Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 4

Does ‘‘Science’’ Make You Moral?

The Effects of Priming


Science on Moral Judgments and Behavior
Christine Ma-Kellams*, Jim Blascovich
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of California Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, California, United States of America

Abstract
Background: Previous work has noted that science stands as an ideological force insofar as the answers it offers to a variety
of fundamental questions and concerns; as such, those who pursue scientific inquiry have been shown to be concerned
with the moral and social ramifications of their scientific endeavors. No studies to date have directly investigated the links
between exposure to science and moral or prosocial behaviors.

Methodology/Principal Findings: Across four studies, both naturalistic measures of science exposure and experimental
primes of science led to increased adherence to moral norms and more morally normative behaviors across domains. Study
1 (n = 36) tested the natural correlation between exposure to science and likelihood of enforcing moral norms. Studies 2
(n = 49), 3 (n = 52), and 4 (n = 43) manipulated thoughts about science and examined the causal impact of such thoughts on
imagined and actual moral behavior. Across studies, thinking about science had a moralizing effect on a broad array of
domains, including interpersonal violations (Studies 1, 2), prosocial intentions (Study 3), and economic exploitation (Study
4).

Conclusions/Significance: These studies demonstrated the morally normative effects of lay notions of science. Thinking
about science leads individuals to endorse more stringent moral norms and exhibit more morally normative behavior. These
studies are the first of their kind to systematically and empirically test the relationship between science and morality. The
present findings speak to this question and elucidate the value-laden outcomes of the notion of science.

Citation: Ma-Kellams C, Blascovich J (2013) Does ‘‘Science’’ Make You Moral? The Effects of Priming Science on Moral Judgments and Behavior. PLoS ONE 8(3):
e57989. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0057989
Editor: Luis M. Martinez, CSIC-Univ Miguel Hernandez, Spain
Received October 26, 2012; Accepted January 31, 2013; Published March 6, 2013
Copyright: ß 2013 Ma-Kellams, Blascovich. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which
permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Funding: The authors have no support or funding to report.
Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
* E-mail: christinema@fas.harvard.edu

Introduction ‘‘followers’’. On the other hand, apart from the model of the
scientific method of acquiring information about the world, we
Science has stood as a powerful force in shaping human contend that there is a lay image or notion of ‘‘science’’ that is
civilization and behavior. As both an ideological system and a associated with concepts of rationality, impartiality, fairness,
method for acquiring information about the world, it offers technological progress, and ultimately, the idea that we are to
explanations for the origins of the physical universe and answers to use these rational tools for the mutual benefit of all people in
a variety of other fundamental questions and concerns [1]. Past society [10]. Philosophers and historians have noted that scientific
research has noted that personal values influence both the inquiry began to flourish when Western society moved from one
questions that are asked and the methods used in arriving at the centered on religious notions of God’s will to one in which the
answers; as such, scientists have often been concerned with the rational mind served as the primary means to understand and
moral and social ramifications of their scientific endeavors [2,3]. improve our existence [10]. As such, the notion of science contains
Not surprisingly, the general consensus is that science is value- in it the broader moral vision of a society in which rationality is
laden [4–8]. However, no studies to date have directly investigated used for the mutual benefit of all.
the link between exposure to science and moral or prosocial We predict that this notion of science as part of a broader moral
behaviors. Here, we empirically examined the effects of thinking vision of society facilitates moral and prosocial judgments and
about science on moral judgments and behavior. behaviors. Consistent with the notion that science plays a key role
It is important to note that ‘‘science’’ is multi-faceted construct in the moral vision of a society of mutual benefit, scholars have
that takes on distinct forms. On the one hand, the scientific style of long argued that science’s systematic approach to studying causes
thinking employed by scientists is unusual, difficult, and uncom- and consequences allows for more informed opinions about
mon [9]. Although science can serve as a belief system, it is distinct questions of good and evil [11], and many have argued that the
from other belief systems (e.g., religion) insofar as its counterin- classic scientific ethos stands as an ethically neutral, but morally
tuitive nature and the degree to which it does not rely on universal, normative, set of principles that guides scientific inquiry [12]. We
automatic, unconscious cognitive systems [9]; as a consequence, contend that the same scientific ethos that serves to guide
relative to other belief systems like religion, science has few explicit

PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 1 March 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 3 | e57989


Science, Moral Judgments and Behavior

empirical inquiries also facilitates the enforcement of moral norms to receive either a science or control prime. The primes involved a
more broadly. series of sentence unscrambling tasks based on the materials used
by Shariff and Norenzayan [14]. Both primes involved ten sets of
Methods five scrambled words, from which participants had to choose four
in order to form a complete sentence. For those in the science
The ethics committee at the Department of Psychology, condition, half of the sentences contained the key words: logical,
University of California, Santa Barbara, specifically approved this hypothesis, laboratory, scientists, and theory. Participants in the
study. All participants provided written informed consent. control condition completed a similar prime except that all the
sentence scrambles contained neutral words (e.g., ‘‘shoes give
Participants replace old the’’; ‘‘more paper it once do’’).
Study 1. 48 undergraduates (18 men and 30 women ranged
18 to 24 years, mean age = 19.11, SD = 1.34) from the University Study 2: Interpersonal Violation
of California, Santa Barbara’s research participation pool were In Study 2, all participants read the same vignette about date
recruited and received course credit for participation. rape and completed the same moral judgment rating used in Study
Study 2. 33 undergraduates (16 men and 17 women ranged 1.
from 18 to 22 years, mean age = 18.67, SD = 1.02) from the
University of California, Santa Barbara’s research participation
Study 3: Prosocial Intentions
pool were recruited and received course credit for participation.
In Study 3, participants completed a prosocial intentions
Study 3. 32 volunteers (16 men and 16 women ranged from
measure [15]. Participants indicated the likelihood of engaging
18 to 28, mean age = 20.61, SD = 2.01) from the greater Santa
in each of several behaviors in the following month, including
Barbara county were recruited via a variety of means, including
prosocial activities (donating to charity, giving blood, volunteering)
word of mouth and online appeals.
and distractor activities (attending a party, going on vacation,
Study 4. 43 participants (15 men and 28 women from 18 to
seeing a movie); the order of activities was randomly presented.
22 years, mean age = 19.35, SD = 1.04) from the University of
California, Santa Barbara’s research participation pool were
recruited and received course credit for participation. Study 4: Economic Exploitation
In Study 4, participants completed a behavioral measure of
economic exploitation post-experimental manipulation. Partici-
Design and Procedure
pants played an economics dictator game modeled after the
Across four studies, we investigated whether science promotes procedures used by Shariff and Norenzayan [14]. Participants
moral or prosocial behavior. Morality is used broadly throughout were given five one-dollar bills, and told that their job was to
this paper and refers globally to a wide range of evaluations and divide the money between themselves and an anonymous other
behaviors that include wrongness, appropriateness, and other participant. Participants were told that they could keep the
judgments. While we acknowledge that these specific evaluations amount of money they allocated to themselves, and that the other
and behaviors are not identical, we contend that it is nevertheless participant would receive the remaining amount, if any. Upon
useful to rely on a commonsense notion of morality that completion of the study, all participants were debriefed and
encompasses all such behaviors. Study 1 used naturalistic measures received the five dollars as a gift, regardless of their allocation
of exposure to and belief in science and tested whether it predicted decision in the economics game.
the likelihood of enforcing moral norms. Studies 2–4 manipulated
thoughts about science and examined the causal impact of such Results
thoughts on both imagined (Studies 2, 3) as well as actual moral
behavior (Study 4). Across studies, we examined the effects of Study 1: Interpersonal Violations
science on a broad array of domains, including interpersonal Prior to data analysis, participants’ field of study was coded as
violations (Studies 1, 2), prosocial intentions (Study 3), and either a science (e.g. biology, chemistry, physics, psychology) or a
economic exploitation (Study 4). non-science field (e.g., art, communication, history, languages/
literature, music, sociology, theater).
Materials Gender was not related to any of the variables of interest (all p’s
..14), so it will not be discussed. A point-biserial correlation was
Study 1: Interpersonal Violations computed for the relationship between field of study as a predictor
In Study 1, participants read a date rape vignette [13] about of moral judgment. Studying science was positively correlated with
John and Sally, two acquaintances who are out on a date. After both greater moral condemnation of the date rape act (i.e.,
John drives Sally home, Sally invites him in for a drink; afterwards, studying science, relative to studying a non-science field, was
John engages in non-consensual sex with her. After reading the associated with rating the act of date rape as more wrong), r = .36,
vignette, participants were asked judge the wrongness of John’s p = .011. Belief in science in general was also positively correlated
behavior (i.e., of forcing non-consensual sex with Sally) on a scale with moral condemnation of the date rape act (i.e., those who
from 1 (completely right) to 100 (completely wrong). Afterwards, reported greater belief in science rated the date rape as more
all participants answered questions regarding their concentrated wrong), r = .65, p,.001. Importantly, moral condemnation did not
field of study and the question ‘‘How much do you believe in correspond with the other demographic variables, religiosity or
science?’’ on a scale from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very much). In ethnicity (all p’s ..46).
addition, participants completed basic demographic information
(age, sex, whether or not they were religious, ethnicity). Study 2: Interpersonal Violation
There was no main effect of gender nor any gender by condition
Studies 2–4: Experimental Manipulations interactions on the dependent variable of interest in Study 2 (all p’s
Studies 2–4 relied on experimental manipulation of science- ..22), so gender will no longer be mentioned. In Study 2, those
related vs. control thoughts. Participants were randomly assigned primed with science responded more severely to the moral

PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 2 March 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 3 | e57989


Science, Moral Judgments and Behavior

transgression (i.e., condemned the act as more wrong; M = 95.95, noted that science is value-laden insofar as the extent to which
SD = 4.37) relative to those in the control condition (M = 81.57, personal values influence both the questions that are asked and the
SD = 5.09), F(1, 31) = 4.58, p = .040. methods used in arriving at the answers [2–8]. These findings
suggest that beyond these individual differences in previously-
Study 3: Prosocial Intentions formed values that scientists introduce to the process of scientific
Likewise in Study 3, there was no main effect of gender nor any investigation, the act of thinking about science itself produces
gender by condition interactions on the dependent variable of important psychological consequences.
interest. Those primed with science reported greater prosocial The present findings may also help elucidate the effects of
intentions (i.e., increased likelihood of donating to charity, giving subscribing to the broader ‘‘ethos’’ of science. Past scholars have
blood, and volunteering; M = 4.14, SD = 1.49) relative to those in argued that the classic scientific ethos stands as an ethically
the control condition (M = 3.44, SD = 0.98), F(1, 31) = 5.64, neutral, but morally normative, set of principles that guides
p = .024. scientific inquiry [12]. These findings suggest the same scientific
ethos that serves to guide empirical inquiries also facilitates the
Study 4: Economic Exploitation enforcement of moral norms more broadly.
There was a main effect of gender on the dependent variable of Our results should be considered in the light of a number of
interest, money allocated, F(1, 55) = 4.98, p = .030. Women limitations of our design. First, it is possible that a number of
allocated more money to themselves (M = 3.32, SD = 1.23) than additional factors may have accounted for the natural correlation
men (M = 2.35, SD = 1.32). However, no gender by condition between exposure to science and enforcement of moral norms in
interaction emerged, F(2, 55) = 1.22, p = .30. As predicted, those in Study 1. Although we accounted for, and cast strong doubt on, the
the science condition allocated less money to themselves (M = 2.71, confound of religiosity–i.e., the alternative explanation that greater
SD = 1.43) than those in the control condition (M = 2.84, religiosity predicts both less exposure to science and greater
SD = 1.11), t(41) = 2.06, p = .046. endorsement of moral norms against interpersonal violations–
there are nevertheless other factors that may have potentially
Discussion accounted for the observed relationship. Studies 2–4 serve to
address this limitation by relying on experimental primes of
Across the four studies presented here, we demonstrated the
science.
morally normative effects of thinking about science. Priming lay
Second, the present studies examined morality primarily in the
notions of science leads individuals to endorse more stringent
moral norms (Studies 1, 2), report greater prosocial intentions domains of harm/care (i.e., interpersonal violation–Studies 1, 2;
(Study 3), and exhibit more morally normative behavior (Study 4). prosocial behaviors–Study 3) and fairness (i.e., economic exploi-
The moralizing effects of science were observed both by using tation–Study 4). Existing frameworks regarding the foundations of
naturalistic measures of exposure to science (e.g., field of study) as moral judgments suggest that other moral concerns exist, including
well as laboratory manipulations of thought-accessibility, and authority/respect, ingroup/loyalty, and purity/sanctity [17]. It
emerged across a broad array of domains, including interpersonal remains unclear whether science would also exert a moral effect on
violations (Study 1), academic dishonesty (Studies 2), prosocial these additional domains of morality, and the boundary conditions
behaviors (Study 3), and economic exploitation (Study 4). of science’s moralizing outcomes remains an empirical question to
It is important to note that the primes used across all studies be tested in the future.
activated broad, general, lay notions of science rather than specific These limitations notwithstanding, these studies are the first of
scientific findings. The key words used the science primes (logical, their kind to systematically and empirically test the relationship
hypothesis, laboratory, scientists, and theory) were likely associated between science and morality. No studies to date have directly
with semantic notions of rationality, impartiality and progress– investigated the link between beliefs in science and moral or
notions that are a part of the broader moral view of science as a prosocial outcomes. The present findings speak to such questions
way of building a mutually beneficial society in which rational and elucidate the value-laden outcomes of science.
tools are used to improve the human condition. The moralizing
effects of priming this broad idea of science diverges from previous Acknowledgments
studies that have focused on the effects of activating specific
We are grateful to Jonathan Lanman of Queen’s University Belfast for his
scientific findings–for example, Vohs and Schooler’s finding that
insightful comments regarding an earlier version of this paper.
those exposed to scientific findings about humans lacking free will
were more likely to cheat [16].
Taken together, the present results provide support for the idea Author Contributions
that the study of science itself–independent of the specific Conceived and designed the experiments: CM. Performed the experi-
conclusions reached by scientific inquiries–holds normative ments: CM. Analyzed the data: CM. Contributed reagents/materials/
implications and leads to moral outcomes. Previous research has analysis tools: CM. Wrote the paper: CM JB.

References
1. Preston J, Epley N (2009) Science and God: An automatic opposition between 7. Fiske DW, Shweder RA (1986) Metatheory in social science. Chicago:
ultimate explanations. J Exp Soc Psych 45: 238–241. University of Chicago Press.
2. Howard GS (1985) The role of values in the science of psychology. Amer 8. Toulmin S (1953) The philosophy of science. New York, NY: Harper & Row.
Psychologist 40: 255–265. 9. McCauley RN (2011) Why religion is natural and science is not. Oxford,
3. Kurtines W, Alvarez M, Azmitia M (1990) Science and Morality: The Role of England: Oxford University Press.
Values in Science and the Scientific Study of Moral Phenomena. Psych Bull 103: 10. Taylor C (2004) Modern Social Imaginaries. Durham,. NC: Duke University
283–295. Press.
4. Becker L (1968) Is science moral? J Sci Relig 3: 335–342. 11. Thorndike EL (1940) Human nature and the social order. New York:
5. Bhaskar R (1975). A realist theory of science. Leeds, England: Leeds Books. Macmillan.
6. Bronowski J (1956) Science and human values. Higher Ed Qtrly 11: 26–42. 12. Merton R (1973) The Sociology of Science. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.

PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 3 March 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 3 | e57989


Science, Moral Judgments and Behavior

13. Emmers-Sommer T (2001) Sexual coercion and resistance. In Allen M, Preiss 15. Jordan J, Mullen E, Murnighan JK (2011). Striving for the moral self: the effects
RW, Gayle BM, Burrell N, editors. Interpersonal communication research: of recalling past moral actions on future moral behavior. Pers Soc Psychol Bull
advances through meta-analysis. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 37: 701–713.
Inc. 315–329. 16. Vohs K, Schooler J (2008) The value of believing in free will; encouraging a
14. Shariff AF, Norenzayan A (2007) God is watching you: Priming God concepts belief in determinism increases cheating. Psych Sci 19: 49–54.
increases prosocial behavior in an anonymous economic game. Psych Sci 18: 17. Graham J, Haidt J, Nosek B (2009) Liberals and conservatives use different sets
803–809. of moral foundations. J Pers Soc Psychol 96: 1029–1046.

PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 4 March 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 3 | e57989

You might also like