Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Development at Age 36 Months in Children With Deformational Plagiocephaly

Brent R. Collett, Kristen E. Gray, Jacqueline R. Starr, Carrie L. Heike, Michael L.


Cunningham and Matthew L. Speltz
Pediatrics 2013;131;e109; originally published online December 24, 2012;
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2012-1779

The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is
located on the World Wide Web at:
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/131/1/e109.full.html

PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly


publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned,
published, and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point
Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 2013 by the American Academy
of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Univ Of Arizona on January 21, 2013


ARTICLE

Development at Age 36 Months in Children


With Deformational Plagiocephaly
AUTHORS: Brent R. Collett, PhD,a,b Kristen E. Gray, MS,c WHAT’S KNOWN ON THIS SUBJECT: Infants and toddlers with
Jacqueline R. Starr, PhD,c,d,e Carrie L. Heike, MD,f,g Michael L. deformational plagiocephaly (DP) score lower on developmental
Cunningham, MD, PhD,f,g and Matthew L. Speltz, PhDa,b measures than children without DP and lower than expected
Departments of aPsychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, relative to test norms.
cEpidemiology, and fPediatrics, University of Washington, Seattle,

Washington; Department of bPsychiatry and Behavioral Medicine, WHAT THIS STUDY ADDS: This study is the first to examine
and gSeattle Children’s Craniofacial Center, Seattle Children’s
Hospital, Seattle, Washington; dDepartment of Clinical and
developmental outcomes in preschool-aged children with DP
Translational Research, The Forsyth Institute, Cambridge, relative to demographically similar children without DP using
Massachusetts; and eDepartment of Oral Health Policy and a standardized, clinician administered assessment.
Epidemiology, Harvard School of Dental Medicine, Boston,
Massachusetts
KEY WORDS
plagiocephaly, developmental assessment, preschool age, Back to
Sleep
ABBREVIATIONS
abstract
3D—3 dimensional OBJECTIVES: Infants and toddlers with deformational plagiocephaly
BSID-III—Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, Third (DP) have been shown to score lower on developmental measures
Edition
CI—confidence interval than unaffected children. To determine whether these differences
DP—deformational plagiocephaly persist, we examined development in 36-month-old children with
RR—relative risk and without a history of DP.
SES—socioeconomic status
METHODS: Participants included 224 children with DP and 231 children
Dr Collett assisted in study conceptualization and design,
participated in recruitment and data collection, supervised without diagnosed DP, all of who had been followed in a longitudinal
infant/toddler examiners, led data analyses, drafted the initial study since infancy. To confirm the presence or absence of DP, pedia-
manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted; tricians blinded to children’s case status rated 3-dimensional cranial
Ms Gray assisted with conceptualization and study design,
particularly data analyses and interpretation; assisted with images taken when children were 7 months old on average. The
manuscript preparation; provided a critical review of the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, Third Edition
manuscript; and approved the final manuscript as submitted; Dr (BSID-III) was administered as a measure of child development.
Starr assisted in conceptualization and study design,
particularly data analyses and interpretation; provided a critical RESULTS: Children with DP scored lower on all scales of the BSID-III
review of the manuscript; and approved the final manuscript as than children without DP. Differences were largest in cognition,
submitted; Dr Heike assisted with conceptualization and study
language, and parent-reported adaptive behavior (adjusted
design, particularly collection of 3-dimensional cranial imaging
data; provided a critical review of the manuscript; and approved differences = –2.9 to –4.4 standard score points) and smallest in
the final manuscript as submitted; Dr Cunningham assisted with motor development (adjusted difference = –2.7). Children in the
study conceptualization and design, provided medical oversight control group who did not have previously diagnosed DP but who
during participant enrollment (eg, to determine eligibility when
questions arose for children with other medical conditions), were later rated by pediatricians to have at least mild cranial
provided a critical review of the manuscript, and approved the deformation also scored lower on the BSID-III than unaffected controls.
final manuscript as submitted; and Dr Speltz conceptualized and
designed the study, obtained National Institutes of Health
CONCLUSIONS: Preschool-aged children with a history of DP continue
funding, supervised data collection, assisted with data analyses to receive lower developmental scores than unaffected controls. These
and interpretation, provided a critical review of the manuscript, findings do not imply that DP causes developmental problems, but DP
and approved the final manuscript as submitted.
may nonetheless serve as a marker of developmental risk. We
(Continued on last page) encourage clinicians to screen children with DP for developmental
concerns to facilitate early identification and intervention.
Pediatrics 2013;131:e109–e115

PEDIATRICS Volume 131, Number 1, January 2013 e109


Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Univ Of Arizona on January 21, 2013
Deformational plagiocephaly (DP) refers time of their child’s diagnosis at the Measures
to flattening of the infant skull secondary Seattle Children’s Hospital Craniofacial
Severity of Cranial Deformation
to external forces. The prevalence of DP in Center (see Speltz et al10). Patients were
the United States has increased from 5% eligible if they had been diagnosed with Three-dimensional (3D) cranial images
in the 1990s to 20% to 30% at present,1–3 DP by a craniofacial specialist, were were obtained during participants’
an increase largely attributed to the aged 4 to 11 months, and families were Time 1 study visit using the 3dMD Cranial
successful “Back to Sleep” campaign.4 able to complete a study visit within 4 System (see Speltz et al10). Images were
Many clinicians consider DP to be a mi- weeks of the child’s diagnosis. Exclu- deidentified, randomly sorted, and rated
nor cosmetic condition, although DP has sions were (1) history of prematurity for severity by 2 craniofacial pedia-
been associated with heightened risk for (,35 weeks’ gestation); (2) a diagnosed tricians (MC and CH) who were unaware
developmental delays in infants and neurodevelopmental condition, brain of DP status. A 4-point ordinal scale
toddlers.5–10 Data on the persistence of injury, or significant hearing or vision (none, mild, moderate, severe) was used
DP-associated delays are less consis- impairment; (3) presence of a major to rate cranial deformation. The mean
tent.11–13 Existing studies are limited by malformation or $3 minor extracranial of the 2 raters’ scores was used
the use of parent observations rather anomalies15; (4) a non-English-speaking to represent the overall severity of
than clinician-administered measures, mother; (5) history of adoption or out-of- each participant’s cranial deformation.
and most have relied on retrospective home placement; and (6) family plans Interrater agreement (weighted k) was
evaluations of development and com- to move out of state before project 0.72 for severity category and (k) 0.80
parisons to test norms. completion. We recruited 235 infants for the presence or absence of any
with DP between June 2006 and deformation.
To determine whether DP is associated
withdevelopmentfromdiagnosisthrough February 2009, representing 52% of
eligible patients. Participants were Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler
age 3 years, we initiated a longitudinal
similar to nonparticipants with re- Development, Third Edition (BSID-III)
study of 235 children diagnosed with
DP and 237 demographically similar gard to demographic characteristics The BSID-III14 yields composite scores
controls. Participants were previously and DP severity.10 for cognitive, language, and motor de-
assessed at an average age of 7 and 18 velopment and for parent reports of
Infants Without DP the child’s adaptive behavior. Subscale
months (Time 1 and Time 2, respec-
tively) using the Bayley Scales of Infant In addition to the exclusions listed for scores are derived for expressive and
and Toddler Development–Third Edition infants with DP, infants without DP were receptive language and for fine and
(BSID-III).14 At both assessments, chil- excluded if they had been diagnosed gross motor development. Raw scores
dren with DP received lower BSID-III with DP or any other craniofacial are converted to norm-referenced
scores than controls.5,10 In this study, anomaly. The first 8 infants in this group standard scores (average = 100, SD =
we sought to examine whether (1) were identified through pediatric prac- 15) for composite scales and scaled
these group differences persisted at tices. Remaining infants were identified scores (average = 10, SD = 3) for lan-
age 36 months (Time 3), (2) findings from a pool of families who agreed to be guage and motor subscales. Gesta-
were altered by participation in de- contacted for research participation tional age was calculated using
velopmental interventions, and (3) when their child was born. Parents were maternal report of due date and birth
outcomes among cases were affected contacted by phone when their child was date. We corrected BSID-III scores for
by demographic and clinical variables. 4 to 11 months old, and those who prematurity for children born between
expressed interest in the project were 35 and 37 weeks’ gestation and for
METHODS screened to determine eligibility. We those born at 37 weeks’ gestation but
selected controls who were most weighing ,6 pounds. The BSID-III was
Participants
similar to infants in the DP cohort in administered by trained psychome-
Participants were enrolled after obtain- terms of infants’age, gender, ethnicity, trists, who were blinded to children’s
ing informed consent using procedures and family socioeconomic status (SES).16 case status, although on occasion this
approved by the Institutional Review Two hundred thirty-seven infants with- may have been compromised by some
Board at Seattle Children’s Hospital. out known DP were recruited between children’s appearance or information
March 2007 and February 2009, repre- shared by parents. Assessments were
Infants With DP senting 90% of those eligible for partic- videotaped, and ∼10% were reviewed
The parents of infants with DP were ipation. Twenty-seven families declined for reliability by one of the authors
approached for participation at the participation. (BC). Scoring agreement on individual

e110 COLLETT et al
Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Univ Of Arizona on January 21, 2013
ARTICLE

items (k) was 0.84 to 0.90. Mothers were (infants with a diagnosis of DP, con- Table 1) and may have improved their
asked to complete the BSID-III adaptive firmed with clinician ratings), Unaf- developmental outcomes on average.
development scale and return the form fected Controls (infants with no previous We therefore repeated the primary
by mail. diagnosis of DP and no evidence of DP analyses using censored normal re-
with clinician ratings), and DP Controls gression.17 This approach assumes
Medical and Intervention History (infants without previously diagnosed that we cannot know how participants
Interviews were completed with moth- DP but detectable dysmorphology based receiving intervention would have
ers at Time 1 to document demographic on our pediatricians’ ratings of 3D scored without treatment, but their
characteristics and medical history, images). estimated scores would likely have
including history of suspected or con- Linear regression analyses with robust been at least as low as their observed
firmed torticollis. At Time 3, an abbre- standard errors were used to compare scores (ie, their scores are “left cen-
viated interview was conducted for Time 3 standard scores for DP cases sored”). This method therefore pro-
cases and controls to obtain informa- and unaffected controls on the BSID-III vides an estimate of bias by accounting
tion about newly diagnosed medical cognitive, language, motor, and adap- for the anticipated increase in BSID-III
conditions and participation in de- tive behavior composites. We also ex- scores after intervention. Analyses
velopmental interventions. Because the amined differences in scaled scores on were completed by using the Stata SE
duration of services varied, partic- BSID-III language and motor subscales. 12.0 software package.18
ipants were only classified as receiving In categorical analyses, we used Pois-
an intervention if they had $4 treatment son regression models with robust RESULTS
sessions or $2 months of monitoring in standard errors to estimate the relative
a “Birth-to-Three” early intervention Two hundred fifteen children with and
risk (RR) of children with and without
224 children without diagnosed DP
program. These cutoffs were used to DP receiving a standard score ,85 on
differentiate children receiving only completed the BSID-III at Time 3. A
the BSID-III composite scales, a conven- parent interview or BSID-III adaptive
assessments from those participating tional threshold for risk of develop-
in ongoing intervention. For cases, the behavior scale was completed for an
mental delay. We adjusted all analyses additional 9 children with DP and 7
Time 3 interview also documented the for children’s age (in months), gender,
use of orthotic treatments for DP. controls, resulting in partial or com-
race/ethnicity (white and non-Hispanic plete data for 224 children with DP and
vs non-White or Hispanic), and SES. 231 children without DP (95% and 97%
Assessment Procedures In secondary analyses, we used linear of the original cohorts, respectively).
We scheduled participants’ Time 3 visit regression to compare average BSID-III Compared with participants, families
within 12 weeks of the child’s 3-year scores of unaffected controls and DP lost to follow-up (11 children with DP, 6
birthday and set an upper age limit of controls. Among cases, we examined children without DP) had lower SES
42 months. Parents unable to partici- developmental outcomes as a function (71% Hollingshead categories III–IV
pate in a full assessment completed of initial severity, coded as “mild” or compared with 28% among partic-
the interview by phone and were “moderate to severe.” In separate re- ipants), and a greater proportion were
mailed the BSID-III adaptive behavior gression analyses, we also examined non-White or Hispanic (59% compared
scale. After testing, psychometrists in- developmental outcomes among cases with 35% among participants). Partic-
dicated whether they considered the as a function of children’s gender; his- ipants and nonparticipants were similar
evaluation “valid” or “invalid” due to tory of suspected or confirmed torti- in their average DP severity at Time 1.
child behavior (eg, noncompliance) or collis; and history of orthotic treatment. Children with and without DP were
testing circumstances (eg, child ill- In addition to adjusting for demographic predominantly male, of white race, and
ness). One or more BSID-III scores were covariates, we adjusted these analyses of middle to upper SES (Table 1). The
dropped for 1 child with DP and 6 for Time 1 DP severity (continuous). mean age at Time 3 for children in both
children without DP due to examiner The foregoing analyses do not account groups was ∼36 months. Seventy-nine
ratings of invalidity. for the fact that some children received children with DP (35.3%) received or-
developmental interventions before thotic treatment. Eighty-two children
Data Analyses being assessed. This could bias case- with DP (36.6%) and 15 children with-
We used pediatricians’ ratings of Time 1 control differences toward the null out DP (6.5%) participated in $4 in-
3D surface images to categorize par- because these interventions were far tervention sessions or $2 months of
ticipants into 3 groups: DP Cases more common among DP cases (see Birth-to-Three monitoring.

PEDIATRICS Volume 131, Number 1, January 2013 e111


Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Univ Of Arizona on January 21, 2013
TABLE 1 Demographic and Clinical Characteristics for Children With DP (Cases) and Without The smallest difference was in motor
Deformational Plagiocephaly (Controls)
development (adjusted difference = –
Characteristic Cases (n = 224) Controls (n = 231) 2.7, 95% CI = –5.1 to –0.3). DP cases
n % n % also scored lower than unaffected
Gender controls on the receptive language,
Male 145 64.7 139 60.2 expressive language, and fine motor
Female 79 35.3 92 39.8
subscales.
Age in months at Time 3 (mean, SD)a 36.5(1.2) 36.0(1.1)
Race/ethnicity DP cases were more likely than un-
Caucasian 153 68.3 144 62.3 affected controls to receive scores ,85
Asian/Pacific Islander 12 5.4 12 5.2
Black/African American 0 – 6 2.6 in language (RR = 7.9, 95% CI = 1.8–
Hispanic/Latino 26 11.6 28 12.1 35.1), motor (RR = 4.3, 95% CI = 1.0–
Mixed race/Other 33 14.7 41 17.7 17.9), and cognitive development (RR
Familial SES (mean, SD) 47.1 (12.4) 46.9 (11.6)
I (high) 80 35.7 60 26.0
was not calculated because none of the
II 89 39.7 100 43.3 controls scored in the delayed range;
III 34 15.2 46 19.9 Table 3). Children with DP were only
IV 15 6.7 20 8.7
slightly more likely than controls to
V (low) 6 2.7 5 2.2
History of torticollisb score in the delayed range on the
Suspected 9 4.0 3 1.3 adaptive behavior (parent report)
Confirmed 89 39.7 2 0.9 composite (RR = 1.7, 95% CI = 0.7–4.4).
None 126 56.3 226 97.8
Orthotic helmet or band therapyb
Yes 79 35.3 0 – Secondary Analyses
No 145 64.7 231 100.0
Developmental interventionsb DP controls scored lower than unaf-
Physical or occupational therapy 104 46.4 11 4.8 fected controls on all BSID-III composite
Speech/language therapy 34 15.2 14 6.1 scales, with adjusted group differences
Birth-to-Three monitoring 31 13.8 16 6.9
Other 38 17.0 18 7.8
of –2.9 to –3.5 standard score points.
Any Developmental Interventionsc 122 54.5 38 16.5 DP controls also scored lower on the
$4 intervention sessions or $2 mo Birth-to-Three early 82 36.6 15 6.5 receptive language (adjusted differ-
intervention monitoring
ence = –0.56, 95% CI = –1.1 to 0.0) and
DP severity in infancy
None 2 0.9 163 70.6 fine motor subscale (adjusted differ-
Mild 101 45.1 66 28.6 ence = –0.76, 95% CI = –1.5 to 0.0).
Moderate-severe 121 54.0 2 0.9
a
Among DP cases, outcomes were sim-
Range in age = 34.0 to 41.7 mo in cases, 33.3 to 40.9 mo in controls.
b Variables assessed at Time 3, including children with any history of the condition or treatment. ilar for children with “mild” and
c Refers to children who received $1 of the listed interventions by Time 3.
“moderate-severe” DP and for those
with and without a history of orthotic
Fifty-four percent of cases had “mod- other medical conditions that could af- treatment. Female DP cases scored
erate” or “severe” DP based on pedia- fect neurodevelopment.5 lower than male DP cases in cognitive
tricians’ reviews of subjects’ 3D images development (adjusted difference = –
This left 215 DP cases, 163 unaffected
at Time 1 (Table 1). Among controls, 163 2.7, 95% CI = –5.1 to –0.3), although
controls and 66 DP controls foranalysis.
(70.6%) had no discernible skull dys- their scores were similar in all other
morphology, 66 (28.6%) had “mild” areas. Average scores were similar for
dysmorphology, and 2 (0.9%) had Developmental Outcomes for DP children with and without a history of
“moderate” to “severe” dysmorphol- Cases Versus Unaffected Controls torticollis except on the composite
ogy. We excluded from further analyses Cases scored lower on average than motor and gross motor scales, on
2 children with diagnosed DP who did unaffected controls on all BSID-III which cases with torticollis scored on
not have discernible skull dysmorphol- composite scales (Table 2). The larg- average 4.2 and 0.6 points higher, re-
ogy. We also excluded from analysis 7 est differences were observed in lan- spectively (95% CI = 1.0–7.4 and 0.1–
children with DP and 2 children without guage (adjusted difference = –4.4, 95% 1.1, respectively).
DP (both of who had evidence of dys- confidence interval [CI] = –6.8 to –2.0) In censored normal regression analyses,
morphology on 3D imaging) who, after and cognitive development (adjusted the magnitude of all differences between
study enrollment, were diagnosed with difference = –2.9 95% CI = –4.6 to –1.1). DP cases and unaffected controls were

e112 COLLETT et al
Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Univ Of Arizona on January 21, 2013
ARTICLE

TABLE 2 Average Standardizeda BSID-III Scores and Adjustedb Group Comparisons for Children vate the risk of developmental delays
With DP (Cases) and Without DP (Controls)c
(eg, prematurity, sensory impairments).
BSID-III Domain Cases, n = 215 Controls, n = 163 Case vs unaffected control differences
We observed few differences among DP
mean (SD) mean (SD)
95% CI cases as a function of clinical and de-
Adj. diff. Lower Upper P value mographic characteristics. Consistent
Cognitive d
97.3 (8.3) 99.4 (8.2) 22.9 24.6 21.1 .001 with the study by Miller and Clarren,12
Languaged 105.0 (11.9) 108.7 (10.8) 24.4 26.8 22.0 ,.0005 a large proportion of children with DP
Receptive languaged 10.9 (2.1) 11.3 (1.9) 20.6 21.1 20.2 .005
received developmental services in the
Expressive languaged 10.8 (2.2) 11.6 (2.1) 20.9 21.3 20.4 ,.0005
Motord 100.4 (11.1) 102.5 (10.5) 22.7 25.1 20.3 .030 community. Assuming even a modest
Fine motord 10.6 (2.4) 11.1 (2.3) 20.6 21.1 20.1 .022 treatment effect, use of interventions
Gross motord 9.5 (1.8) 9.7 (1.9) 20.3 20.7 0.1 .185
Adaptive behaviore 101.1 (13.9) 104.8 (13.0) 24.0 27.0 20.9 .011
likely reduced the magnitude of ob-
a Standard scores for the cognitive, language, motor, and adaptive behavior composite scales have a normative mean of 100
served group differences, as is sug-
and SD of 15. Scaled scores for the receptive language, expressive language, fine motor, and gross motor subscales have gested by our findings using censored
a normative mean of 10 and SD of 3.
b Adjusted for child age (in mo), gender, SES (Hollingshead total, measured continuously), and ethnicity (white, non-Hispanic normal regression analyses. Finally,
vs nonwhite or Hispanic). “DP controls” (ie, infants enrolled as
c Data were dropped for 2 children in the DP group who did not have evidence of DP based on clinician ratings of 3D head

photographs and 68 children in the non-DP group who had mild or greater posterior skull flattening or asymmetry. Data were
healthy controls without previously di-
also dropped for 7 children with DP and 2 children without DP who were diagnosed with medical conditions with de- agnosed DP, but later found to have skull
velopmental implications after enrollment in the study.
d Scores rated as “invalid” by the examiner were dropped from analyses. At Time 3, cognitive scores were rated invalid for 1 dysmorphology on 3D imaging) consis-
child without DP; language scores were rated invalid for 3 children without DP; and motor scores were rated invalid for 1 tently scored lower on the BSID-III than
child with DP and 5 children without DP.
e Adaptive behavior data were missing for 31 children with DP and 18 children without DP at Time 3. Another 3 children with unaffected controls. This suggests that
DP and 3 children without DP were not seen for a clinic assessment, and only parent-reported adaptive behavior data were the association between DP and de-
available.
velopment is not due merely to bias in
patients referred to a specialty clinic.
TABLE 3 Percentage of Children With DP (Cases) and Without DP (Controls)a Who Received
Standard Scores ,85 on the BSID-III and Adjusted RRb These observations are consistent with
BSID-III domain Percent Delayed (Standard Score ,85) Adjusted RR
comparisons at ages 7 and 18 months in
this cohort,5,10 and they provide the
95% Confidence
Interval
first evidence of a persistent associa-
tion between DP and development in
Cases (n = 215) Controls (n = 163) RR Lower Upper
preschoolers using clinician-administered
Cognitivec,d 3.1 0.0 — — —
Languagec 5.3 1.2 7.9 1.8 35.1
assessments. Although the strength of
Motorc 5.8 1.8 4.3 1.0 17.9 the association between DP and de-
Adaptivee 6.2 4.8 1.7 0.7 4.4 velopment is relatively modest, its clini-
a Data were dropped for 2 children in the DP group who did not have evidence of DP based on clinician ratings of 3D head cal relevance is suggested by the
photographs, and 68 children in the non-DP group who had mild or greater posterior skull flattening or asymmetry. Data
were also dropped for 7 children with DP and 2 children without DP who were diagnosed with medical conditions with consistency of this association over time
developmental implications after enrollment in the study. and the persistence of group differences
b Adjusted for child age (in mo), gender, SES (Hollingshead total, measured continuously), and ethnicity (white, non-Hispanic

vs nonwhite or Hispanic). despite the high proportion of children


c Scores rated as “invalid” by the examiner were dropped from analyses. At Time 3, cognitive scores were rated invalid for 1
with DP who received developmental
child without DP; language scores were rated invalid for 3 children without DP; and motor scores were rated invalid for 1
child with DP and 5 children without DP. intervention. Compared with those ob-
d RR was not calculated for cognitive scores given that no children in the unaffected control group scored ,85.
e Adaptive behavior data were missing for 31 children with DP and 18 children without DP at Time 3. Another 3 children with
served during infancy, group differences
DP and 3 children without DP were not seen for a clinic assessment, and only parent reported adaptive behavior data were in gross motor development were di-
available. minished, whereas differences per-
sisted or increased in language and
greater after accounting for the esti- DISCUSSION cognition. This specificity may reflect the
mated effects of developmental inter- nature of the developmental inter-
Children with a history of DP continued
ventions. Group differences favoring to score lower than unaffected controls ventions received because physical and
controls increased by 2.0 to 3.3 stan- on the BSID-III at age 36 months. Im- occupational therapy were the most
dard score points on the BSID-III com- portantly, this observation is based on common interventions (46.4% of cases,
posites and 0.6 to 0.7 scaled score a sample from which we excluded cases compared with 4.8% of controls) and
points on BSID-III subscales. and controls with conditions that ele- may have ameliorated motor deficits.

PEDIATRICS Volume 131, Number 1, January 2013 e113


Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Univ Of Arizona on January 21, 2013
We know of only 1 other study that positioning practices and neurodevelop- population in ways that would be difficult
tracked the development of children mental vulnerability. Although the ma- to measure but that might nonetheless
with DP to preschool age,11 which relied jority of children are now placed in affect developmental outcomes. Fur-
on parent-reported developmental supine position for sleep, only a mi- thermore, the participation rate among
outcomes and test norms for compar- nority develop DP. Those with de- children with DP was lower than desired,
ison. In that study, 11% of children with velopmental concerns, who may be in part because of the requirement that
DP aged 3 to 5 years scored in the less able to reposition themselves, may families be seen for the study #4 weeks
“delayed” range relative to test norms be more likely to develop skull de- after the child’s diagnosis. However, we
on the Ages and Stages Question- formation. This idea is consistent with did not find evidence of participation
naire.19 One risk when relying on test data reported by Fowler et al6 who bias as a function of demographic (eg,
norms for comparison is that research found that infants with DP were more SES) or clinical (eg, DP severity) varia-
participants may differ from the nor- likely than controls to have abnormal bles.10 The rate of developmental in-
mative sample in characteristics that muscle tone on neurologic exams. DP tervention might have been elevated by
are associated with development (eg, might therefore serve as a physical feedback provided to participants as
SES), potentially leading to an un- “marker” of developmental risk, evi- part of this study. We provided all fam-
derestimation of the risk of scoring dent before delays are fully manifest ilies with feedback regarding their
below a clinical cutoff relative to de- and testable, which could be used to child’s BSID-III scores, and when chil-
mographically similar peers. In our identify children who need additional dren scored in the “at-risk” range on the
sample, few children scored in the “at- evaluation and intervention. BSID-III we recommended follow-up with
risk” range compared with the pro- That the severity of DP in infancy was the child’s primary care provider. This
portion indicated by test norms (see unrelated to developmental outcomes likely prompted some families who
Table 3), likely reflecting the middle to at Time 3 argues further against the otherwise would not have received as-
high SES of the families in our sample notion that DP causes later de- sessment to seek out developmental
and the exclusion of children with velopmental delays. Using the model intervention in the community. However,
known neurodevelopmental liabilities. proposed here, it may be that early this should have had a similar effect on
Nonetheless, children with DP in this neurodevelopmental concerns place an families in both groups, and therefore
study were ∼2 to 8 times more likely to infant at risk for some degree of DP, with does not account for the disparity be-
receive “at-risk” scores than unaffected severity of deformation mediated by tween DP cases and controls. Moreover,
controls, and we would expect the ab- parents’ positioning practices. Or censored regression analyses sug-
solute percentage of delayed children parents may be better motivated to gested that accounting for this source
with DP to be higher in samples with participate in repositioning and other of bias would only magnify the observed
broader representation of SES. exercises when their child’s skull de- differences.
Although we cannot rule out DP as the formity is severe, with beneficial
cause of developmental delays, a more effects on later development. CONCLUSIONS
parsimonious explanation for the ob- Strengths of this study included ob- Developmental differences between
served case-control differences is the jective measures of development, 3D children with and without DP persist
reverse: infants with early develop- imaging of children with and without through age 36 months. Additional
mental risk are more apt to develop DP. diagnosed DP to identify skull dysmor- study is needed to determine whether
There is some evidence for this possi- phology, the inclusion of demogra- these differences continue in school-
bility in population-based studies,2 and phically similarcontrolsforcomparison, aged children and to confirm these
the high prevalence of previously un- and an excellent retention rate. Limi- findings in prospectively studied
diagnosed medical conditions that tations include recruitment of partic- population-based samples. In the
emerged in our sample of children with ipants through a specialty craniofacial meantime, we encourage clinicians to
DP suggests an underlying develop- clinic (DP cases) and via a participant provide developmental screening and
mental liability that preceded skull pool (controls), rather than population- monitoring for infants with DP and to
deformation. It may be that DP is based sampling. Children in both sam- offer early intervention services when
the result of the interaction between ples might differ from the general warranted.

e114 COLLETT et al
Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Univ Of Arizona on January 21, 2013
ARTICLE

REFERENCES
1. Argenta LC, David LR, Wilson JA, Bell WO. An 7. Hutchison BL, Stewart AW, Mitchell EA. 105(2). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/
increase in infant cranial deformity with Characteristics, head shape measure- cgi/content/full/105/2/E26
supine sleeping position. J Craniofac Surg. ments and developmental delay in 287 13. Steinbok P, Lam D, Singh S, Mortenson PA,
1996;7(1):5–11 consecutive infants attending a plagioce- Singhal A. Long-term outcome of infants
2. McKinney CM, Cunningham ML, Holt VL, phaly clinic. Acta Paediatr. 2009;98(9): with positional occipital plagiocephaly.
Leroux B, Starr JR. Characteristics of 2733 1494–1499 Childs Nerv Syst. 2007;23(11):1275–1283
cases diagnosed with deformational pla- 8. Kennedy E, Majnemer A, Farmer JP, Barr 14. Bayley N. Manual for the Bayley Scales of
giocephaly and changes in risk factors RG, Platt RW. Motor development of infants Infant and Toddler Development. 3rd ed.
over time. Cleft Palate Craniofac J. 2008;45 with positional plagiocephaly. Phys Occup San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corpo-
(2):208–216 Ther Pediatr. 2009;29(3):222–235 ration; 2006
3. Kane AA, Mitchell LE, Craven KP, Marsh JL. 9. Kordestani RK, Panchal J. Neuro- 15. Leppig KA, Werler MM, Cann CI, Cook CA,
Observations on a recent increase in pla- development delays in children with de- Holmes LB. Predictive value of minor
giocephaly without synostosis. Pediatrics. formational plagiocephaly. Plast Reconstr anomalies. I. Association with major mal-
1996;97(6 pt 1):877–885 Surg. 2006;118(3):808–809, author reply formations. J Pediatr. 1987;110(4):531–537
4. Turk AE, McCarthy JG, Thorne CH, Wisoff JH. 809–810 16. Hollingshead AB. Four Factor Index of So-
The “back to sleep campaign” and de- 10. Speltz ML, Collett BR, Stott-Miller M, et al. cial Status. New Haven, CT: Yale University;
formational plagiocephaly: is there cause Case-control study of neurodevelopment in 1975
for concern? J Craniofac Surg. 1996;7(1): deformational plagiocephaly. Pediatrics. 17. Tobin MD, Sheehan NA, Scurrah KJ, Burton
12–18 2010;125(3). Available at: www.pediatrics. PR. Adjusting for treatment effects in
5. Collett BR, Starr JR, Kartin D, et al. De- org/cgi/content/full/125/3/e537 studies of quantitative traits: antihyper-
velopment in toddlers with and without 11. Hutchison BL, Stewart AW, Mitchell EA. De- tensive therapy and systolic blood pres-
deformational plagiocephaly. Arch Pediatr formational plagiocephaly: a follow-up of sure. Stat Med. 2005;24(19):2911–2935
Adolesc Med. 2011;165(7):653–658 head shape, parental concern and neuro- 18. StataCorp. Stata Statistical Software; Re-
6. Fowler EA, Becker DB, Pilgram TK, Noetzel development at ages 3 and 4 years. Arch lease 12. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP
M, Epstein J, Kane AA. Neurologic find- Dis Child. 2011;96(1):85–90 19. Squires J, Twombly E, Bricker D, Potter R.
ings in infants with deformational pla- 12. Miller RI, Clarren SK. Long-term de- Behavior—Ages & Stages Questionnaires.
giocephaly. J Child Neurol. 2008;23(7): velopmental outcomes in patients with de- 3rd ed. Eugene, OR: University of Oregon,
742–747 formational plagiocephaly. Pediatrics. 2000; Center on Human Development; 2009

(Continued from first page)


www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/peds.2012-1779
doi:10.1542/peds.2012-1779
Accepted for publication Sep 17, 2012
Address correspondence to Brent R. Collett, PhD, Seattle Children’s Research Institute, 2001 8th Ave, Suite 600, Mailstop CW8-6, Seattle, WA 98121. E-mail: bcollett@u.
washington.edu
PEDIATRICS (ISSN Numbers: Print, 0031-4005; Online, 1098-4275).
Copyright © 2013 by the American Academy of Pediatrics
FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.
FUNDING: This publication was made possible in part by grant 1 R01 HD046565 from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHHD) to Dr
Speltz and grant 1 UL1 RR025014 from the National Center for Research Resources (NCRR), components of the National Institutes of Health (NIH). Its contents are
solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official view of NICHHD, NCRR, or NIH. Funded by the National Institutes of Health (NIH).

PEDIATRICS Volume 131, Number 1, January 2013 e115


Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Univ Of Arizona on January 21, 2013
Development at Age 36 Months in Children With Deformational Plagiocephaly
Brent R. Collett, Kristen E. Gray, Jacqueline R. Starr, Carrie L. Heike, Michael L.
Cunningham and Matthew L. Speltz
Pediatrics 2013;131;e109; originally published online December 24, 2012;
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2012-1779
Updated Information & including high resolution figures, can be found at:
Services http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/131/1/e109.full.h
tml
References This article cites 15 articles, 5 of which can be accessed free
at:
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/131/1/e109.full.h
tml#ref-list-1
Subspecialty Collections This article, along with others on similar topics, appears in
the following collection(s):
Developmental/Behavior
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/developme
ntal:behavior
Permissions & Licensing Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures,
tables) or in its entirety can be found online at:
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/site/misc/Permissions.xh
tml
Reprints Information about ordering reprints can be found online:
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/site/misc/reprints.xhtml

PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly


publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned, published,
and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elk
Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 2013 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All
rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Univ Of Arizona on January 21, 2013

You might also like