Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/286640615

Assessment of Social Cognition and Theory of Mind: Initial Validation of the


Geneva Social Cognition Scale

Article  in  European Neurology · December 2015


DOI: 10.1159/000442412

CITATIONS READS

6 1,849

6 authors, including:

Martory Marie-Dominique Alan J Pegna

19 PUBLICATIONS   472 CITATIONS   
The University of Queensland
139 PUBLICATIONS   4,171 CITATIONS   
SEE PROFILE
SEE PROFILE

Laurent Sheybani Jean-Marie Annoni


Hôpitaux Universitaires de Genève Université de Fribourg
12 PUBLICATIONS   111 CITATIONS    274 PUBLICATIONS   5,612 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Diagnostics View project

Static mechanical allodynia (SMA) is a paradoxical painful hypoaesthesia in CRSP'patient View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Jean-Marie Annoni on 22 December 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Original Paper

Eur Neurol 2015;74:288–295 Received: June 23, 2015


Accepted: November 11, 2015
DOI: 10.1159/000442412
Published online: December 12, 2015

Assessment of Social Cognition and


Theory of Mind: Initial Validation of
the Geneva Social Cognition Scale
Marie-Dominique Martory a Alan John Pegna a, b Laurent Sheybani c
     

Mélanie Métral a Françoise Bernasconi Pertusio a Jean-Marie Annoni d


     

a
Neuropsychology Unit and Laboratory of Experimental Neuropsychology, Department of Neurology, Geneva
 

University Hospital, b Faculty of Psychology and Educational Science and c Department of Neuroscience, University
   

Medical Center (CMU), University of Geneva, Geneva, and d Neurology Unit, Department of Medicine, Faculty of
 

Sciences, University and Hospital of Fribourg, Fribourg, Switzerland

Key Words left CVAs were impaired in verbal/discourse tasks (social cog-
Clinical scale · Neuropsychological evaluation · Brain nition, inferences, absurd stories, and cartoons. Conclu-
damage · Social emotions · False beliefs · Inferences · sions: The GeSoCS is a medium duration assessment tool
Absurdity judgement · Executive planning that appears to detect and characterize significant social im-
pairment in neurological patients. © 2015 S. Karger AG, Basel

Abstract
Background: Social cognition is widely studied in neurolo-
gy. At present, such evaluations are designed for research or Introduction
for specific diseases and simple general clinical tools are
lacking. We propose a clinical evaluation tool for social cog- The last decade saw the development of social neuro-
nition, the Geneva Social Cognition Scale (GeSoCS). Meth- science, which integrated social psychology, cognitive
ods: The GeSoCS is a 100-point scale composed of 6 subtests neuropsychology and neurosciences and raised the ques-
(theory of mind stories, recognition of social emotions, false tion of whether social cognition exists as a specific do-
beliefs, inferences, absurdity judgement and planning abili- main (for review see Ward [1]). Social cognition refers to
ties) chosen from different validated tests of social and cog- the abilities and processes allowing people to interact and
nitive evaluation. Eighty-four patients with neurological dis- to understand the behaviour of others. Among these dif-
orders and 52 controls participated in the study. Evaluation ferent processes, the capacity to attribute mental states
duration lasted 20–60 min. Results: Mean scores were 92.6 ± such as feelings and beliefs and then to predict behaviour
4.5 for controls and 76.5 ± 15.3 for patients and differentiate is called theory of mind (ToM) and plays a determining
patients and controls in all subtests. With a cut-off score of role in social adaptation. ToM was first studied in pri-
84, the scale had a sensitivity of 62% and a specificity of 94%. mates [2], and subsequently in developmental psycholo-
In our stroke subgroup, right CVAs failed in cartoons, infer- gy [3]. Its first clinical application involved autistic per-
ences, ‘mind in the eyes’, and in the temporal rule task while sons [4]. Social cognition has also been addressed exten-
149.126.78.17 - 12/14/2015 5:44:51 PM

© 2015 S. Karger AG, Basel Dr. Marie-Dominique Martory


Bibliotheque Cantonale

0014–3022/15/0746–0288$39.50/0 Neuropsychology Unit, Department of Neurology


Geneva University Hospital
Downloaded by:

E-Mail karger@karger.com
4, Gabrielle-Perret-Gentil, 1205 Geneva (Switzerland)
www.karger.com/ene
E-Mail mdmartory @ bluewin.ch
sively using brain imaging techniques. Along these lines, Table 1. Demographic characteristics of control and patient groups
fMRI studies, for example, have shown that the temporal
lobe, the temporal-parietal-junction, the medial prefron- Control group Patient group
(n = 52) (n = 84)
tal cortex and the posterior cingulate cortex are specifi-
cally involved in ToM [5–7]. Age, mean (SD) 42.2 (16.4) 53.3 (15.5)
In the field of clinical neuroscience, investigations of Gender
brain-injured patients with acute or neurodegenerative Male 20 49
Female 32 35
disease have also revealed different types of impairments Handedness
in social cognition and the understanding of people’s be- Right 51 71
haviour [8]. Indeed, ToM constitutes a complex domain Left 1 13
that involves numerous cognitive processes, including Level of education, years
executive functions [9, 10] and, in certain conditions, <12 1 11
>12 51 73
normal language development [11].
Thus, the evaluation of social cognition and ToM in
adults relies on a wide range of tasks of varying complex-
ity. The most common involves false beliefs and can be
used with young children. In particular, first-order beliefs Table 2. Patient group aetiologies
necessitate understanding another person’s mental state, Patient Stroke Trauma Tumor Dementia Encephalopathy
while second-order beliefs, which are more complex, pre- group (n = 31) (n = 17) (n = 7) (n = 17) (n = 11)
dict what a person thinks about a third person’s feelings (n = 84)
[3, 4]. More complex tasks used to assess the mental state
Age,
of others require that participants identify a person’s emo- mean
tional state on the basis of the expression in their eyes (the (SD) 58 (12.8) 41 (13) 51 (14) 66.4 (8.8) 44.8 (16.6)
‘mind in the eyes’ test) [12, 13], detect a social ‘faux pas’ Gender
Male 15 13 4 11 7
[14], or understand mental inferences or indirect speech Female 16 4 3 6 4
as in metaphors and jokes. Children are able to resolve Handedness
these tasks at different ages (age 4 for the first order false Right 28 14 6 14 8
belief and 9 or 11 for the faux pas). In brain-damaged pa- Left 3 3 1 3 3
tients, a number of authors have proposed that patients
with brain injuries would process simple false beliefs sto-
ries correctly but would fail in more complex tasks [14].
Despite the great number of studies in social neurosci- neurological patients rather than a short screening tool.
ence investigating patients with acute acquired or neuro- Deficits identified with this tool would then allow subse-
degenerative brain damage, research in the field has not quent detailed examination using the existing tests.
yet provided tests that are broad enough for use in the
clinic and have focused either on specific pathologies,
such as multiple sclerosis [15] or neurodegenerative
Method
groups (for review see Henry et al. [16]), or alternately,
have addressed specifics such as false beliefs [17], or faux Participants
pas recognition [14]. Additional global testing, as de- All subjects gave written informed consent for their participa-
scribed earlier, is often too long for clinical practice. To tion in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The study was
our knowledge, there is yet no general test that allows approved by the local Ethics Committee, (Commission d’Ethique
de la Recherche Clinique, Geneva University Hospital, Switzerland;
neuropsychologists to assess ToM and social cognition CER 10-073).
function more globally in everyday clinical practice. The population was composed of 52 healthy French-speaking,
The aim of this study is thus to offer a short assessment volunteers, with no known neurological or psychiatric disorders.
tool that is easy to use in everyday neuropsychological Demographic data are summarized in table 1.
practice, which has been developed from existing tests de- Eighty-four brain-damaged French-speaking patients were ex-
amined consecutively during their hospitalization in the neuro-
scribed in the literature such as the mind in the eyes test logical clinic. The patient population presented various aetiologies
or false beliefs. This scale was intended as a clinical tool (stroke, epilepsy, trauma, tumour, dementia; table 2). Participants
for potential impairment in social cognition found in with CVA were assessed in the post-acute stage (about 10 days to
149.126.78.17 - 12/14/2015 5:44:51 PM

GeSoCS as a ToM Clinical Tool Eur Neurol 2015;74:288–295 289


Bibliotheque Cantonale

DOI: 10.1159/000442412
Downloaded by:
3 weeks after their stroke and after stabilization for other aetiolo- Is Willy rather affectionate with his wife this evening? (response:
gies). All patients’ diagnoses were based on a clinical evaluation, a yes).
laboratory evaluation including an MRI, and a formal neuropsy- Tasks 5 and 6 are beyond ToM strictly speaking but are used to
chological assessment. Exclusion criteria included a history of pri- determine clinically a general executive impairment, which can be
or psychiatric disorders, delirium, global aphasia and severe de- associated (although not necessarily) with deficits in social cogni-
mentia (≥CDR3). tion [9].
Subsequent statistical inquiries as to the effects of levels of edu- Task 5 required that patients correctly detect an absurdity in
cation on our tests showed that this factor did not influence the 4  stories and accept a control story (e.g. ‘The management of a
results. The different levels were therefore merged. railroad company noticed that in train accidents, injuries were for
more serious in the tail wagons. Consequently, it was decided to
Assessment of ToM and Social Cognition remove the last wagon of each train. What do you think of that?’).
The test was developed internally on the basis of the literature This test was retained as it constituted an interesting match with
reported above and spanned aspects most frequently explored in the social cognition stories that also required a ‘meta’ representa-
social cognition. Each of them was evaluated through specific tasks tion, albeit without the social component (adapted from Binet and
as described in the literature and combined in order to form a scale Simon [22]).
with a total score of 100 points (online suppl. appendix, see www. Task 6 was not directly related to social cognition, but was pre-
karger.com/doi/10.1159/000442412). sented as a general measure of executive function. It is a French
Tasks 1, 2, 3 and 4 were specific to social cognition and com- adaptation of the Counter test [23] and consists in determining the
prehension of inferences. For each of them, the patients were asked rules underlying the temporal appearance of colour counters (e.g.
to read a story and to answer specific questions involving ToM, but black-white-white, black-white-white…). This test was chosen for
also to respond to simple control questions in order to test linguis- its ease of use and its good prediction of mental flexibility in fron-
tic comprehension. The lengths of the stories were similar. In ad- tal impairment [24].
dition, precautions were taken to ensure that the questions and the Every verbal or picture task contained control questions
syntactic structures of the sentences were equivalent and simple measuring literal comprehension but excluding ToM or social
enough to be understood by most patients. aspect. The control questions were not scored but were includ-
Task 1 was composed of 5 written ToM and social cognition ed to ensure correct linguistic comprehension. The item ‘Jim’
stories: one ‘faux pas’ situation, one ‘normal’ situation (‘Helen’ and that constituted a full question in the test was an exception. The
‘Jim’ stories [14]), one ToM story (‘burglar’ story [18]), and one duration of the whole test ranged from about 40 min in a
in-house ToM story involving a self-perspective1. healthy subject to between 60 and 70 min in the brain-damaged
Task 2 involved 10 items chosen from the ‘reading the mind in patients. In this latter group, participants could re-read the sto-
the eyes’ test [12, 13]. The original test is composed of 36 pairs of ries or obtain repetitions of the questions. Patients and control
eyes each evoking an emotion. The subject has to choose between subjects were examined by clinically qualified neuropsycholo-
4 written propositions the one that best describes the emotion. The gists.
items selected (1-4-5-8-14-16-18-30-33-35) were retained as being
the most easily identifiable on the basis of a pilot experiment with Procedure
25 participants. The Geneva Social Cognition Scale (GeSoCS) was tested in ad-
Task 3 was made up of 5 ToM picture stories [19] representing dition to the patients’ standard neuropsychological examination,
first- and second-order false beliefs (e.g. A man is sitting on a which will not be detailed in this study. In controls, no additional
bench with his dog beside him and he is preparing to eat a sand- evaluation was performed, as participants were considered to be
wich; while he is throwing the paper packaging, a bird steals the cognitively normal. However, a semi-structured interview was car-
sandwich; on seeing the sandwich missing, the man scolds the ried out to confirm the absence of any indicators of cognitive im-
dog. Question: why does the man scold his dog?) and one story pairment, either anamnestically or behaviourally. The subjects
not requiring inference (A boy sits on his sofa when he hears the were individually tested in a single session of about 40–60 min. The
doorbell ring. He thinks that it is the mailman but he smiles when sessions took place at the University Hospital of Geneva, in the
he sees that it is his friend. Question: Why does the boy seem Neuropsychology Unit.
happy?). During the test, they were asked to answer the questions freely
Task 4 was composed of 5 stories that the patient had to read and no solutions were proposed. If necessary, the question was re-
before answering questions about possible or impossible infer- peated. For each item, full points were given when the answer was
ences [20, 21] e.g. ‘The more Willy loses at the casino, the more complete and correct. Half points were given if the answer was in-
affectionate he is with his wife; apparently, he was not very lucky complete, or if the answer was correct but not the explanation.
in his game this evening’. Control question without inference:
Did Willy win a lot at the casino this evening? (response: no). Statistical Analysis
Question with impossible inference to make: Is Willy’s wife rich? The results of the whole group are described first, followed by
(response: we cannot know). Question with an inference to make: that of the patients and controls (global scores, then subtests).
Significant differences and interactions between the controls and
the patients were calculated using an analysis of variance
1
  In a fMRI study with healthy subjects, Vogeley et al. [2001], found differ- (ANOVA; statistical analyses were performed using Statistica
ent patterns of brain activation, as the subjects took the first or third person 12  software). For the smaller and more heterogeneous groups
perspective and Gambini, Barbieri, Scarone [2004], supported this idea in a (TBI, dementia), additional testing was carried out using non-
study involving schizophrenic subjects. parametrical tests.
149.126.78.17 - 12/14/2015 5:44:51 PM

290 Eur Neurol 2015;74:288–295 Martory/Pegna/Sheybani/Métral/


Bibliotheque Cantonale

DOI: 10.1159/000442412 Bernasconi Pertusio/Annoni


Downloaded by:
Social Mind in Absurd Temporal
cognition the eyes Cartoons Inferences stories rules Total
0.80

0.40

–0.40

Controls
Patients
Fig. 1. Comparison of mean z-scores of –0.80
controls and patients for all the tasks.

Results Table 3. Results: mean scores and SD for controls and patients for
all tasks
Descriptive statistics for the controls and the patients
Tasks Controls (n = 52), Patients (n = 84),
are provided in table  3. The controls obtained a total mean (SD) mean (SD)
mean score of 92.6/100 (SD 4.5); for each test, the result
was close to the maximum score with a standard devia- Social cognition 19.36 (1.20) 16.02 (4.08)
tion equal to 1, with the exception of the ‘mind in the eyes’ Mind in the eyes 15.09 (2.28) 12.11 (3.81)
test, (mean 15/20, SD 2.3). There was no significant dif- ToM cartoons 19.55 (0.79) 17.22 (3.5)
Inferences 19.17 (1.49) 15.83 (4.4)
ference due to gender (women 92.4, men 93). The pa- Absurd stories 9.65 (0.94) 7.78 (2.32)
tients obtained a total mean score lower than the controls Temporal rules 9.76 (0.85) 7.54 (3.42)
(76.51) with a large dispersal (SD 15.32) and performanc- Total 92.61 (4.51) 76.51 (15.32)
es for each test were lower than the controls.
For every task, the performance of the patients and the
controls were expressed as z-scores of the mean total
group’s performances (patients + controls; fig. 1). The ANOVA confirmed that the difference was sig-
An ANOVA comparing the 2 groups’ z-scores revealed nificant (F(15, 395)  = 4.4,651, p  < 0.05). However, when
a significant difference in performance between the con- considering the side of the lesion, a post hoc Fisher test
trols and the patients (F(1, 134) = 55.035, p < 0.00001), and showed different patterns of impairment in the case of
a post hoc test showed that the patients were impaired right CVA, left CVA or bilateral CVA.
compared to the healthy subjects in all tasks. Right CVAs did not differ from the controls in the first
We now considered the mean z-score for the CVA pop- task of social cognition and were quite correct in ‘faux pas’
ulation compared to the mean z-score of the control group. stories, which required criticism (p = 0.06), but they failed
in cartoons, in inferences and in tasks in which they had to
Comparison of z-Scores: CVA Patients vs. Controls guess a feeling, such as the ‘mind in the eyes’ test (p < 0.05),
The 31 patients with CVA demonstrated approxi- or an intention, such as in the temporal rule task (p < 0.05).
mately the same pattern as the entire patient group with Left CVAs were impaired in all verbal tasks, social cog-
altered performances compared to the controls as shown nition tasks (p < 0.05), inferences (p < 0.05), absurd sto-
in figure 2. ries, and also in cartoons (p < 0.05). Conversely, they have
149.126.78.17 - 12/14/2015 5:44:51 PM

GeSoCS as a ToM Clinical Tool Eur Neurol 2015;74:288–295 291


Bibliotheque Cantonale

DOI: 10.1159/000442412
Downloaded by:
Controls ight C A eft C A ilateral C A
. 0

0.80

0.40

–0.40
Social cognition
Mind in the eyes
–0.80
Cartoons
Inferences
– . 0 Absurd stories
Temporal rules
Fig. 2. Comparison of mean z-scores of
right CVA, left CVA, bilateral CVA and – . 0
controls for all the tasks.

shown significant difficulties in the ‘mind in the eyes’ task Table 4. Cut-off for each subtest and the total test
(p = 0.07).
Finally, bilateral CVAs were significantly impaired Tasks Controls, Cut-off (rounded
mean (SD) to nearest unit)
(p < 0.05) in all 6 tasks.
Social cognition 19.36 (1.20) 17
Determining Pathological Function: The Cut-Off Score Mind in the eyes 15.09 (2.28) 11
Based on these data, we attempted to determine a cut- ToM cartoons 19.55 (0.79) 18
Inferences 19.17 (1.49) 16
off score for clinical use. The values for each test are given Absurd stories 9.65 (0.94) 8
in table 4 using a cut-off score of minus 2 SDs. Temporal rules 9.76 (0.85) 8
Using these values, 52 patients fell below the cut-off Total 92.61 (4.51) 84
(84) and 32 remained above; among the 31 CVA patients,
19 were below the cut off (9/15 left, 5/8 right and 5/7 bi-
lateral CVA); 3 healthy subjects fell below the cut-off and
49 were above. The cut-off of 84 allowed to significantly rological patients in a daily clinical practice. This screening
differentiate between controls and patients (χ2(1) = 42.02; test was constructed with items taken from existing tasks
p  < 0.0001). Sensitivity was at 61.9% and specificity at previously described in the literature for evaluating the
94.23%. An analysis computing the receiver operating ToM. These tasks were subsequently adapted and grouped
curve (ROC) with these cut-off scores showed an esti- in order to form a scale graded from 0 to 100. To the best of
mated area of 0.78 under the curve. Thus, the probability our knowledge, it is the first global tool adapted for every-
of correctly classifying a neurological patient as patho- day clinical work as most of the studies on clinical patients
logical on the basis of our test is 78% (fig. 3). until now have used extensive batteries. From a clinical per-
spective, GeSoCS appears to conform to the criteria for a
consistent scale. First, the GeSoCS uses multiple aspects of
Discussion validated tests for assessment of ToM, a ‘complex, multi-
faceted cognitive ability’ [25]. On average, GeSoCS lasts
In order to provide a simple tool for evaluating social 40–60 min. Generally, this duration is easily tolerated by
cognition, we designed the GeSoCS, which allows rapid neurological patients, although one cannot exclude poten-
and easy assessment of social cognition in hospitalized neu- tial attentional deficits during evaluation.
149.126.78.17 - 12/14/2015 5:44:51 PM

292 Eur Neurol 2015;74:288–295 Martory/Pegna/Sheybani/Métral/


Bibliotheque Cantonale

DOI: 10.1159/000442412 Bernasconi Pertusio/Annoni


Downloaded by:
orated verbal response) and ToM pictures (cartoons) but

Color version available online


Total
performed correctly in the cognition scale. The bilateral
00 strokes, as expected, had more difficulty in all tasks.
As regards CVA, the recent data in the literature show
80 that social cognition is affected during the acute phase,
particularly if the lesion involves anatomical regions such
as the parieto-temporal junction and fronto-parietal ar-
0 eas. However, this disorder can evolve favourably in the
Sensiti ity

same way as other cognitive functions [27].


40
Interest and Limitations of Current Study
The GeSoCS scale has the advantage of being com-
0 posed of tests that are described and validated in the lit-
erature as being sensitive to social cognition and ToM.
Instructions and scoring methods are simple to use for
0
neuropsychologists and behavioural neurologists. Its du-
0 0 40 0 80 00
ration and the absence of heavy equipment allow it to be
00 – Specificity
used quite easily with any patient suspected of presenting
disorders of social cognition.
Fig. 3. ROC with an estimated area of 0.78 under the curve. In our study, the number of healthy controls was
slightly limited, but their scores were high and homoge-
neous, and most tests have already been reported exten-
This scale was administered by experienced neuropsy- sively in the literature and their use has been confirmed.
chologists to 136 volunteers: 52 healthy subjects and Their average age was younger (11 years) than the average
84 patients who were either hospitalized in the post-acute age of the patients. This difference might have influenced
stage or stabilized. The results showed that patients per- our findings. However, a recent study that considers the
formed significantly more poorly than healthy controls effect of aging on mind-reading abilities in healthy people
on average (p  < 0.05). In our sample, patients differed has suggested the absence of any difference between
from controls not only in the global score but also in all younger and older subjects [28]. The test cannot be un-
subtests. This ability to differentiate between both groups, derstood as a screening, but must be considered as a more
despite the patients’ heterogeneous results (SD 15.368 in descriptive test. It took often between 30 min and 1 h, and
the patients’ group vs. 4.5 in the controls), offers a satisfy- had to be done in a different session than the usual neu-
ing perspective in the use of this tool. ropsychological evaluation. Such durations have also
The CVA patients, who constituted the main group, been used in other populations with clinically oriented
performed in a significantly worse manner in all events. test (see social cognition impairments in relation to gen-
Interestingly, the results differed according to the lateral- eral cognitive deficits, injury severity, and prefrontal le-
ization of the lesion: left CVAs were significantly different sions in traumatic brain injury patients [29] or in fronto-
from the controls in all the tests (p < 0.05) except the ‘mind temporal dementia [30]). So it must be considered a clin-
in the eyes’ test. Some of these left CVA patients suffered ical test with a capacity of describing the type of deficit in
from aphasia and, although it has been shown that ToM clinical cognition in neurological patients. Our experi-
abilities are not fundamentally impaired in this population ence suggests that patients easily support this non-chal-
[26], it is possible that language difficulties and deficient lenging evaluation, except strongly demented patients
working memory may play a part in these results, and also and severe aphasia.
in ToM picture tasks where all the answers require a verbal Our data showed a good specificity of 94% for a cut-off
argumentation. Nevertheless, this group was not signifi- of 84, making the GeSoCS a highly efficient method for
cantly different from the controls in the interpretation of detecting deficits of social cognition. The sensitivity of
the ‘mind in the eyes’ test (p > 0.5). The right CVAs showed 61% is of course a limitation in our study and the GeSoCS
a different pattern with respect to the controls and to the cannot be considered an interesting screening tool for
left CVAs: they failed in the ‘mind in the eyes’ test, in tem- neurological disorders. It has, however, a strong clinical
poral rule tasks (which nevertheless do not require an elab- interest because it can detect, among the neurological pa-
149.126.78.17 - 12/14/2015 5:44:51 PM

GeSoCS as a ToM Clinical Tool Eur Neurol 2015;74:288–295 293


Bibliotheque Cantonale

DOI: 10.1159/000442412
Downloaded by:
tients those who have effectively a deficit in social cogni- task while left CVAs were impaired in verbal/discourse
tion. As mentioned, the high specificity allows predicting tasks (social cognition, inferences, absurd stories, and
that impaired results at the GeSoCS in neurological pa- cartoons). Furthermore, we did not attempt any qualita-
tients are indeed related to the disease and less probably tive analysis of the answers or the different questions for
to non-neurological effects and are a sign of an acquired social cognition (ToM, point of view, faux pas) and did
deficit in social cognition. In order to confirm the eco- not establish correlations between the GeSoCS and neu-
logical validity of the GeSoCs, we made a post hoc corre- ropsychological (e.g. executive functions and language)
lation between the GeSoCS scores and scores obtained at or ecological measures. Finally, the test was intended to
the dysexecutive questionnaire (DEX) [31], a standard- be as short as possible and thus, the test samples used may
ized 20 items rating scale used for quantifying behav- not have had the same diagnostic value as the test as a
ioural disturbances commonly associated with executive whole, e.g. the ‘reading the mind in the eyes’ test consists
impairment. In an unselected subgroup of 28 patients, a of 36 items, but only 10 were selected for our task.
close relative completed the DEX questionnaire. In this More elaborate studies on the different patient groups
subgroup, there was a correlation between total GeSoCS should be considered and, in particular, their correlation
score and DEX score (CC = –0.450, p = 0.02). with other ecological measures, including questionnaires
Another limitation of this study is the fact that various and behavioural scales, could be examined in more detail.
aetiologies were included, some of which contained Nevertheless, the data obtained from the current-screen-
smaller samples of patients, with the exception of the ing test for disorders of social cognition and ToM provide
CVA group. Aetiologies therefore could not be investi- a useful bedside assessment tool that should allow the cli-
gated systematically and only the effect of the side of the nician to determine the presence of ToM deficits in brain-
lesion was taken into account because of the absence of damaged patients.
prior known behavioural deficits. The aim was actually to
test different neurological populations to show that dif-
Acknowledgements
ferent pathologies could be detected as causing ToM and
social cognition deficits. However, in the second part of We thank S. Baron Cohen, A. Duchêne May-Carle, M. Monfort
the results, we analysed separately dementia, stroke pa- and I. Monfort, Ortho Editions for allowing us to use parts of their
tients and the other aetiologies, in order to show that all tests to devise our scale and the neuropsychologists who adminis-
pathologies may have social cognition impairment, but trated the GeSoCS.
the pattern varies with the aetiologies. GeSoCS sub scores
seemed also sensitive to the side of the lesions as described Disclosure Statement
in our stroke subgroup: right CVAs failed in cartoons,
inferences, ‘mind in the eyes’, and in the temporal rule The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References
1 Ward J: The Student’s Guide to Social Neuro- rietal junction in ‘theory of mind’. Neuroim- dren’s understanding of mind; in Astington
science. Hove and New York, Psychology age 2003;19:1835–1842. JW Baird JA (eds): Why Language Matters for
Press, 2012. 7 Saxe R, Powell LJ: It’s the thought that counts: Theory of Mind. Oxford, Oxford University
2 Premack D, Woodruff G: Does the chimpan- specific brain regions for one component of Press, 2005, pp 50–69.
zee have a theory of mind? Behav Brain Sci theory of mind. Psychol Sci 2006;17:692–699. 12 Baron-Cohen S, Jolliffe T, Mortimore C,
1978;1:515–526. 8 Samson D: Reading other people’s mind: in- Robertson M: Another advanced test of the-
3 Wimmer H, Perner J: Beliefs about beliefs: sights from neuropsychology. J Neuropsychol ory of mind: evidence from very high func-
representation and constraining function of 2009;3:3–16. tioning adults with autism or Asperger syn-
wrong beliefs in young children’s under- 9 Martin-Rodriguez JF, Leon-Carrion J: Theory drome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1997; 38:
standing of deception. Cognition 1983; 13: of mind deficits in patients with acquired 813–822.
103–128. brain injury: a quantitative review. Neuropsy- 13 Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste
4 Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, Frith U: Does the chologia 2010;48:1181–1191. Y, Plumb I: The ‘reading the mind in the eyes’
autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’? Cogni- 10 Aboulafia-Brakha T, Christe B, Martory MD, test revised version: a study with normal
tion 1985;21:37–46. Annoni JM: Theory of mind tasks and execu- adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or
5 Frith U, Frith CD: Development and neuro- tive functions: a systematic review of group high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psy-
physiology of mentalizing. Philos Trans R Soc studies in neurology. J Neuropsychol 2011; 5: chiatry 2001;42:241–251.
Lond B Biol Sci 2003;358:459–473. 39–55. 14 Stone VE, Baron-Cohen S, Knight RT: Fron-
6 Saxe R, Kanwisher N: People thinking about 11 Dunn J, Brophy M: Communication, rela- tal lobe contributions to theory of mind. J
thinking people. The role of the temporo-pa- tionships, and individual differences in chil- Cogn Neurosci 1998;10:640–656.
149.126.78.17 - 12/14/2015 5:44:51 PM

294 Eur Neurol 2015;74:288–295 Martory/Pegna/Sheybani/Métral/


Bibliotheque Cantonale

DOI: 10.1159/000442412 Bernasconi Pertusio/Annoni


Downloaded by:
15 Henry A, Tourbah A, Chaunu MP, Rumbach 21 Duchêne May Carle A: La gestion de 28 Castelli I, Baglio F, Blasi V, Alberoni M, Falini
L, Montreuil M, Bakchine S: Social cognition l’implicite. Théorie et évaluation. Ortho Edi- A, Liverta-Sempio O, Nemni R, Marchetti A:
impairments in relapsing-remitting multiple tion, 62330, Isbergues, 2007. Effects of aging on mindreading ability
sclerosis. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2011; 17: 22 Binet A, Simon T: Le développement de through the eyes: an fMRI study. Neuropsy-
1122–1131. l’intelligence chez les enfants. L’année Psy- chologia 2010;48:2586–2594.
16 Henry JD, Cowan DG, Lee T, Sachdev PS: Re- chologique, 1907, vol 14, pp 1–94. 29 Spikman JM, Timmerman ME, Milders MV,
cent trends in testing social cognition. Curr 23 Luria AR, Prieram KH, Homskaya ED: An ex- Veenstra WS, van der Naalt J: Social cognition
Opin Psychiatry 2015;28:133–140. perimental analysis of the behavioral distur- impairments in relation to general cognitive
17 Desgranges B, Laisney M, Bon L, Duval C, bance produced by a left frontal arachnoidal deficits, injury severity, and prefrontal lesions
Mondou A, Bejanin A, et al: TOM-15: une endothelioma (meningioma). Neuropsycho- in traumatic brain injury patients. J Neu-
épreuve de fausses croyances pour évaluer la logia 1964;2:257–280. rotrauma 2012;29:101–111.
théorie de l’esprit cognitive. Rev Neuropsy- 24 Messerli P, Seron X, Tissot R: Quelques as- 30 Bertoux M, Volle E, Funkiewiez A, de Souza
chol 2012;4:216–220. pects des troubles de la programmation dans LC, Leclerq D, Dubois B: Social Cognition
18 Happé F, Brownell H, Winner E: Acquired le syndrome frontal. Arch Suisses Neurol and Emotional Assessment (SEA) is a mark-
‘theory of mind’ impairments following Neurochir Psychiatrie 1979;125:23–35. er of medial and orbital frontal functions: a
stroke. Cognition 1999;70:211–240. 25 Muller F, Simion A, Reviriego E, Galera C, voxel-based morphometry study in behav-
19 Monfort M, Monfort I: L’esprit des autres. Mazaux JM, Barat M, Joseph PA: Exploring ioral variant of frontotemporal degenera-
Madrid, Entha Ediciones Pour Ortho Edition, theory of mind after severe traumatic brain tion. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2012; 18: 972–
62330, Isbergues, 2001. injury. Cortex 2010;46:1088–1099. 985.
20 Duchêne May-Carle A: La gestion des in- 26 Apperly IA, Samson D, Carroll N, Hussain S, 31 Wilson BA, Alderman N, Burgess PW, Emslie
férences chez les cérébro-lésés droits. Thèse Humphreys G: Intact first- and second-order H, Evans J: Behavioural Assessment of the
de doctorat en neuropsychologie. France, false belief reasoning in a patient with severe- Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS) Manual.
Université de Lyon, 1997. ly impaired grammar. Soc Neurosci 2006; 1: Bury St Edmonds, Thames Valley Test Com-
334–348. pany, 1996.
27 Bertrand-Gauvin C, Faucher L, Bocti C, Ga-
gnon MJ, Joanette Y: Cognition sociale et ac-
cident vasculaire cérébral: perspective neuro-
psychologique. Rev Neuropsychol 2014; 6:
99–109.

149.126.78.17 - 12/14/2015 5:44:51 PM

GeSoCS as a ToM Clinical Tool Eur Neurol 2015;74:288–295 295


Bibliotheque Cantonale

DOI: 10.1159/000442412
Downloaded by:

View publication stats

You might also like