
raised the question of whether locus of control interactions, should benefit more from the
might influence how persons make use of po- moderating effects of social supports.
tential social supports. Because previous re- Our hypotheses are that the loci of control
search had indicated that internals were better for affiliation and for achievement and both
able to find and to make use of information the affiliation and autonomy scales of the PRF
than were externals (Lefcourt, Gronnerud, & will prove to be salient in producing the mod-
McDonald, 1973; Wolk & DuCette, 1974), erator effects of social supports. Specifically,
Sandier and Lakey hypothesized that similar persons who are internal for affiliation and
differences between internals and externals achievement, low in the need for affiliation,
might be found in the ways that social supports and high in the need for autonomy will more
are used to alleviate the impacts of stress. In commonly exhibit moderator effects from so-
their investigation, they found interactions be- cial support than will their opposites.
tween negative life events and social supports
when they made predictions of anxiety and
depression among internals but not when they Study 1
made such predictions for externals. Simple Method
effects revealed that both anxiety and depres-
sion following negative events were moderated Subjects
for internals by the presence of social supports. Forty-six (22 male and 24 female) members of a first-
The same was not true for externals. year introductory psychology course agreed to participate
in a study on life stress among university students. During
The present studies represent a replication successive class sessions, subjects completed each of the
of and expansion on the Sandier and Lakey following measures:
(1982)findings.In instances in which Sandier 1. College Student Recent Life Events Schedule (CSRLES;
Sandier & Lakey. 1982) This checklist is composed of
and Lakey made use of the personal control 112 experiences that are considered germane to college
factor of Rotter's locus of control scale (Mirels, students. The subjects were instructed to check off the
1970), we have used the affiliation and events that had happened to them during the preceding
achievement locus of control scales from the year and to rate the effect that each event had had on
their lives (very negative, slightly negative, slightly positive,
Multidimensional-Multiattributional Causal- or very positive). We obtained a weighted negative life
ity Scale (MMCS; Lefcourt, 1981; Lefcourt, events score for each subject by adding only events that
von Baeyer, Ware, & Cox, 1979). These scales were rated as having had a negative impact, weighting
are used to assess the degree to which subjects them 1 (slightly negative) or 2 (very negative).
believe that they can exercise control over two 2. Inventory of Socially Supportive Behaviors (ISSB,
reinforcement areas that are presumably of Barrera, Sandier, & Ramsay, 1981). This is a 40-item scale
in which respondents report the frequency of receiving
high value to university students. Lefcourt supportive actions from others. Examples of such support
(1981) argued that locus of control measures are "having had someone stay with you [the subject] in a
concerning outcomes in goal areas that are stressful situation," "having had someone watch your pos-
highly pertinent to particular samples should sessions when you were away," having had someone "pro-
vide you with a place where you could get away for a
afford greater predictive power within those while," and so on. The scale has been found to be internally
samples than more general measures that consistent and temporally reliable over a short time span,
380 H. LEPCOUKT, R. MARTIN, AND W. SALEH

Table 1
Intercorrelations Between Variables in Study 1

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

1. NLE — .10 -.03 -.02 .17 .04 -.20


2. SS .28* -.15 .07 .01 -.08
3. N-aff -.53***» .36»** .25* -.25*
4. N-aut -.19 .02 .17
5. LC-aff .35** .18
6. LC-ach .00
7. TMD —

Note. NLE = Negative Life Events; SS = Social Support; N-aff = Need for Affiliation; N-aut = Need for Autonomy;
LC-aff = Affiliation locus of control; LC-ach = Achievement locus of control; TMD = Total Mood Disturbance.
• p < .10. " p < .05. • « p < .02. •••• p < .001.

and is related to measures of social network and family Results


support.
3. Muiudimensional-Mullattnbulional Causality Scale Table 1 represents the intercorrelation ma-
(MMCS; Lefcourt. 1981; Lefcourt, von Baeyer, Ware, &
Cox, 1979). This scale comprises two 24-item Likert scales
trix for the variables in this study. It is evident
concerned with achievement and affiliation. The items are that neither Negative Life Events nor Social
balanced for success and failure and are equally divided Support are strongly related to the personality
among four sources of outcomes: ability, effort, task or measures. There is a slight tendency for highly
situation constraints, and chance. Total scores reflect the affiliative persons to report having more social
composite of endorsed external attributions minus the
composite of endorsed internal attributions. High scores, support (r = .28, p < .10), though the mag-
therefore, indicate externality for the specific goal area. nitude of that relation is low. The strongest
Full descriptions of the scale with reliability and validity relation obtained was between the needs for
data are included in the articles noted earlier. autonomy and affiliation (r = —.53,/? < .001),
4. Personality Research Form (PRF, Jackson, 1967). which were negatively related, as common
The needs for affiliation and autonomy are derived from
the larger PRF and consist of 20 true-false items each. sense would suggest. Finally, highly affiliative
These needs are inferred from the frequency with which persons (high N-aff) also seem to express more
subjects endorse items indicating activity in the respective externality with regard to affiliative outcomes
areas. Thus doing things alone frequently and preferring (r = .36, p < .02), which suggests that inse-
to be on one's own would indicate a need for autonomy.
There is considerable literature pertaining to the reliability curity in social relations may play some part
and validity of PRF scales. in gregariousness. With regard to the depen-
5. Profile of Mood States (POMS; McNair, Lorr, & dent measure, Total Mood Disturbance was
Droppleman, 1971) This 65-item scale consists of de- unrelated to any of the predictor measures,
scriptive terms that subjects rate for occurrence. The mea- except for one borderline relation with N-aff
sure yields scores on five negative moods (Tension,
Depression, Anger, Fatigue, and Confusion) and one pos- (r = -.25, p < .10).
itive mood (Vigor). These six moods are highly mtercor- Total Mood Disturbance, which consists of
related; to avoid redundancy, we conducted the analyses the sum of the negative affects (tension,
using the Total Mood Disturbance score, which is computed depression, etc.) minus the measure of vigor,
by summing the five negative scores and subtracting the
Vigor score. The POMS has been used extensively in the was the primary dependent variable. In four
study of moods and mood change and its reliability and separate analyses, hierarchical regressions were
validity are well documented. computed for subjects who were high or low
on each of the individual difference measures
as determined by median splits. For example,
Procedure subjects scoring as internal were treated sep-
arately from those scoring as external with
During weekly three-hour classes, the middle hour was regard to affiliation. For each group, Negative
devoted to participation in this study. The measures were
each administered in their typical fashion. The POMS, Life Events (CSRLES) was entered as the first
however, was scored as to how the students had most typ- variable in the regression analysis, Social Sup-
ically felt during the past few months. port (ISSB) was entered as the second variable,
MODERATORS OF STRESS 381

and the product of the two was entered as the .03). No main effects for social support were
third variable. Thefirsttwo variables produce found in either group. On the other hand, an
the equivalent of main effects, whereas the interaction of borderline significance, F{1,
third produces the equivalent of the interaction 20) = 3.60, p < .10, was found among affili-
term in an analysis of variance (ANOVA). In ation internals. As is evident in Figure 1, in-
each case then, the analyses reveal whether ternals with social support responded rather
social support blunts the effects of negative differently to negative life events than did in-
life events within either group (internals and ternals with less social support.
externals, for example) in producing mood With increasing negative experiences, so-
disturbance. cially supported internals exhibited a decrease
Table 2 presents the results for each of the in mood disturbance, whereas less supported
four personality variables. With regard to the internals showed increases in mood distur-
two locus of control measures, the results were bances. In essence, affiliation internals with
not overly impressive. When subjects were social support seemed to have thrived on po-
classified by the affiliation locus of control tential adversities, possibly converting them
scale, one main effect attained significance. into "doable" challenges, whereas less sup-
Among those who were external for affiliation, ported internals regarded negative life events
we found a strong relation between negative as sources of disturbance.
life events and mood disturbance (r = .53), We found no such interaction among affil-
F(l, 20) = 8.00, p < .025). The equivalent iation externals or, for that matter, among ei-
among affiliation internals was negligible (r = ther internals or externals classified by the

Table 2
Hierarchically Arranged Multiple Regression Analyses Between Stress and Moderator
Variables on Total Mood Disturbance Scores

Variable r *2A F df p r *2A F df P

Internals Externals
LC-aff*
NLE .03 .001 0.02 1,22 _ .53 .286 8.00 .20 <.O25
SS .25 .061 1.36 1,21 — .10 .000 0.00 , 19
NLEXSS _ .143 3.60 1,20 <.10 .004 0.11 , 18
LC-achb
NLE .47 .226 5.54 1, 19 <.05 .07 .005 0.12 ,23
SS .30 .012 0.28 1, 18 — .08 .004 0.10 .22
NLEXSS — .003 0.06 1, 17 — — .028 0.60 ,21 —

Low-need affil High-need affil


Need-affilc
NLE .16 .024 0.42 1, 17 — .40 .163 4.29 1,22 <.05
SS .02 .000 0.00 1, 16 — .01 .006 0.15 1,21
NLEXSS — .428 11.75 1, 15 <.OO5 — .018 0.4S 1.20 —

Low-need aut High-need aut


1
Need-auf
NLE .51 .263 7.86 1,22 <.025 .14 .019 0.33 1, 17
SS .13 .002 0.05 1,21 — -.07 .007 0.12 1, 16
NLEXSS — .062 1.84 1,20 — — .383 9.72 1, 15 <.01

Note. NLE = Negative Life Events; SS = Social Support; LC-aff = Affiliation locus of control; LC-ach = Achievement
locus of control; Need-affil = need for affiliation; Need-aut = need for autonomy.
1
For internals, score < 43, N = 24; for externals, score > 43, N = 22.
b
For internals, score < 38, N = 21; for externals, score > ii, N = 25.
c
For low-need affil, score < 13, N = 19; for high-need affil, score > 13, N = 24.
d
For low-need aut, score < 9, N = 24; for high-need aut, score > 9, N = 19.
382 H. LEPCOURT, R. MARTIN, AND W. SALEH

achievement locus of control measure. Here, In Table 2, the regression analyses for the
in contrast, we found one significant main ef- needs for affiliation and autonomy are also
fect. Among achievement internals, negative presented. As might have been expected, those
life events had a significant impact upon mood scoring higher in affiliative tendencies tended
disturbance (r = .47), F(l, 19) = 5.54, to score more highly on the measure of social
p < .05. support (r = .28, p < .10) and less highly on

Low Need Affiliation High Need Autonomy

too- 100

80-

60- a 60'
o
Z
Z
40-

20 20

10 20 30 40 10 20 30 40
NLE NLE

Internal for Affiliation

too-

80

a 60
z
»- 40

20

10 20 '30 40

Low Social Support

High Social Support


Figure 1. Regression lines predicting Total Mood Disturbance scores (TMD) from Negative Life Events
(NLE) for low and high social support groups for Study 1.
MODERATORS OF STRESS 383

mood disturbance (r = —.25, p <. 10). Despite realm (i.e., internal locus of control for affil-
the curtailment of score ranges, there was a iation).
significant main effect for negative life events
on mood disturbance (r = .40), F{1, 22) = Study 2
4.29, p < .05, among those who were highly
affiliative. On the other hand, there were no Though the results from Study 1 offered
main effects found in the analysis of mood some confirmation for our hypotheses, the data
data obtained from those who were low in the obtained with the locus of control measures
need for affiliation. However, as predicted, a were not as strong as we had anticipated. Be-
strong interaction between negative life events cause the sample in thefirststudy was limited
and social supports, F[l, 15) = 11.75, p < in size, allowing for only 24 subjects to be
.005, was obtained. As is evident in Figure 1, classified as affiliation internals (21 as achieve-
the interaction bears similarity to that obtained ment internals) and 22 as affiliation externals
among affiliation internals. Highly supported (25 as achievement externals), we conducted
persons who were low in affiliative needs a replication study during the following se-
seemed to be undaunted by the occurrence of mester when a larger sample became available.
negative life events. In contrast, less supported Subjects for the second study were drawn
low-affiliative persons exhibited rapid increases from the university subject pool, which con-
in mood disturbance with increases in negative sisted of students enrolled in first-year intro-
life events. ductory psychology courses. Participation was
The results for need for autonomy were voluntary, although course credit was granted
nearly the same as for need for affiliation. A for participation.
significant main effect and no interaction was
found among those who were less autonomous Method
(r = .51), 2^1, 22) = 7.86, p < .025, showing
that negative life events were associated with Ninety-nine subjects (58 men and 41 women) were seen
individually. In the course of 1 '/> hrs, the same measures
greater mood disturbances regardless of social of life stress, social support, affiliation, and achievement
support in this group as they were in the highly locus of control and mood disturbance were obtained. The
affiliative group. PRF was omitted because of time constraints.
In contrast, among the more autonomous
subjects a significant interaction was found, Results
i^l, 15) = 9.72, p < .01, which is illustrated
in Figure 1. It is clear that the form of this Table 3 shows the interrelations among the
interaction is nearly identical to that obtained variables. With this larger sample the expected
from the less affiliative subject group. zero-order correlations were obtained. Nega-
tive life events were associated with mood dis-
Discussion turbance (r = .33, p < .001), as was externality
for affiliation (r = .33, p < .001), and social
The results from this first study offer some support was negatively related to mood dis-
confirmation for our hypotheses, though cau- turbance (r = -.20, p < .05). Only locus of
tion is necessary in acceptingfindingsderived control for achievement was unrelated to
from samples of such limited size. It seemed mood scores confirming the zero-level relation
evident in this sample, however, that person- found in the first study. The other finding of
ality variables that were pertinent to inter- note was that externality for affiliation was
personal concerns interacted more readily with associated with negative life events (r = .25,
social support than did the one achievement- p < .02), which suggests that many of the neg-
focused variable (locus of control for achieve- ative life events might be related to social un-
ment) when we predicted mood disturbance. certainties, either in a causal way with social
Apparently the moderator effects of social shocks leading to external attributions for so-
support were more salient among those who cial outcomes, or in the opposite fashion with
were less generally gregarious (i.e., high-need externality for affiliation begetting the bungled
autonomy and low-need affiliation) and more social interactions perceived as negative ex-
self-attributing for outcomes in the affiliative periences.
384 H. LEFCOURT, R. MARTIN, AND W. SALEH

Table 3
Intercorrelations Between the Variables in Study 2

Variable 1

1. NLE .06 .03 .33~


2. SS .05 .11 -.20*
3. LC-aff .16 .33"
4. LC-ach .12
5. TMD

Note. NLE = Negative Life Events; SS = Social Support; LC-aff = Affiliation locus of control; LC-ach = Achievement
locus of control; TMD = Total Mood Disturbance.
• p < .05. • • p < .02. •*• p< .001.

Hierarchical regressions were computed for the same decline in mood disturbance with
subjects classified as internal and external by increasing negative life events, less supported
median splits for each of the two goal areas. internals did show the same increases in dis-
Table 4 shows the results for both the affiliation turbance with increases in negative experiences
and achievement scales. as was found earlier. It seems once again that
It is evident that the effects of negative life social support operated as a moderator variable
events and social support are more marked among affiliation internals.
among affiliation internals than they are In the equivalent analyses for locus of con-
among affiliation externals. Negative life events trol for achievement, main effects for negative
produced a significant main effect (r = .44), life events were found among achievement in-
F{1, 44) = 10.31, p < .005, and interaction, ternals (r = .31), F{\, 42) = 4.91, p < .05, and
/=X1, 42) = 5.48, p < .025) among affiliation externals (r = .33), F\l, 48) = 5.78, p < .025.
internals. Among affiliation externals, only a Additionally, social support produced a main
borderline effect for social support was found effect among externals (r = -.25), F\\, 47) =
( r = - . 2 2 ) , # U , 50) = 3.11,/><.10). 5.62, p < .025. However, only among achieve-
As may be noted in Figure 2, the interaction ment internals did the interaction term attain
pattern between negative life events and social significance, F{\, 45) = 4.86, p < .05, and, as
supports among affiliation internals is similar is evident in Figure 2, the pattern of that in-
to that found in the first study. Though highly teraction is very similar to that found with
supported affiliation internals did not show affiliation locus of control. Again, internals

Table 4
Hierarchically Arranged Multiple Regression Analyses Between Stress and Moderator
Variables on Total Mood Disturbance Scores

Internals Externals

Variable r *>A F df P r *>A F df P

LC-aff*
NLE .44 .19 10.31 1,44 < .005 .17 .03 1.48 1,51
SS -.19 .04 2.30 1,43 — -.22 .06 3.11 1,50 < .10
NLEXSS .09 5.48 1,42 <.025 .00 0.17 1,49
LC-achb

NLE .31 .09 4.91 1,47 <.05 .33 .11 5.78 1,48 <.025
SS -.14 .02 0.83 1,46 — -.25 .10 5.62 1,47 <.O25
NLEXSS — .09 4.86 1,45 <.05 — .02 1.10 1,46 —

Note. NLE = Negative Life Events; SS = Social Support; LC-aff = Affiliation locus of control; LC-ach = Achievement
locus of control.
" For internals, score < 46, N =• 46; for externals, score > 46, N = 53.
b
For internals, score < 40, N = 49; for externals, score > 40, N = 50.
MODERATORS OF STRESS 385

bereft of social support tend to increase in the number of subjects within each study is
mood disturbance as negative life events in- not sufficient to allay all fears concerning the
crease, whereas supported internals seem to reliability of the moderator effects that we have
be relatively unaffected by changing frequen- witnessed. Ideally, a sample of several
cies of negative events. hundreds of subjects would allow us to include
all variables in one regression analysis such
Discussion that we could observe three-way interactions
between our variables. Given the limitations
In these first two studies, the patterns appear of our sample sizes, we have opted to examine
to be similar with occasional exceptions. the reliability of the moderator effect through
Though the main effects vary between samples replications of the basic study itself. We con-
and variables, the interactions indicating ducted this final study during the semester
moderator effects seem to be reliable. Only after the one in which Study 2 was completed.
among internals, among those who are more Subjects for this third study were drawn
autonomous, and among those who are less from a third-year psychology course concerned
gregarious, are interactions found between with measurement; class members were all
negative life events and social supports. In each honors students. Participation was required as
case the internal with support seems resilient part of the class's exposure to different types
in the face of stress, whereas the internal with- of measuring devices.
out support seems to be in jeopardy.
Method
Study 3
Sixty-six subjects (17 men and 49 women) were provided
Though the sample size in the second study packets in which each of the measures included in the
was considerably larger than that of the first, second study were enclosed. They were given a two-week

Internal for Affiliation Internal for Achievement

too 100-

80' 80

Q 60- Q 60
2 z
•" 40-
H
40

20-
20

10 20 30 40 10 20 30 40
NLE NLE

Low Social Support

High Social Support


Figure 2 Regression lines predicting Total Mood Disturbance scores (TMD) from Negative Life Events
(NLE) for low and high social support groups for Study 2.
386 H. LEFCOURT, R. MARTIN, AND W. SALEH

Table 5 and conflict than would be affiliation expec-


Intercorrelations Between the Variables tancies.
in Study 3 Table 6 shows the regression analyses for
Variable I 2 3 4 5
subjects classified as internals and externals
by median splits for each goal area. In contrast
1. NLE — .16 -.14 .06 .25* to the previous studies, classification .by affil-
2. SS -.12 -.07 .06 iation locus of control did not produce the
3. LC-aff .25* .16 same interaction between negative life events
4. LC-ach .37"
5. TMD — and social support. We found only one main
effect: Affiliation internals showed a significant
Note. NLE «= Negative Life Events; SS = Social Support; main effect (r = -44), JM(1, 31) = 7.29, p <
LC-aff = Affiliation locus of control; LC-ach = Achieve- .01, between negative life events and mood
ment locus of control; TMD = Total Mood Disturbance.
* p < .05. ** p < .01. disturbance. On the other hand, achievement
locus of control does reveal the interaction
that was found in the preceding study with
period in which to complete the measures. All personal achievement internals, F{1, 30) = 3.80, p <
identifying information was eliminated; each subject had .06). As is evident in Figure 3, the shape of
a code number by which his or her measures were rec-
ognized. the interaction is the same as was found in
the previous studies.
Results Achievement internals with social support
seem to be resilient in the face of stressors,
Table S represents the intercorrelation ma- whereas internals without social support ap-
trix for the variables. The major difference pear to be more disturbed given increasing
between this matrix and the preceding ones is numbers of negative life events. In contrast,
that in this one, achievement locus of control externals for both affiliation and achievement
was related to mood disturbance (r = .37, seem to be unpredictable on the basis of either
p < .01) within this sample, whereas affiliation negative life events or social support.
locus of control was not significantly related
to the mood measure. This may reflect the
Discussion
fact that for this more advanced student group,
achievement is the more salient goal concern, In this third study the results bear a simi-
and externality with regard to this high concern larity to the other studies, but they also reveal
area may be more indicative of current malaise distinct differences. The fact that these students

Table 6
Hierarchically Arranged Multiple Regression Analyses Between Stress and Moderator
Variables on Total Mood Disturbance Scores

Internals Externals

Variable r F df P r *»A F df p

LC-aff'
NLE .44 .19 7.29 1 ,31 <.01 • .06 .00 0.13 1 ,31 —
SS -.03 .00 0.10 1,30 .15 .02 0.58 1 ,30 —
NLEXSS .03 1.08 1,29 .02 0.57 1,29 —
LC-achb
NLE .27 .07 2.50 1 ,32 .12 .21 .04 1.36 1,30 —
SS .08 .00 0.10 1 ,31 — .01 .00 0.07 1,29 —
NLEXSS — .10 3.80 1 ,30 .06 — .00 0.03 1 ,28 —

Note. NLE = Negative Life Events; SS = Social Support; LC-aff = Affiliation locus of control; LC-ach = Achievement
locus of control.
* For internals, score < 44, N = 33; for externals, score > 44, N = 33.
b
For internals, score < 42, N = 34; for externals, score > 42, N = 32.
MODERATORS OF STRESS 387

were more advanced and were engaged in an General Discussion


honors course that had portents for career
choices meant that these students were likely Our results tend to corroborate those that
to be more serious about both their coursework Sandier and Lakey (1982) obtained using the
and their career goals than were the less ad- locus of control variable. In addition, those
vanced students who participated in the first who seem to be less generally sociable or more
two studies. Consequently, achievement locus autonomous appear to benefit the most from
of control proved to be more important than the presence of social supports. Though the
affiliation locus of control in predicting mood results were neither uniform nor of a very high
states; that is, the sense of well-being of these magnitude in every case, therelationsbetween
advanced students seemed to be more contin- negative life events and mood disturbance were
gent on their beliefs regarding achievement, substantially reduced by social support for
perhaps because of the greater salience of this those who were internal for affiliation, internal
goal area at this point in their lives. The sim- for achievement, less generally affiliative, and
ilarity to the preceding studies inheres in the more highly autonomous. In no instance did
interaction obtained between social support their opposites seem to receive the same degree
and negative life events among achievement of benefit from social support. We could also
internals. Once again, unsupported internals arguefromour data that social support is more
showed elevated mood disturbances as negative necessary for those who are presumably more
life events increased, whereas socially sup- instrumental in their social interactions (i.e.,
ported internals exhibited minimal effects internals, low-need affiliation and high-need
from such events. autonomy) because negative life events had
strong effects on mood disturbance among
those subjects when they did not have access
Internal for Achievement to social support. Whether such persons have
greater need of social support than do others
or whether they make better use of support as
100 we have hypothesized is not clearly answerable
with the current data. However, if need for
80- affiliation reflects need for social support, ev-
idence from the first study in which need for
affiliation was associated with externality for
60H affiliation (r = .36, p < .02) offers some support
for the latter alternative; that is, that internals
express less need of but show better effects
40 from having social support than do externals
who show more need of but obtain fewer ben-
20
efits from such support.
With specific reference to locus of control,
the currentfindingsalong with those of Sandier
and Lakey (1982) may help to explain some
10 20 30 40
of the discrepancies with research generated
N LE within the learned helplessness framework.
Seligman, Abramson, Semmel, and von Baeyer
(1979) and Metalsky, Abramson, Seligman,
Semmel, and Peterson (1982) contended that
Low Social Support internal-global-stable attributions result in
depression when negative outcomes are ex-
perienced. In contrast, Lefcourt (1982), after
High Social Support reviewing the literature concerned with locus
Figure 3. Regression lines predicting Total Mood Distur- of control and stress, concluded that internality
bance scores (TMD) from Negative Life Events (NLE) for more often seems to have a moderating effect
low and high social support groups for Study 3. on the relation between stress and mood dis-
388 H. LEFCOURT, R. MARTIN, AND W. SALEH

turbances. In our research, it is evident that experiences. However, that role is complex, interacting as
internals devoid of social support bear simi- it does with other variables such as time of life stress,
social support, and no doubt other variables as well,
larity to those internals who have been used (p. 110)
in helplessness investigations. Though we have
no evidence to support the hypothesis from As our currentfindingssuggest, the interaction
our data, those who are more global and stable with social support is one of the more reliable
in their self-blaming tendencies might also be findings that may help to clarify the role that
those who do not have the benefit of social locus of control plays in determining how per-
supports when they are needed. It is tempting sons react to negative life experiences. Though
to believe that the more instrumental use of it is a powerful predictor in interaction with
social supports would provide one with social personality variables, social support produced
comparisons, perspective taking, and advice, only one significant main effect in all 16 of
which in turn could lessen the tendency to be the analyses, which indicates that the presence
self-blaming for failure (ie., making internal- of social support did not have an all-pervasive
stable-global attributions) with its accompa- effect on mood disturbance. Only among those
nying dysphoric moods. Internals who use persons who we suspect are more instrumental
supports in this manner would then be less and purposeful in their social relations was
prone to depression in the face of adverse cir- the moderator effect found.
cumstances. In essence, internality can be ex- Research concerned with stress is apparently
pected to predict depressive tendencies on the becoming more complex. Although studies of
one hand and resilience on the other, and it the effects of life stress per se once held center
may be the access to social supports that de- stage, research into the functioning of mod-
termines the direction of the relations. The erators has more recently attained prominence.
exact linkages between locus of control, help- Now it seems that a more interactive and com-
lessness attributions, and social supports await plex model is becoming more salient. Processes
further investigation. However, the present such as the receipt of social support are not
data do suggest that social support plays a ma- found to have universal ameliorating effects.
jor role in determining how internals respond In fact, some researchers such as Kobasa
to stress. (1982) have found that social supports can
When examining the main effects for neg- increase the impact of stressors in certain
ative life events throughout these three studies, groups such as lawyers. As Kobasa noted, so-
one may conclude that contrary to the findings cial interactions among lawyers may be a "way
of earlier researchers (Johnson & Sarason, of avoiding the solution of those problems
1978; Kobasa, 1979), locus of control per se brought on by stressful life events, and pre-
may not always operate as a reliable stress venting the development of commitment"
moderator. Although the differences between (1982, p. 716). Social supports, then, may have
subjects classified by the achievement locus of different meanings for persons with differing
control scale are not very large with regard to orientations toward their work and their social
the negative-life-event-mood-disturbance re- interactions.
lation, the important fact is that the results In the studies described in this article, it is
do not support the hypothesized direct mod- equally obvious that locus of control by itself
erator effects of locus of control. The differ- does not produce consistent ameliorating ef-
ences between subjects classified with regard fects. Only in interaction with measures of
to affiliation locus of control, on the other social support do we witness a reliable mod-
hand, almost suggest the opposite. In two of eration of stress effects. Conceivably, it is the
the three studies, internals were found to have more internal individuals who, by regarding
higher magnitude relations between stress and themselves as responsible for their disappoint-
mood disturbance than externals. The reversals ments, draw more useful information from
and uncertainties generated by these data mir- and succor close companionships. If failures
ror conclusions drawn by Lefcourt (1982) were experienced by persons who held them-
when he attempted to summarize the findings selves responsible for those events, emotional
from previous investigations: distress would be likely unless guilt feelings
It is obvious . . . that locus of control does play some could be assuaged. Our guess is that social
role in affecting the ways in which people cope with their support with its implied provision of alter-
MODERATORS OF STRESS 389

native perspectives and opportunities for reat- Lefcourt, H. M., Gronnerud, P., & McDonald, P. (1973).
tributions of blame and shared fate may be Cognitive activity and hypothesis formation during a
double entendre word association test as a function of
the very balm that serves to protect the more locus of control and field dependence. Canadian Journal
solitary adventurers who regard themselves as of Behavioral Science, 5, 161-173.
the agents of their own experiences. Lefcourt, H. M., Miller, R. S., Ware, E E., & Sherk, D.
Finally, we conclude with the obvious point (1981). Locus of control as a modifier of the relationship
to be drawn from these investigations: Inter- between stressors and moods. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 41. 357-369.
actions between personal proclivities and mi- Lefcourt, H. M., von Baeyer, C. L., Ware, E. E., & Cox,
lieu characteristics should always provide bet- D. J. (1979). The multidimensional-multiattributional
ter predictions of criteria such as mood dis- causality scale. Canadian Journal ofBehavioral Science,
turbance than should simpler, unifactor 11, 286-304.
models. Martin, R. A., & Lefcourt, H. M. (1983). The sense of
humor as a moderator of the relation between stressors
and moods. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
References 45, 1313-1324.
Martin, R. A., & Lefcourt, H. M. (1984). The Situational
Humor Response Questionnaire: A quantitative measure
Barrera, M., Sandier, I. N., & Ramsay, T. B. (1981). Pre-
of the sense of humor. Journal ofPersonality and Social
liminary development of a scale of social support Studies
Psychology, 47. 145-155.
on college students. American Journal of Community
McNair, D. M., Loir, M., & Droppleman, L. F. (1971).
Psychology, 9, 435-447.
The profile of mood states San Diego: Educational and
Brown, G. W., Bhrolchain, M. N., & Harris, T. (1975). Industrial Testing Service.
Social class and psychiatric disturbance among women
in an urban population. Sociology, 5, 225-254. Metalsky, G. I., Abramson, L. Y, Seligman, M. E. P.,
Semmel, A., & Peterson, C. (1982). Attribution styles
Eaton, N. W. (1978). Life events, social supports, and
and life events in the classroom: Vulnerability and in-
psychiatric symptoms: A reanalysis of the New Haven
vulnerability to depressive mood reactions. Journal of
data. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 19, 230-
Personality and Social Psychology. 43, 612-617.
234.
Mirels, H. L. (1970). Dimensions of internal versus external
Garmezy, N. (1981). Children under stress: Perspectives
control. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
on antecedents and correlates of vulnerability and re-
34, 226-228.
sistance to psychopathology. In A. I. Rabin, J. Aronoff,
A. M. Barclay, & R. A. Zucker (Eds.), Further explo- Murphy, L. B., & Mortality, A. (1976). Vulnerability, cop-
rations in personality (pp. 196-270). New York: Wiley. ing, and growth from infancy to adolescence. New Haven,
CT: Yale University Press.
Holmes, T. H., & Rahe, R. H. (1967). The social read- Phillips, S. L., & Fischer, C. S. (1981). Measuring social
justment rating scale. Journal of Psychosomatic Re- support networks in general populations. In B. S. Doh-
search, 11, 213-218. renwend & B. P. Dohrenwend (Eds.), Stressful life events
Husaini, B. A. (Ed.) (1982). Stress and psychiatric symp- and their contexts (pp. 223-233). NY: Prodist
toms: Personality and social support as buffers [Special Sandier, I. N., & Lakey, B. (1982). Locus of control as a
issue]. Journal of Community Psychology, iO(4). stress moderator. The role of control perceptions and
Jackson, D. N. (1967). Personality Research Form. Goshen, social support. American Journal of Community Psy-
NY: Research Psychologists Press. chology, 10, 65-80.
Johnson, J. G., & Sarason, I. G. (1978). Life stress, depres-
Sarason, I. G. (1980). Life stress, self-preoccupation, and
sion and anxiety: Internal-external control as a mod-
social supports. In I. G. Sarason and C. D. Spielberger
erator variable. Journal ofPsychosomatic Research, 22,
(Eds.), Stress and anxiety, Vol. 7. Washington, DC:
205-208.
Hemisphere.
Johnson, J. H. & Sarason, I. G. (1979). Moderator variables
Schaefer, C , Coyne, J. C , & Lazarus, R. S. (1981). The
in life stress research. In I. G. Sarason & C. D. Spielberger
health related functions of social support Journal of
(Eds.), Stress and anxiety. Vol. 6 (pp. 151-167). Wash-
Behavioral Medicine. 4, 381-406.
ington, DC: Hemisphere.
Seligman, M. E. P., Abramson, L. Y, Semmel, A., & von
Kobasa, S. C. (1979). Stressful life events, personality, and
Baeyer, C. (1979). Depressive attributional style. Journal
health: An inquiry into hardiness. Journal ofPersonality
of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 242-247.
and Social Psychology, 37, 1-11.
Silver, R. I., & Wortman, C. B. (1981). Coping with un-
Kobasa, S. C. (1982). Commitment and coping in stress
desirable life events. In J. Garber & M. E. P. Seligman
resistance.among lawyers. Journal of Personality and
(Eds.), Human Helplessness. Theory and application (pp.
Social Psychology, 42, 707-717.
279-340). New York: Academic Press.
Lefcourt, H. M. (1981). The construction and development
of the muln'dimensional-multiattributional causality Wolk, S., & DuCette, J. (1974). Intentional performance
scales. In H. M. Lefcourt (Ed.), Research with the Locus and incidental learning as a function of personality and
of Control Construct (Vol. 1, pp. 245-277). New York: task directions. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Academic Press. chology, 29, 90-101.
Lefcourt, H. M. (1982). Locus of control: Current trends
in theory and research, second edition. Hillsdale, NJ: Received October 14, 1982
Erlbaum. Revision received March 18, 1983 •

View publication stats

You might also like