Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

CHILD

Bugental,MALTREATMENT
Shennum / GENDER
/ FEBRUARY
AND VIOLENCE
2002

Gender, Power, and Violence in the Family

Daphne Blunt Bugental


University of California, Santa Barbara
William Shennum
Five Acres, Altadena, California

Attributions for parent-child relationship outcomes were as- Perceived Power and Maltreatment
sessed as a function of childhood maltreatment history. In
Past research on the role of attributions in child mal-
Study 1, the attributions of maltreated children were compared
treatment has focused on biased maternal attributions.
with those of nonmaltreated children from a similar back-
Those mothers who are more likely to use harsh or abusive
ground. Maltreated girls (but not boys) were found to be more
tactics with their children have been found to attribute
likely than nonmaltreated girls to attribute lower power or con-
higher intentionality, control, and blame to children than
trol to self than to parents. Effects of maltreatment on chil-
to self (e.g., Bradley & Peters, 1991; Bugental, Blue, &
dren’s attributions were found for physical but not sexual
Cruzcosa, 1989; Nix, Pinderhughes, Dodge, & Bates,
abuse. In Study 2, the attributions of parents with a history of
1999). The causal role of such biases is suggested by the
abuse as children were compared with those of parents who had
fact that prebirth attributions of mothers serve to predict
not been abused. Women (but no men) with a history of abuse
subsequent child maltreatment during the first year of the
attributed less power or control to self than to children. Impli-
child’s life (Bugental, 1999).
cations are drawn for gender differences in the role of biased
The attributional patterns that are more characteristic
attributions as a consequence of physical abuse.
of harsh or physically abusive mothers have been concep-
tualized in power terms within Bugental’s program of

Violence in the home is a commonly occurring event. research. Physically abusive mothers tend to view them-
selves as victims and children as blameworthy and power-
In any given year, more than 2 million cases of suspected ful; this interpretation, in turn, promotes their efforts to
child abuse are reported (Daro & McCurdy, 1990). The reestablish power within the relationship (Bugental,
experience of abuse not only fosters a variety of undesired Lewis, Lin, Lyon, & Kopeikin, 2000). The notion of exag-
outcomes in terms of children’s social behavior, cognitive gerated power assertion among those who see themselves
performance, and health, but also influences the possibil- as lacking power is well-supported within the social power
ity that those who are abused will themselves become abu- literature. That is, those who see themselves as lacking
sive as parents (Putallaz, Costanzo, Grimes, & Sherman, control or power are more likely than others to make exag-
1998). In this article, we explore the extent to which a
history of abuse influences the attributions acquired by
children for the relationship between parents and chil-
dren. In particular, we ask whether a history of abuse fos-
ters perceived powerlessness. Do those who have experi-
enced abuse think of themselves as much less powerful
than parents? Our central predictions in this study are for
the effects of physical abuse.

Authors’ Note: This research was supported by National Institute of


Mental Health Grant No. 5 R01 MH 19095 and NSF Grant No. BNS
C CHILD MALTREATMENT, Vol. 7, No. 1, February 2002 55-63
H© 2002 Sage Publications 9021221.

55
56 Bugental, Shennum / GENDER AND VIOLENCE

gerated use of coercive force (e.g., Kipnis, 1976; Raven & among boys than among girls (Crane-Ross, Tisak, &
Kruglanski, 1970). Tisak, 1998; Egan, Monson, & Perry, 1998; Steinberg &
Relatively little is known, however, about the origins Dodge, 1983). Little evidence has been found for a rela-
of such power-oriented attributional biases. That is, do tionship between girls’ experience of harsh parenting,
adults acquire such biases as a result of their own early their subsequent cognitive biases, and later instances of
experiences? The current evidence for this possibility is physically aggressive behavior with peers. Girls typically
indirect. As one suggestive indication, Grusec and lack the contextual support for redirecting aggression to
Mammone (1995) found that mothers with an insecure peers.
attachment history (as measured by the Adult Attach- Relatively little is known concerning the sequelae of
ment Interview) showed attributional biases (as measured child maltreatment on children’s attributions regarding
by Bugental’s Parent Attribution Test). Specifically, they parent-child relationships. As suggestive support,
were more likely to conceptualize their relationships in Fincham, Beach, Arias and Brody (1998) found an associ-
terms of power inequities; that is, they were more likely to ation between parent-child conflict and the presence of
see children and parents as having unequal rather than conflict-promoting attributions among the children
balanced power. As an another suggestive indication, themselves. However, the differential effect of maltreat-
Dodge and his colleagues (e.g., Dodge, Pettit, Bates, & ment on the attributional biases of boys and girls remains
Valente, 1995) have observed that children’s biased attri- an open question.
butions partially mediate the relationship between a his-
Research Questions
tory of harsh parenting and their subsequent
aggressiveness. The research presented here asks whether those who
Although there is abundant evidence to show the neg- have been abused as children hold distinctive
ative affective and cognitive consequences that result attributional patterns concerning the causes of caregiving
from a history of childhood maltreatment (e.g., Cicchetti conflict. It also asks whether there are gender differences
& Toth, 1997; Margolin & Gordis, 2000), less is known in the power-based attributional biases shown by males
about the various ways in which children respond to this and females in response to a history of harsh or abusive
experience. As suggested by Downey and her colleagues parenting. Within our program of research, “low power”
(e.g., Downey, Feldman, Khuri, & Friedman, 1994), mal- attributions have been defined in terms of the individuals’
treatment may have quite different consequences for dif- belief that negative events are under the control of others
ferent children; whereas some children are more likely to and outside the control of self. Such attributions are pre-
respond to such experiences with aggression, others dicted—and have consistently been found—to be related
respond with depression. In this article, we explore the to high efforts to exercise power in contexts that allow
possibility that gender may serve as a moderator of the such responses and submissiveness and withdrawal in con-
effects of physical abuse on children’s power-based texts that constrain such responses. For example, those
attributional patterns. with low perceived power show high power assertion in
The domains in which and the means by which males interaction with those who pose no threat and low power
and females manifest and think about interpersonal power assertion with those who do pose a threat (see Bugental &
(including aggression) begin in early childhood (e.g., Goodnow, 1998; Bugental et al., 2000; and Bugental &
Coie & Dodge, 1998; Ruble & Martin, 1998). Research Johnston, 2000, for summaries of this program of
on the role of power-based attributions among parents has research).
generally focused on mothers (Bugental & Johnston, We assessed gender differences in attributions in two
2000). There are some indications that mothers’ attribu- different settings. We initially explored the attributions
tions regarding their competence and power have stron- made by maltreated versus nonmaltreated children. We
ger effects on offspring aggression than do fathers’ attribu- then went on to explore the attributions made by parents
tions (e.g., Bugental & Martorell, 1999). However, who had experienced maltreatment in their own
information on fathers within this literature is sparse. childhood.
In the same way, concern with power-based attribu-
tions in children’s peer relations has generally focused on
STUDY 1
boys (Coie & Dodge, 1998). The indications are that boys
who experience harsh parenting—and who develop hos- Method
tile attributional biases—receive subsequent support for
Research Participants
such biases in their peer relationships (Dodge et al.,
1995). When gender comparisons have been made, hos- The study involved 22 physically abused and 28
tile attributional biases have been found to be more nonabused children from similar demographic back-
closely associated with reactive physical aggression grounds. The abused children in this study had been

CHILD MALTREATMENT / FEBRUARY 2002


Bugental, Shennum / GENDER AND VIOLENCE 57

removed from their homes or previous placements and aminers for the maltreated group were blind to children’s
were subsequently relocated in a residential treatment specific history but necessarily were aware that children
center for maltreated children. were living in a residential facility for maltreated children.
The nonabused children were recruited from a low- The PIXAT’s interview format involves questions
income neighborhood; their ethnic and socioeconomic concerning the causes of conflictual interaction between
characteristics were similar to those of children within the parents and children. Children are presented with car-
residential facility. Flyers were placed in public locations toon pictures that depict a problem in a family followed by
in a low-income area of Santa Barbara, California. The cartoon pictures that depict a solution. Two problems
flyers, which included both English and Spanish text, involve a mother or father who is shown as angry with a
were placed in such locations as grocery store bulletin child, and two involve a mother or father who is shown
boards, laundry room bulletin boards, and the unemploy- spanking a child. The sex of the child in the pictures was
ment office. As far as possible, families were matched for matched with the sex of the research participant.
ethnicity and education of mother. They were also The children’s task was to tell a story about events
matched for gender and age of child. The sample of leading up to the problem and the solution. In doing so,
nonabused children is viewed as a comparison group they were asked to select from an array of pictures; they
drawn from a demographically similar background rather were told that they may use as many or as few pictures as
than as a fully matched group. they wish. Coding of children’s picture selection was,
The abused group included 15 boys and 7 girls; the however, limited to their first three choices.
nonabused sample included 18 boys and 7 girls. Within Our past work has revealed significant effects only for
the abused sample, 14 children were White, Anglo chil- children’s attributions for problems (Bugental &
dren and 8 children were Latino. Within the nonabused Martorell, 1999; Bugental et al., 1992). Thus, analyses in
sample, 10 children were White, Anglo children, and 8 the present study were limited to children’s attributions
children were Latino. The mean age in both groups was for negative events.
9.0 years (range = 7 to 11 years). The mean education of Perceived power was defined by the proportion of pic-
mothers in the abused group was 11.5 years; the mean edu- tures the child selected in which the child—as opposed to
cation of mothers in the nonabused sample was 13.0 years. other people—was depicted as the primary causal agent in
Within the abused group, 51% of the children (58% of the story. Test-retest reliability of PIXAT scores is .71
boys and 44% of girls) had experienced only physical (Cortez & Bugental, 1994). Those children who have low
abuse, 11% (11% of boys and 11% of girls) only sexual perceived power, as measured by the PIXAT, have been
abuse, 27% (20% of boys and 44% of girls) both physical found to be highly reactive to potential social threat. For
and sexual abuse, and 11% (11% of boys and 11% of girls) example, they are attentionally avoidant and show perfor-
neither sexual or physical abused but neglect. The gender mance errors when confronted with potential threat from
differences in abuse history did not approach significance. an adult (Bugental, Lin, & Susskind, 1995; Cortez &
Within the abused group, 13% had been removed from Bugental, 1994). In contrast, they have been found to
a home environment that included at least one biological show high levels of verbal aggression when placed in com-
parent, 41% had been removed from a foster home, 13% petitive settings with peers (Bugental & Martorell, 1999).
had been removed from a group home, 17% had been The combination of avoidance (when they lack veridical
removed from a juvenile detention facility, and 17% had control) and aggression (when they have an equal level of
been removed from a psychiatric facility. There were no power with others) is consistent with the response pat-
differential gender patterns in children’s prior residence. terns shown by adults with low perceived power (dis-
Because of the extremity of the past experiences of chil- cussed in Study 2).
dren within the abused group, the nonabused group must
Results and Discussion
be considered as imperfectly matched with the abused
group. That is, the abused group is atypical in ways other Preliminary analyses were conducted to determine
than their abuse history. whether children’s attributional responses were influ-
enced by maternal education or child ethnicity. No signif-
Procedure
icant relationship was found between maternal education
The Picture Attribution Test. Children were given the and child PIXAT scores (r = –.01). No significant rela-
Picture Attribution Test (PIXAT) (Bugental, Cortez, & tionship was found between children’s minority or
Blue, 1992). The test was administered by undergraduate nonminority status and their PIXAT scores (t < 1).
assistants with both samples. Examiners watched video- Children’s attributional responses were analyzed in a 2
taped instructions on test administration and were given × 2 factorial ANOVA. Grouping variables were gender
practice in all procedures. Examiners for the and maltreatment history (presence or absence). The
nonmaltreated group were blind to children’s history. Ex- dependent variable was perceived power (i.e., the propor-

CHILD MALTREATMENT / FEBRUARY 2002


58 Bugental, Shennum / GENDER AND VIOLENCE

tion of pictures selected in which the child was depicted as TABLE 1: Power-Biased Attributions of Boys and Girls as a Func-
agentic). tion of Maltreatment History
An interaction was found between gender and mal-
Perceived Power
treatment history, F(1, 55) = 4.41, p = .04. As shown in (PIXAT scores)
Table 1, compared to nonmaltreated girls, maltreated girls Maltreatment
depicted themselves as having significantly less power (or History Gender n M SD
as less causal). In contrast, maltreated boys depicted
Yes Males 19 .39 22
themselves as having slightly greater power than did Females 9 .22a 15
nonmaltreated boys; however, this difference did not No Males 20 .34 18
reach statistical significance. Females 11 .39a 15
Follow-up analyses were conducted separately within NOTE: Scores represent the proportion of pictures in which the “con-
the two samples (maltreated versus nonmaltreated chil- troller” of a bad event is the child. Means followed by shared subscripts
dren). An initial analysis was conducted within the are significantly different at better than the .05 level of confidence.
nonmaltreated sample to test for gender effects. No signif-
icant effects or trends were found (F < 1).
A second analysis was conducted within the mal-
It is important to recognize the special nature of this
treated sample to test for the effects of type of abuse. The
first ANOVA included gender and physical abuse as population. Children with extreme early histories are very
grouping variables; placement history (direct removal likely to have experienced multiple types of victimization,
from a home environment versus prior placement in and reporting history is not entirely reliable. To the
another institution), sexual abuse (presence or absence), extent reporting is accurate, it should be recognized that
and neglect (presence or absence) were introduced as physical abuse was by far the most commonly reported
covariates. A significant effect was found for gender, F(1, type of maltreatment; that is, 78% of children within this
84) = 4.84, p = .039. As shown in Table 1, girls in the mal- sample were known to have been physically abused. In
treated population showed lower levels of perceived addition, only physical maltreatment was associated with
power than did boys. In addition, the effect of physical generally low levels of perceived power among abused
abuse approached significance, F(1, 21) = 4.22, p = .053. children. Therefore, it is likely that findings are more
All children who had been physically abused showed applicable to a history of physical abuse and cannot be
lower levels of perceived power (mean PIXAT score = applied to children who have been sexually abused.
.22) than did children who had not been physically
abused (mean PIXAT score = .54). A second ANOVA
included gender and sexual abuse as grouping variables; STUDY 2
placement history, physical abuse, and neglect were intro-
duced as covariates. History of sexual abuse (as a main The next question then becomes: Does the experience
effect or interaction) produced no significant effects or of maltreatment in childhood carry forward to influence
trends; however, the main effect of gender continued to be the power-based attributional biases made by parents with
significant, F(1, 21) = 7.99, p = .01. respect to their own children? Do women who experi-
A third analysis was conducted in which a test was enced abuse as children show a pattern of perceived pow-
made of institutional history. That is, children who had erlessness with their own children? In the same way, do
been placed in the current facility directly from a home men who experienced abuse as children show a pattern of
environment were compared with children who had been exceptionally high perceived power with their own
moved to the current facility from another institutional children?
setting. Perceived power was tested as a combined effect of To answer these questions, we assessed gender differ-
gender and institutional history. No significant effects or
ences among adults as a function of maltreatment history.
trends were found as a function of institutional history.
This analysis was part of a larger longitudinal study that
As a general pattern, it appears that boys and girls
assessed prebirth parental predictors of infant maltreat-
respond in different ways to victimization in childhood.
Abused girls were significantly more likely than boys to ment during the first year of the child’s life (Bugental,
respond with depressed levels of self-perceived power in 1999). Our specific concern here was with parents’ own
relationships with adults. Maltreated boys did not reveal recollected history of abuse as a child and the extent to
any significant differences in their attributions as a func- which this history predicts caregiver attributions as adults.
tion of abuse history. The observed trend for maltreated In addition, we asked whether men and women differed in
boys (very high perception of self-perceived power) will the attributions they revealed as a function of their own
require further support before it can be safely interpreted. childhood history.

CHILD MALTREATMENT / FEBRUARY 2002


Bugental, Shennum / GENDER AND VIOLENCE 59

Method score whereas rating “being sick” as an important cause of


caregiving failure contributed to a low ACF score; rating
Sample
“child stubbornness” as very important for caregiving fail-
The sample included 39 couples identified as at risk to ure contributed to a high CCF score and rating “child
become abusive with their children. Couples were drawn being tired” as very important for caregiving failure con-
from a larger sample that also included single parents. Par- tributed to a low CCF score.
ticipants were recruited through local clinics and obstetri- The two scales that measure attributions for caregiving
cians. The Family Stress Checklist (FSC) (Murphy, failure (ACF and CCF) are orthogonal. They are usually
Orkow, & Nikola, 1985) was used as a screening instru- combined to provide a “perceived control over failure”
ment. Kempe’s FSC provides an indication of the level of (PCF) scale. The scale should be conceptualized as mea-
difficulty families are experiencing. A score of 0 to 20 suring the amount of power or control that is attributed to
indicates low risk (for child maltreatment), a score of 25 adult caregivers versus the amount of control that is
or more indicates moderate risk, and a score of more than attributed to dependent children. That is, an adult who
40 has been correlated with a high potential for abuse. attributes high power to adults and low power to the child
Those families who scored as being at moderate risk for has the highest possible perceived balance of power or
abuse (scores between 20 and 40) were invited to partici- control (high PCF) in the relationship. Conversely, an
pate. About 37% of parents who have scores in this range adult who attributes high power to children and low
have been found to abuse their children early in life. power to adults has the lowest possible perceived balance
All research participants were Latino. Our analysis was of power in the relationship. Our choice of terms has been
restricted to partnered couples to ensure that any gender conceptually driven by the social power literature. As
differences did not reflect (a) differences reflecting pres- noted earlier, some of the most punitive social responses
ence or absence of a partner (54% of our total sample have been found for those individuals who see themselves
included single mothers) or (b) differences in marital his- as at a “power disadvantage,” that is, they see others as
tory (e.g., spousal violence frequently preceded the depar- more powerful than they are themselves (e.g., Kipnis,
ture of a male partner; thus, single mothers had a more 1976; Raven & Kruglanski, 1970). The concept of bal-
extensive history of spousal battery). ance of power is dyadic in nature. As discussed in Bugental
and Johnston’s (2000) review of this literature, increasing
Measures
attention is being given to dyadic constructs within the
Parent Attribution Test (PAT). Prospective parents parent cognition literature.
were administered the PAT at the point of intake to the Extensive testing of the validity of the PAT (perceived
study. The PAT (Bugental & Shennum, 1984; Bugental balance of power) has been made during the past 15 years,
et al., 1989) was developed to assess the attributions adults both in our own research program and in other programs
make for the causes of their successful and unsuccessful in- focused on parent-child relationships (Bugental,
teractions with children. Respondents rate the relative Johnston, New, & Silvester, 1998). Women with a low
importance they would assign (on 7-point scales) to po- perceived balance of power have been found to react neg-
tential causes of caregiving outcomes. For example, they atively to potential challenge or threat within caregiving
rate the importance they assign to such factors as child relationships. When caregiver challenge is combined
stubbornness, child mood, or adult lack of parenting skill with a high veridical level of adult control, adults with low
as causes of an unsuccessful interaction. PAT items were perceived power often show exaggerated efforts to assert
initially selected on the basis of free responses given by control (e.g., Bradley & Peters, 1991; Bugental et al.,
mothers in reply to questions concerning the causes of 1989; Bugental, Blue, & Lewis, 1990; Bugental &
caregiving success and failure. Happaney, 2000; Bugental & Lewis, 1998; Bugental,
Based on the results of a multidimensional scaling Lewis, Lin, et al., 2000; Lewis, Bugental, & Fleck, 1991;
analysis and a subsequent factor analyses, PAT scales were Mills, 1998). In contrast, when adults with low perceived
developed that measure the perceived controllability of power are confronted with caregiving challenge com-
caregiving outcomes by the self and by children. The des- bined with lack of veridical control, they are often submis-
ignated meaning of scales was based on judgments of a sive and show high levels of false positivity (Bugental,
group of mothers; that is, an item was coded as controlla- Brown, & Reiss, 1996; Bugental & Lewis, 1998; Bugental
ble if the cause presented was generally seen as controlla- & Shennum, 1984). Adults with low perceived power also
ble by adults. For negative caregiving outcomes, the two show elevations in autonomic mobilization in response to
dimensions identified were Adult Control over Failure caregiving challenge (e.g., Bugental et al., 1993, 2000).
(ACF) and Child Control over Failure (CCF). So, for Despite their vigilance for possible caregiving threat, par-
example, rating “using the wrong strategy” as an impor- ents with low perceived power show deficits in their abil-
tant cause of caregiving failure contributed to a high ACF ity to make fine-grained distinctions in children’s behav-

CHILD MALTREATMENT / FEBRUARY 2002


60 Bugental, Shennum / GENDER AND VIOLENCE

ior and to respond adaptively to these distinctions Results


(Lovejoy, Polewko, & Harrison, 1996). When paired with
Participants’ scores on the PCF scale were compared in
“easy,” responsive children, however, the responses of low
a 2 × 2 ANOVA. The two grouping variables were gender1
power parents are indistinguishable from those of other
and maltreatment history. The analysis yielded a signifi-
parents.
cant gender effect, F(1, 71) = 7.68, p = .007, and a signifi-
In short, the construct of balance of power operates to
cant interaction between gender and maltreatment his-
produce conditional responses to caregiving situations; it
tory, F(1, 71) = 7.66, p = .04. As shown in Table 2,
serves to activate defensive responses when the caregiving
prospective mothers with a history of maltreatment
situation can be seen as a challenge or threat. Thus, it is
scored lower on the PCF scale than did prospective moth-
important to note that perceived power imbalance, as
ers without such a history. In contrast, prospective fathers
measured by the PAT, is related to insecure attachment
with a history of maltreatment scored higher on the PCF
categories on the Adult Attachment Interview (Grusec,
than did prospective fathers without such a history. How-
Adam, & Mammone, 1993; Grusec & Mammone, 1995).
ever, this difference did not reach significance.
Besides showing excellent construct validity, PAT
scores have been found to have good convergent and In summary, maltreated women, like maltreated girls,
divergent validity. For example, perceived balance of showed a pattern of perceived powerlessness. Just as mal-
power has been found to be unrelated to individual mea- treated girls saw themselves as having low power relative
sures of perceived control (as opposed to balance of con- to parental adults, maltreated women saw themselves as
trol within a dyad) or affective measures (Lovejoy, Verda, having low power relative to dependent children. In con-
& Hays, 1997). At the same time, low power scores on the trast, males with a history of maltreatment as children
PAT have been found to be significantly correlated with revealed trends suggesting the possibility that abuse his-
high attributions to powerful others (a scale on tory is associated with very high levels of perceived power.
Levenson’s Locus of Control Test, 1974) (Bugental &
Shennum, 1984). GENERAL DISCUSSION
Test-retest reliability (after 1 week) of PCF scores was
measured in two samples: a sample of undergraduate So what can we conclude? The attributional biases
women (N = 55) and a sample of mothers (N = 57). A reli- shown in response to childhood maltreatment appear to
ability coefficient (r) of .63 was obtained for mothers, and differ for males and females. Females who experienced
a coefficient of .61 was obtained for undergraduate maltreatment as children revealed a pattern of perceived
women. powerlessness. This same attributional bias was shown
both by girls who had recently been abused and by women
History of abuse. Participants’ own history of abuse as a
who reported themselves as having been abused as chil-
child was measured on the basis of an adapted version of
dren. In addition, the observed bias transcends means of
the Conflict Tactics Scale (CTS) (Straus, 1979). The
assessing abuse. Among children, abuse history was based
CTS asks for frequency of occurrence of different ways of
on the reports of social workers. Among parents, abuse
responding to family conflict. The instrument can be used
history was based on self-report. The attributional pattern
either in a questionnaire or interview format. Reliability
found for abused females is also consistent with the
coefficients of the CTS are .62 for the use of violent tactics
attributional pattern that has been found for women who
and .77 for verbal aggression for parent-child conflicts.
are physically abusive with their own children (Bugental
Among couples, the percentage of agreement is 77% for
et al., 1989).
husband’s violence and 80% for wife’s violence (Straus &
No significant effects were found for males as a func-
Gelles, 1990). It represents the most commonly used mea-
tion of abuse history. However, there was a consistent
sure of family violence, and it has been extensively used to
trend indicating the possibility that maltreated males are
assess prevalence rates of child maltreatment and spousal
more likely to show exaggerated perceptions of power. As
battery (Straus, Hamby, Finkelhor, & Moore, 1998).
these results did not reach significance nor were they pre-
CTS items were revised here to reflect participants’
dicted, it will be important to replicate this finding before
own history. Thus, participants were asked whether their
any conclusions can be drawn.
parents had ever used specific conflict tactics with them.
Whenever a respondent reported any instance of abusive Gender, Maltreatment History, and Attributional
tactics (e.g., being beat up by a parent), that individual Biases
was categorized as having been abused.
The observed gender differences in attributions may be
thought of as reflecting different accommodations to the
experience of parental maltreatment. Females’
attributional biases are congruent with their history. That

CHILD MALTREATMENT / FEBRUARY 2002


Bugental, Shennum / GENDER AND VIOLENCE 61

TABLE 2: Perceived Balance of Power Among Adults With Mal- As a second cautionary note, it should be pointed out
treatment History
that both child and adult samples involved high-risk par-
Perceived Balance
ticipants from homes with relatively low education. Both
of Power groups also included a large representation of Latino par-
(PAT scores) ticipants. However, in Study 1 (a sample that included
Maltreatment greater ethnic and educational variability than did the
History Gender n M SD
sample employed in Study 2), neither education or eth-
Yes Males 17 77a 57 nicity was found to qualify the observed effects. As an
Females 27 –02ab 86 additional caution, full information on trauma history was
No Males 31 39 40 only available for respondents in Study 2.
Females 21 39b 46
As a major limitation of the study, findings are based
NOTE: Scores range from –200 (low balance of power) to 200 (high bal-
ance of power). Means followed by shared subscripts are significantly dif-
on small samples. At the same time, the consistency of the
ferent at better than the .05 level of confidence. observed effects suggests the possibility that effects for
males might be significant in larger samples.

is, those who have been maltreated by their parents reveal Implications for Future Risk for Aggression
a pattern of low perceived power. In general, there is less
The different attributional biases shown by males and
contextual support for direct power assertion among girls
females in response to a childhood history of physical
than among boys (Maccoby, 1990). As a result, they may
abuse can be seen to have implications for subsequent risk
be more likely to construct relationship schemas that are
for aggression. Although the cognitive maintenance of
consistent with their history as victims of power assertion.
peer aggression is more pronounced among males than
That is, they develop power schemas in which they see
females, the cognitive maintenance of aggression in par-
themselves as occupying a position of low power relative
ent-child relations appears to be stronger among females
to the power of others.
than males. Supporting this notion, we have observed
These findings suggest the congruence between fe-
that power-biased attributions serve to predict physical
males’ perceived powerlessness and both (a) their own
abuse of infants among mothers but not fathers (Bugental,
history of abuse and (b) their subsequent risk for abusive-
1999). For women, the perceived powerlessness that fol-
ness with dependent others. When they themselves as-
lows from a history of abuse may be thought of as defensive
sume a position of power with respect to their own
in nature. That is, women with a low perceived balance of
children, their biased attributions may lead not only to
power are easily triggered to the possibility that depend-
negative interpretations of the behaviors of the young, but
ent children pose a threat—to which they respond “in
also to direct reactive aggression against the presumed
challenges of the young. As suggested by Egan et al. kind” with use of physical force. Although constrained by
(1998),
2 limitations in physical power and contextual support at
younger ages, mothers have both the veridical power
Perhaps these children (those who are victimized but do (physical strength) and the legitimate power (authority
not retaliate) should be considered “latent aggressors”— role) to exert physical power against the young.
people who are prone to behave aggressively when situa- Implications may also be drawn for remediation efforts.
tional constraints are relaxed, as when one is confronted In other ongoing research, we have made use of these find-
with an opportunity to aggress covertly or one is frustrated ings in an attributionally based child abuse prevention
by a weaker, younger, defenseless person (including, in
program (Bugental, 1999). That is, we designed a program
later years, a defenseless child). (p. 1003)
directed to reducing the extent to which mothers frame
Limitations their interaction with children as a power contest. In
doing so, we were able to demonstrate reductions in the
The findings obtained here can be generalized only to prevalence of child maltreatment. Thus, interventions
physical abuse. Although no attributional effects were that direct causal explanations away from interpersonal
found as a function of children’s history of sexual abuse, power have benefits for adaptive conflict resolution
there were a relatively small number of sexually abused within the family.
children within that sample. As a limitation, no informa-
tion was available on sexual abuse history of adult respon-
dents. Therefore, the possibility must still be considered NOTES
that gender differences may reflect the greater likelihood 1. Inclusion of both parents in the same analysis was viewed
of a combined history of physical abuse and sexual abuse as justified in the absence of any association between the PCF
among females. scores of husbands and wives.

CHILD MALTREATMENT / FEBRUARY 2002


62 Bugental, Shennum / GENDER AND VIOLENCE

2. Egan et al. were referring specifically to boys with hostile Cortez, V., & Bugental, D. B. (1994). Children’s visual avoidance of
threat: A strategy associated with low social control, Merrill-Palmer
biases who had been victimized by peers and subsequently re-
Quarterly (Special issue on Emotion Regulation), 40, 82-97.
duced their aggression. However, the same principle is more Crane-Ross, D., Tisak, M. S., & Tisak, J. (1998). Aggression and con-
broadly applicable. ventional rule violation among adolescents: Social-reasoning pre-
dictors of social behavior. Aggressive Behavior, 24, 347-365.
Daro, D., & McCurdy, K. (1990). Current trends in abuse reporting and
REFERENCES facilities: The results of the 1990 annual 50-state survey. Chicago:
National Committee for the Prevention of Child Abuse.
Bradley, E. J., & Peters, R. D. (1991). Physically abusive and nonabusive Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., & Valente, E. (1995). Social
mothers’ perceptions of parenting and child behavior. American information-processing patterns partially mediate the effect of early
Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 61, 455-460. physical abuse on later conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Psy-
Bugental, D. B. (1999, October). Power-oriented cognitions as predictors of chology, 104, 632-643.
family violence. Paper presented at the meetings of the Society for Downey, G., Feldman, S., Khuri, J., & Friedman, S. (1994). Maltreat-
Experimental Social Psychology, St. Louis, MO. ment and childhood depression. In W. M. Reynolds & H. F.
Bugental, D. B., Blue, J., Cortez, V., Fleck, K., Kopeikin, H., Lewis, J., & Johnston (Eds.), Handbook of depression in children and adolescents
Lyon, J. (1993). Social cognitions as organizers of autonomic and (pp. 481-508). New York: Plenum.
affective responses to social challenge. Journal of Personality and Egan, S. K., Monson, T. C., & Perry, D. G. (1998). Social-cognitive
Social Psychology, 64, 94-103. influences on change in aggression over time. Developmental Psychol-
Bugental, D. B., Blue, J. B., & Cruzcosa, M. (1989). Perceived control ogy, 34, 996-1006.
over caregiving outcomes: Implications for child abuse. Developmen- Fincham, F. D., Beach, S.R.H., Arias, I., & Brody, G. H. (1998).
tal Psychology, 25, 532-539. Children’s attributions in the family: The Children’s Relationship
Bugental, D. B., Blue, J., & Lewis, J. (1990). Caregiver beliefs and Attribution Measure. Journal of Family Psychology, 12, 481-493.
dysphoric affect to difficult children. Developmental Psychology, 26, Grusec, J. E., Adam, E., & Mammone, N. (1993, April). Mental represen-
631-638. tations of relationships, parent belief systems, and parenting behavior.
Bugental, D. B., Brown, M., & Reiss, C. (1996). Cognitive representa- Paper presented at the Meetings of the Society for Research in Child
tions of power in caregiving relationships: Biasing effects on inter- Development, New Orleans.
personal interaction and information-processing. Journal of Family Grusec, J. E., & Mammone, N. (1995). Features and sources of parents’
Psychology, 10, 397-407. attributions about themselves and their children. In N.
Bugental, D. B., Cortez, V., & Blue, J. (1992). Children’s affective Eisenberg (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 15,
responses to the expressive cues of others. In N. Eisenberg & R. pp. 49-73). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Fabes (Eds.), New directions in child development (pp. 75-90). San Kipnis, D. (1976). The powerholders. Chicago: University of Chicago
Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Press.
Bugental, D. B., & Goodnow, J. G. (1998) Socialization processes. In N. Larrance, D. T., & Twentyman, C. T. (1983). Maternal attributions and
Eisenberg (Volume Ed.). Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, child abuse. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92, 449-457.
emotional, and personality development (pp. 389-462). New York: John Lewis, J. C., Bugental, D. B., & Fleck, K. (1991). Beliefs as moderators of
Wiley. reactions to computer-simulated responsive and unresponsive chil-
Bugental, D. B., & Happaney, K., (2000). Parent-child interaction as a dren. Social Cognition, 9, 277-293.
power contest. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 21, 267- Lovejoy, M. C., Polewko, J., & Harrison, B. (1996). Adult perceptions of
282. interpersonal control and reactions to disruptive child behaviors.
Bugental, D. B., & Johnston, C. (2000). Parental and child cognitions in Social Cognition, 14, 227-245.
the context of the family. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 315-344. Lovejoy, M. C., Verda, M. R., & Hays, C. E. (1997). Convergent and
Bugental, D. B., Johnston, C., New, M., & Silvester, J. (1998). Mea- discriminant validity of measures of parenting efficacy and control.
suring parental attributions: Conceptual and methodological issues. Journal of Clinical and Child Psychology, 26, 366-376.
Journal of Family Psychology, 12, 459-480. Maccoby, E. (1990). The role of gender identity and gender constancy:
Bugental, D. B., & Lewis, J. (1998). Interpersonal power repair in A developmental account. American Psychologist, 45, 513-520.
response to threats to control from dependent others. In M. Kofta, G. Margolin, G., & Gordis, E. B. (2000). The effects of family and commu-
Weary, & G. Sedek (Eds.), Personal control in action: Cognitive and nity violence on children. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 445-479,
motivational mechanisms (pp. 341-362). New York: Plenum. Mills, R.S.L. (1998). Paradoxical relations between perceived power
Bugental, D. B., Lewis, J. C., Lin, E., Lyon, J., & Kopeikin, H. (2000). In and maternal control. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 30, 132-136.
charge but not in control: The management of authority-based rela- Murphy, S., Orkow, B., & Nikola, R. M. (1985). Prenatal prediction of
tionships by those with low perceived power. Developmental Psychol- child abuse and neglect: A prospective study. Child Abuse and
ogy, 35, 1367-1378. Neglect, 9, 225-235.
Bugental, D. B., Lin, E. K., & Susskind, J. E. (1995). Influences of affect Nix, R. L., Pinderhughes, E. E., Dodge, K. A., & Bates, J. E. (1999). The
on cognitive processes at different ages: Why the change? In N. relation between mothers’ hostile attribution tendencies and chil-
Eisenberg (Ed.), Social development (Review of Personality and Social dren’s externalizing behavior problems: The mediating role of moth-
Psychology, Vol. 15, pp. 159-184). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. ers’ harsh discipline practices. Child Development, 70, 896-909.
Bugental, D. B., & Martorell, G. (1999). Competition between friends: Putallaz, M., Costanzo, P. R., Grimes, C. L., & Sherman, M. (1998).
The joint influence of self, friends, and parents. Journal of Family Psy- Intergenerational continuities and their influences on children’s
chology, 13, 1-14. development. Social Development, 7, 389-427.
Bugental, D. B., & Shennum, W. A. (1984). “Difficult” children as Raven, B. H., & Kruglanski, A. W. (1970). Conflict and power. In P.
elicitors and targets of adult communication patterns: An Swingle (Ed.), The structure of conflict (pp. 69-110). San Diego, CA:
attributional-behavioral transactional analysis. Monographs of the Academic Press.
Society for Research in Child Development, 39(No. 1). Ruble, D. N., & Martin, C. L. (1998). Gender development. In N.
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (Eds.). (1997). Developmental perspectives on Eisenberg (Volume Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social,
trauma: Theory, research, and intervention. Rochester, NY: University emotional, and personality development (pp. 933-1016). New York:
of Rochester Press. John Wiley.
Coie, J. D., & Dodge, K. A. (1998). Aggression and antisocial behavior. Steinberg, M. S., & Dodge, K. A. (1983). Attributional bias in aggressive
In N. Eisenberg (Volume Ed). Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. adolescent boys and girls. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 1,
Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 779-862). New 312-321.
York: John Wiley. Straus, M. (1979). Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence: The
Conflict Tactics Scale. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 41, 75-88.

CHILD MALTREATMENT / FEBRUARY 2002


Bugental, Shennum / GENDER AND VIOLENCE 63

Straus, M. A., & Gelles, R. J. (1990). Physical violence in American fami- of social and developmental psychology, and she has contributed chap-
lies: Risk factors and adaptation to violence in 8,145 families. New ters to a number of books.
Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.
Straus, M., Hamby, S. L., Finkelhor, D., & Moore, D. W. (1998). Identi-
fication of child maltreatment with the Parent-Child Conflict Tac- William Shennum is the director of research at Five Acres, a non-
tics Scales: Development and psychometric data for a national sam- profit child and family services agency serving the greater Los Angeles
ple of American parents. Child Abuse and Neglect, 22, 249-270. area. He is responsible for evaluation research in the agency’s multiple
programs, which include family-based services, foster care, and group
Daphne Blunt Bugental is professor of developmental and social residential treatment. He earned his Ph.D. in psychology at the Uni-
psychology with the Department of Psychology and codirector of the versity of California, Santa Barbara. Within the field of child mal-
Interdisciplinary Program in Human Development at the University treatment, his interests include intervention research, outcome mea-
of California, Santa Barbara. Her research is concerned with the surement, and analysis of individual differences in outcome. He
influence of cognitive, affective, and neurohormonal variables within chairs research and evaluation committees for the California Alliance
conflictual family systems—with a particular focus on violence and of Child and Family Services and the Inter-University Research Con-
violence prevention. Her work is published in journals within the fields sortium for Family Preservation.

CHILD MALTREATMENT / FEBRUARY 2002

You might also like