Download as docx, pdf, or txt
Download as docx, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1of 5

Expressed emotion and relapse

Origin of the term


To understand the origins of the concept of “expressed emotion” (EE), one must go back
to the 1950’s, to the seminal work of George Brown. To understand the basis for symptom
relapse soon after release from long term treatment, a study was initiated by George Brown and
his colleagues with 229 men discharged from psychiatric hospitals, 156 of them with a diagnosis
of schizophrenia (as cited in Leff, 2000). From the study, it was observed that the strongest link
with relapse and readmission was the type of home to which patients were discharged. They
observed that the relapse rate in patients who went home to critical, over-involved, or hostile
families was more than double that of patients from other types of families. This effect was more
pronounced if patients spent more than 35 hours a week in close contact with their families.
Although this observation matched with clinical experience, it was not universally welcomed.
Most of the objections came from families who did not like being labeled as high expressed
emotion; they felt they were being blamed for the patient’s illness. Another argument against the
concept was that the effect of EE on relapse was not of the same magnitude as that of medication
or other variables, and thus was not to be taken into account. But despite these reservations, the
evidence for a relation between EE and relapse remains strong (The Lancet, 1992).
The components of expressed emotion
George Brown explained five components of EE which includes critical comments,
hostility, emotional over-involvement, positive remarks, and warmth (as cited in Wearden,
Tarrier, Barrowclough, Zastowny, & Rahill, 2000). The quantification of critical comments and
hostility is greatly reliant on the way in which the respondent uses their tone of voice to convey
their feelings (anger, rejection, irritability, ignorance, blaming, negligence etc.), while the
judgment of over-involvement takes into account reported behavior such as caregivers blaming
themselves, sacrificing things, being overprotective of patients, excessively being concerned for
patients, neglecting own personal needs, and similar others. Venkatasubramanian & Amaresha
(2012) explain:
1. Critical comments: Patients who are unable to get up in the morning, who fail to wash
regularly, or who do not participate in household tasks etc., are criticized for being
lazy and selfish; unfortunately, in this context, the caregivers fail to understand that
these could be potential manifestations of negative symptoms of schizophrenia or any
other psychotic disorder. By contrast, low EE caregivers are more capable of
recognizing aspects of the patient’s behavior which are a manifestation of the illness.
(For example: Family caregivers may express in increased tone, tempo, or volume
that the patient frustrates them and deliberately causes problems for them; that family
members feel the burden of managing the patient’s needs and that living with him/her
is hard; or even commenting that patient is ignoring or not following their advice
which is why the illness continues).
2. Hostility: It is a consequence of anger and irritation towards the patient. Hostility is
expressed by general criticisms or attitudes that accuse, reject or demean the patient.
(For example: Caregivers state that the patient intentionally causes problems for
them, wish aloud to live away from the patient, shouts at the patient, accuse the
patient of being able to control himself but acting as though not).
3. Emotional overinvolvement (EOI): Manifests itself by over-emotionality, excessive
self-sacrifice, over-identification, and extremely overprotective behavior with the
patient. Parents of children, who develop schizophrenia, oftentimes feel guilty for the
child’s illness. This chronic guilt leads them to initiate excessive reparative efforts to
make things better for the child. Unfortunately, this has the effect of discouraging the
person’s skills and self-reliance, so that in the long run, overprotectiveness hampers
the person’s recovery. It also leads to dependence of the patient on their caregiver.
(For example: Caregivers blame themselves for everything that goes wrong and feels
like everything is their fault; does not allow or encourage the patient to carry out
his/her day-to-day activities by themselves; gives less importance to personal needs
than patient needs).
4. Warmth: Is expressed through kindness, concern, and empathy of the caregiver. It
depends greatly on vocal qualities of speech, with smiling and positive verbalizations
being common accompaniments, which often conveys an empathic attitude by the
relative. Warmth is a significant characteristic of the low EE family. (For example:
Caregivers state that patient tries to get along with everyone, makes a lot of sense, is
easy to get along with, and is good to have around, etc).
5. Positive regard: This comprises of statements that express appreciation or support for
patient’s behavior and verbal/nonverbal reinforcement by the caregiver. (For
example: Family states that they feel very close to the patient, they appreciate
patient’s little efforts in his day-to-day functioning, state that they can cope with the
patient and enjoy being with him/her).
Assessment of expressed emotion
EE measures the attitudes, quality of relationships and interaction patterns among
psychiatric patients and their close family members. There are many instruments which assess
the family environment in terms of EE. Some of these important EE assessment tools are:
1. Camberwell Family Interview (CFI): During the interview, relative’s speech is
recorded and later used for coding. This semi-structured interview consists of
questions which address the onset of the patient’s illness and the symptoms that were
noticeable to the relative in the months earlier to the patient’s worsening of illness or
admission to the hospital; which provides an understanding about relative’s
involvement as well as attitudes toward the patient.
2. The Five-Minute Speech Sample (FMSS): The five-minute speech sample (FMSS) is
similar to the CFI in that family members talk about their patient and their
relationship for five uninterrupted minutes and the speech is recorded and later coded
for the overall level of EE, criticism, and EOI.
3. Level of expressed emotion scale (LEE scale): This is a 60-item, self-report scale that
measures the emotional environment in the patient’s most important relationships. It
has 60 items that form the four subscales, namely Intrusiveness, Emotional Response,
Attitude toward Illness, and Tolerance and Expectations. Items are rated in a true–
false format.
4. Perceived criticism scale (PCS): This scale recognizes that the most important
element of EE is criticism. It consists of only one question, namely “How critical do
you consider your relative to be of you?” It is administered as a 10-point Likert-type
scale and anchored with values between “not at all critical” and “very critical indeed.”
5. Family emotional involvement and criticism scale (FEIC scale): It is a 14-item scale
which assesses two dimensions of EE: EOI and perceived criticism (PC) in the
family.
6. Family attitude scale: It is a 30-item, self-report measure of EE which emphasizes on
criticism and hostility. Respondents report how often each statement is true on a scale
ranging from “Everyday” (4) to “Never” (0).
Models of expressed emotion
Locus of control model: Studies show that locus of control and EE has significant
associations, especially with an internal locus of control. High EE caregivers, particularly those
who are high in criticality, have been shown to have a more internally based locus of control than
do low EE caregivers and to be more self-blaming. It was also found that highly critical
caregivers are more likely than low critical caregivers to have an internal locus of control
(Hooley, 1998).
Stress-diathesis model or the stress-vulnerability model (Zubin & Spring, 1977): In this
model, the degree of vulnerability to a given psychiatric episode is determined by each person’s
tolerance to traumatic or stressful life events. As EE appears to accurately determine relapse
among patients, the research suggested that family environment may be a major contributing
factor to critical stress levels among persons with schizophrenia. Stress-diathesis approach
facilitates the view that both patients and caregivers are involved in a system of mutual influence
in which each contributes to the stress that acts on the intrinsic vulnerabilities of the other; this
perspective emphasizes upon interactions between vulnerability and stress variables.
Considerable research data on EE with regard to illness relapse also shows EE as a form of
psychosocial stress.
Cultural variables
Most of the studies that contribute to our understanding of EE are largely from Western
countries, all of them cross-sectional, and problems regarding the cross-cultural validity of
assessments of EE are not usually discussed. The cross-cultural validity of EE has been
examined in a few non-Western cultures, with controversial findings (Cheng, 2002). The
frequency and intensity of individual types of EE vary widely across cultures, with the
proportion of high EE among relatives of individuals with schizophrenia defined by the Western
operational criteria ranging from 8% in rural India (Wig et.al, 1987b) to 67% in urban USA
(Vaughn et.al, 1984). The finding that a very low proportion (4%) of the relatives of people with
schizophrenia in the Indian study was rated high in emotional over-involvement was attributed to
issues of inter-rater reliability resulting from training problems (Wig et.al, 1987a). A
corresponding figure of 7.0% was reported in China (Phillips & Xiong, 1995). The authors
commented that the Western criteria for high emotional over-involvement are inappropriate in
Asian culture, where most unmarried persons with schizophrenia stay with their parents. This
may bring about much more contact between patients and their parents, leading to greater
exposure to EE in Chinese and other non-Western societies than in their Western counterparts. It
is known that Asian families are parent-child dominated, rather than husband-wife dominated as
observed in Western societies (Hsu, 1973). Unlike the Western family, in which the autonomy
and independence of children are stressed, in the Asian family children's obedience and
acceptance of parents' criticism, and interdependence between generations, are encouraged. The
implications of such differential exposure to EE may deserve further inquiry (Cheng, 2002).
Psychosocial intervention to reduce expressed emotion
Family-focused psychosocial interventions primarily emphasize on reducing the levels of
EE of caregivers by enhancing their knowledge about the illness. Family psychoeducation (FP)
aims at reducing re-hospitalization by controlling the relapse of symptoms and helping patient
adhere to the treatment, and it also aims to reduce the distress faced by family caregivers and
improve patient-family relations and communication patterns. FP includes education to the
patient and caregiver about illness, crisis management, problem-solving skills, clarifying myths
and misconceptions, and emotional support. The length and duration of the intervention vary
across the cultures. It depends upon the person’s socioeconomic, education, and domicile status
(Venkatasubramanian & Amaresha, 2012).
Conclusion
Caring for a person with schizophrenia is highly challenging, and it might result in a
negative emotional atmosphere in the patient’s family which can cause not only relapse of
symptoms and re-hospitalization, but significantly affect the course of the illness. The symptoms
displayed by the patient influence the caregiver’s EE and this, in turn, influences symptom
relapse in patients. Hence, treatment should attempt at a holistic, multidisciplinary, bio-
psychosocial approach which addresses all dimensions of the patient and his/her family
environment.
References
Cheng, A. T. (2002). Expressed emotion: a cross-culturally valid concept? The British Journal of
Psychiatry, 181(6), 466-467. doi:10.1192/bjp.181.6.466
Expressed emotion in schizophrenia. (1992). The Lancet, 340(8826), 1007-1008.
doi:10.1016/0140-6736(92)93014-e
Hsu, F. L. K. (1973) Kinship is the key. The Center Magazine, 11(6), 4 -14.
Hooley, J. M. (1998). Expressed Emotion and Locus of Control. The Journal of Nervous &
Mental Disease, 186(6), 374-378. doi:10.1097/00005053-199806000-00009
Leff J. (2000) Expressed emotion: Measuring relationships. In: Harris T, editor. Where inner and
outer worlds meet: Psychosocial research in the tradition of George W Brown. London:
Routledge; p. 97-110.
Phillips, M. R. & Xiong, W. (1995) Expressed emotion in mainland China: Chinese families
with schizophrenic patients. International Journal of Mental Health, 24, 54-75.
Vaughn, C. E., Snyder, K. S., Jones, S., et al (1984) Family factors in schizophrenic relapse:
replication in California of British research on expressed emotion. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 41, 1169 -1177.
Venkatasubramanian, G., & Amaresha, A. (2012). Expressed Emotion in Schizophrenia: An
Overview. Indian Journal of Psychological Medicine, 34(1), 12. doi:10.4103/0253-
7176.96149
Wearden, A. J., Tarrier, N., Barrowclough, C., Zastowny, T. R., & Rahill, A. A. (2000). A
review of expressed emotion research in health care. Clinical Psychology Review, 20(5),
633-666. doi:10.1016/s0272-7358(99)00008-2
Wig, N. N., Menon, D. K., Bedi, H., et al (1987a) Expressed emotion and schizophrenia in north
India. I: Cross-cultural transfer of ratings of relatives' expressed emotion. British Journal
of Psychiatry, 151, 156 -160.
Wig, N. N., Menon, D. K., Bedi, H., et al (1987b) Expressed emotion and schizophrenia in north
India. II: Distribution of expressed emotion components among relatives of schizophrenic
patients in Aarhus and Chandigarh. British Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 160 -165.
Zubin, J., & Spring, B. (1977). Vulnerability: A new view of schizophrenia. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 86(2), 103-126. doi:10.1037//0021-843x.86.2.103

You might also like