Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders (2020) 50:4230–4242

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-020-04488-2

ORIGINAL PAPER

Wisconsin Card Sorting Test‑Learning Potential: Usefulness


for Assessing Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder
M. Mar Gómez‑Pérez2 · Sara Mata1,3 · Francisca Serrano1,2 · M. Dolores Calero1

Published online: 3 April 2020


© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2020

Abstract
This study analyzes the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test-Learning Potential (WCST-LP) in children with Autism Spectrum
Disorder (ASD) versus children with typical development (TD). Its main aim was to assess: the test’s construct validity; the
effect of IQ on its pretest and LP scores; and whether the WCST-LP held any relationship to cognitive/EF and social abili-
ties. Participants were 105 children (43 with ASD/62 with TD). Results showed evidence of construct validity in an ASD
population (improvements from pretest to posttest), that full IQ influenced pretest performance but did not affect LP, and
that a relationship between LP and verbal and social abilities existed only in children with ASD. Conclusions indicate the
appropriateness of the WCST-LP in ASD prognosis assessment.

Keywords  Autism spectrum disorders · Wisconsin card sorting test · Response training · Learning potential · Prognosis

Introduction EFs are important for adequate psychological adjustment


and proper cognitive, social and psychological develop-
Executive functions (EFs) refer to a number of higher-order, ment. Specifically, according to some authors, EF is directly
interrelated neurocognitive processes that are involved in the related to social competence in childhood and adolescence
organization of action and thought. They are considered nec- (Diamond 2013; Landry et al. 2009; Maddio and Greco
essary to deal with new, complex situations involving objec- 2010); therefore, deficits in EFs hinder people’s adaptation,
tives or goals for which we have no established, automated affecting their problem-solving processes, decision-making,
behavior (Blijd-Hoogewys et al. 2014; Diamond 2013; Hill inhibition of inappropriate behavior that may offend others,
2004a), and for successful achievement in changing environ- emotional intelligence, and the generalization of learning
ments (Blijd-Hoogewys et al. 2014; Diamond 2013). Several (Diamond 2013; Montgomery et al. 2013).
processes are considered as part of EFs (such as cognitive Many psychological disorders have associated deficits in
flexibility, inhibitory control, working memory, planning, EFs, as is the case in Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). The
initiation of behavior, and self-regulation), the main being DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association 2013) classifies
working memory, cognitive flexibility, and inhibitory control ASD among neurodevelopmental disorders, as characterized
(Hill 2004a; Robinson et al. 2009). by persistent deficits in communication and social interac-
tion skills, non-verbal communication, social participation,
and restricted, repetitive patterns of behaviour, interests,
or activities. The ASD diagnosis involves specifications
* Sara Mata about the presence or absence of either intellectual deficit
saramata@ugr.es
or severe language deficit (pragmatic language impairment,
1
Centro de Investigación Mente, Cerebro y Comportamiento expressive-receptive language disorder) or both, because
(CIMCYC), Granada, Spain these deficits can be an index of symptom severity, accord-
2
Departamento de Psicología Evolutiva y de la Educación, ing to the DSM-5.
Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Granada, Granada, Research approaches have differed in their consideration
Spain of the specific ASD deficits (De Vries et al. 2015; Mont-
3
Departamento de Personalidad, Evaluación y Tratamiento gomery et al. 2013). For example, the Executive Functions
Psicológico, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Granada, Theory proposes that a general cognitive disturbance in
Campus Cartuja S/N, 18071 Granada, Spain

13
Vol:.(1234567890)
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders (2020) 50:4230–4242 4231

EFs—cognitive flexibility, inhibitory control, self-regu- study the utility of the WCST in an ASD population. In
lation, planning and working memory—is responsible for general, they analyze which WCST scores were used most
problems in interaction, social adaptation, and everyday often in the studies reviewed, and which presented greater
behavioral characteristics in ASD (Blijd-Hoogewys et al. discriminant validity between persons with ASD and per-
2014; Happé et al. 2006; Hill 2004a, b; Leung et al. 2015; sons with TD. The results from these studies show that the
Pugliese et al. 2016). perseverations score (cognitive flexibility) followed by the
There is some controversy, however, as to which EFs are number of complete categories (global indicator of concep-
damaged and if there are concrete factors that either mediate tual problem-solving skills) were the WCST scores most
or cause the difficulties, such as age, IQ, task’s characteris- often used in the research on EFs, in children, youth and
tics or ASD symptomatology (Hill 2004b; Van Eylen et al. adults with ASD, and were the scores where persons with
2015). Currently, heterogeneous findings have signalled dif- ASD showed the greatest difficulties.
ferent affected EFs: some research points out deficits in some Adapted from the classical WCST, the Wisconsin Card
of the main EFs but not in the others (Brunsdon et al. 2015; Sorting Test-Learning Potential (WCST-LP) was devel-
Robinson et al. 2009), and some research has even found oped by Wiedl and Wienöbst (1999) and uses 64 cards in a
similar results when testing EFs in populations with typical pretest-training-posttest design. The main goal is to test the
development (TD) and populations with ASD (De Vries and individual’s ability to personally benefit from a short inter-
Geurts 2012). The variations seem to be due to the different vention with regard to the tasks included in the assessment
assessment methods used—open vs. structured tasks, self- procedure; in other words, it tests the individual’s capacity
reports vs. third-party assessment—and differences in the for learning, or Learning Potential (LP). The WCST-LP is
definition of EFs (Gómez-Pérez et al. 2016; Robinson et al. based on an assessment methodology known as LP Assess-
2009; Van Eylen et al. 2015). ment or as Dynamic Assessment methodology. LP meth-
Nevertheless, many research studies have demonstrated odological approaches emerged in the second half of the
executive dysfunction in children with ASD, namely, in cog- twentieth century as a complement to the information pro-
nitive flexibility (Mostert-Kerckhoffs et al. 2015; Pooragha vided by conventional intelligence tests, especially in indi-
et al. 2013), inhibitory control (Montgomery et al. 2013; viduals with intellectual disability. LP assessment follows
Pooragha et al. 2013) and planning (Geurts et al. 2004; a pretest-training-posttest format, where a training phase is
Brunsdon et al. 2015). Such studies have shown that diffi- included in the middle of two standard tests (also known as
culties appear more consistently in cognitive flexibility and mediation). While conventional assessment (intelligence and
inhibition tasks, such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test aptitude tests) emphasizes the individual’s final task per-
(WCST), Planning Drawing Task and Trail Making Test formance as a product, LP assessment focuses on cognitive
(Brunsdon et al. 2015; Van Eylen et al. 2015). and metacognitive processes involved in learning, and the
Several neuropsychological tools for assessing EFs in change involved in reaching the solution to the task, in other
children with ASD are available for the purpose of study- words, cognitive modifiability. Consequently, the informa-
ing how EFs are altered in these children. Among them, tion provided by traditional assessment and LP assessment is
the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) (Heaton 1981) different in nature, the former is static (measuring the result
is most often used for assessing EFs in the framework of of knowledge/skills that have already been acquired) and the
ASD, and is known as “the gold standard EF task” (Ozonoff second is dynamic (measuring the capacity for change or
et al. 2005; p. 532). This test assesses EFs such as strategic learning) (Calero 2012; Haywood and Lidz 2007).
planning, organized searching, and environmental feedback The theoretical basis of LP methodology rests on the
to shift cognitive sets. Moreover, it is one of the tests most work of Vygostky (1978) and the idea of Zone of Proximal
often used for testing EFs in school age, especially from Development (ZPD) and Feuerstein et al. (1979) and the
age six to eight (Romine et al. 2004). The WCST is a clas- Theory of Structural Cognitive Modifiability and the con-
sification task based on set-shifting skills. It consists of 128 cept of “mediated learning experiences”. Both theoretical
cards that have to be sorted according to a certain matching approaches maintain that certain populations with difficul-
criterion (color, shape, and number), based on the experi- ties cannot manifest their actual abilities in the context of
menter’s feedback. The matching criterion changes after 10 traditional assessment. LP methodology has been broadly
consecutive correct matches, but without explicit instruc- validated in populations with severe conditions, like schiz-
tion to the participant. Researchers and practitioners have ophrenia, dementia, learning disabilities, and Down syn-
extensively used the WCST in the ASD population. Since drome, showing that it is possible to design and/or adapt
the year 2000, two review articles and four meta-analyses any intervention as a function of the specific learning strate-
about WCST in ASD can be found (Hill 2004a, b; Landry gies of persons, by considering the LP index (for a review,
and Al-Taie 2016; Leung and Zakzanis 2014; Romine et al. see Calero 2012; Haywood and Lidz 2007). Nevertheless,
2004; Westwood et al. 2016). These reviews/meta-analyses only five studies to date have addressed the LP assessment

13

4232 Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders (2020) 50:4230–4242

methodology in the ASD population, and only one of them These results offered evidence of the construct validity of the
focuses on WCST-LP testing (Aljunied and Frederickson WCST-LP in an ASD population, because participants made
2011a, b; Bonete et al. 2010; Calero et al. 2015; Donaldson significant pretest-to-posttest improvements (the WCST-LP
and Olswang 2007). uses EF measures to evaluate learning capacity as a main
Donaldson and Olswang (2007) investigated the capac- construct). The measure of LP in correct answers signifi-
ity of preschoolers with ASD to make requests (request cantly correlated with emotion recognition measures and
information from an adult, request an action or ask for an interpersonal conflict solution skills.
object), using two conditions of structured play. The first All the previous studies have tried to determine the use-
condition assessed the ability to request information in a tra- fulness of LP methodology in children with ASD, as well as
ditional assessment situation (assessor is a nonparticipating the relationship of LP to relevant factors such as intelligence,
observer), compared to a second LP assessment condition, Theory of Mind and social abilities.
where the assessor manipulated conditions of the context, Intelligence may influence the WCST performance of
the choice of materials/activities and the linguistic vari- children with ASD, as occurs with other types of measures,
ables involved in the game. The results of this study showed but there have not been conclusive results to date. Thus,
that including the LP assessment along with the traditional Ozonoff (1995) found normative WCST performance in
assessment made it easier to identify ASD children who had children with ASD with an IQ over 70. For their part, Liss
greater difficulties in requesting information, and to identify et al. (2001) found no differences in WCST between children
the type of assistance that was most adequate for improving with ASD and children with specific language impairments,
their performance. On the other hand, Bonete et al. (2010) once verbal IQ was controlled. In fact, several studies have
used LP methodology to study the ability to learn how to shown that once verbal-IQ or full-IQ effect is controlled,
solve matrices, along with an assessment of interpersonal many of the differences between participants with and
skills, in 10 children with ASD and 10 children with TD without ASD also disappear, specifically, in the percent of
(ages 11 to 16). Results showed that the children with ASD perseverative errors, the number of perseverative errors and
presented similar LP to that of children with TD, and that LP the number of non-perseverative errors (Hill 2004b; Liss
did not correlate to deficits in interpersonal skills, leading et al. 2001; Rumsey 1985). These studies have concluded
these authors to conclude that social deficits were not related that flexibility deficits, as measured by WCST in children
to a limited capacity for learning. Aljunied and Frederick- with ASD, could be related with IQ; this measure should
son (2011a) investigated relationships between nonverbal therefore be controlled in this area of research. By contrast,
intelligence, central coherence, and LP—assessed with an some other studies like Filley et al. (1999) have pointed out
analogies task in a pretest-training-posttest format—in 52 that the diagnostic validity of the WCST may be independ-
children (ages 9 and 10) with ASD. They found that all chil- ent of intelligence in children with ASD. However, all these
dren showed LP (they showed gains from pretest to posttest, studies have analyzed scores from the traditional version of
after receiving a training phase), although these improve- the WCST, without having information about how IQ might
ments were associated with the children’s scores in nonver- influence LP scores from the WCST-LP.
bal intelligence and central coherence. In fact, even chil- Based on the foregoing, the main aim of this study was
dren with a non-verbal IQ lower than 85 and weak central to analyze the utility of a WCST adaptation designed for
coherence (field-independence) showed some improvement testing LP (WCST-LP) in EFs tasks, for comparing chil-
on the LP assessment test. In a second study, Aljunied and dren with ASD to children with typical development (TD).
Frederickson (2011b) investigated the diagnostic utility of It attempted to ascertain the relationship among test per-
intelligence measures (total IQ), LP measures and Theory of formance, intelligence, EF (switching) and social abilities.
Mind measures in 52 children with ASD from three different More specific goals were:
educational settings: conventional/unsupported, with mini-
mal educational support, with moderate support. The results • To obtain evidence that supports the construct validity
showed that 82.7% of participants were correctly discrimi- of WCST-LP in children with ASD, because the WCST-
nated and grouped according to their educational setting, LP uses EF measures to evaluate learning potential as a
by considering 3 factors: full IQ, performance on Theory main construct. Hypothesis 1 (H.1): Children with ASD
of Mind tasks, and LP score. Finally, Calero et al. (2015) will benefit from the training in the WCST-LP obtain-
was the only study to use the WCST-LP in an ASD popu- ing significantly higher results in the posttest than in the
lation (45 children from 7 to 13 years old). They showed pretest, thus demonstrating LP.
that children with ASD obtained significant gains from pre- • To determine the role of a static measure of intelligence
test to posttest in the WCST-LP measures of perseverative (full IQ) in the diagnostic validity of the pretest and gain
answers, perseverative errors and number of complete cat- scores from the WCST-LP, by observing its sensitiv-
egories; although the study identified different gain profiles. ity for discriminating between children with ASD and

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders (2020) 50:4230–4242 4233

children with TD, once full IQ has been controlled as a determined as exclusion criteria for both groups (ASD/TD).
covariable. Hypothesis 2.1 (H.2.1): Full IQ will have a None of the selected children showed any sensory or physi-
significant effect on level of performance in the WCST- cal disease.
LP pretest scores (performance scores similar to those For ethical reasons, the groups were selected from autism
obtained in a standard application of the WCST), for centers and schools; we respected the percentages of male
children with ASD and children with TD. Hypothesis and female students in each of the groups proposed by the
2.2 (H.2.2): Full IQ will not have a significant effect on professionals responsible for the students. Consequently,
WCST-LP gain scores (pretest-to-posttest improvement there are initial differences in the number of female partici-
after receiving training), for either children with ASD or pants in the group of children with ASD, compared to the
children with TD. group of children with TD.
• To explore the relationship between WCST-LP measures
(pretest and gain scores) and the indexes of IQ, EF and Instruments
social abilities in children with ASD vs. children with
TD. Hypotheses were not established; the purpose of the Measure of Intelligence
study was exploratory, due to lack of prior consistent
evidence. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV (WISC-IV;
Wechsler 2005). The scale contains 15 subtests, 10 core
subtests and 5 complementary subtests, which assess the
Method intellectual capacity of children from the ages 6 to 16. The
scale provides four composite scores: verbal comprehension
Participants index, perceptual reasoning index, working memory index
and processing speed index. The scale also provides a total
Participants were 105 children from ages 7 to 13  years IQ score that reflects the child’s general cognitive capacity.
(MAge = 9.88, SD = 1.60), divided into two groups: 43 chil- The assessment lasted approximately one hour and fifty min-
dren diagnosed with ASD (38 male and 5 female), without utes. Reliability data on the core indices fall between 0.86
associated intellectual disability or language impairment (or and 0.95, with an average stability coefficient of 0.85. Analy-
Asperger Syndrome when the diagnosis was prior to DSM- ses of content and internal structure validity have shown
5), MAge = 10.07 (SD = 1.65) and MIQ = 94.16 (SD = 14.80); satisfactory results, with a factorial structure corresponding
and 62 children (33 males and 29 females) with TD, to the 4 indices.
MAge = 9.74 (SD = 1.56) and MIQ = 91.56 (SD = 12.20).
The general inclusion criteria were age between 7 and Measure of EF and LP
13 years, and full IQ between 70 and 130, as tested by the
WISC-IV. For the ASD group, a diagnosis of ASD with- Wisconsin Card Sorting Test-Learning Potential (WCST-LP)
out accompanying intellectual or language impairment was (Wiedl et al. 2001). This reduced version of the WCST (64
established as a specific inclusion criterion for the ASD cards) is applied in three phases (pretest-training-posttest),
group (or Asperger syndrome if the diagnosis preceded the thus measuring LP with respect to the various EFs evalu-
DSM-5). This subgroup of children with ASD was selected ated, namely, cognitive flexibility, inhibition, self-regulation
because the training included in the WCST-LP consists and planning. The assessment starts with the standard static
of instruction, assistance and feedback provided through test (pretest), with the reduced version of the WCST (64
oral language. All children with ASD had a previous ASD cards). Participants must sort the cards according to a cer-
diagnosis provided by mental health professionals, based tain matching criterion (color, shape, number), based on the
on assessments with the Autism Diagnostic Observation experimenter’s feedback. The matching criterion changed
Schedule (ADOS; Lord et al. 2002) and the Autism Diag- after 10 consecutive correct matches, but without explicit
nostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R; Le Couteur et al. 2003), instruction to the participant. The WCST provides nine main
as well as an overall assessment of their skills. In addition, measurements: (1) correct answers (CA), that is, number
they were receiving treatment in autism centres or in chil- of right matches; (2) total errors (E), number of incorrect
dren’s mental health services at hospitals. The previous ASD matches; (3) perseverative answers (PA), repeated responses
diagnosis was verified through available clinical reports for using the wrong matching-criterion—they could be correct
all children, along with any additional comorbid disorders and incorrect answers; (4) perseverative errors (PE), incor-
(ADHD, behaviour problems, sensory problems) or use of rect responses following the perseveration principle; (5) non-
medication. Based on these reports submitted by the profes- perseverative errors (NPE), incorrect responses not follow-
sionals who made the diagnosis, the presence of additional ing the perseveration principle; (6) conceptual level answers
comorbid psychological disorders or use of medication were (CLA), answers showing comprehension of the matching

13

4234 Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders (2020) 50:4230–4242

criterion—these are counted as groups of 3 or more correct must name the color of the typeface, ignoring any conflict
responses; (7) number of complete categories (NCC), as the with the word meaning, which may be a different color. An
number of complete sequences (10 correct matches) follow- interference index is also obtained (Stroop-I), which assesses
ing the right matching criteria; (8) tries to complete the first interference in the participant and his/her attention control.
category (TC1stC), the number of tries needed to complete Test–retest reliability is 0.89 for Stroop-W, 0.84 for Stroop-
one category for the first time; (9) failure to maintain accu- C, 0.73 for Stroop-CW and 0.70 for Stroop-I (Golden 2006).
racy (FMA), that is, 5 consecutive correct responses fol-
lowed by one error. Measures of Interpersonal Conflict Resolution Skill
Several studies have pointed out that the different meas-
urements are associated with different EFs. That is, CA and Social Comprehension Index–Factor proposed by Golstein
E test task-related functions; PA and PE will relate to cogni- et al. (2008) for high-functioning ASD children and adults,
tive flexibility—the best predictor of prefrontal dysfunction; corresponding to social cognition in these populations. It is
CLA relates to abstract thinking; NCC constitutes a global composed of the WISC-IV supplemental subtests of Picture
indicator of conceptual problem-solving skills; TC1stC Completion and Word Reasoning.
would serve as an indicator of initial conceptualization Ekman 60 Faces test (Young et  al. 2002). This is a
prior to criterion-change; finally, FMA would test attention computerized task assessing emotion recognition in facial
(Leung and Zakzanis 2014; Tsuchiya et al. 2005). expressions, as a key component of interpersonal conflict
After the pretest (standard WCST procedure with 64 solving skill. The computer screen shows photographs of
cards), a training phase followed. The experimenter told faces expressing the six basic emotions (anger, disgust, fear,
the participant that he/she would redo the task with help. happiness, sadness, and surprise). The images (first pub-
Before starting the second block of cards, the experimenter lished by Ekman and Friesen 1976) are presented at random
explained to the participant that there were three card-match- for 5 s each, and the participant is required to indicate his/
ing-rules (according to color, shape, and number, respec- her impression of the emotion by pressing the corresponding
tively). Each time that the participant placed a card, he/she key on the keyboard. During the task, a small sign placed
was told if his/her answer was right or wrong. After 10 con- over the keyboard indicates what key should be pressed
secutive correct responses, the experimenter informed the for each of the emotions. Separate reliability values have
participant about the change in matching criteria. Finally, been obtained for the emotion types (anger: 0.68; disgust:
once the training was done, the posttest assessment was 0.92; fear: 0.88; sadness: 0.65; and surprise: 0.33) with
administered without any help. The pretest assessment pro- adequate results (p < 0.001); the value for happiness was
vided the same nine measurements as the traditional version. the only exception (0.18), where scores showed a ceiling
However, the WCST-LP added the possibility to calculate effect (Young et al. 2002). Regarding validity of this instru-
gains and LP after the posttest assessment. Gain scores could ment (Young et al. 2002) compared to the original (Ekman
be obtained for any of the traditional measurements using and Friesen 1976), a strong correlation was found with the
the formula posttest-pretest = simple gain score. The WCST- recognition rate presented by Ekman and Friesen (r = 0.81,
LP has shown evidence of construct validity in patients with t = 10.35, p < 0.001). In Spain, this test has been adapted and
schizophrenia, showing gains from the pretest to the posttest, standardized, with similar results to those of the original
that is, after the intervention; there is also evidence of con- sample; Cronbach alpha for the entire sample is 0.93 (hap-
current validity with the Stroop test and the Auditory Verbal piness: 0.82; anger: 0.69; disgust: 0.73; fear: 0.72; sadness:
Learning Test (Heubrock 1992); and, finally, evidence of 0.68; and surprise: 0.73) (see Molinero et al. 2015).
predictive validity regarding the effectiveness of intervention Assessment of Interpersonal Conflict Solving [Evaluación
programs for job skills and cognitive recovery in patients de Solución de Conflictos Interpersonales (ESCI) (García-
with schizophrenia (Wiedl et al. 1999,2004). Martín and Calero 2018)]. The test assesses interpersonal
conflict solving skills, namely, emotion recognition in other
Measure of EF (Switching) people, skill in identifying situational agreement during a
conflict, and skills for generating a viable solution which
Stroop: Color and Word test (Golden 2006). This test is a will benefit everyone. The test consists of 17 sequences of
measure of switching ability and involves inhibitory con- sketches that represent an interpersonal conflict, shown on a
trol, assessing, and attentional control. It is applicable in computer monitor. The first four sketches show a single per-
ages 7 to 80, and consists of three tasks with a duration of son, while the remaining sketches show two or more charac-
45 s each. The tasks are word reading (Stroop-W), in which ters in a conflict situation. The participant is required to give
the participant must read the written names of colors; color written answers to the following questions: (1) How does
naming (Stroop-C), in which he/she must name the color of the main character in the drawing feel? (2) Why does he/
the typeface; and color-word (Stroop-CW), in which he/she she feel this way? (3) What could he/she do to remedy this

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders (2020) 50:4230–4242 4235

situation? The assessment provides a total score (ESCI-T) of normality and homogeneity using Kolmogorov–Smirnov
and a score for each construct: emotion (ESCI-E), situational and Levene statistics.
agreement (ESCI-SA) and solutions (ESCI-S). The instru-
ment was constructed according to the 7-phase model of • To test H.1, a repeated-measures ANOVA was carried
interpersonal problem solving by Pelechano (1996), and has out with the Greenhouse–Geisser correction.
been validated in Spain (García-Martín and Calero 2018). • To test H.2.1 and H.2.2, multivariate analyses of covari-
The Cronbach index obtained was α = 0.90 for the whole ance (MANCOVA) were used, including full IQ as a
test, with indexes of α = 0.69 for emotion, α = 0.91 for situ- covariate to control its possible effect on WCST-LP pre-
ational agreement, and α = 0.81 for solutions. Exploratory test and gain (LP) scores.
factor validity analysis revealed three main factors (emotion, • To test objective 3, the Pearson correlation coefficient
situational agreement, solutions) and confirmatory factor was used.
validity analysis revealed the same three-factor structure,
as well as a single second-order factor named interpersonal
skills. The test also showed evidence of concurrent validity, Results
the emotion factor correlating significantly with the Ekman
60 Faces test, the situational agreement factor with Figure Preliminary analysis included analyses of variance
Completion and Picture Arrangement from the Wechsler (ANOVA), showing that the two groups were matched by
Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III; Wechsler 1997), and age F(1,104) = 1.07, p = 0.303 and full IQ F(1,104) = 0.97,
the solutions factor with the rational solution sub-scale from p = 0.328, but not by sex χ2 = 14.32, p = 0.0001.
the Social Problem-Solving Inventory-Revised (D’Zurilla The main objective of this study was to analyze the utility
et al. 2002) (see García-Martín and Calero 2018). of the set of LP measurements provided by the WCST-LP in
children with ASD, aiming to provide a source of evidence
of both construct and diagnostic validity (controlling for full
Procedure
IQ), and also to test the relationships between WCST-LP test
performance, cognitive measures (WISC-IV indexes), EF
Firstly, permission from the Ethics Committee of Univer-
(switching), and social abilities.
sity of Granada was obtained. Secondly, participants were
Regarding the first specific objective, that is, to obtain
selected by requesting voluntary participation from ASD
evidence that supports the construct validity of the WCST-
Associations in several Spanish cities. Neither material nor
LP for testing LP in children with ASD, hypothesis (H.1)
money incentives were given for participation. After agree-
stated that children with ASD would benefit from the train-
ing to participate, the psychologists of the associations were
ing that forms part of the WCST-LP, obtaining significantly
asked to pre-select the participants, following the established
higher posttest results than pretest results, thus demon-
inclusion/exclusion criteria. Thirdly, once the participants
strating LP (see Table 1). In this respect, results from the
with ASD were identified, a group of children with TD,
repeated measures ANOVA showed significant improve-
matched to the ASD group in age and IQ, was selected from
ments from pretest to posttest in all WCST-LP measures.
different schools. Finally, information about the research
Specifically, CA (F(1,42) = 89.40, p = 0.0001, η2partial = 0.68,
goals, procedures and implications was offered in group
s.p. = 1.00), PA (F(1,42) = 23.69, p = 0.0001, η2partial = 0.36,
meetings with the parents. These meetings were organized
s.p. = 0.99), PE (F(1,42) = 29.45, p = 0.0001, η2partial = 0.41,
at the ASD centres and the schools. Informed parental con-
s.p. = 1.00), NPE (F(1,42) = 12.56, p = 0.001, η2partial = 0.23,
sent was obtained for 107 children, but two of them were
s.p. = 0.93), CLA (F(1,42) = 71.84, p = 0.0001,
excluded because they had additional comorbid disorders
η 2 partial  = 0.63, s.p. = 1.00), NCC (F(1,42) = 46.50,
(ADHD). None of the final 105 participants withdrew from
p = 0.0001, η 2 partial  = 0.53, s.p. = 1.00), TC1stC
the study.
(F(1,42) = 11.08, p = 0.002, η2partial = 0.21, s.p. = 0.91), and
Participants were individually assessed in three sessions
FMA (F(1,42) = 6.16, p = 0.017, η2partial = 0.13, s.p. = 0.68)
of approximately one hour each; testing with all the instru-
in the ASD group (see Table 1).
ments (Spanish versions) was counterbalanced, to avoid a
The second aim was to determine whether the full IQ
possible effect of testing order.
score significantly influenced the diagnostic validity of
the WCST-LP (pretest and gain scores) by comparing its
Design and Data Analysis sensitivity for discriminating between children with ASD
and children with TD, once full IQ had been controlled as
A retrospective ex post facto design with two groups was a covariable. Hypothesis (H.2.1) stated that full IQ would
used. First, we verified that the sample fulfilled assumptions have a significant effect on performance level in WCST-LP
pretest scores (performance scores similar to those obtained

13

4236

13
Table 1  Descriptive statistic, range, minimum and maximum of WCST-LP in ASD and TD groups and multivariate analyses del repeated measures ANOVA with one within-subjects factor-
assessment moment (from pre-treatment to post-treatment) in ASD group
Group Variable Test range Pre-test Post-test Gain score Multivariate analyses
M SD Min Max M SD Min Max M SD Min Max F(1,42) p η2p s.p

Group ASD (n = 43) WCST-LP


   CA 0–64 34.35 12.69 13.00 57.00 50.81 9.48 13.00 63.00 16.47 11.42 − 3.00 44.00 89.40 .0001 .68 1.00
   PA 0–63 18.79 16.62 2.00 62.00 7.93 8.02 2.00 52.00 − 10.86 14.63 − 56.00 13.00 23.69 .0001 .36 .99
   PE 0–63 15.71 12.36 2.00 46.00 6.83 6.03 1.00 39.00 − 8.88 10.73 − 40.00 8.00 29.45 .0001 .41 1.00
   NPE 0–64 12.33 12.64 .00 42.00 5.91 6.97 .00 42.00 − 6.43 11.89 − 38.00 12.00 12.56 .001 .23 .93
   CLA 0–61 28.81 16.32 .00 55.00 46.14 11.84 .00 60.00 19.33 14.95 − 4.00 56.00 71.84 .0001 .63 1.00
   NCC 0–6 1.81 1.47 .00 5.00 3.33 1.46 .00 6.00 1.51 1.45 − 2.00 5.00 46.50 .0001 .53 1.00
   TC1stC 10–64 26.88 23.02 10.00 65.00 16.12 13.13 10.00 65.00 − 10.77 21.21 − 55.00 24.00 11.08 .002 .21 .91
   FMA 0–10 0.56 0.73 .00 3.00 1.07 1.06 .00 3.00 0.51 1.35 − 2.00 3.00 6.16 .017 .13 .68
Group TD (n = 62) WCST-LP
   CA 0–64 31.77 9.90 13.00 52.00 49.82 8.15 22.00 61.00 18.05 12.64 − 14.00 43.00
   PA 0–63 17.13 13.28 4.00 62.00 6.53 4.28 1.00 24.00 − 10.60 13.41 − 59.00 7.00
   PE 0–63 14.77 9.61 4.00 46.00 6.19 3.70 1.00 19.00 − 8.58 9.91 − 43.00 6.00
   NPE 0–64 17.44 10.00 1.00 42.00 7.94 5.48 1.00 29.00 − 9.50 11.66 − 37.00 14.00
   CLA 0–61 23.47 12.35 0.00 47.00 45.73 10.43 10.00 59.00 22.26 15.54 − 19.00 50.00
   NCC 0–6 1.69 1.08 0.00 4.00 3.15 1.27 1.00 5.00 1.45 1.60 − 2.00 5.00
   TC1stC 10–64 25.69 21.01 10.00 65.00 15.84 10.80 10.00 53.00 − 9.85 21.94 − 55.00 33.00
   FMA 0–10 0.44 0.67 0.00 3.00 1.26 1.09 .00 4.00 0.82 1.33 − 2.00 4.00

WCST-LP Wisconsin Card Sorting Test-Learning Potential; CA Correct answers, PA Perseverative answers, PE perseverative errors, NPE Non-perseverative errors, CLA Conceptual level
answers, NCC Number of complete categories, TC1stC Tries to complete the first category; FMA Failure to maintain the attitude
Inverse variable in bold
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders (2020) 50:4230–4242
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders (2020) 50:4230–4242 4237

on a standard application of the WCST), for both ASD and the full IQ effect was controlled (see Table 2). Regarding
TD children. Accordingly, the results of the MANCOVA did the covariable, results indicate that full IQ did not have a
not show significant between-group differences in WCST-LP significant effect on results obtained in gain scores, except
pretest scores Ʌ = 0.87, F(8,95) = 1.73, p = 0.101, η2 = 0.13, for the TC1stC measure (see Table 2), thereby confirming
s.p. = 0.72 when controlling for full IQ. Univariate analy- the hypothesis for the most part. For the TC1stC measure,
ses only showed significant differences between the two b-value results indicate that intelligence had a positive direc-
groups in the NPE pretest score (see Table 2), favoring the tion (b = 0.368).
ASD group (see Table 2). Results also indicated a signifi- The third aim was to explore the relations between
cant effect of full IQ on pretest scores in CA, PA, PE, CLA, WCST-LP measurements (pretest and gain scores) and the
TC1stC, FMA (see Table 2) confirming our hypothesis. indexes of IQ, EF and social abilities in children with ASD
Regarding the direction of full IQ, b-value results indicate vs. children with TD. In this case, results for the ASD group
a positive relationship in CA (b = 0.210), CLA (b = 0.265) showed statistically significant, there were positive Pearson
and FMA (b = 0.012); and a negative relationship in meas- correlations between the WCST-LP measures in CA, CLA,
urements of PA (b = − 0.296), PE (b = − 0.222) and TC1stC NCC and FMA, with VC, PR, WM, PS, SC, Stroop PC
(b = − 0.330) for the pretest. and ESCI-T; and negative correlations between WCST-LP
The second hypothesis of this objective (H.2.2) con- measures of PA, PE and TC1stC, with VC, PR, WM, PS,
sidered that full IQ would not have a significant effect on SC, Stroop PC and ESCI-T (see Table 3). Gain measures
WCST-LP gain scores (pretest-to-posttest improvement (estimated LP), however, showed a lower number of signifi-
after receiving training), for either children with ASD or cant correlations, limited to a few WISC-IV indexes: VC
children with TD. MANCOVA results did not show sig- index correlated significantly with gain scores in PA, PE and
nificant between-group differences in the gain scores when TC1stC; WM index correlated with gain scores in NCC and

Table 2  Mean difference (MANCOVA-Full-IQ covariate) between Group ASD and TD in the WCST-L’s measures in pretest assessment and
gain score as a function of Group (ASD and TD)
Variable Group TD Group ASD Mean difference Covariate
(n = 62) (n = 43)
M* SD M* SD F(1.103) p η2partial s.p F(1.103) p η2partial s.p

WCST-LP pretest
 CA 32.00 9.90 34.03 12.69 0.89 .349 .01 .15 6.88 .010** .06 .74
 PA 16.81 13.28 19.24 16.62 0.73 .395 .01 .14 7.89 .006** .07 .80
 PE 14.53 9.61 16.02 12.36 0.54 .464 .01 .11 8.61 .004** .08 .83
 NPE 17.46 10.00 12.29 12.65 5.35 .023* .05 .63 0.10 .757 .001 .06
 CLA 23.75 12.35 26.41 16.32 0.94 .334 .01 .16 6.81 .010** .06 .73
 NCC 1.71 1.08 1.79 1.47 0.10 .750 .001 .06 3.01 .086 .03 .41
 TC1stC 25.34 21.01 27.39 23.02 0.23 .634 .002 .08 4.31 .040* .04 .54
 FMA 0.45 0.67 0.54 0.73 0.45 .505 .004 .10 6.05 .016* .06 .68
WCST-LP gain score
 CA 17.95 12.64 16.61 11.42 0.30 .583 .003 .09 1.13 .291 .01 .18
 PA − 10.41 13.42 − 11.12 14.63 0.07 .798 .001 .06 2.83 .096 .03 .38
 PE − 8.44 9.91 − 9.09 10.73 0.10 .749 .001 .06 3.23 .075 .03 .43
 NPE − 9.53 11.66 − 6.38 11.90 1.80 .183 .017 .26 0.14 .714 .001 .07
 CLA 22.15 15.54 19.48 14.95 0.76 .385 .01 .14 0.83 .363 .008 .15
 NCC 1.44 1.60 1.52 1.45 0.07 .798 .001 .06 0.41 .521 .004 .10
 TC1stC − 9.46 21.94 − 11.33 21.21 0.20 .659 .002 .07 5.51 .021* .05 .64
 FMA 0.82 1.33 0.51 1.35 1.38 .243 .01 .21 0.02 .888 .001 .05

WCST-LP Wisconsin card sorting test-learning potential, CA Correct answers, PA Perseverative answers, PE Perseverative errors, NPE Non-
perseverative errors, CLA Conceptual level answers, NCC Number of complete categories, TC1stC Tries to complete the first category, FMA
Failure to maintain the attitude
Inverse variable in bold
M*: Means are estimated as a function of a full IQ covariate value = 92.6286
p < .05*. p < .01**

13

4238

13
Table 3  Bivariated correlations in pretest and gain scores WCST-LP’s measures with indexes of WISC-IV, Stroop-PC, Stroop-I, Ekman total and ESCI-Total (EF measures and social compe-
tency) as a function of Group (ASD and TD)
WCST-LP Group ASD (n = 43) Group TD (n = 62)
VC PR WM PS Stroop-PC Stroo-I* SC Ekman ESCI-T VC RP WM PS Stroop-PC Stroop-I* SC Ekman ESCI-T

Pretest
 CA .42** .31* .37* .48** .32* .0001 .48** .14 .31* − .035 .71 − .03 .08 .07 − .02 .08 .22 .16
 PA − .47** − .33* − .28 − .35* − .19 − .04 − .54** .10 − .13 .004 − .12 − .07 − .03 .05 − .03 − .05 − .09 − .10
 PE − .46** − .34* − .28 − .36* − .22 − .06 − .54** .07 − .13 .001 − .11 − .10 − .08 .02 − .04 − .06 − .10 − .12
 NPE .03 .01 − .03 − .17 − .01 .11 .09 − .29 − .31* − .03 .04 .12 − .01 − .09 .05 − .02 − .13 − .05
 CLA .43** .30 .39** .48** .30* − .02 .49** .18 .35* − .02 .03 − .04 .04 .07 − .01 .01 .15 .07
 NCC .37* .21 .34* .32* .18 − .11 .37* .22 .26 − .10 .01 − .15 .04 .05 − .02 .08 .06 .03
 TC1stC − .46** − .26 − .38* − .38* − .14 .27 − .56** − .21 − .34* .11 − .09 .10 − .04 .10 .13 − .05 .02 − .05
 FMA .32* .19 .26 .40** .34* .16 .42* − .06 .20 .12 .13 .09 − .002 .05 .15 − .02 .13 − .01
Gain score
 CA − .21 − .18 − .21 − .29 − .24 .02 .43** − .21 .05 .04 − .002 .09 − .17 − .19 − .10 − .002 − .23 − .11
 PA .30* .19 .15 .27 .14 .09 .43** − .19 − .16 − .01 .08 .05 .06 − .01 .08 .05 .11 .08
 PE .31* .20 .16 .29 .19 .12 − .19 .05 − .12 − .01 .06 .06 .11 .03 .09 .05 .12 .09
 NPE .07 .01 .05 .06 .03 − .14 − .21 .17 .14 − .03 − .05 − .14 .08 .19 .03 − .04 .15 .04
 CLA .17 − .16 − .20 . − 27 − .19 .04 − .13 .23 − .03 .02 .05 .06 − .12 − .20 − .11 .07 − .17 − .04
 NCC − .21 − .08 − .31* .01 − .09 .06 .47** .06 .02 .08 − .01 .18 − .18 − .21 − .13 − .01 .01 .14
 TC1stC .51** .28 .48** .35* .24 − .19 − .13 − .01 .28 − .09 .12 − .13 .16 − .01 − .11 .07 .01 .02
 FMA .01 − .06 .05 − .29 − .17 .02 .48** .14 − .01 − .02 .14 − .01 .13 .002 .01 .07 − .17 − .17

WCST-LP Wisconsin card sorting test-learning potential, CA Correct answers, PA Perseverative answers, PE Perseverative errors, NPE Non-perseverative errors, CLA Conceptual level answers,
NCC Number of complete categories, TC1stC Tries to complete the first category, FMA Failure to maintain the attitude
VC: Verbal Comprehension index WISC-IV; PR: Perceptive Reasoning index WISC-IV; WM: Working Memory index WISC-IV; PS: Processing Speed index WISC-IV; Stroop-PC: Stroop Test
word-color measure; Stroop-I: Stroop Test interference index; SC: Social Comprehension index WISC-IV; Ekman: Total score Ekman test; ESCI-T: Evaluación de Solución de Conflictos Inter-
personales [Assessment of Interpersonal Conflict Resolution] Total score
Inverse variable in bold
p < .05*. p < .01**
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders (2020) 50:4230–4242
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders (2020) 50:4230–4242 4239

TC1stC; PS index correlated with gain scores in TC1stC; the WCST-LP in general, and with an ASD population spe-
and SC index correlated with gain scores in CA, PA, NCC cifically. These results add supplemental evidence to other
and FMA. All correlations were in a positive direction (see previous studies of LP in patients with schizophrenia, which
Table 3). In the TD group, there were no significant corre- have traditionally reduced LP measures to Correct responses
lations between WCST-LP pretest and gain scores with the or Perseverative answers (Bonete 2012).
other measures (see Table 3). The second goal was to determine the influence of intelli-
gence in the diagnostic validity of the WCST-LP, by observ-
ing whether its pretest and gain scores were sensitive for
discriminating between children with ASD and children
Discussion with TD. The study has shown that when intelligence is
controlled, children with ASD do not differ from children
The main objective of this study was to analyze the use- with TD, except in the NPE measure. These results high-
fulness of the WCST-LP set of measurements in children light the influence of intelligence on performance in the
with ASD. We sought to find some source of evidence of pretest level of the different WCST-LP scores. This result
both construct validity (improved scores after a training was to be expected, given that the data are showing the
phase) and diagnostic validity (discriminating between influence of a static cognition measure (full IQ) on other
children with ASD and children with TD in pretest and gain measures that are also static (WCST-LP pretest scores); in
scores, when controlling for full IQ), and to test the relation- other words, measures that assess performance ability and
ships between WCST-LP performance, cognitive measures demonstrate acquired skills at the moment of an EF task
(WISC-IV indexes), EF (switching), and social abilities. assessment (Calero 2012; Haywood and Lidz 2007). In this
The findings represent a step forward in the field of regard, these findings agree with those of Rumsey (1985),
neuropsychological assessment, providing evidence of the Ozonoff (1995), and Liss et al. (2001), indicating that level
validity of a WCST adaptation that measures capacity for of intelligence plays a role in one’s performance on tradi-
learning in EFs-related tasks in the ASD population. Until tional versions of the WCST, and that it should be controlled
now, only one published study had reported data showing the in diagnostic assessments of children with possible ASD.
possible diagnostic and prognostic utility of these measures However, the present study represents an advance in the
(Calero et al. 2015). Now, we have taken a further step by existing knowledge, by showing how dynamic measures of
showing the differential influence of intelligence on pretest LP assessment in EF tasks are independent of one’s former
and gain scores, and by exploring the relations between the IQ (Calero 2012; Haywood and Lidz 2007). In this way,
measures supplied by this version of the WCST, and impor- this version of the WCST is shown to contribute qualita-
tant performance variables of children with ASD, such as tively different information from the traditional version of
their verbal and social comprehension skills. The funda- the test, and allows us to assess cognitive modifiability, or
mental importance of these skills is that they inform us of the capacity for change (learning) in significant tasks for the
the individual’s competence for daily life and for relating ASD population. The fact that LP scores are not influenced
to his or her environment. This study can thus contribute to by intelligence leads us to interpret them as a prognostic
the development of neuropsychological measures of LP in indicator that can identify the capacity for learning in ASD
children with ASD, having shown its relationship to verbal children prior to planning their intervention, regardless of
and social skills. previous factors like IQ.
Overall, the findings support the idea that children with The third exploratory objective allowed us to determine
ASD can benefit from brief training included in a classic EF that pretest measures of the WCST-LP were related to intel-
assessment task (WCST-LP); thus, they demonstrate LP, like ligence, flexibility and social abilities in children with ASD
children with TD. The test therefore offers some evidence (positively, in that more right answers accompanied higher
of construct validity in an ASD population. These results IQ; and negatively, in that more perseverations accompanied
agree with the few previous studies that investigate LP lower IQ); this relationship was not found in the group of
and children with ASD (Aljunied and Frederickson 2011a, children with TD. However, the most significant gains in
2011b; Bonete et al. 2010; Calero et al. 2015; Donaldson WCST-LP scores (CA, PE) correlated to performance on
and Olswang 2007) and with previous research on LP in measures of verbal comprehension and social skills only in
populations with severe developmental conditions (Calero the children with ASD, while there were no correlations in
2012; Haywood and Lidz 2007). Furthermore, this study the TD children. This may indicate their prognostic value
shows how LP extends to all types of WCST-LP responses, for these competencies, beyond that of intelligence and/or
and hence, to the different EF-related domains assessed by performance measures. These children’s LP is related to
these (cognitive flexibility, inhibition, self-regulation, plan- their verbal and social competencies, not to their IQ. Simi-
ning), extending the available information regarding use of lar findings had been found by Wiedl and Wienöbst (1999)

13

4240 Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders (2020) 50:4230–4242

in their study of adults with a schizophrenia diagnosis, by in individualized intervention designs; it informs about the
Calero and Navarro (2007) in older adults with dementia, individual’s ability to profit from training, which may be a
and by Calero et al. (2015), where significant relationships possible predictive index of change after the intervention.
were found between LP scores in CA on the WCST-LP, and Further testing of the utility of this test in an ASD popula-
emotion recognition measures and conflict resolution skills. tion is still required, because only two studies have pursued
In the studies cited above, this sensitivity to verbal and social this goal to date. The stability of these results must be veri-
comprehension was accounted for by the fact that the inter- fied, on one hand, to understand the utility of LP for deter-
vention supplied within the LP assessment is based on ver- mining inter-individual differences in children with ASD
bal interaction, and requires collaborative work between the (investigating different LP profiles/levels), and on the other
assessor and the subject; it is act of learning within a social hand, to analyze its predictive power of intervention success.
context, modulated by verbal skills. On the other hand, these Finally, in light of these findings, it would be interesting
results do not agree with those of Bonete et al. (2010), who to compare test results in groups of children with ASD who
did not find significant correlations between LP score and have different intelligence levels, to try to clarify this test’s
deficits in interpersonal skills, probably due to their sample discriminant validity for ASD children with different IQ lev-
size of 10 participants with ASD, a number that limits the els (Aljunied and Frederickson 2011a, b; Ozonoff 1995; Liss
validity of their results. et al. 2001; Rumsey 1985).
Another interesting finding is the absence of significant
correlations between any of the WCST-LP measures and the
other measurements in children with TD. For this reason, the
WCST-LP appears to be a sensitive task for revealing EF Conclusions
deficits that relate to the social and non-social behavior of
children with ASD in their everyday routine; these deficits This study has provided a source of construct-based evi-
cannot be observed in children with TD (Happé, et al. 2006; dence for the validity of the WCST-LP set of measurements,
Hill 2004a; Leung et al. 2015; Pugliese et al. 2016). and for its greater prognostic sensitivity in children with
Overall, these findings must be interpreted with consid- ASD, based on their capacity for learning when full IQ is
eration of certain limitations affecting the study methodol- controlled. The results obtained here have indicated the per-
ogy. Firstly, information about prior intervention received tinence of including the WCST-LP set of measures in ASD
by the participants with ASD could not be controlled: these prognosis assessment. Moreover, the WCST-LP is a measure
children had a prior diagnosis and therefore were receiving of LP, which provides information about possible satisfac-
regular therapy sessions, but that information could not be tory teaching styles for each person.
accessed or controlled in this study. Secondly, although our Finally, this study has confirmed the relation between LP
sample of children with ASD might be large enough, and and verbal and social comprehension, again highlighting
certainly has surpassed the sample size of previous stud- the need for comprehensive assessment and intervention in
ies (Bonete et al. 2010; Donaldson and Olswang 2007), the children with ASD.
present sample size cannot guarantee ecological validity of
these findings. The small size of the ASD group has made Acknowledgment  We want to thank to all children for their participa-
tion, their parents as well as educators & psychologist working with
it difficult to compare these children’s performance accord- them in the different ASD Associations who collaborated in this study.
ing to their IQ levels, and there were also discrepancies
between the IQ indexes. Finally, the small number of girls Funding  The research reported in this paper is funded by the Junta de
in the sample precluded any consideration of possible gender Andalucía (HUM-129 & HUM-820), and the Research project (Excel-
differences. lence call, 2012) [P12-SEJ-560]
Therefore, generally speaking, this study shows that the
WCST-LP may be a useful assessment tool in children with Compliance with Ethical Standards 
ASD both for making a diagnosis (pretest performance) and
Conflict of interests  The authors declare that they have no conflict of
for establishing a prognosis (based on gain scores), because interest.
it offers construct-based evidence of the validity of the
WCST-LP in children with ASD. The availability of a meas-
ure for effectively assessing learning capacity profiles could
help in understanding the ASD prognosis. The WCST-LP References
can be complementary to traditional assessment measures,
offering dynamic measures that identify the individual’s Aljunied, M., & Frederickson, N. (2011a). Does central coherence
cognitive modifiability. In other words, a measure with relate to the cognitive performance of children with autism
special emphasis in LP can be included for consideration

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders (2020) 50:4230–4242 4241

in dynamic assessments? Autism, 17, 172–183. https​: //doi. Theory, instruments and techniques. Baltimore: University Park
org/10.1177/13623​61311​40996​0. Press.
Aljunied, M., & Frederickson, N. (2011b). Cognitive indicators of Filley, C. M., Young, D. A., Reardon, M. S., & Wilkening, G. N.
different levels of special educational support needs in autism. (1999). Frontal lobe lesions and executive dysfunction in children.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(1), 368–376. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology,
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statisti- 12(3), 156–160.
cal manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). American Psychiatric García-Martín, M. B., & Calero, M. D. (2018) ESCI: Solución de Con-
Association: Washington, DC. flictos Interpersonales. Cuestionario y Programa de Entrenami-
Blijd-Hoogewys, E. M., Bezemer, M. L., & Van Geert, P. L. (2014). ento [ESCI: Interpersonal Conflict Resolution. Questionnaire and
Executive functioning in children with ASD: An analysis of the Intervention Program]. Bogotá: El Manual Moderno.
BRIEF. Journal of Autism Development Disorder, 44, 3089– Goldstein, G., Allen, D. N., Minshew, N. J., Williams, D. L., Volkmar,
3100. https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1080​3-014-2176-9. F., Klin, A., et al. (2008). The structure of intelligence in children
Bonete, S. (2012). Evaluación del potencial de aprendizaje en psico- and adults with high functioning autism. Neuropsychology, 22(3),
sis y trastornos del desarrollo [Assessment of learning potential 301.
in psychosis and developmental disorders.]. In M. D. Calero Gómez-Pérez, M. M., Calero, M. D., Mata, S., & Molinero, C. (2016).
(Ed.), Evaluación del potencial de aprendizaje: Fundamentos y Discrepancies between direct and indirect measures of interper-
aplicaciones [Assessment of learning potential: Fundamentals sonal and neurocognitive skills in autism spectrum disorder chil-
and applications] (pp. 163–206). Madrid: EOS. dren. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 38,
Bonete, S., Vives, M. C., Fernández-Parra, A., Calero, M. D., & 875–886. https​://doi.org/10.1080/13803​395.2016.11701​06.
García-Martín, M. B. (2010). Potencial de aprendizaje y habi- Geurts, H. M., Verté, S., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H., & Sergeant, J. A.
lidades sociales en escolares con el Trastorno de Asperger (2004). How specific are executive functioning deficits in atten-
[Learning potential and social skills in schoolchildren with tion deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism? Journal of Child
Asperger’s Disorder]. Behavioral Psychology, 18, 473–490. Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 836–854. https​://doi.org/10.111
Brunsdon, V. E. A., Colvert, E., Ames, C., Garnett, T., Gillan, N., 1/j.1469-7610.2004.00276​.x.
Hallett, V., … Happé, F. (2015). Exploring the cognitive fea- Golden, C. J. (2006). Stroop: Test de Colores y Palabras [Stroop: Color
tures in children with autism spectrum disorder, their co-twins, and Word test]. Madrid: TEA ediciones.
and typically developing children within a population-based Happé, F., Booth, R., Charlton, R., & Hughes, C. (2006). Executive
sample. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56, function deficits in autism spectrum disorders and attention defi-
893–902. https​://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12362​ cit/ hyperactivity disorder: Examining profiles across domains and
Calero, M. D. (2012). Evaluación del potencial de aprendizaje: ages. Brain and Cognition, 61, 25–39. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.
fundamentos y aplicaciones [Learning potential assessment: bandc​.2006.03.004.
Fundamentals and applications]. Madrid: EOS. Heaton, R. K. (1981). The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test manual.
Calero, M. D., Mata, S., Bonete, S., Molinero, C., & Gómez-Pérez, Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
M. M. (2015). Relations between learning potential, cognitive Haywood, H. C., & Lidz, C. S. (2007). Dynamic assessment in prac-
and interpersonal skills in Asperger children. Learning and tice: Clinical and educational applications. Cambridge: Univer-
Individual Differences, 44, 53–60. https​: //doi.org/10.1016/j. sity Press.
lindi​f.2015.07.004. Heubrock, D. (1992). The Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT)
Calero, M. D., & Navarro, E. (2007). Cognitive plasticity as a mod- in clinical and experimental neuropsychology: Administration,
ulating variable on the effects of memory training in elderly evaluation and research findings. Zeitschrift fuer Differentielle und
persons. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22(1), 63–72. Diagnostische Psychologie, 3, 161–174.
https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2006.06.020. Hill, E. L. (2004a). Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cogni-
De Vries, M., & Geurts, H. M. (2012). Cognitive flexibility in tive Science, 8, 26–32. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.003.
ASD: Task switching with emotional faces. Journal of Autism Hill, E. L. (2004b). Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction
and Developmental Disorders, 42, 2558–2568. https​: //doi. in autism. Developmental Review, 24, 189–233. https​://doi.
org/10.1007/s1080​3-012-1512-1. org/10.1016/j.dr.2004.01.001.
De Vries, M., Prins, P. J., Schmand, B. A., & Geurts, H. M. (2015). Landry, O., & Al-Taie, S. (2016). A meta-analysis of the Wisconsin
Working memory and cognitive flexibility-training for children Card Sort Task in Autism. Journal of Autism Development Disor-
with an Autism Spectrum Disorder: A randomized controlled der, 46, 1220–1235. https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1080​3-015-2659-3.
trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(5), Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., & Swank, P. R. (2009). New directions
566–576. in evaluating social problem solving in childhood: Early precur-
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychol- sors and links to adolescent social competence. New Directions
ogy, 64, 135–168. https​://doi.org/10.1146/annur​ev-psych​-11301​ for Child and Adolescent Development, 123, 51–68. https​://doi.
1-14375​0. org/10.1002/cd.235.
Donaldson, A. L., & Olswang, L. B. (2007). Investigating requests Le Couteur, A. N. N. E., Lord, C., & Rutter, M. (2003). The autism
for information in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders: diagnostic interview-revised (ADI-R) (pp. 659–685). Los Angeles,
Static versus dynamic assessment. Advances in Speech-Language CA: Western Psychological Services.
Pathology, 9, 297–311. https:​ //doi.org/10.1080/144170​ 40701​ 4137​ Leung, R. C., & Zakzanis, K. K. (2014). Brief report: Cognitive flex-
20. ibility in autism spectrum disorders: A quantitative review. Jour-
D’Zurilla, T. J., Nezu, A. M., & Maydeu-Olivares, A. (2002). Social nal of Autism Development Disorder, 44, 2628–2645. https​://doi.
problem-solving Inventory—revised (SPSI-R). North Tonawanda, org/10.1007/s1080​3-014-2136-4.
NY: Multi-Health Systems. Leung, R. C., Vogan, V. M., Powell, T. L., Anagnostou, E., & Taylor,
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1976). Pictures of facial affect. Palo Alto, M. J. (2015). The role of executive functions in social impair-
CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. ment in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Child Neuropsychology, 22,
Feuerstein, R., Rand, Y., & Hoffman, M. B. (1979). Dynamic assess- 336–344. https​://doi.org/10.1080/09297​049.2015.10050​66.
ment of retarders performers. The learning assessment device: Liss, M., Harel, B., Fein, D., Allen, D., Dunn, M., Feinstein, C.,
… Rapin, I. (2001). Predictors and correlates of adaptive

13

4242 Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders (2020) 50:4230–4242

functioning in children with developmental disorders. Journal Rumsey, J. M. (1985). Conceptual problem-solving in highly verbal,
of  Autism  Developmental Disorder, 32, 219-230. https​://doi. nonretarded autistic men. Journal of Autism and Developmental
org/10.1023/A:10107​07417​274 Disorders, 15, 23–36.
Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (2002). Escala de Tsuchiya, E., Oki, J., Yahara, N., & Fujieda, K. (2005). Computer-
Observación de Diagnóstico de Autismo [Autism Diagnostic ized version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in children with
Observation Schedule, ADOS]. Madrid: TEA Ediciones. high-functioning autistic disorder or attention-deficit/hyperac-
Maddio, S. L., & Greco, C. (2010). Cognitive flexibility to solve inter- tivity disorder. Brain & Development, 27, 233–236. https​://doi.
personal problems. Does this ability differ among peers in chil- org/10.1016/j.brain​dev.2004.06.008.
dren from elementary public urban and urban-marginal schools? Van Eylen, L., Boets, B., Steyaert, J., Wagemans, J., & Noens, I. (2015).
Interamerican Journal of Psychology, 44, 98–109. Executive functioning in autism spectrum disorders: influence of
Molinero, C., Bonete, S., Gómez-Pérez, M. M., & Calero, M. D. task and sample characteristics and relation to symptom severity.
(2015). Normative study of Ekman’s 60-face test for Spanish European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 24, 1399–1417. https​
adolescents. Behavioral Psychology, 23, 361–371. ://doi.org/10.1007/s0078​7-015-0689-1.
Montgomery, J. M., Stoesz, B. M., & McCrimmon, A. W. (2013). Vygostky, L. S. (1978). El desarrollo de los procesos psicológicos
Emotional intelligence, theory of mind, and executive functions superiores. [Development of higher psychological processes.]
as predictors of social outcomes in young adults with Asperger Barcelona: Grijalbo.
Syndrome. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabili- Wechsler, D. (1997). WAIS-III: Escala de inteligencia Wechsler para
ties, 28, 4–13. https​://doi.org/10.1177/10883​57612​46152​5. adultos-III [Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Adults-III]. Madrid:
Mostert-Kerckhoffs, M. A. L., Staal, W. G., Houben, R. H., & De- Manual Moderno.
Jonge, M. V. (2015). Stop and change: Inhibition and flex- Wechsler, D. (2005). Escala de inteligencia de Wechsler para niños-IV
ibility skills are related to repetitive behavior in children and (WISC-IV) [Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV (WISC-
young adults with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism IV)]. Madrid: TEA ediciones.
and Developmental Disorders, 45, 3148–3158. https ​ : //doi. Westwood, H., Stahl, D., Mandy, W., & Tchanturia, K. (2016). The set-
org/10.1007/2Fs10​803-015-2473-y. shifting profiles of anorexia nervosa and autism spectrum disorder
Ozonoff, S. (1995). Reliability and validity of the Wisconsin Card using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: A systematic review and
Sorting Test in studies of autism. Neuropsychology, 9, 491–500. meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 46, 1809–1827. https​://
Ozonoff, S., Goodlin-Jones, B. L., & Solomon, M. (2005). Evidence doi.org/10.1017/S0033​29171​60005​81.
based assessment of Autism Spectrum Disorder in children and Wiedl, K. H., Schottke, H., & Calero, M. D. (2001). Dynamic assess-
adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychol- ment of cognitive rehabilitation potential in schizophrenic per-
ogy, 34, 523–540. https:​ //doi.org/10.1207/s15374​ 424jc​ cp340​ 3_8. sons and in elderly persons with and without dementia. European
Pelechano, V. (Ed.). (1996). Habilidades interpersonales: Teoría Journal of Psychological Assessment, 17, 112–119. https​://doi.
mínima y programas de intervención [Interpersonal skills: org/10.1027//1015-5759.17.2.112.
Minimum theory and intervention programs] (Vol. 1). Valencia: Wiedl, K. H., Schöttke, H., Green, M. F., & Nuechterlein, K. H. (2004).
Promolibro. Dynamic testing in schizophrenia: Does training change the con-
Pooragha, F., Kafi, S. M., & Sotodeh, S. O. (2013). Comparing struct validity of a test? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 30, 703–711. https​
response inhibition and flexibility for two components of execu- ://doi.org/10.1093/oxfor​djour​nals.schbu​l.a0071​24.
tive functioning in children with autism spectrum disorder and Wiedl, K. H., & Wienöbst, J. (1999). Inter-individual differences in
normal children. Iranian Journal of Pediatrics, 23, 309–314. cognitive remediation research with schizophrenic patient’s indi-
Pugliese, C. E., Anthony, L. G., Strang, J. F., Dudley, K., Wallace, cators of rehabilitation potential? International Journal of Reha-
G. L., Naiman, D. Q., et al. (2016). Longitudinal examination bilitation Research, 22, 1–5.
of adaptive behavior in autism spectrum disorders: Influence of Wiedl, K. H., Wiënobst, J., & Schöttke, H. (1999). Estimation reha-
executive function. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- bilitation potential in schizophrenic subjects. In F. Böker, H. D.
ders, 46, 467–477. https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1080​3-015-2584-5. Brenner, & R. Genner (Eds.), Rehabilitation of cognitive disorders
Robinson, S., Goddard, L., Dritschel, B., Wisley, M., & Howlin, P. in schizophrenic patients (pp. 221–242). Bern: Huber, P.
(2009). Executive functions in children with Autism Spec- Young, A. W., Perrett, D., Calder, A., Sprengelmeyr, R., & Ekman,
trum Disorders. Brain and Cognition, 71, 362–368. https​://doi. P. (2002). Facial expressions of emotions: Stimuli and test. San
org/10.1016/j.bandc​.2009.06.007. Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.
Romine, C. B., Lee, D., Wolfe, M. E., Homack, S., George, C., & Ric-
cio, C. A. (2004). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test with children: A Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
meta-analytic study of sensitivity and specificity. Archives of Clin- jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
ical Neuropsychology, 19, 1027–1041. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.
acn.2003.12.009.

13
Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders is a copyright of Springer, 2020. All Rights
Reserved.

You might also like