Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 26

ELECTION LAW JOURNAL

Volume 00, Number 0, 2019 Articles


# Mary Ann Liebert, Inc.
DOI: 10.1089/elj.2018.0539

Does Compulsory Voting Foster Civic Duty to Vote?

Fernando Feitosa, André Blais, and Ruth Dassonneville

ABSTRACT
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

Research has established that compulsory voting rules have a strong effect on turnout. There are good rea-
sons to think compulsory voting has such an effect because it leads citizens to feel a stronger sense of civic
duty. In this study, we put the link between compulsory voting rules and civic duty to a test by examining
whether Chile’s abolition of compulsory voting rules, in 2012, affected citizens’ belief in the duty to vote.
Our results are consistent with the claim that compulsory voting rules strengthen citizens’ sense of civic
duty to vote, a finding that stands up to several robustness checks.

Keywords: compulsory voting rules; abolition; civic duty to vote; Chile; quasi-experiment; synthetic
control model

INTRODUCTION elections (Engelen 2007). Research has tested the


latter argument and found no evidence that civic

T he mobilizing effect of compulsory voting


is one of the most consistent findings in the
field of elections and voting behavior. Both individ-
duty is higher when voting is mandated than when
it is not (Maldonado 2015; Rangel 2017). We
argue that those null findings are in fact due to the
ual and aggregate level research shows that turnout methodology employed by researchers—which we
is higher in countries and regions that mandate vot- discuss in more detail below. Studying the relation-
ing (Cancela and Geys 2016; Stockemer 2017). In ship between compulsory voting rules and civic
countries that enforce compulsory voting rules in par- duty by leveraging the abolition of compulsory vot-
ticular, a substantial part of the electorate turns out to ing in Chile, we believe we are better capable of
vote (Birch 2009; Singh 2011). assessing whether those rules exert indeed an effect
However, the ways in which compulsory voting on civic duty or not.
influences turnout are still debated. Some argue In taking advantage of this quasi-experiment, we
that compulsory voting has an indirect effect on perform a synthetic control model to estimate a
turnout, by increasing the costs of abstaining (Pan- post-2012 Chile that simulates a context in which
agopoulos 2008). Others claim that compulsory vot- Chile still mandated voting after 2012. Comparing
ing augments the belief in the duty to vote and, Chile to its synthetic control suggests that Chile’s
consequently, the number of citizens who vote in transition to voluntary voting led to a substantial de-
cline in citizens’ belief of the duty to vote, which
might explain the strong decline in turnout follow-
ing the 2012 reform, given the attitude’s strong in-
Fernando Feitosa is a PhD candidate in the Department of Polit-
ical Science at the University of Montréal in Montréal, Canada.
fluence in the voting decision (Blais 2000; Clarke
André Blais is a full professor in the Department of Political et al. 2004). Our findings are confirmed by placebo
Science at the University of Montréal in Montréal, Canada, tests in which we replicate the analyses with all con-
and holds the Research Chair in Electoral Studies. Ruth Das- trol units, and by several robustness checks.
sonneville is an assistant professor in the Department of Polit-
ical Science at the University of Montréal in Montréal, Canada, In shedding light on the mechanism through which
and holds the Canada Research Chair in Electoral Democracy. compulsory voting likely affects turnout, we make a

1
2 FEITOSA ET AL.

number of important theoretical contributions. First, conclude that compulsory voting rules indeed reduce
our study connects aggregate-level work on the deter- income inequality, in line with Lijphart’s (1997) prop-
minants of turnout to individual-level theories of what ositions. Not all scholarly work on compulsory voting
explains turnout: While scholars often refer to the ef- highlights its benefits, however. A number of scholars
fects of the sanctions that are associated with non- warn that compulsory voting has negative side effects
voting to explain the positive effect of compulsory as well, specifically when it comes to the ‘‘quality’’ of
voting on citizens’ electoral participation, research the vote (Dassonneville et al. 2018; Selb and Lachat
on the individual-level determinants of voter turnout 2009), satisfaction with democracy (Singh 2018),
has shown that the cost of voting contributes little and the amount of invalid and blank balloting that
to explaining why citizens turn out to vote (Blais is cast in elections (Barnes and Rangel 2018; Singh
2000). In contrast, civic duty is a strong individual- 2017).
level predictor of citizen engagement in elections, Given that the mobilizing effect of compulsory
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

thus representing a more plausible explanation for voting is not debated, recent scholarship has shifted
why compulsory voting bolsters the number of voters the focus to studying its indirect effects. We argue,
in elections. Second, the origins of civic duty are still however, that key questions regarding the link be-
very much unknown. Research has often focused on tween compulsory voting and turnout remain to be
individual-level determinants of civic duty, thus ig- answered. In particular: How is a legal obligation
noring macro-level factors. We show that institutions to vote translated into higher turnout rates? Few
can also affect citizens’ adherence to the voting studies offer insights into this question. Among
norm, a finding that suggests the importance of look- them, Panagopoulos (2008) argues that turnout is
ing at the bigger picture when investigating what higher in compulsory voting systems because man-
drives individuals to believe in the duty to vote. datory voting implies the introduction of an addi-
tional cost to abstention. Panagopoulos expands
Downs’ classical rational calculus of voting to in-
COMPULSORY VOTING AND CIVIC DUTY corporate two cost terms: one, regarding the act of
voting (C’’); the other, the act of abstaining in elec-
Compulsory voting entails a legal obligation to tions (C#$’’). Panagopoulos argues that compulsory
visit the polling station on Election Day or to par- voting rules increase the value of C#$’’, while C’’ re-
ticipate in a given election by other means, such as mains constant. Because he does not observe a dif-
by proxy voting, advance voting, or voting by mail ference between systems with symbolic penalties
(Birch 2009). Past work has documented sev- and those with voluntary voting, Panagopoulos con-
eral benefits from the legal compulsion to vote. cludes that compulsory voting has no effect on turn-
Countries that adopt compulsory voting rules and out beyond the abstention costs, an argument that is
that enforce them, in particular, generally have a often used to explain the positive effect of compul-
higher number of citizens who vote in all levels sory voting on turnout.
of elections than countries that adopt compulsory Still, there are good reasons to think other mech-
voting rules but that do not enforce them and anisms are at play as well. Singh and Thornton
countries in which voting is left for individuals (2013) and De Leon and Rizzi (2016), for instance,
to decide (Birch 2009; Norris 2004).1 Some re- demonstrate that partisanship increases when vot-
searchers go further and argue that, by augmenting ing is mandatory. The authors argue that when cit-
turnout, compulsory voting serves as an impor- izens have the legal obligation to vote, they are
tant mechanism for reducing socio-demographic
inequalities in voting (Dassonneville and Hooghe
2017; Lijphart 1997) and for adopting redistribu- 1
A recent study by Barnes and Rangel (2018) contradicts this
tive policies (Bechtel et al. 2016; Fowler 2013). argument, by showing that the abolition of compulsory voting
rules, combined with the transition to automatic registration,
The findings of Carey and Horiuchi (2017) are led to a lower electoral participation in some Chilean districts,
particularly insightful in this regard. Analyzing but not in others. For this reason, and to account for a potential
the consequences of the abolition of compulsory overrepresentation of urban respondents, we conduct additional
voting rules in Venezuela they find that the abo- tests with individuals living in rural areas only. We furthermore
acknowledge as a limitation of our study our focus on the con-
lition of compulsory voting was followed by a sequences of the abolition of compulsory voting to citizens’
decline of redistributive policies. They therefore sense of civic duty at the national level.
DOES COMPULSORY VOTING FOSTER CIVIC DUTY TO VOTE? 3

likely to resort to shortcuts—such as partisanship— to vote, including civic duty.4 Maldonado tests this ar-
for deciding which candidate to support in an elec- gument empirically in a lab experiment in which 760
tion and, thus, to reduce the cost associated with the Peruvians were exposed to three different messages.
voting decision. Other studies offer evidence that These messages served to remind participants of the
compulsory voting rules spur the acquisition of po- monetary, the non-monetary, or both monetary and
litical information either voluntarily or accidentally non-monetary punishments for abstention, and thus
(Gordon and Segura 1997; Sheppard 2015; Shine- of the fact that voting is compulsory. Participants in
man 2018), which may lead to a preference over the control group are not exposed to any message.
which candidate should be elected.2 By acting on Maldonado finds that reminding participants of the
those variables, compulsory voting might thus affect monetary and social consequences of abstention
the expected benefit from voting and in this way in- does not affect their sense of civic duty. That is,
fluence turnout. We contribute to this literature by there are no statistically significant differences in
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

focusing on a third factor that might similarly ex- civic duty between those in the treatment groups
plain the link between compulsory voting rules and in the control group. These findings led Maldo-
and turnout: civic duty. nado to conclude that compulsory voting does not in-
Civic duty represents a belief that one has a moral fluence civic duty.
obligation to vote in an election (Blais 2000). Civic Rangel (2017) further tested the association be-
duty is generally thought of as an important turnout tween compulsory voting rules and civic duty. She
determinant. Campbell et al. (1960), for instance, did so by means of a cross-national analysis of
report a turnout difference of close to 60 percentage data from the 2010 Latinobarómetro. More specifi-
points between those who consider voting a duty cally, Rangel regressed civic duty on compulsory
and those who consider it a matter of personal voting and found that civic duty is not significantly
choice. In addition, Blais et al. (2000) offer evi- different, in statistical terms, in Latin American
dence that civic duty moderates the impact of com- countries where voting is mandated compared to
ponents of rational choice models. More precisely, those in which voting is a personal decision. As
the expected benefit of voting, the cost of perform- such, like Maldonado, Rangel concludes that com-
ing that action, and the probability of casting a piv- pulsory voting does not boost civic duty: ‘‘if com-
otal vote lose their force as a citizen becomes more pulsory voting has any effect beyond increased
dutiful. Given the importance of civic duty as a mo- turnout, it is likely not because it makes citizens
tivation to vote, it seems relevant to study whether
compulsory voting can augment citizens’ sense of
duty to vote.
We are not the first to argue that compulsory vot- 2
Supporting this contention, past work reports that political
ing rules likely influence citizens’ sense of civic knowledge makes it more likely that differences between can-
duty. Quintelier et al. (2011), for instance, argue didates are perceived and fewer individuals are indifferent
with respect to the voting choices (Palfrey and Poole 1987).
that compulsory voting can function as a signal of 3
Reinforcing the argument that compulsory voting rules posi-
what actions are expected from a good citizen. tively influence civic duty, research has established that politi-
Chapman (2018) follows this argument and notes cal trust (Lundell 2012), partisanship (De Leon &and Rizzi
that even though only about one in four Australians 2016; Singh and Thornton 2013), and political knowledge (Gor-
don and Segura 1997; Sheppard 2015; Shineman 2018) are
incur a monetary sanction for failing to vote, turnout higher in contexts of mandatory voting than where voting is a
is high in that country, which she interprets as evi- personal decision. Given that these variables are associated
dence that compulsory voting can act on turnout be- with a belief in the duty to vote (Bowler and Donovan 2013;
Galais and Blais 2017; Prior 2003; Riker and Ordeshook
yond its effect on the cost of abstention—which 1968), it is reasonable to expect that compulsory voting rules
Chapman (2018) calls the ‘‘expressive’’ function will also positively affect civic duty.
4
of compulsory voting rules.3 Recent studies contradict the crowding-out argument and
claim that compulsory voting in fact might serve to reinforce
To the best of our knowledge, only two studies preexisting internal motivations to vote: for example, Chapman
have empirically investigated the connection between (2018) and Elliot (2017) argue that the fact that Australians had
compulsory voting and civic duty. Maldonado (2015), been obliged to vote since 1924 and have one of the highest
who builds on work in social-psychology and educa- rates of sense of civic duty can be taken as evidence that exter-
nal and internal motivations to vote—which differ in terms of
tion, argues that (external) punishment in compulsory the locus of action, i.e., where the initial thrive to vote comes
voting settings might crowd out internal motivations from—are not enemies, but in fact reinforce each other.
4 FEITOSA ET AL.

feel more civically minded and consider voting to in 2012, to study the impact of compulsory voting
be a duty’’ (Rangel 2017, 44).5 rules on citizens’ belief that voting is a duty. Before
While both Maldonado’s and Rangel’s findings that year, voting was mandatory for citizens who
offer useful contributions to the literature, we be- were registered to vote. However, registration was
lieve the debate is not settled, as those studies suffer not automatic or compulsory. The few registration of-
from important limitations. By exposing individuals fices and their opening only on certain days yielded
to one of three messages—or none at all—regarding very low registration rates—especially among youth
the penalties for abstaining in Peru, Maldonado (Corvalan and Cox 2013, 53). Furthermore, only
likely assessed the impact of being reminded about few registered citizens were penalized by means of
those sanctions on civic duty, not the impact of com- a fee or even imprisonment (Barnes and Rangel
pulsory voting per se. Further, given that participants 2014).
came from a country in which citizens are obliged This scenario changed drastically in January,
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

by law to vote, those in the control group likely do 2012, with the promulgation of law 20.568. Most
not differ from those in the treatment groups, in importantly, this new law modified the article 60
terms of their sense of duty. of law 18.700, which regulated that all registered
Rangel’s study is limited in an important respect citizens had the legal obligation to vote. Law
as well. Her study relies on a cross-national investi- 20.568 also modified registration rules in the coun-
gation of the relation between compulsory voting try: specifically, it established automatic registra-
rules and civic duty. The difficulty in accounting tion for all citizens as well as foreign nationals
for potential confounders of this relation in such a who have lived in Chile for at least five years and
set-up, however, is a reason for concern. For that who comply with the age requirement (Barnes and
precise reason, Hirczy (1994) recommends analyz- Rangel 2014; Biblioteca del Congresso National
ing within-country variation and leveraging changes de Chile 2018).
within a country to study the effects of institutional Technically, we are thus studying the impact of
settings, such as the abolition of compulsory voting the abolition of compulsory voting combined with
rules, on citizens’ voting behavior. the introduction of automatic registration. Such
In conclusion, past research claims that compul- changes in the registration rules should bias our es-
sory voting rules bolster turnout through the cost of timates downwards, however. Braconnier et al.
abstaining in elections. Researchers have enlarged (2017) have provided compelling experimental evi-
this narrative and included the expected benefit of dence that, when registration is made easier, which
voting and the civic duty to vote as mechanisms is the case in Chile, individuals are more likely to be
through which compulsory voting rules affect turn- interested, pay attention to, and participate in polit-
out. Focusing on the latter mechanism, civic duty, ical affairs. Because civic duty is positively associ-
two studies have investigated it by means of a lab ated with these variables, a rise in civic duty should
experiment and a cross-national analysis. The con- follow from Chile’s transition to automatic registra-
clusion was that compulsory voting rules have no tion. We return to this point in the section in which
effect on whether citizens conceive voting in dutiful we conduct and report the results from robustness
terms. Because those studies can be criticized on checks of our findings.
methodological grounds and given that there are By means of a case study of Chile’s 2012 reform,
theoretical reasons for believing in a positive effect we hence address some of the limitations of earlier
of compulsory voting rules on civic duty, we pro- work. In contrast to Maldonado (2015), we study
pose a new study, based on a quasi-experiment, civic duty in a direct way and also gain external
which we elaborate below.
5
Rangel expected a positive relationship between those vari-
ables as compulsory voting rules likely send a signal that the
DATA AND METHOD government values electoral participation, and, in doing so,
they might increase the number of citizens who believe in the
We assess the impact of compulsory voting rules duty to vote. Furthermore, children are likely more socialized
on citizens’ sense of civic duty by means of a quasi- into voting—and, with it, into civic duty—in countries where
voting is mandatory than where voting is a personal decision,
experimental approach. Specifically, we take advan- as their parents will be more likely to participate in elections
tage of Chile’s transition to a voluntary voting system, in the former but not in the latter (Rangel 2017).
DOES COMPULSORY VOTING FOSTER CIVIC DUTY TO VOTE? 5

validity compared to his lab experimental setting. different compulsory voting countries in Latin
Furthermore, in contrast to Rangel (2017), we focus America before and after 2012, when the Chilean
on what happens within a single country and, as government abolished compulsory voting. This esti-
such, we are better able to isolate the effect that com- mation strategy, however, comes with a number of
pulsory voting might have on civic duty.6 assumptions. An important one regards the pre-
For studying the influence of Chile’s abolition of treatment duty trends. Specifically, scholars who
compulsory voting on civic duty, we make use of use the difference-in-differences approach assume
data from six Latinobarómetro; the 2007, 2008, that trends in the dependent variable in the control
2009, 2010, 2011, and 2015 surveys (Latinobaró- and the treatment groups would have been the
metro 2018). The Latinobarómetro, which is used same in the absence of the treatment (Angrist and
extensively in comparative research (Farrer and Pischke 2009).
Zingher 2018; Houle et al. 2018; Singh 2018), Insights into the validity of this assumption can
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

constitutes—to our best knowledge—the sole avail- be obtained from studying trends in the control and
able dataset that allows an analysis of the impact of treatment groups in the period preceding the treat-
the abolition of compulsory voting rules on civic ment. Visually inspecting trends in civic duty be-
duty. The project has consistently measured Latinos’ fore 2012 in Chile and other compulsory voting
belief in the duty to vote before and after the aboli- countries in Latin America suggests differences
tion of compulsory voting in Chile. Furthermore, by in the evolution of civic duty between Chile and
focusing on Latin America, the Latinobarómetro al- the other countries before 2012 (see Appendix B).
lows more refined analysis of the impact of Chile’s When regressing (pre-2012) civic duty among
abolition of compulsory voting on civic duty, as compulsory voting systems on survey year, a
the participating countries share political and so- country dummy (Chile = 1; All other compulsory
cial characteristics, including the legal obligation voting [CV] countries = 0) and their interactions,
to vote (see Appendix A for additional information we indeed find a statistically significant differ-
on the Latin American countries’ voting rules). ence in civic duty trends between Chile and all
We additionally note the high quality of the data other Latin American compulsory voting systems
from the Latinobarómetro project. The dataset before the treatment (see Appendix D).7 As such,
includes, for each survey-wave, nationally repre- these tests suggest that an important assumption
sentative samples of country nationals that are in the difference-in-differences approach may be
interviewed face-to-face in each of the participat- violated. Given this violation, we rely mainly on an
ing countries. The Chilean sample included around alternative empirical strategy to estimate a synthetic
2,000 citizens who were sampled randomly in a control that resembles the pre-treatment civic duty
three-step approach in regions, a variable number trends in Chile.
of blocks, and routes. This strategy results in a rep- The synthetic control method is not new within
resentative sample of individuals. In 2015, for exam- the comparative politics literature. Researchers
ple, the year of the first study following the 2012 have used it to study the impact of state policy on
electoral reform, 18 percent of Chilean respondents tobacco consumption (Abadie et al. 2010), of feder-
come from rural areas—defined as cities with less alism on social spending (Arnold and Stadelmann-
than 5,000 inhabitants—and 82 percent from Steffen 2017), and of a natural disaster—the 2007
urban areas, a distribution that is consistent with of-
ficial census data (Latinobarómetro 2018; Instituto
6
Nacional de Estadı́sticas, 2015). Even so, we ex- We have reasons to assume that a public rejection of manda-
tory voting is not what motivated lawmakers to implement vol-
plore the validity of our aggregate-level tests in untary voting in Chile. In particular, if the change in voting
the section where robustness checks are discussed. rules was endogenous to a decline in civic duty, we should
Difference-in-difference estimation has been a see a lower sense of civic duty preceding Chile’s transition to
voluntary voting. The data do not show evidence of such a
common empirical strategy in quasi-experimental trend. Furthermore, before the reform, civic duty was higher
studies such as ours (e.g., Bullock and Clinton in Chile than it was in other compulsory voting systems:
2011; Ferwerda 2014; Lucardi 2017). For studying while the mean proportion of individuals who considered vot-
the effect of the abolition of compulsory voting ing a duty in Chile, before the intervention, was .67, in Brazil,
where voting is also mandatory, the mean was .52.
rules on civic duty, such a design would take the 7
More specifically, three of the four tests reported in Appendix
form of a comparison of civic duty means between D fail to confirm the parallel trend assumption.
6 FEITOSA ET AL.

Ica earthquake—on support for the political system synthetic control estimation, with country weights,
(Katz and Levin 2016). Acknowledging the advan- reads:
tages of this approach, students of compulsory vot-
1
ing have started to rely on the synthetic control aChile ¼ Ychile  wi  Y i 0 ‚
method. They have done so, for instance, to investi-
gate the impact of compulsory voting rules on turn- where a corresponds to the treatment effect of the
out (Bechtel et al. 2018), on parties’ vote share abolition of compulsory voting on Chileans’ sense
(Fowler 2013), and on their vote-seeking strategies of civic duty, Y, the outcome value (i.e., of civic
(Singh 2019). duty) with treatment (marked by the superscript 1)
The synthetic control procedure resembles the and without treatment (marked by the superscript
difference-in-differences approach in that the im- 0), and W, the country weights. The subscript i de-
pact of a treatment is obtained by subtracting the notes the set of countries that contribute to the syn-
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

outcome value in the treated unit by its value in thetic control estimation.
the control unit. Formally, we can write this as: For estimating synthetic Chile, we follow previ-
ous work that used the synthetic control method in
ai ¼ Yi1  Yi0 their comparative work and select a set of political
and socio-demographic variables that likely explain
where Y entails the outcome value with treat- the Chilean levels and trends in civic duty. Impor-
ment (marked by the superscript 1) and without tantly, the strength of the chosen predictors is not
treatment (marked by the superscript 0). The sub- of primary interest. ‘‘All that matters is that they
script i denotes the observation. In our case, the ef- help model the pre-treatment outcome as accurately
fect of compulsory voting rules results from the as possible’’ (Koehler and König 2015). We first add
subtraction of civic duty mean between treated in our synthetic control estimation education, age
Chile and non-treated Chile. Given that we are not and religion, as the more educated, the elderly and
capable of having both conditions simultaneously— the religious are more likely to consider voting a
and that no other country resembles Chile before the duty (Blais 2000; Galais and Blais 2017).9 Data
treatment—we estimate a non-treated Chile, which on education come from the Varieties of Democracy
is our synthetic control, and then use it as a counter- Project, on age, from the World Bank, and on reli-
factual for a comparison with ‘‘real’’ Chile (Abadie gion, from the Latinobarómetro. Education is mea-
et al. 2010; 2015). sured by average years of formal education, age is
To estimate ‘‘synthetic’’ Chile, we calculate and the percentage of 19–34 year-olds among the
assign weights (summing up 1) to all Latin Ameri- voting-age population, and religion is constructed
can countries in our data—including compulsory based on answers to the question ‘‘How would
and voluntary voting systems. These weights are you describe yourself? Very devout, Devout, Not
given according to three pieces of information: the very devout, or Not devout at all’’ (for a distribution
values of the treated unit and the synthetic-control
donors on variables that are associated with the out-
8
come, the outcome itself (before the treatment), and Abadie et al. (2011) note that a perfect match is not frequent,
their linear combinations (Abadie et al. 2015). as the characteristics of the treated unit might differ substan-
tially from those that contribute to the development of the syn-
Countries that mostly—though not perfectly— thetic control. In this case, the latter will approximate as best as
resemble the treated unit with respect to the out- possible the former, while being a valid counterfactual.
9
come value, its predictors, and the association be- It is important to note that the synthetic control model is a
static estimation of a one-time external shocks effect on the out-
tween these variables are assigned a higher weight come variable: the model estimates a counterfactual that resem-
than countries that do not, thus contributing to a bles the treated unit, at each individual time point, from a pool
larger degree to the estimation of the synthetic of potential donor countries, based on their pre-treatment values
on variables associated with the outcome variable, the outcome
control. To be sure, the formation of a synthetic variable itself, and the linear association between these vari-
counterfactual based on the weighted contribution ables and the outcome variable (Abadie et al. 2015). It is
of donor units allows a more precise control than if hence not a problem that low-variance variables, which is the
any country were individually taken as a compar- case of these variables and compulsory voting as well, are
used in these estimations. As such, explaining the pre-treatment
ison with the treated unit, or if all compulsory vot- duty variance—through a dynamic model with high-variance
ing systems served that role.8 The formula for the variables—is out of the scope of this article.
DOES COMPULSORY VOTING FOSTER CIVIC DUTY TO VOTE? 7

of these variables, see Appendix E). Including these by the question ‘‘Which of the following things do
variables should help in the construction of a syn- you believe that a person must do if she wants to be
thetic Chile that resembles real Chile before the considered a citizen? Vote; Pay taxes; Obey all laws;
treatment. Participate in social organizations; Participate in polit-
Past scholarship has further demonstrated that trust ical organizations; Buy environmentally responsible
in parliament, support for democracy, and democrati- products; Help those in need; Serve in the military.’’
zation level are positively associated with the belief in Respondents can choose as many response op-
the duty to vote (Galais and Blais 2017). We thus in- tions as they want. Those who mentioned ‘‘vote’’
clude indicators of these concepts to estimate syn- are coded as 1, and those who did not mention it
thetic Chile. Data on trust in parliament come from are coded as 0. In the pre-treatment period (from
the Latinobarómetro, on support for democracy, 2007 to 2011), 68 percent of Chileans report a be-
also from the Latinobarómetro, and on democratiza- lief in the duty to vote. This number is slightly
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

tion level, from Freedom House. Trust in parliament lower than among the full sample of Latin Amer-
is measured by the question ‘‘How much trust do icans (70 percent). As with other variables, we use
you have in each of the following groups, institutions aggregate civic duty values by country and year in
or persons: a lot, some, a little or no trust? National the synthetic control estimations.
Congress/Parliament.’’ Support for democracy, by Previous research has used the same measure in
the question ‘‘With which of the following state- studies of civic duty—most notably, Rangel (2017).
ments do you agree most? Democracy is preferable We acknowledge, however, that this is not sufficient
to any other kind of government; Under some cir- to ascertain the validity of this measure. We hence
cumstances, an authoritarian government can be correlate ‘‘vote’’ with ‘‘obey all laws’’ in all compul-
preferable to a democratic one; For people like sory and voluntary voting systems. If the correlation
me, it doesn’t matter whether we have a democratic between these two items is stronger in compulsory
or non-democratic regime.’’ And democratization than voluntary voting systems, it might suggest that
level by a classification of countries as ‘‘not free,’’ citizens in compulsory voting countries simply iden-
‘‘partly free,’’ or ‘‘free.’’ Since our comparison tify that voting is a legal duty. If not, we have a good
takes place at the national level, religion, trust in reason to believe that our civic duty measure is tap-
parliament, and support for democracy—origi- ping citizens’ belief in the civic duty to vote. The re-
nally at the individual level—are aggregated by sults indicate that the correlation between voting and
country and year. ‘‘obey all laws’’ is marginally stronger under com-
Finally, we include compulsory voting in the es- pulsory voting: the corresponding gamma statistics
timation of synthetic Chile. Countries are divided are .83 under compulsory voting and .81 under vol-
in either compulsory or voluntary voting system untary voting. Tests with likely registered and non-
as defined by the 2018 Varieties of Democracy registered Chileans—specifically, those aged 50 or
Project. Further, in line with past work that has re- more, and 50 or less—suggest the same results as
lied on the synthetic control method (Abadie et al. well: the corresponding gamma statistics are .86
2010; 2015; Heersink and Peterson 2017), we also and .81. Furthermore, in both cases, these correla-
include pre-2012 civic duty levels in the estimation tions would be classified as very strong. It thus
of synthetic Chile. Doing so should help find the seems very likely that our measure captures citizens’
best donors and the optimal share of individual con- sense of duty.
tributions to the estimation of a synthetic control that It must be noted that while we lack data on civic
mostly resembles pre-treatment Chile. Given the risk duty beyond those six years, this does not impede
of overfitting with respect to the pre-treatment data, the use of the synthetic control method. Previous
in additional tests we replace this variable for the work has implemented this method over as few as
civic duty mean levels in 2007, in 2011 and its two or three time points (Dowling et al. 2012;
growth change between these years (Beckley et al. Fowler 2013; Heersink and Peterson 2017). Yet, be-
2018), whose results are reported in the robustness cause of a lack of data in the years immediately fol-
checks section. lowing the abolition of compulsory voting (i.e., 2012,
Data on civic duty come from six Latino- 2013, and 2014), we cannot tell whether the effect of
barómetro—from 2007, 2008, 2009, 2010, 2011, the abolition of compulsory voting rules on citizens’
and 2015. In those studies, civic duty is measured belief in the duty to vote strengthens, weakens or
8 FEITOSA ET AL.

Table 1. Mean Level of Variables Before the Abolition of Compulsory Voting (in 2012)

‘‘Real’’ Chile ‘‘Synthetic’’ Chile All countries in the donor pool

Compulsory voting (2007–2011) 1.00 1.00 0.75


Civic duty (2007–2011) 0.68 0.69 0.71
Education (2007–2011) 10.20 8.57 7.24
Religion (2007–2011) 2.29 2.30 2.52
Democratization level (2007–2011) 3.00 3.00 2.54
Age (2007–2011) 0.34 0.39 0.42
Trust in parliament (2007–2011) 2.16 1.97 2.09
Support for democracy (2007–2011) 2.35 2.47 2.41
Notes: The countries that contributed to the estimation of ‘‘synthetic’’ Chile are Peru, Uruguay and Panama (for the weights given to each Latin
American country, see Appendix F). While some differences are observed, ‘‘real’’ Chile and ‘‘synthetic’’ Chile have approximately the same civic
duty mean level before the treatment. Because we focus on this variable in our subsequent analyses, this may be taken as strong evidence that the
‘‘synthetic’’ Chile can be used as a valid counterfactual.
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

remains stable after the change in law, an important in all pre-treatment years. They differ substantially,
limitation of our study. however, after 2012, when compulsory voting was
abolished in the country. Civic duty declines to
about 62 percent in ‘‘real’’ Chile, whereas it in-
RESULTS creases to about 72 percent in ‘‘synthetic’’ Chile.
In short, while Chile and its synthetic control had
We first estimate a synthetic Chile, which we very similar levels of civic duty before the treat-
then use to assess the impact of the abolition of ment (and parallel trends), a large difference is ob-
compulsory voting on citizens’ sense of duty to served after the abolition of compulsory voting,
vote. We do so based on Latin American countries which may be taken as an indication that this insti-
pre-treatment values on eight variables: compul- tutional reform negatively affected civic duty.
sory voting, civic duty, education, religion, age, According to the estimates of our synthetic control
support for democracy, trust in parliament, and de- method, had compulsory voting not been abolished
mocratization level.10 Table 1 displays how close in Chile, civic duty would have been about 10 per-
synthetic Chile is to real Chile before the interven- centage points higher in 2015 than what it was in
tion. We can observe a high correspondence between reality.
Chile and synthetic Chile—that was created from This effect seems rather large, but it is reassur-
Peru (.69), Uruguay (.29), and Panama (.02)—on sev- ing to note that an effect of this size is in line with
eral variables. For instance, both constitute compul- the effect sizes of compulsory voting regarding
sory voting systems and have a relatively high level other outcome variables. For example, previous
of education and democratization. They also have studies report a 28-percentage-point increase in
an older population in comparison with other Latin partisanship and a 20-percentage-point increase
American countries. Most importantly, ‘‘synthetic’’ in support for leftist policy positions—in a refer-
Chile has approximately the same civic duty mean endum—due to compulsory voting alone (Bechtel
as ‘‘real’’ Chile before the treatment. Given that we et al. 2016; Singh and Thornton 2013). We should
are mainly interested in obtaining a control with sim- also note that a substantial effect on civic duty cor-
ilar (and parallel) duty trends, this may be taken as responds with the sizeable effects that the change
strong evidence that the estimated ‘‘synthetic’’ Chile of law had on citizens’ electoral participation.
is a valid counterfactual to the ‘‘real’’ Chile. More specifically, national-level turnout in Chile
Considering that the ‘‘synthetic’’ Chile seems to declined from 59 percent before 2012 to 45 and
be a very good approximation of ‘‘real’’ Chile be- 49 percent in the 2013 and 2017 presidential elec-
fore its transition to voluntary voting, we measure tions, even though Chile’s mandatory voting rules
the impact of compulsory voting on civic duty by were weakly enforced (IDEA 2018; Singh 2011).
comparing the civic duty mean level in ‘‘real’’
Chile and in ‘‘synthetic’’ Chile after 2012. As Fig-
ure 1 shows, ‘‘real’’ Chile and ‘‘synthetic’’ Chile 10
We perform these estimations by means of the ‘‘synth’’ pack-
have approximately the same mean civic duty levels age that is available in R (Abadie et al. 2011).
DOES COMPULSORY VOTING FOSTER CIVIC DUTY TO VOTE? 9
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

FIG. 1. Civic duty levels in ‘‘real’’ Chile and ‘‘synthetic’’ Chile before and after the abolition of compulsory voting. Notes:
‘‘Real’’ Chile and ‘‘synthetic’’ Chile have the same civic duty levels before the abolition of compulsory voting in 2012. A difference
is observed after that year, however, when Chile shifted to voluntary voting. We characterize this difference as the effect of com-
pulsory voting on civic duty.

ROBUSTNESS CHECKS Chile, and, hence, as a consequence of the aboli-


tion of compulsory voting rules.
Given the specificity of the Chilean case, and Figure 2 displays the results of these placebo
particularities related to the synthetic control tests. It indicates the ratio of the root-mean-squared
model, we perform a series of robustness checks. difference in civic duty between each country in the
We start by assessing the likelihood of finding a donor pool and their synthetic control after the inter-
similar effect in other Latin American countries, vention divided by the same difference before the
where no change in the electoral rules related to intervention (see Appendices G and H for the corre-
the legal obligation to vote were observed. To this sponding civic duty trends in all Latin American
end, we follow previous studies that relied on the countries and their synthetic controls).11 Chile is
same model, and conduct placebo tests (Abadie clearly an exceptional case. Indeed, while the ratio
et al. 2015). More specifically, we estimate a syn- is between 0 and 3 among all other countries, it is
thetic control for each Latin American country in about 10 for Chile. Such a large gap may be taken
the donor pool. This includes those who contributed as evidence that the difference between ‘‘real’’
to the estimation of ‘‘synthetic’’ Chile and those Chile and its synthetic control after 2012 is specific
who did not. If the difference that is observed be- to this country, and likely the result of its transition
tween ‘‘synthetic’’ Chile and ‘‘real’’ Chile also oc-
curs with other countries and their respective
synthetic control, it means that such a difference 11
Mathematically, this ratio can be expressed
may not be due to Chile’s abolition of compulsory
voting and, thus, we have a good reason to be skep- as (Abadie
tical about its influence on civic duty to vote. In
contrast, if we only find an effect of this size et al., 2015). If Latin American countries that did not abolish
when applying the method to studying differences compulsory voting rules display a ratio as high as Chile, it
in civic duty between ‘‘real’’ Chile and its syn- means that another event might explain the decline of civic
duty. In contrast, if their corresponding ratio is lower than in
thetic control, we can characterize with greater Chile, it means that Chile is unique and, consequently, the aboli-
confidence the observed difference as unique to tion of compulsory voting is likely the cause of such a decline.
10 FEITOSA ET AL.
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

FIG. 2. Placebo tests with all Latin American countries in the dataset. Notes: This figure indicates the ratio of the root-mean-
squared difference in civic duty between each country in the donor pool and their synthetic control after the intervention divided
by the same difference before the intervention. We observe that Chile is clearly an exceptional case. We attribute the observed
difference to its change to a voluntary voting system in 2012.

to a system in which citizens have the choice to vote mean gap .030. We may thus reject the possibil-
or abstain in elections. The placebo tests indicate ity of an over-fitting bias with respect to the pre-
that the observed decay of civic duty does not repli- treatment data.
cate in countries that were not exposed to the treat- Although an over-fit is unlikely, we still perform
ment, namely, the abolition of compulsory voting. the synthetic control model without the lagged values
Consequently, we are more confident in attributing of the dependent variable. Recent research by Beck-
the observed decline of civic duty to the abolition ley et al. (2017) argues against the use of such values
of compulsory voting rules in Chile. and suggests their replacement for the mean of the
We additionally analyze the possibility that our outcome variable instead. We do so by including
synthetic control estimations over-fit the pre-treatment the means in 2007 and in 2011 (i.e., in the first and
data. More precisely, we replicate the model for last time point before the intervention), and a mea-
each of the countries in the sample, creating a syn- sure of its growth rate between these years in order
thetic version of the country. We calculate the dif- to avoid an over-fit with the pre-treatment data.
ference between each country and its synthetic Replacing the lagged civic duty for these variables
control in each survey year, then compute the pre- does not alter the results: the attitudes mean value
treatment and the post-treatment means of these still increases in ‘‘synthetic’’ Chile after the abolition
differences within each country, divide these num- of compulsory voting, whereas it declines in the
bers, and then average them with all countries in ‘‘real’’ Chile in the same period, with a difference
the dataset, except Chile. We observe that the in these values of 10 percentage points (see Appen-
post-treatment gaps are not substantially lower dix I). Therefore, we are confident that we do not in-
than the pre-treatment gaps: the mean gap across advertently over-fit the estimated synthetic control
all Latin American countries is indeed .047 before with the pre-treatment data, which would result in
2012 and .055 after 2012. When we remove coun- a large post-treatment difference in the duty mean
tries whose pre-treatment gaps are twice larger levels between ‘‘real’’ Chile and its synthetic control.
than Chile, the differences remain minimal: The To further verify the robustness of the results
pre-2012 mean gap is now .019 and the post-2012 from the synthetic control model, we conduct the
DOES COMPULSORY VOTING FOSTER CIVIC DUTY TO VOTE? 11

same analyses with a more restricted sample. That of compulsory voting rules on Chileans’ belief that
is, instead of using all six Latinobarómetro, we voting is a duty, it is possible that another political
used only three of them (from 2007, 2011, and event explains the decay in civic duty after 2012.
2015). Doing so results in a set of data points with We discuss two possibilities: the simultaneous transi-
an equal interval of four years between each civic tion to automatic registration, and student protests
duty measure. We find that, with less information that took place between 2011 and 2013. First, Chile
on the civic duty trend, Brazil is given some weight was unique in that only citizens who registered vol-
(.09) in the estimation of the synthetic control. Peru untarily in the electoral lists were obliged to vote.
and Uruguay remain the biggest donors (.57 and With the 2012 electoral reform, the decision to vote
.31), followed by Panama (.02). Even though the became a personal decision, while registration be-
control units are somewhat different, with a new came automatic. We are hence talking about a double
country contributing to the estimation of the syn- change in the voting rules. However, it is unlikely
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

thetic control, the observed difference between that the change in registration rules accounts for
post-2012 ‘‘real’’ Chile and its synthetic control re- the observed decline in civic duty attitudes.
mains 10 percentage points (see Appendix J). Research by Braconnier et al. (2016) shows that
Besides withdrawing some years, we also tested making registering easier increases attention to
the robustness of the results with the synthetic con- and interest in politics and, consequently, turnout.
trol model by withdrawing one country from the Following this line of reasoning, the shift to auto-
donor pool each at a time. We started with Peru, matic registration in Chile should have increased
then Uruguay, and then Panama, countries that citizens’ sense of civic duty to vote, and thus their
contribute from the most to the least to the estima- likelihood of turning out to vote, which contrasts
tion of ‘‘synthetic’’ Chile. While the fit between Chile with the observed decline in the extent to which
and its synthetic control declines when we exclude Chileans consider voting in elections a duty. Conse-
one of these countries, the difference between them quently, this explanation for the duty decline is im-
after the abolition of compulsory voting rules remains plausible. Instead, our results may be conservative
10 percentage points, with civic duty increasing in given the potential growth of civic duty in the
‘‘synthetic’’ Chile and declining in ‘‘real’’ Chile (see same period (i.e., post-2012) due to the transition
Appendices K to M). from voluntary to automatic registration.
Varying the sample of potential donor countries While theoretically the change to automatic
may not alter the results, but varying the sample registration seems the unlikely culprit for the de-
of individuals from whom country-level data is pro- cline in civic duty, we acknowledge that the syn-
duced may do so. More specifically, it may be the thetic control—in particular prior to 2012—may
case that citizens from urban centers are overrepre- not be perfectly comparable to ‘‘real’’ Chile, be-
sented in the data, thus biasing the estimates. We cause no other country combined compulsory vot-
hence conducted the same tests with those coming ing with voluntary registration. Given that this is a
from rural areas, defined as cities with less than very important point, we have sought other ways
5,000 inhabitants. We do not find evidence of a to account for the simultaneous change to volun-
bias: that is, restricting the aggregate-level tests to tary voting and automatic registration. More spe-
these individuals does not change the observed de- cifically, we performed additional tests in which
cline in civic duty after 2012 in ‘‘real’’ Chile and we limit our analysis to citizens aged 50 years
its increase in ‘‘synthetic’’ Chile. The difference be- old or more. This is the group that was most likely
tween Chile and its synthetic counterfactual remains already registered in elections before 2012, partly
10 percentage points as well (see Appendix N). as a consequence of the massive registration ef-
In the previous tests, we tackled the robustness of forts that followed the fall of Pinochet’s authoritarian
the results from the synthetic control model. All tests regime (Barnes and Rangel 2018; Corvalan and Cox
suggest that the surge of civic duty between 2011 and 2013). As such, these older individuals were likely
2015 is exceptional to Chile and that the results are not affected much by the transition to automatic reg-
robust to a different model specification and a differ- istration, meaning that the sole changing rule that af-
ent set of years, donor countries, and respondents. fected them was the abolition of compulsory voting.
Yet, while the synthetic control model provides con- When replicating the analysis for a sample of respon-
sistent evidence of a negative effect of the abolition dents of 50 years and older, civic duty still declines
12 FEITOSA ET AL.
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

FIG. 3. Synthetic control model with citizens aged 50+ years old. Notes: The post-treatment difference in the civic duty mean
levels between ‘‘real’’ Chile and its synthetic control is slightly lower (eight percentage points) when restricting the analysis to
respondents aged 50 years old or more, who are likely registered to vote before 2012, and, hence, upon whom the transition to
automatic registration likely exerted no influence.

after 2012, while it increases in ‘‘synthetic’’ Chile (see dence on the strong (positive) link between political
Figure 3). The difference, however, is somewhat interest and civic duty). In short, the two most visi-
smaller than previously estimated: eight percentage ble political events in Chile that might have affected
points. As a second test, we added the changing regis- citizens’ civic duty are unlikely the cause of the ob-
tration rates in the estimation of the synthetic control. served decline in civic duty after 2012, since both
Doing so does not alter the main findings. This test events should have resulted in a rise (not a decline)
still suggests a decline of civic duty in ‘‘real’’ Chile of civic duty in this period.
following the 2012 electoral reform, and an increase Results from the synthetic control model hence
in ‘‘synthetic’’ Chile in the same period, with a differ- consistently point towards a decline of civic duty
ence of 10 percentage points (see Appendix O). In attitudes in post-2012 Chile, which can be safely
short, accounting for the change to automatic registra- attributed to the abolition of compulsory voting
tion does not alter our findings.12 rules. We think that the synthetic control model has
Another possible event that may have affected a key property for studying the connection between
citizens’ sense of civic duty is the student protests compulsory voting and civic duty: the method—that
that took place in Chile between 2011 and 2013. analyzes within-unit, country-level variation—im-
During these years, Chilean students took to the plicitly controls the factors that do not change or
streets in massive demonstrations to fight for uni- change little between 2007 and 2015. To gain insights
versal education. Research indicates an influence in the link between compulsory voting and civic duty,
from these demonstrations, also referred to as the however, we have also pursued a more traditional
‘‘Chilean winter,’’ on the candidates’ agenda during
the 2013 presidential election (Bernasconi 2014), a
finding that indicates how important such an event 12
On the possible link between registration rules and civic duty,
was to the politics of the country. These demonstra- it should be further noted that while previous research has pro-
tions are unlikely the cause of the decline of duty, vided compelling evidence of a pre-2012 decline of registration
however. Given that they might have bolstered pub- rates in Chile, especially among new generations (Barnes and
lic interest in politics, the consequence should be Rangel 2018), such an event was not accompanied by a constant
decline in the extent to which Chileans consider voting a duty or
an augmentation of citizens’ sense of civic duty to not. A connection between registration rules and civic duty is
participate in elections (see Carreras [2018] for evi- hence very unlikely.
DOES COMPULSORY VOTING FOSTER CIVIC DUTY TO VOTE? 13

estimation approach that leverages between country- process is not clear. In this article, we have empiri-
and individual-level variation in compulsory voting, cally tested the role of civic duty in linking compul-
and in civic duty.13 sory voting and turnout. By means of a synthetic
As can be seen from the results in Appendix P, control method, we evaluated the impact of the ab-
estimating a hierarchical model on the pooled olition of compulsory voting rules in Chile on levels
individual-level data from the Latinobarómetro pro- of civic duty in the country. We find, through a syn-
ject offers little indication of a positive relation be- thetic control design, that civic duty declined in
tween compulsory voting rules and levels of civic post-2012 Chile, when compulsory voting was
duty. However, when estimating effects in the larger, replaced by a voluntary voting system. We esti-
and for that reason more appropriate, International mated a series of additional models and conducted
Social Survey Program (ISSP) dataset, the results placebo tests. Our results appear to be robust,
are more supportive of our theory. That is, we find strengthening our confidence in them and suggest-
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

that levels of civic duty are, on average, significantly ing that the abolition of compulsory voting in
higher in countries that implement a system of com- Chile did indeed have a negative effect in Chilean
pulsory voting. citizens’ levels of civic duty.
To further assess the robustness of these results, Our results seem robust, but it is also important to
we performed additional regressions with both acknowledge some limitations in our study. First,
country and year fixed effects. As shown in Appen- we lack data from a longer timeframe. For estimat-
dix Q, the impact of compulsory voting remains in- ing a synthetic Chile, we rely on six Latinobaróme-
significant with the Latinobarómetro sample but tro surveys (five from before 2012 and one from
significant and positive with the ISSP data. after 2012). This limits the amount of information
We further investigate whether we would arrive at that we have for estimating the impact of compul-
the same conclusions when employing a difference- sory voting rules on civic duty. Second, as previ-
in-differences estimation strategy. Comparing Chile ously noted, we do not have data from 2012,
with all Latin American countries where voting is 2013, and 2014. The citizenship question that we
compulsory immediately before and after treatment, use as a measure of civic duty was not employed
we find that the effect is still about 10 percentage in Latinobarómetro studies in those years. This lim-
points (see Appendix R). To take into account the its the interpretation of our results, in particular be-
over-time trends of civic duty prior to the treat- cause of the possibility that the impact of the
ment, however, we also estimate the difference-in- abolition of compulsory voting has decayed over
differences model with data from all pre-treatment time. Third, our results might be driven by the
years (i.e., 2007, 2008, 2009, and 2010). In this fact that we rely on a single item to measure civic
more conservative test, we obtain a much lower ef- duty. Past scholarship has supported the use of mul-
fect of the abolition of compulsory voting on civic tiple indicators for capturing a latent concept such
duty: it is now 2.9 percentage points, or 3.4 points as the belief in the duty to vote (Blais and Galais
if we exclude Honduras, which seems to be an out- 2016). As we are constrained to using the Latino-
lier in the dataset.14 These effects are substantially Barometro data, however, there is no other option
lower than those obtained with the synthetic control than to capture civic duty by means of a single
model, but they are in the same direction, that is, item. Fourth, our synthetic control is not perfectly
they suggest a decline in civic duty after the aboli-
tion of compulsory voting. While this result leads
us to be careful when interpreting the size of the ef- 13
The Latinobarómetro data have a hierarchical structure, with
fect of the compulsory voting on the duty to vote, it individuals nested in years and countries. As our variables of in-
also strengthens our confidence that there is indeed terest are either at the country level (e.g., compulsory voting) or
a positive relation between these variables. the individual level (e.g., civic duty), we account for this data
structure by estimating multi-level models in which individuals
are nested within countries. The over-time variation within
countries is accounted for by means of year fixed effects.
14
DISCUSSION While 64 percent of Latin Americans report a sense of duty to
vote before the treatment—i.e., in 2011—only 41 percent of
Hondurans do so. Furthermore, from 2007 to 2010, on average
That compulsory voting increases turnout is well 69 percent of Hondurans indicate that voting is a duty, which is
known, but whether civic duty plays a role in the a lot higher than the 41 percent found in 2011.
14 FEITOSA ET AL.

identical to Chile before the intervention when all that compulsory voting as well can serve as a source
pre-treatment trend is taken into account. Conse- for the development of civic duty.
quently, some of the observed difference between Finally, our results offer an important contribution
Chile and its synthetic control may be due to some to politics in general. They suggest one way in which
unaccounted differences between the two. Fifth, the decline of civic duty, that is observed in many ad-
while our analyses suggest a decline in civic duty vanced democracies, may be deterred and civic duty
following 2012—which likely negatively affected augmented. Research has shown that new genera-
turnout—others have drawn attention to the fact that tions adhere less to the voting norm and, as such,
the abolition of compulsory voting yielded a turnout they have been less keen to vote in elections (Blais
increase in some Chilean districts (Barnes and Rangel et al. 2004; Dalton 2008). We demonstrate that com-
2018). This suggests that the effects might be hetero- pulsory voting constitutes an important mechanism
geneous and differ between individuals or regions. that might be able to foster a civic-spirited citizenry
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

Unfortunately, because of data limitations, we cannot that vote in elections. We note, however, that despite
investigate this possibility further. the evidence of an association between compulsory
The main take-away from the observation that voting and civic duty, an increase of blank and in-
turnout increased after the reform in many districts, valid voting likely follows the legal obligation to
however, is probably that mandatory voting may not vote (Barnes and Rangel 2018; Singh 2017). In
be a panacea for fostering civic duty—and in this this case, the implementation of mandatory voting
way turnout. is not necessarily good for democracy, given that it
Despite these limitations, our study represents a might increase the number of blank and invalid
first attempt to explore the association between votes in elections while pushing more eligible citi-
compulsory voting and civic duty through a quasi- zens to the polls.
experimental approach. In showing evidence of a
link between these variables, it connects aggregate-
level work on the determinants of turnout to
REFERENCES
individual-level theories of what explains turnout:
specifically, the well-documented association be- Abadie, A., A. Diamond, and J. Hainmueller. (2010). ‘‘Syn-
tween electoral participation and individuals’ sense thetic Control Methods for Comparative Case Studies:
of civic duty to vote. While previous research on Estimating the Effect Oo Californias Tobacco Control
the connection between compulsory voting and Program.’’ Journal of the American Statistical Associa-
turnout referred to the costs from abstaining to tion, 105(490): 493–505.
Abadie, A., A. Diamond, and J. Hainmueller. (2011). ‘‘Synth: An
explain such association, individual-level research R Package for Synthetic Control Methods in Comparative
suggests that such rational considerations only Case Studies.’’ Journal of Statistical Software, 42(13): 1–17.
have little impact on the decision to vote or abstain. Abadie, A., A. Diamond, and J. Hainmueller. (2015). ‘‘Compa-
In showing that civic duty, a far more important rative Politics and the Synthetic Control Method.’’ Ameri-
turnout determinant, is also affected by compul- can Journal of Political Science, 59(2): 495–510.
Angrist, J., and J. Pischke. (2009). Mostly Harmless Economet-
sory voting, we offer a more plausible explanation
rics: An Empiricists Companion. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
of the positive link between compulsory voting and University Press.
citizens turning out to vote than previously given Arnold, T., and I. Stadelmann-Steffen. (2017). ‘‘How Federal-
by researchers in the field. ism Influences Welfare Spending: Belgium Federalism
Our study contributes to the scholarly literature Reform Through the Perspective of the Synthetic Control
on the origins of civic duty as well. Only a handful Method.’’ European Journal of Political Research, 56(3):
680–702.
of studies have investigated where civic duty comes Barnes, T.D., and G. Rangel. (2014). ‘‘Election Law Reform in
from, even though it constitutes one of the most im- Chile: The Implementation of Automatic Registration and
portant reasons why individuals bother to vote. Voluntary Voting.’’ Election Law Journal, 13(4): 570–582.
These studies also focus on the role of individual- Barnes, T., and G. Rangel. (2018). ‘‘Subnational Patterns of Par-
level factors for explaining why some individuals ticipation: Compulsory Voting and the Conditional Impact
of Institutional Design.’’ Political Research Quarterly,
develop a sense of civic duty to vote and others do
71(4): 826–841.
not (Galais 2018). Macro-level factors have so far Bechtel, M.M., D. Hangartner, and L. Schmid. (2016). ‘‘Does
not received attention in this field. We hence Compulsory Voting Increase Support for Leftist Policy?’’
make an important contribution by demonstrating American Journal of Political Science, 60(3): 752–767.
DOES COMPULSORY VOTING FOSTER CIVIC DUTY TO VOTE? 15

Bechtel, M.M., D. Hangartner, and L. Schmid. (2018). ‘‘Com- methods-guides/10-things-you-need-know-about-multiple-


pulsory Voting, Habit Formation, and Political Participa- comparisons>.
tion.’’ Review of Economics and Statistics, 100(3): 467– Corvalan, A., and P. Cox. (2013). ‘‘Class-Biased Electoral Par-
476. ticipation: The Youth Vote in Chile.’’ Latin American Poli-
Beckley, M., Y. Horiuchi, and J. Miller. (2018). ‘‘America’s tics and Society, 55(3): 47–68.
Role in the Making of Japan’s Economic Miracle.’’ Journal Dalton, R.J. (2008). ‘‘Citizenship Norms and the Expansion of
of East Asian Studies, 18(1): 1–21. Political Participation.’’ Political Studies, 56(1): 76–98.
Bernasconi, A. (2014). ‘‘Policy Path Dependence of a Research Dassonneville, R., F. Feitosa, M. Hooghe, R.R. Lau, and D.
Agenda: The Case of Chile in the Aftermath of the Student Stiers. (2018). ‘‘Compulsory Voting Rules, Reluctant Voters
Revolt of 2011.’’ Studies in Higher Education, 39(8): 1405– and Ideological Proximity Voting.’’ Political Behavior,
1416. 41(1): 209–230. doi: 10.1007/s11109-018-9448-6.
Biblioteca del Congresso National de Chile. (2018). Ordena- Dassonneville, R., and M. Hooghe. (2017). ‘‘Voter Turnout
miento Nacional Chileno. Retrieved from <https://www Decline and Stratification: Quasi- Experimental and Com-
.bcn.cl>. parative Evidence of a Growing Educational Gap.’’ Politics,
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

Birch, S. (2009). ‘‘The Case for Compulsory Voting.’’ Public 37(2): 184–200.
Policy Research, 16(1): 21–27. De Leon, Fernanda L. L., and R. Rizzi. (2016). ‘‘Does Forced
Blais, A. (2000). To Vote or Not to Vote?: The Merits and Lim- Voting Result in Political Polarization?’’ Public Choice,
its of Rational Choice Theory. Pittsburgh, PA: University of 166(1): 143–160.
Pittsburgh Press. Dowling, C.M., R.D. Enos, A. Fowler, and C. Panagopoulos.
Blais, A., C. Galais. (2016). ‘‘Measuring the Civic Duty to Vote: (2012). ‘‘Does Public Financing Chill Political Speech?
A Proposal.’’ Electoral Studies, 41: 60–69. Exploiting AaCourt Injunction as a Natural Experiment.’’
Blais, A., E. Gidengil, and N. Nevitte. (2004). ‘‘Where Does Election Law Journal, 11(3): 302–315.
Turnout Decline Come From?’’ European Journal of Polit- Elliott, K.J. (2017). ‘‘Aid for Our Purposes: Mandatory Voting
ical Research, 43(2): 221–236. as Precommitment and Nudge.’’ Journal of Politics, 79(2):
Blais, A., R. Young, and M. Lapp. (2000). ‘‘The Calculus of 656–669.
Voting: An Empirical Test.’’ European Journal of Political Engelen, B. (2007). ‘‘Why Compulsory Voting Can Enhance
Research, 37(2):181–201. Democracy.’’ Acta Politica, 42(1): 23–39.
Bolzendahl, C., and H. Coffé. (2013). ‘‘Are Good Citizens Good Farrer, B., and J.N. Zingher. (2018). ‘‘A Global Analysis of How
Participants? Testing Citizenship Norms and Political Partic- Losing an Election Affects Voter Satisfaction with Democ-
ipation Across 25 Nations.’’ Political Studies, 61(1): 45–65. racy.’’ International Political Science Review, 40(4): 518–
Bowler, S., and T. Donovan. (2013). ‘‘Civic Duty and Turnout 534. doi: 0192512118779213.
in the UK Referendum on AV: What Shapes the Duty to Ferwerda, J. (2014). ‘‘Electoral Consequences of Declining Par-
Vote?’’ Electoral Studies, 32(2): 265–273. ticipation: A Natural Experiment in Austria.’’ Electoral
Braconnier, C., J. Dormagen, and V. Pons. (2017). ‘‘Voter Regis- Studies, 35: 242–252.
tration Costs and Disenfranchisement: Experimental Evi- Fowler, A. (2013). ‘‘Electoral and Policy Consequences of
dence from France.’’ American Political Science Review, Voter Turnout: Evidence from Compulsory Voting in Aus-
111(3): 584–604. tralia.’’ Quarterly Journal of Political Science, 8(1): 159–
Bullock, W., and J.D. Clinton. (2011). ‘‘More a Molehill Than a 182.
Mountain: The Effects of the Blanket Primary on Elected Galais, C. (2018). ‘‘How to Make Dutiful Citizens and Influ-
Officials Behavior from California.’’ Journal of Politics, ence Turnout: The Effects of Family and School Dynamics
73(3): 915– 930. on the Duty to Vote.’’ Canadian Journal of Political Sci-
Campbell, A., P.E. Converse, W.E. Miller, and D.E. Stokes. ence, 51(3): 599–617.
(1960). The American Voter. Chicago, IL: University of Galais, C., and A. Blais. (2017). ‘‘Duty to Vote and Political
Chicago Press. Support in Asia.’’ International Journal of Public Opinion
Cancela, J., and B. Geys. (2016). ‘‘Explaining Voter Turnout: A Research, 29(4): 631–656.
Meta-Analysis of National and Subnational Elections.’’ Gordon, S.B., and G.M. Segura. (1997). ‘‘Cross-National Varia-
Electoral Studies, 42: 264–275. tion in the Political Sophistication of Individuals: Capabil-
Carey, J.M., and Y. Horiuchi. (2017). ‘‘Compulsory Voting and ity or Choice?’’ Journal of Politics, 59(1): 126–147.
Income Inequality: Evidence for Lijpharts Proposition from Heersink, B., and B.D. Peterson. (2017). ‘‘Truman Defeats
Venezuela.’’ Latin American Politics and Society, 59(2): Dewey: The Effect of Campaign Visits on Election Out-
122–144. comes.’’ Electoral Studies, 49: 49–64.
Chapman, E.B. (2018). ‘‘The Distinctive Value of Elections and Houle, C., C. Park, and P. Kenny. (2018). ‘‘The Structure of Eth-
the Case for Compulsory Voting.’’ American Journal of nic Inequality and Ethnic Voting.’’ Journal of Politics,
Political Science. 63(1): 101–112. doi: 10.111/ajps.12393. 81(1): 187:200. doi: 10.1086/700200.
Clarke, H., D. Sanders, M. Stewart, and P. Whiteley. (2004). Hirczy, W. (1994). ‘‘The Impact of Mandatory Voting Laws on
Political Choice in Britain. Oxford: Oxford University Turnout: A Quasi-Experimental Approach.’’ Electoral
Press. Studies, 13: 64–76.
Coppock, A. (2015). ‘‘10 Things to Know About Multiple Com- IDEA. (2018). Voter Turnout Database. Retrieved from
parisons [Methods Guide].’’ Retrieved from: <https://egap.org/ <https://www.idea.int/data- tools/data/voter-turnout>.
16 FEITOSA ET AL.

Instituto Nacional de Estadı́sticas. (2015). Compendio Estadı́s- Selb, P., and R. Lachat. (2009). ‘‘The More, the Better? Coun-
tico [Statistical Report, English translation]. Retrieved terfactual Evidence on the Effect of Compulsory Voting on
from: <http://historico.ine.cl/canales/menu/publicaciones/ the Consistency of Party Choice.’’ European Journal of
calendario_de_publicaciones/pdf/compendio_estadistico_ Political Research, 48(5): 573–597.
ine_2015.pdf>. Sheppard, J. (2015). ‘‘Compulsory Voting and Political Knowl-
Katz, G., and I. Levin. (2016). ‘‘The Dynamics of Political Sup- edge: Testing a Compelled Engagement Hypothesis.’’ Elec-
port in Emerging Democracies: Evidence from a Natural toral Studies, 40: 300–307.
Disaster in Peru.’’ International Journal of Public Opinion Shineman, V.A. (2018). ‘‘If You Mobilize Them, They Will
Research, 28(2): 173–195. Become Informed: Experimental Evidence That Information
Koehler, S., and T. König. (2015). ‘‘Fiscal Governance in the Acquisition Is Endogenous to Costs and Incentives to Partic-
Eurozone: How Effectively Does the Stability and Growth ipate.’’ British Journal of Political Science, 48(1): 189–211.
Pact Limit Governmental Debt in the Euro Countries?’’ Singh, S. (2011). ‘‘How Compelling Is Compulsory Voting? A
Political Science Research and Methods, 3(2): 329–351. Multilevel Analysis of Turnout.’’ Political Behavior, 33(1):
Latinobarómetro. (2018). 1995–2017 Cross-National Studies. 95–111.
Retrieved from <http://www.latinobarometro.org>.
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

Singh, S. (2017). ‘‘Politically Unengaged, Distrusting, and Dis-


Lijphart, A. (1997). ‘‘Unequal Participation: Democracy’s affected Individuals Drive the Link Between Compulsory
Unresolved Dilemma, Presidential Address, American Voting and Invalid Balloting.’’ Political Science Research
Political Science Association, 1996.’’ American Political and Methods, 7(1): 107–123. doi: 10.1017/psrm.2017.11.
Science Review, 91(1): 1–14. Singh, S. (2018). ‘‘Compulsory Voting and Dissatisfaction with
Lucardi, A. (2017). ‘‘The Effect Of District Magnitude on Elec- Democracy.’’ British Journal of Political Science, 48(3):
toral Outcomes: Evidence from Two Natural Experiments 843–854.
in Argentina.’’ British Journal of Political Science, 49(2): Singh, S. (2019). ‘‘Compulsory Voting and Parties Vote-
557–577. doi: 10.1017/S0007123416000740. Seeking Strategies.’’ American Journal of Political Science.
Lundell, K. (2012). ‘‘Civic Participation and Political Trust: 63(1): 37–52. doi: 10.1111/ajps.12386.
The Impact of Compulsory Voting.’’ Representation, 48(2): Singh, S., and J. Thornton. (2013). ‘‘Compulsory Voting and the
221–234. Dynamics of Partisan Identification.’’ European Journal of
Maldonado, A. (2015). The Origins and Consequences of Com- Political Research, 52(2): 188–211.
pulsory Voting in Latin America (Doctoral dissertation). Stockemer, D. (2017). ‘‘What Affects Voter Turnout? A Review
Retrieved from <https://etd.library.vanderbilt.edu/available/ Article/Meta-Analysis of Aggregate Research.’’ Govern-
etd-11202015-184530/unrestricted/Maldonado.pdf>. ment and Opposition, 52(4): 698–722.
Norris, P. (2004). Electoral Engineering: Voting Rules and Polit- Varieties of Democracy Project (2018). V-Dem Dataset (Country-
ical Behavior. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Year Extended): Version 8.
Palfrey, T.R., and K.T. Poole. (1987). ‘‘The Relationship Retrieved from <https://www.v-dem.net/en/data/data-version-
Between Information, Ideology, and Voting Behavior.’’ 8/>.
American Journal of Political Science, 31(3): 511–530.
Panagopoulos, C. (2008). ‘‘The Calculus of Voting in Compul-
Address correspondence to:
sory Voting Systems.’’ Political Behavior, 30(4): 455–467.
Prior, M. (2003). ‘‘Any Good News in Soft News? The Impact Fernando Feitosa
of Soft News Preference on Political Knowledge.’’ Political Department of Political Science
Communication, 20(2): 149–171. Université de Montréal
Quintelier, E., M. Hooghe, and S. Marien. (2011). ‘‘The Effect 3150 Jean-Brillant St., Room C-3123
of Compulsory Voting on Turnout Stratification Patterns: A
Montréal, Quebec H3T 1N8
Cross-National Analysis.’’ International Political Science
Review, 32(4): 396–416. Canada
Rangel, G. (2017). Voting as a (Mandatory) Duty: Citizen Atti- E-mail: f.feitosa.ribeiro@umontreal.ca
tudes, Political Engagement, and Party Outreach Under
Compulsory Voting (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from
Received for publication December 8, 2018; re-
<https://uknowledge.uky.edu/polysci_etds/21/>.
Riker, W.H., and P.C. Ordeshook. (1968). ‘‘A Theory of the ceived in revised form October 10, 2019; accepted
Calculus of Voting.’’ American Political Science Review, October 30, 2019; published online December 18,
62(1): 25–42. 2019.

(Appendix follows/)
DOES COMPULSORY VOTING FOSTER CIVIC DUTY TO VOTE? 17

Appendix
Appendix A

Table A1. Voting Rules from the 17 Latin


American Countries in Our Sample

Compulsory Enforcement
Country Voting and severity

Argentina Yes 1
Bolivia Yes 2
Brazil Yes 2
Chile Yes (until 2012) 1
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

Colombia No -
Costa Rica Yes 1
Dominican Yes 1
Republic
Ecuador Yes 2
El Salvador No -
Guatemala No -
Honduras Yes 1
Mexico Yes 1
Nicaragua No -
Panama Yes 1
Paraguay Yes 1
Peru Yes 3
Uruguay Yes 3
Notes: Data come from the 2018 Varieties of Democracy Project. 3
means ‘‘sanctions are enforced, and impose considerable costs’’; 2,
‘‘sanctions are enforced, but they impose minimal costs’’; 1, ‘‘there
are no sanctions or they are not enforced.’’ We exclude Venezuela as
it also abolished compulsory voting rules, in 1993. Previous work has
made use of this classification of voting systems (Singh 2019), as
well as the same trichotomous categorization of compulsory voting sys-
tems (Panagopoulos 2008).

Appendix B

FIG. B1. Civic duty trends in Chile and in all CV countries (except Chile). Notes: The trend in all Latin American compulsory voting
systems (except Chile) differs from that in Chile before the 2012 intervention. Particularly, we observe a decline of civic duty right
before Chile’s electoral reform among all other countries and, consequently, an important dissonance of the duty trends. See Appendix
C for the civic duty trends in each Latin American country, and Appendix D for a formal test of the parallel trend assumption.
(Appendix continues/)
18 FEITOSA ET AL.

Appendix C
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

FIG. C1. Mean levels of civic duty in Latin American countries. Notes: Values correspond to the aggregate civic duty mean lev-
els, by country. In most countries where voting is mandatory, civic duty increased from 2011 to 2015.

(Appendix continues/)
DOES COMPULSORY VOTING FOSTER CIVIC DUTY TO VOTE? 19

Appendix D

Table D1. Formal Tests of the Parallel Trend Assumption

DV: Civic Duty


Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4

Chile -51.458** -0.042* -42.935* -0.041


(18.815) (0.019) (18.215) (0.021)
Year -0.019* -0.015*
(0.008) (0.007)
Chile · Year 0.026** 0.021*
(0.009) (0.009)
2008 -0.001 -0.003
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

(0.027) (0.029)
2009 0.031 0.037
(0.026) (0.027)
2010 -0.008 -0.005
(0.026) (0.028)
2011 -0.092** -0.073*
(0.033) (0.031)
Chile · 2008 -0.040 -0.037
(0.027) (0.029)
Chile · 2009 -0.008 -0.015
(0.026) (0.027)
Chile · 2010 0.033 0.031
(0.026) (0.028)
Chile · 2011 0.092* 0.073
(0.033) (0.031)
Constant 39.034* 0.717*** 30.511* 0.715***
(15.282) (0.019) (14.531) (0.021)
N 66,217 66,217 61,524 61,524
Notes: *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. Clustered standard errors (by country-year) in parentheses. Only compulsory voting systems are ana-
lyzed, including Chile. In models 1 and 3, year is taken as a continuous variable. In models 2 and 4, it is taken as a categorical variable. Following
Coppock (2015), we correct for multiple comparisons by means of the Holm’s approach. For the sake of parsimony, the original and adjusted p-
values of the interaction terms in models 2 and 4 are not printed in the table. To test whether Honduras—where civic duty declined to below 50
percent in 2011—drives the results, we excluded it in additional analyses. They correspond to models 2 to 4. Three of the four models fail to con-
firm the parallel trend assumption.
DV, dependent variable.

Appendix E

Table E1. Descriptive Statistics of All Variables in the Synthetic Control Estimations (2007–2011 period)

Mean Standard deviation Minimum Maximum N

Civic duty* 0.71 0.08 0.42 0.85 85


Voting system 0.76 0.43 0.00 1.00 85
Religion* 2.51 0.23 1.84 2.91 85
Education 7.42 1.32 5.01 10.35 85
Age 0.42 0.05 0.31 0.51 85
Support for democracy* 2.41 0.18 2.04 2.78 85
Trust in parliament* 2.09 0.25 1.45 2.78 85
Democratization level 2.56 0.50 2.00 3.00 85
Notes: Variables marked with an asterisk (*) correspond to country-level aggregations of individual-level variables. At that level, 1 = ‘‘duty,’’
0 = ‘‘choice.’’ 4 = ‘‘very devout,’’ 3 = ‘‘devout,’’ 2 = ‘‘not very devout,’’ 1 = ‘‘not devout at all.’’ 3 = ‘‘Democracy is preferable to any other kind of
government,’’ 2 = ‘‘under some circumstances, an authoritarian government can be preferable to a democratic one,’’ 1 = ‘‘For people like me, it
doesn’t matter whether we have a democratic or non-democratic regime.’’ 4 = ‘‘trust a lot,’’ 3 = ‘‘some,’’ 2 = ‘‘a little,’’ 1 = ‘‘do not trust.’’ Variables
not marked with an asterisk (*) are originally country-level variables. 1 = ‘‘compulsory,’’ 0 = ‘‘voluntary.’’ 3 = ‘‘free,’’ 2 = ‘‘partly free,’’ 1 = ‘‘not
free.’’ Education corresponds to average years of formal schooling. Age corresponds to percentage of 19–34 year-olds in the voting-age population.

(Appendix continues/)
20 FEITOSA ET AL.

Appendix F

Table F1. Weights Assigned to Latin


American Countries

Synthetic Synthetic
control control
Country weight Country weight

Peru .69 Honduras 0


Argentina 0 Mexico 0
Bolivia 0 Panama .02
Brazil 0 Paraguay 0
Costa Rica 0 Uruguay .29
Dominican 0 Colombia 0
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

Republic
Ecuador 0 El Salvador 0
Nicaragua 0 Guatemala 0
Notes: Weights were assigned based on a country’s pre-treatment score
on the following variables: civic duty, voting system, religion, educa-
tion, age, support for democracy, trust in parliament, and democratiza-
tion level. Higher values mean that a country contributed more to the
estimation of synthetic Chile and 0 means that a country did not contrib-
ute at all. Consistent with previous studies (Arnold and Stadelmann-
Steffen 2017; Fowler 2013), many countries did not contribute to the
estimation of ‘‘synthetic’’ Chile.
Values in bold correspond to the estimated contribution of donor coun-
tries.

Appendix G

FIG. G1. Placebo tests with Chile and 16 Latin American countries. Notes: In these placebo tests, a synthetic control is estimated
for Chile and all 16 countries in the donor pool. Values in the figure correspond to the difference between the mean values of civic
duty in the treated unit and its synthetic control.

(Appendix continues/)
DOES COMPULSORY VOTING FOSTER CIVIC DUTY TO VOTE? 21

Appendix H
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

FIG. H1. Placebo tests with Chile and three countries in the donor pool (Argentina, Colombia, and El Salvador). Notes: In these
placebo tests, a synthetic control is estimated for Chile and three countries whose difference in the mean levels of civic duty was not
twice larger than Chile in Appendix G. They are Argentina, Colombia, and El Salvador.

Appendix I

FIG. I1. Synthetic control with civic duty means in 2007 and 2011, and their growth rate, as suggested by Beckley et al. (2017).
Notes: To further test whether results are the same when accounting for over-fitting, we follow Beckley et al. (2017) and substitute
the lagged civic duty for its 2007 and 2011 values, and the corresponding growth rate between these years, as well. Results are
essentially the same, with a remaining 10-percentage-point difference between ‘‘real’’ Chile and its synthetic control after the ab-
olition of compulsory voting.

(Appendix continues/)
22 FEITOSA ET AL.

Appendix J
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

FIG. J1. Analysis with three Latinobarómetro studies (from 2007, 2011, and 2015). Notes: In this analysis, we use the data from
only three Latinobarómetro (from 2007, 2011, and 2015). Each study is thus separated by a four-year gap. Results point to a dif-
ference between ‘‘real’’ Chile and synthetic Chile after the abolition of compulsory voting of 10 percentage points.

Appendix K

FIG. K1. Synthetic control without Peru as a potential donor country. Notes: Before the estimation of the synthetic control
model, Peru is excluded from the data and, thus, from the pool of potential donor countries. After this exclusion, the synthetic con-
trol is then calculated based on the same set of eight variables.

(Appendix continues/)
DOES COMPULSORY VOTING FOSTER CIVIC DUTY TO VOTE? 23

Appendix L
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

FIG. L1. Synthetic control without Uruguay as a potential donor country. Notes: Before the estimation of the synthetic control
model, Uruguay is excluded from the data and, thus, from the pool of potential donor countries. After this exclusion, the synthetic
control is then calculated based on the same set of eight variables.

Appendix M

FIG. M1. Synthetic control without Panama as a potential donor country. Notes: Before the estimation of the synthetic control
model, Panama is excluded from the data and, thus, from the pool of potential donor countries. After this exclusion, the synthetic
control is then calculated based on the same set of eight variables.

(Appendix continues/)
24 FEITOSA ET AL.

Appendix N
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

FIG. N1. Synthetic control model with rural respondents (i.e., those living in cities with under 5,000 inhabitants). Notes: Before
the calculation of the mean civic duty levels by country, we exclude respondents who do not live in a city with less than 5,000
inhabitants, which we define as rural area, from the data. We subsequently estimate the synthetic control model with such new data.

Appendix O

FIG. O1. Synthetic control model with registration rates as a predictor. Notes: To account for the temporal change in registration
rates across countries, we added this variable in the estimation of the synthetic control. Such inclusion does not alter the main find-
ings of a negative (10-percentage-point) effect of the abolition of compulsory voting on civic duty.

(Appendix continues/)
DOES COMPULSORY VOTING FOSTER CIVIC DUTY TO VOTE? 25

Appendix P

Table P1. Effect of Compulsory Voting Rules on Sense of Civic Duty to Vote, Estimated by Multi-Level Models

Latinobarómetro ISSP
Duty: 0–1 Variable Duty: 1–7 Scale Duty: 0–1 Variable
(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)

Compulsory Voting 0.029 -0.048 0.392*** 0.376*** 0.363*** 0.314***


(0.065) (0.081) (0.076) (0.080) (0.087) (0.091)
Age 0.004*** 0.021*** 0.021***
(0.001) (0.001) (0.001)
Education 0.051*** 0.072*** 0.103***
(0.007) (0.006) (0.008)
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

Female 0.057*** 0.074*** 0.061**


(0.016) (0.014) (0.019)
Religion -0.011 0.064*** 0.075***
(0.010) (0.004) (0.005)
Support for Democracy 0.190***
(0.010)
Trust in Parliament 0.098***
(0.009)
Trust in Politicians 0.129*** 0.219***
(0.007) (0.010)
Democratization 0.205*** -0.109 0.058
Level (0.049) (0.100) (0.137)
Constant 0.882*** -0.384* 1.421*** -0.701
(0.077) (0.157) (0.085) (0.406)
Var (Country) 0.055 0.054 0.262 0.245 0.299 0.276
Fixed Effects: Year Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes
N (countries) 17 17 45 45 45 45
N (respondents) 102169 79335 82623 74214 84146 75253
Notes: ***p < 0.01; **p < 0.05; *p < 0.1. Values correspond to log-odds. They are calculated by means of (ordered) logistic multi-level regressions
of sense of civic duty with pooled Latinobarómetro and International Social Survey Program (ISSP) data. Individuals are nested within countries
(n = 17 and 45 in the Latinobarómetro and the ISSP data, respectively). Year fixed effects are included but not shown. Computing the average mar-
ginal effect from the dichotomized civic duty measure in the ISSP data (column 6), we obtain a four-percentage-point increase in civic duty with the
presence of compulsory voting (results not shown).

(Appendix continues/)
26 FEITOSA ET AL.

Appendix Q
Table Q1. Effect of Compulsory Voting Rules on Sense of Civic Duty to Vote,
Estimated by Country and Year Fixed Effects

Latinobarómetro ISSP
Duty: 0–1 Variable Duty: 1–7 Scale Duty: 0–1 Variable

Compulsory voting -0.022 0.328*** 0.263**


(0.101) (0.073) (0.099)
Age 0.004*** 0.021*** 0.021***
(0.001) (0.002) (0.001)
Education 0.052*** 0.072*** 0.103***
(0.007) (0.016) (0.008)
Female 0.057*** 0.075** 0.062**
Downloaded by AUT UNIVERSITY (Auckland University of Tech) from www.liebertpub.com at 12/28/19. For personal use only.

(0.016) (0.023) (0.019)


Religion -0.011 0.064*** 0.075***
(0.010) (0.009) (0.005)
Support for democracy 0.189***
(0.010)
Trust in parliament 0.098***
(0.009)
Trust in politicians 0.128*** 0.219***
(0.021) (0.010)
Democratization level 0.233*** -0.081 0.181
(0.053) (0.076) (0.181)
Constant -0.415* -1.116*
(0.203) (0.563)
Fixed effects: Country Yes Yes Yes
Fixed effects: Year Yes Yes Yes
N (countries) 17 45 45
N (respondents) 79335 74214 75253
Notes: ***p < 0.01; **p < 0.05; *p < 0.1. Values correspond to log-odds. They are calculated by means of (ordered) logistic regressions of sense of
civic duty with pooled Latinobarómetro and International Social Survey Program (ISSP) data, with clustered standard errors (by country). Country
and year fixed effects are included but not shown. Computing the average marginal effect with ISSP data we obtain a four-percentage-point increase
in civic duty with the presence of compulsory voting, as in the multi-level regressions (results not shown).

Appendix R
Table R1. Analyses of Compulsory Voting’s Effect on Civic Duty with the Difference-in-Differences Approach

Difference between Chile


All CV countries Chile and all CV countries

With 2011 and 2015 Latinobarómetro:


Before abolition of compulsory voting 0.625 0.674 0.049***
(0.015)
After abolition of compulsory voting 0.674 0.618 -0.056***
(0.016)
Difference-in-differences - - -0.105***
(0.022)
With all Latinobarómetro studies:
Before abolition of compulsory voting 0.703 0.676 -0.027***
(0.007)
After abolition of compulsory voting 0.674 0.618 -0.056***
(0.015)
Difference-in-differences - - -0.029*
(0.017)
With all Latinobarómetro studies and excluding Honduras:
Before abolition of compulsory voting 0.706 0.676 -0.030***
(0.007)
After abolition of compulsory voting 0.683 0.618 -0.064***
(0.015)
Difference-in-differences - - -0.034**
(0.017)
Notes: ***p < 0.01; **p < 0.05; *p < 0.1. In addition to testing with data from immediately before and after the abolition of compulsory voting, we fur-
ther tested the impact of compulsory voting on civic duty including all Latinobarómetro pre-treatment data and excluding Honduras as well, where civic
duty drops to below .50 in 2011–right before the treatment. These more conservative tests yield a lower but still negative (and significant) effect.
Values in bold correspond to the difference-in-difference estimates.

You might also like