Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Journal of Gerontology: PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCES Copyright 2000 b\ The Gerontological Swii'lv of America

2000, Vol. 55B, No. I, P18-P26

Personality Traits and Suicidal Behavior and Ideation


in Depressed Inpatients 50 Years of Age and Older
Paul R. Duberstein, Yeates Conwell, Larry Seidlitz, Diane G. Denning, Christopher Cox, and Eric D. Caine

University of Rochester Medical Center, New York.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/55/1/P18/533273 by guest on 26 December 2022


Completed suicide may be the most preventable lethal complication of depressive disorders in older adults.
Identification of risk factors for suicidal behavior has therefore become a major public health priority. Using data col-
lected on SI depressed patients 50 years of age and older, we report analyses designed to determine the associations be-
tween the personality traits that constitute the Five Factor Model of personality and measures of suicidal behavior and
ideation. We hypothesized that low Extraversion would be associated with a lifetime history of attempted suicide, and
high Neuroticism would be associated with suicidal ideation. Results were generally consistent with the hypotheses. We
also observed a relationship between Openness to Experience and suicidal ideation. These findings suggest that long-
standing patterns of behaving, thinking, and feeling contribute to suicidal behavior and thoughts in older adults and
highlight the need to consider personality traits in crafting and targeting prevention strategies.

model of personality provides a relatively comprehensive cover-


D EPRESSIVE disorders in older adults are common
(Burvill, 1995; Lebowitz et al., 1997) and are associated
with increased all-cause mortality (Gallo, Rabins, Lyketos,
age of personality traits, it can be used to explore and generate
hypotheses about phenomena that have been relatively underin-
Tien, & Anthony, 1997; Penninx et al., 1999; Zubenko, vestigated or about which there is relatively little theorizing.
Mulsant, Sweet, Pasternak, & Tu, 1997). Completed suicide We are aware of no theory that makes explicit predictions
may be the most preventable lethal complication. Although the about the contributions of specific personality traits to specific
greatest number of suicides are committed by young adults, the dimensions of suicidal behavior in particular demographic and
rate increases throughout the lifecourse and peaks in 80-84 diagnostic groups. Most personality theories of suicidal behav-
year olds (Centers for Disease Control [CDC], 1996, 1999). ior lack the specificity warranted by the epidemiological data.
Recognizing the public health impact of completed suicide on For example, despite long-established age and gender differ-
individuals, families, and society, the United States Congress ences in suicidal behavior (Durkheim, 1897/1951; Monk, 1987),
passed resolutions in 1997 and 1998 declaring suicide preven- clinical writings (e.g., Buie & Maltsberger, 1989; Hendin, 1991)
tion a national priority (Congressional Record, 1997, 1998). have typically emphasized the role of hostility, independent of
The ultimate success of these resolutions will depend in part on age, gender, or any other demographic or contextual variable.
the identification of suicide risk factors and correlates. Research Use of an omnibus personality questionnaire grounded in the
aimed at identifying personality traits associated with suicidal FFM increases the likelihood that traits central to late-life suici-
behavior can contribute to prevention efforts by defining groups dal behavior are not overlooked, even if they are ignored in clin-
at high risk, before the development of a major depressive ical and theoretical writings. Indeed, the FFM may be construed
episode or an acute suicidal crisis. The identification of high- as hypothesis-generating. Proponents of the FFM argue that it
risk groups is therefore a critical component of the contempo- provides a fixed reference point from which to assess a variety
rary prevention research agenda (National Institutes of Health, of different scales (Costa & McCrae, 1992; Marshall, Wortman,
1998). Using data collected on a sample of depressed inpatients Vickers, Kusulas, & Hervig, 1994). It therefore overcomes a
50 years of age and older, we report analyses designed to perennial problem in personality psychology: Scales with dif-
determine the direction and strength of associations between the ferent labels measure the same trait, while those with the same
personality traits that constitute the Five Factor Model of per- label measure different traits.
sonality (Digman, 1990; John, 1990) and measures of suicidal Among others, Kagan (1994), McAdams (1994), and Block
behavior. (1995) offer less optimistic opinions of the FFM. Kagan (1994)
critiques its basic premises, including the scientific utility of a
The Five Factor Model (FFM) as a Hypothesis-Testing natural language approach to personality, self-report measures,
and Hypothesis-Generating Tool and factor-analysis itself. He ultimately concedes that, even
Based on decades of factor-analytic research on personality in though the five factors "omit too much information" and are
the natural lexicon and questionnaires, there is considerable "insufficiently differentiated... [they] do tell us something of
(Digman, 1990; John, 1990; McCrae & Costa, 1997), but not interest" (pp. 45-46). McAdams (1994) also takes issue with
complete (Cloninger, Svrakic, & Przybeck, 1993; Tellegen, the basic premises and criticizes trait assessments in general on
1985), agreement that personality attributes can be grouped the grounds that they fail to provide causal explanations for
along five major dimensions: Neuroticism, Extraversion, human behavior, disregard the conditional and contextual na-
Openness, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness. Because this ture of human experience, and fail to provide enough detailed

P18
PERSONALITY AND SUICIDE P19

information to predict specific behaviors in certain circum- Manton, 1986; Moscicki, 1989), but the risk of completed
stances. Block (1995) generally accepts the premises upon suicide increases (CDC, 1999). Following the logic of the cate-
which FFM research is based, though he is somewhat critical of gorical model, we examined the direction and strength of asso-
the "arbitrariness" (p. 189) of factor analysis and the over- ciations between each of the personality traits that constitute the
reliance on self- and peer-report data. He also raises a number FFM and specific variables related to (a) suicide attempts and
of technical concerns, such as the high intercorrelations among (b) suicidal ideation.
the ostensibly uncorrelated five factors. Still, the FFM has with-
stood criticism from those who share, and do not share, its basic The Present Study: Overview and Hypotheses
assumptions (Costa & McCrae, 1995; McCrae & Costa, 1997), The preceding sections point to the need for a study that
and it has proven useful in research on health outcomes in older measures a range of personality traits and distinguishes among

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/55/1/P18/533273 by guest on 26 December 2022


adults (Hooker, Frazier, & Monahan, 1994; Hooker, Monahan, putative categories of suicidal behavior. Data were collected on
Bowman, Frazier, & Shifren, 1998; Hooker, Monahan, Shifren, psychiatric inpatients with major depressive disorder, 50 years of
& Hutchinson, 1992). Those achievements may be sufficient age and older, about half of whom were men. This is a relatively
justification for its continued application to questions of public homogeneous group both diagnostically and demographically,
health significance pertaining to older adults. which should allay concern that relations between personality
and suicidal behavior may be attributable to major depression,
Continua versus Categories? gender, or age.
It is generally believed that one must want to die in order to We tested two hypotheses: (1) Suicide attempters are char-
think about killing oneself, just as one has to have some suici- acterized by low Extraversion, and (2) Suicidal ideation is
dal ideation before making a suicide attempt. And, of course, associated with high Neuroticism. Extraversion refers to pref-
one has to attempt in order to complete suicide. It is precisely erences for social interaction and the tendency to experience
this sort of overlap among suicide constructs that has led to the positive emotion (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Low Extraversion
assumption that suicidal behavior can be conceptualized along increases risk for suicide attempts in relatively younger sam-
a severity continuum, with absence of death ideation at one end, ples (Beautrais, Joyce, & Mulder, 1999; Roy, 1998). Although
completed suicide on the other, and death ideation, suicidal there have been some negative findings, low Extraversion has
ideation, and attempted suicide in the middle. Similarly, it has been empirically associated with poor social support (Krause,
been assumed that people can be "more or less" suicidal. Thus, Liang, & Keith, 1990; Von Dras & Siegler, 1997) and the use
it has been assumed that one's "suicidality" can be captured by of irrational and socially avoidant problem-solving strategies
a single, composite, dimensional variable. (Hooker et al., 1994), characteristics also associated with at-
The notion of a severity continuum has intuitive appeal. tempted suicide (Linehan, Chiles, Egan, Devine, & Laffaw,
Completed suicide is undoubtedly a more severe form of suici- 1986). With respect to the second hypothesis, Neuroticism
dal behavior than suicidal ideation. However, researchers study- refers to the disposition to experience negative affect, such as
ing groups of suicide ideators, suicide attempters, and com- sadness, anxiety, and self-consciousness. People who are high
pleted suicides may be examining categorically discrete in Neuroticism have a tendency to report more severe physical
populations, each characterized by a discrete set of risk factors, (Costa & McCrae, 1987) and depressive (Lyness, Duberstein,
reflecting distinct underlying personality traits or constituent King, Cox, & Caine, 1998) symptoms, one of which is suici-
cognitive, affective, and motivational processes. dal ideation. Given the paucity of previous research on person-
The number and nature of distinct suicidal populations have ality and suicidal behavior in older adults and our interest in
been debated for years (Linehan, 1986; Maris, 1992). This dis- generating novel hypotheses, it seemed premature to restrict
cussion must continue in order to identify and ultimately test our analyses to Neuroticism and Extraversion. We therefore
five of the most significant, yet implicit, assumptions in the explored the contributions of Openness, Agreeableness, and
severity continuum model. These include the notions that (a) re- Conscientiousness to late-life suicidal behavior. Including
search on attempted suicide may be a proxy for research on these three variables in the regression analyses also ensured
completed suicide; that is, conclusions about completed suicide more precise estimates of the effects of Neuroticism and
can be gleaned from studies of suicide attempters; (b) research Extraversion.
on suicidal ideation may substitute for research on attempted
suicide; (c) suicidal ideation is a clinical risk factor for attempted METHODS
suicide and completed suicide; (d) attempted suicide is a clinical
risk factor for completed suicide; and (e) the absence of reported Participants
suicidal ideation indicates decreased risk of attempted or com- Participants were drawn from a larger, ongoing, case-control
pleted suicide in a given population or study group. study of attempted suicide in major depressive disorder.
Whereas the severity continuum model implies shared de- Depressed inpatients 50 years of age and older who were
mographic risk factors across the continuum, a categorical admitted to the hospital following a suicide attempt were com-
model suggests that each putative category of suicidal behavior pared with similarly depressed age- (±5 years) and gender-
may have specific risk factors. The demographic data are gen- matched inpatients whose admissions were not precipitated by
erally consistent with the categorical model. Rates of attempted a suicide attempt. Although there is significant heterogeneity in
suicide are highest in young women (Kessler, Borges, & the prevalent diagnoses of young adult suicides, after age 50,
Walters, 1999), but it is older men who are at greatest risk for the psychiatric diagnoses associated with completed suicide be-
completed suicide (CDC, 1999). Similarly, rates of suicidal come increasingly homogeneous, and affective disorders are
ideation decrease throughout the lifecourse (Blazer, Bachar, & present in over 70% of cases (Conwell et al., 1996). Thus, by
P20 DUBERSTE1NETAL.

choosing 50 as the lower age limit for study entry, we are able Eleven patients (14.2%) had psychotic features. Slightly more
to control for affective disorder without excluding a large por- than half (n = 44; 54.4%) had at least one additional Axis I diag-
tion of people at risk for completed suicide. nosis. The most common comorbid Axis I diagnosis was alco-
The study was conducted at four teaching hospitals in the hol or substance abuse or dependence in full remission (n = 21,
northeastern United States (Rochester, NY), including two 25.9%). Dysthymia was present in about 12% of the sample (n
community hospitals, one tertiary care facility, and one aca- = \ 0). Somatoform disorders (n = 8), active alcohol/substance
demic medical center. Acknowledging that there are problems disorders (n — 8), panic disorder (« = 7), and phobias (n = 7)
inherent in any definition of "suicide attempt" (Beck & were each present in slightly less than 10% of the sample. Scores
Greenberg, 1971; O'Carroll et al., 1996), attempted suicide was on the Beck Hopelessness Scale (Beck, Weissman, Lester, &
defined as an intentional self-destructive act; an expressed wish Trexler, 1974) were elevated (M = 12.4, SD = 5.7), consistent
to die was not necessary.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/55/1/P18/533273 by guest on 26 December 2022


with scores obtained on an older, depressed outpatient sample
Recruitment procedures were as follows. Project coordina- (Hill, Gallagher, Thompson, & Ishida, 1988). Thirty-four of the
tors screened the records of all patients 50 years of age and 81 (41.9%) participants were admitted to the study following a
older admitted to the four hospitals or seen in psychiatric con- suicide attempt; 20 of these participants had made previous at-
sultation on the medical and surgical services following a sui- tempts. Eleven of the 47 patients (13.6%) whose admissions
cide attempt. Because the amount and type of comorbidity were were not precipitated by a suicide attempt had previously at-
important variables that may have distinguished groups, comor- tempted suicide. Excluding the suicide attempts that immedi-
bid medical or psychiatric conditions were not exclusionary ately preceded and precipitated hospitalization, dates of the most
criteria if the diagnosis of major depressive disorder was sus- recent previous suicide attempt ranged from less than 1 week to
pected. Following approval from the patient's attending physi- more than 5 years prior to admission, with the majority occur-
cian, a member of the research team approached patients to ring more than 2 years prior to admission.
obtain their informed consent to be interviewed by one of the
project coordinators (all of whom have masters degrees), and to Materials
complete self-report questionnaires. Psychiatric diagnoses were
made on the basis of an integration of all data sources accord- NEO-Pl-R.—T\ie NEO-PI-R (Costa & McCrae, 1992) is a
ing to DSM-TII-R criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 240-item measure of the five personality dimensions consis-
1987) in a consensus conference attended by members of the tently identified in factor-analytic studies: Neuroticism,
research team. Potential participants were excluded if the labo- Extraversion, Openness to Experience, Agreeableness, and
ratory work-up, physical examination findings, and the tempo- Conscientiousness. An extensive literature supports its reliabil-
ral relation of depressive symptoms to the course of associated ity and validity. Coefficient alphas for the five scales range from
physical illness or substance exposure suggested that the pa- .86 to .92 (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Longitudinal studies con-
tient's mood syndrome was etiologically related to a specific ducted over periods of up to 7 years have frequently reported
medical condition or substance exposure. Of the 87 participants test-retest correlation coefficients greater than .6, attesting to
who completed the NEO Personality Inventory-Revised (NEO- the stability of these five domains (Costa & McCrae, 1992).
PI-R), only 2 suicide attempters and 4 nonattempters were sub- Although the 60-item NEO-FFI has been used in gerontology
sequently excluded because they met criteria for organic mood research (e.g., Hooker et al., 1994) and in research on depressed
disorder; 4 other suicide attempters and 1 nonattempter who outpatients (Bagby et al., 1998), we are unaware of any study
met criteria for that disorder did not complete the NEO-PI-R. that has used the 240-item NEO-PI-R with older, depressed
All participants (n = 81; 34 [42%1 men, 47 [58%] women) inpatients.
who completed the NEO-PI-R and met inclusion criteria were
included in the analyses; 14 others completed the NEO-FFI (60 History and number of suicide attempts.—For decades, the
item short form; Costa & McCrae, 1992), and 50 (34.5%) re- standard approach to research on personality and attempted
fused or were unable to complete any personality inventory de- suicide involved a static group comparison of individuals seek-
spite their participation in other phases of the research and our ing health care following a suicide attempt with individuals
assiduous efforts to increase the return rate. Participants in the seeking care for another reason ("nonattempters"). This ap-
larger study from which these analyses were conducted were, proach is limited primarily because a portion of those de-
on average, about 6 years older, and scored nearly 2 points scribed as nonattempters have attempted suicide in the past. As
lower on the Mini Mental State Exam (Folstein, Folstein, & a general principle, when personality traits increase risk for
McHugh, 1975; M = 25.7, SD = 4.1; M = 27.5, SD = 2.5). The certain adverse health outcomes, such as attempted suicide,
sample was predominantly Caucasian (« = 78; 96.3%), with a risk refers to the entire lifecourse and is not confined to the pe-
mean (SD) age of 61.3 (9.6) years. The age range was 50 to 87 riod of time during which subjects are enrolled in a study.
years. Thirty-three (40.7%) participants were married, 21 (25.9 Thus, in the present study we examined the relationship be-
%) were separated/divorced, and 34 (42%) lived alone at the tween personality and (a) lifetime suicide attempter status, and
time of admission. One-third of the sample (n = 27, 33.3%) was (b) number of suicide attempts.
employed, and slightly less than one-third (30.8%) was either Our data on the number of suicide attempts were based, in
on disability (n = 14) or unemployed (n = 1 1 ) . Thirty-seven part, on participants' responses to the questions: "How many
(45.6%) were in the midst of their first episode of major depres- times all together in your life have you actually done something
sion. Slightly less than half (n - 40,49.4%) was diagnosed with with the intention of taking your life?" and "How many suicide
severe major depression (American Psychiatric Association, attempts have you made in your life?" With respect to the latter
1987); 27 cases were judged to be moderate, and 3 were mild. question, past self-destructive behaviors were coded as suicide
PERSONALITY AND SUICIDE P21

attempts if participants labeled the behavior as a suicide attempt validity in this group of older inpatients with major depression,
even if they disavowed an expressed intention to die. Previous some members of the research team assessed psychiatric inpa-
psychiatric and medical charts were reviewed in an effort to tients while others independently interviewed family infor-
gather additional data on the number of suicide attempts. mants (n = 26 pairs). Kappa coefficients for the diagnoses of
Discrepancies between the number of self-reported suicide at- any substance use disorder, affective disorder, and their comor-
tempts and chart-documented suicide attempts were resolved bidity ranged from 0.61 to 0.75.
by recording the higher number documented or reported.
Mini Mental State Exam (MMSE).—The MMSE measures
Scale of Suicidal Ideation (SSI).—Two outcome measures cognitive function (Folstein et al., 1975). Scores can range from
were extracted from this 19-item, observer-rated measure: (a) 0 to 30. The MMSE score is not used as an inclusion criterion;

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/55/1/P18/533273 by guest on 26 December 2022


presence of suicidal ideation and (b) presence of death ideation rather, it serves solely as a means of characterizing cognitive
(Beck, Kovacs, & Weissman, 1979). Questions pertained to the function.
week prior to the interview or the interval between the suicide
attempt and interview, whichever was shorter. Thus, for those Analytic Plan and Overview of Presentation of Results
hospitalized following a suicide attempt, the SSI provides data First, descriptive statistics and intercorrelations among study
on the presence of suicidal and death ideation following a sui- variables are reported. Kendall's tau and t statistics are pre-
cide attempt. The first three items concern the wish to live, the sented because many of the endpoints were binary or counts
wish to die, and the extent to which one wish outweighs the with skewed distributions. Next, the results of a series of regres-
other. The presence of death ideation was operationally denned sion analyses are reported. Binary endpoints were analyzed by
as a score of 1 or greater in response to these three items, mean- multiple logistic regression. Goodness of fit was examined by
ing that the wish to die outweighed the wish to live. Items 4 and using the Hosmer-Lemeshow (1989) test. Continuous end-
5 concern thoughts of self-destruction, either by active (e.g., points were analyzed by multiple linear regression analysis,
shoot yourself) or passive (e.g., not taking medicine that is which included an examination of residuals as a check on the
needed to survive, refusing to nourish oneself) means. The pres- required assumptions of normally distributed errors with con-
ence of suicidal ideation was operationally defined as an affir- stant variance. If the residual analysis indicated a violation of
mative response to either Question 4 or Question 5. The final assumptions, then the data were logarithmically transformed
14 questions, which concern the frequency, duration, and the and standardized to behave like normal deviates (Chatterjee &
participant's attitude toward suicidal thoughts, were adminis- Hadi, 1988). Cases with standardized residuals greater than 3 in
tered only to those who reported suicidal ideation in response absolute value were excluded as outliers from the regression
to Items 4 or 5. Participants obtain relatively high SSI scores if analysis. Counts and endpoints with skewed distributions were
they report that they "accept" the suicidal thoughts, have little analyzed by Poisson regression (McCullagh & Nelder, 1989),
control over them, have little concern about family, religion, or which also included an outlier analysis. Standardized residuals
other potential deterrents to suicide, have thought extensively are based on components of the Pearson chi-square statistic for
about how to kill themselves, written suicide notes, or changed goodness of fit of the model. All analyses were adjusted for age
wills or life insurance policies. Severity of suicidal ideation was and gender. Predictors included age, gender, and each of the
operationally defined as the total score on the SSI. The SSI has five traits that constitute the FFM: Neuroticism, Extraversion,
established reliability and concurrent validity (Beck et al., Openness to Experience, Agreeableness, and Conscientious-
1979). Coefficient alpha for the current study (ideators only) ness. All reported p values are two-tailed.
was .91.
RESULTS
Spectrum of Suicidal Behavior Scale (SSB).—Project coordi- Descriptive statistics and zero-order correlations (Kendall's
nators used this 5-point ordinal scale (Pfeffer, Stokes, & tau) between the NEO factors and continuous outcomes (SSI
Shindledecker, 1991) to rate the participants' most serious sui- and number of suicide attempts) are presented in Table 1. There
cidal behavior over the past month. Thus, the SSB and SSI were slight, downward trends with age in Neuroticism and
cover different time frames (month prior to hospitalization vs. Openness. The SSI score was positively correlated with
week prior to interview). Participants were rated a 1 (nonsuici- Neuroticism. t tests were conducted to examine the unadjusted
dal) if there was "no evidence of any self-destructive or suicidal relationships between the personality variables and dichoto-
thoughts or actions," a 2 if there is evidence of suicidal ideation, mous endpoints. Table 2 shows that women, suicide ideators,
a 3 if they made a suicidal threat, a 4 if they made a mild sui- and death ideators obtained higher Neuroticism scores, and
cide attempt, or a 5 if they made a serious suicide attempt. In those who had attempted suicide obtained lower scores on
the present study, the SSB served primarily as a measure of sui- Extraversion. Of the 10 intercorrelations among the 5 NEO
cidal ideation in the month prior to admission. For analytic pur- variables, five had absolute values less than .07; the highest
poses, we therefore dichotomized SSB scores (1 vs. other) and value was .46. Therefore, multicollinearity did not appear to
estimated its reliability by means of the kappa-coefficient (K = pose any problems for the regression analyses.
.54) using chart documentation of preadmission suicidal behav-
ior as the criterion. Presence and Number of Suicide Attempts
The first logistic regression sought to determine whether the
Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R.—This instru- personality variables were associated with having made a sui-
ment was used to establish Axis I psychiatric diagnoses cide attempt. As shown in Table 3, those who obtained lower
(Spitzer, Williams, & Gibbons, 1987). In order to examine its Extraversion scores were more likely to have made a lifetime
P22 DUBERSTEIN ET AL.

Table 1. Unadjusted Relationship Between NEO-PI and Continuous Variables: Kendall's Tau
Continuous Variable N M SD Ne Ex Op Ag Co
Age 81 61.30 9.6 -.29*** .04 -.17* -.06 .07
Number of lifetime SA 81 0.85 1.6 .19* -.19* .04 -.04 -.15
Total SSI score 81 7.52 10.3 2g*** -.15 .11 -.04 -.14

Ne = Neuroticism; Ex = Extraversion; Op = Openness to Experience; Ag = Agreeableness; Co = Conscientiousness; SA= Suicide Attempts; SSI = Scale for
Suicidal Ideation.
*/><•05. ***p< .001.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/55/1/P18/533273 by guest on 26 December 2022


Table 2. Unadjusted Relationships Between NEO-PI and Dichotomous Variables: /-Statistics
Categorical Variable Ne Ex Op Ag Co
Gender (« = 47 women, 34 men) 1 .97'* -0.15 0.85 1.91 -1.77
Lifetime SA (n = 45 yes, 36 no) 1 .69" -2.03* -0.44 0.14 1.31
Suicidal ideation - SSB (n = 61 yes, 20 no) 2.66** -1.50 1.48 -1.23 -1.60
Suicidal ideation - SSI (n = 32 yes, 49 no) 3.11*** -1.06 1.80 -1.21 -1.64
Death ideation - SSI (n = 53 yes, 28 no) 3.01*** -1.24 1.76 -0.86 -0.69

Lifetime SA = participants with a lifetime history of at least one suicide attempt were contrasted with all others. Suicidal ideation - SSB = participants who scored
a 1 (absence of suicidal ideation or suicide attempt in the month prior to hospitalization) were contrasted with all others; Suicidal ideation - SSI = participants who
reported suicidal ideation in the week prior to interview (scored a 1 or higher on the Scale for Suicidal Ideation) were contrasted with all others; Death ideation - SSI
= participants who acknowledged that their wish to die outweighed their wish to live in the week prior to interview (scored a 1 or higher on the death ideation items
of the Scale for Suicidal Ideation) were contrasted with all others; Ne = Neuroticism; Ex = Extraversion; Op = Openness to Experience; Ag = Agreeableness: Co =
Conscientiousness. *p < .05; **p < .01 ;***/}< .001. All rf/s = 79, except where otherwise noted.
"Women scored higher than men.
h
Unequal variance (p < .05), there was greater heterogeneity among those with a lifetime SA; d/'tbr t-test = 78.

Table 3. Regression Results

Significant
Predicted Variable Analysis Model Predictor(s) Coeff SE X:(D p value
Lifetime SA 1 Logistic Extraversion -.032 .016 4.43 .035
Number of lifetime S A (outliers removed) 2 Poisson Extraversion -.026 .007 14.06 .0002
No suicidal ideas-SSB 3 Logistic Openness -.054 .025 5.65 .02
Agreeableness +.038 .021 3.73 .05
Suicidal ideation-SSI 4 Logistic Age -.087 .038 6.38 .01
Death ideation-SSI 5 Logistic Openness +.038 .021 3.87 .05
Neuroticism +.037 .018 4.49 .02
Lifetime SA = participants with a lifetime history of at least one suicide attempt were contrasted with all others; Suicidal ideation - SSB = participants who scored
a 1 (absence of suicidal ideation or suicide attempt in the month prior to hospitalization) were contrasted with all others; Suicidal ideation - SSI = participants who
reported suicidal ideation in the week prior to interview (scored a 1 or higher on the Scale for Suicidal Ideation) were contrasted with all others; Death ideation - SSI
= participants who acknowledged that their wish to die outweighed their wish to live in the week prior to interview (scored a 1 or higher on the death ideation items
of the Scale for Suicidal Ideation) were contrasted with all others; CoetT = coefficient.

suicide attempt (Table 3, Analysis 1). The Hosmer-Lemeshow vious analysis and its association with the number of lifetime
goodness of fit was not significant, x2 (8) = 9.90, p = .27, indi- suicide attempts in the unadjusted analyses (Table 2). Next, we
cating a satisfactory fit. Next, we examined predictors of the conducted a Poisson regression to predict the number of suicide
number of previous suicide attempts using Poisson regression. attempts among those with a lifetime history of attempted sui-
Higher Neuroticism and lower Extraversion emerged as signifi- cide (n = 45; analyses not shown). No significant predictors
cant predictors; however, 4 participants were outliers. In each emerged, but there was a trend for those higher in Neuroticism
case, the predicted number of suicide attempts was lower than to make more attempts, x2 (1) = 3.17, p = .07.
the actual number. All 4 had at least two Axis I diagnoses; 3 of
the 4 had psychotic features. Concerned that the nature and in- Suicidal and Death Ideation
terpretation of our findings may have been unduly influenced Two groups were constructed from the SSB data, those who
by this relatively small group reporting numerous suicide at- reported no suicidal ideation or behavior in the past month and
tempts, we removed the outliers and conducted another Poisson those who reported suicidal ideas or made a suicide attempt.
regression. The results (Table 3, Analysis 2) partially duplicated Table 3 (Analysis 3) shows that, in a logistic regression predict-
the previous analysis. Again, Extraversion was a strong predic- ing absence of suicidal ideation, low Openness and high
tor, but Neuroticism was not, despite its significance in the pre- Agreeableness emerged as significant predictors. This contrasts
PERSONALITY AND SUICIDE P23

with the unadjusted analyses, which showed a relationship be- Although our findings are consistent with the notion that
tween Neuroticism and the SSB score (Table 2). The Hosmer- Extraversion is associated with lifetime suicide attempter status,
Lemeshow for the multiple regression was nonsignificant, \2 it is possible that other personality traits (e.g., low Openness,
(8) = 10.75, p = .22, indicating a reasonable fit. When we di- high Neuroticism) are associated with the lethality of attempts.
chotomized the SSI score (0 vs. > 0) and created two groups, This idea could be examined in a study that includes a suffi-
suicide ideators and nonideators, the logistic regression (Analy- cient sample of individuals whose suicide attempts lead to se-
sis 4) yielded one significant predictor (age), in contrast to the vere medical complications.
unadjusted analyses, which implicated Neuroticism in suicidal Second, as hypothesized, Neuroticism is associated with sui-
ideation (Table 2). We also conducted a linear regression with cidal ideation. However, whereas significant relationships
the total score on the SSI as the dependent variable. Again, only between Neuroticism and measures of suicidal ideation were

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/55/1/P18/533273 by guest on 26 December 2022


age was associated with that outcome, F(l,73) - 4.56,p = .04, obtained in all three univariate analyses (SSB, SSI-Suicidal
but there was a trend for those higher in Openness to obtain Ideation, SSI-Death Ideation), the regression analyses told a
higher SSI scores, F( 1,73) = 3.25, p = .07. Next, we created more complex story. Neuroticism was a strong predictor of SSI-
two groups, those who reported death ideation in response to Death Ideation in these analyses, but was not associated with
Items 1-3 of the SSI and those who did not. Table 3 (Analysis the other two suicide ideation variables. Thus, the associations
5) shows that higher Neuroticism and higher Openness between Neuroticism and suicidal ideation in univariate analy-
emerged as significant predictors of death ideation. The ses may be due in part to its associations with other traits, par-
Hosmer-Lemoshow for the overall model was nonsignificant, ticularly Agreeableness and Openness.
X2 (8) = 8.33, p = .40, indicating a satisfactory fit. Third, patients low in self-reported Openness are less likely to
report suicidal ideation. Perhaps patients low in Openness are
DISCUSSION protected from suicidal ideation, and consistent with the severity
These findings reinforce the notion that personality traits continuum model, they are less vulnerable to completed suicide.
ought to be seriously considered as potential risk factors for However, we have previously reported that low informant-
late-life suicidal behavior and ideation. Even in this demo- reported Openness may be a risk factor for completed suicide
graphically and diagnostically homogenous group of psychi- (Duberstein, Conwell, & Caine, 1994). How can this discrep-
atric inpatients, personality traits were important predictors of ancy be reconciled? Perhaps the apparently discrepant findings
suicide attempts and suicidal ideation. Although there were can be ascribed to methodological differences. Self-reported
some negative findings, regression analyses supported hypothe- Openness and informant-reported Openness may not be compa-
sized associations between Extraversion and attempted suicide rable constructs in older, depressed persons. This is unlikely,
and between Neuroticism and suicidal ideation. These analyses given the extensive literature supporting the relationship between
also generated a novel hypothesis linking Openness to suicidal self- and informant-reported data (Costa & McCrae, 1992), even
ideation. in depressed outpatients (Bagby et ah, 1998). In our own sample
of 57 depressed inpatients 50 years of age and older, the relation-
Substantive Findings ship between self- and informant Openness (intraclass correla-
Three findings are especially noteworthy. First, higher tion coefficient = .49) was moderately strong. Acknowledging
Extraversion distinguishes people who have never made an at- that other methodological explanations may account for the ap-
tempt from those who have. Extraversion is positively associ- parently discrepant findings, substantive hypotheses must also be
ated with positive affect (Clark, Watson, & Mineka, 1994) and entertained. People with major depression who are low in
increased social support (e.g., Von Dras & Siegler, 1997), and Openness may be at increased risk for completed suicide precisely
negatively correlated with trait, but not state, hopelessness because they are less likely to feel, or report feeling, suicidal. This
(Young et ah, 1996). Extraverted individuals may be less likely hypothesis represents a genuine challenge to the severity contin-
to engage in suicidal behavior even in the midst of a depressive uum model. Affective muting may be adaptive at earlier points in
episode because they are more likely to recruit and affectively the lifecourse, but could also increase risk for late-life completed
benefit from friendships and family relations, perhaps as a re- suicide (Clark, 1993; Duberstein, 1995). Further research on
sult of better social skills (cf. Zweig & Hinrichsen, 1993). Openness and suicidal behavior is warranted, given the obvious
Suicide attempts among those who are low in Extraversion may implications for risk detection and prevention.
reflect a tendency to take matters into one's own hands, rather
than attempt to recruit help from others. Strategies for treating Limitations and Strengths
young adult suicide attempters (Linehan, 1993) have been in- It cannot be assumed that the present findings generalize to
formed by data linking personal concerns (Linehan et ah, 1986) other diagnostic and demographic subgroups. Participants who
or personality dimensions (Rudd, Joiner, & Rajab, 1996) with completed the NEO-PI-R were about 6 years younger and may
suicidal behavior, but similar data on older adults are rare. have had slightly better cognitive function than those who did
Future research aimed at identifying the mediators of the rela- not complete the 240-item inventory. Nor can it be assumed
tionship between Extraversion and attempter status may lead to that these findings generalize to the small fraction of depressed
interventions designed to decrease the risk of nonfatal suicidal patients with organic mood disorder. The sample size is also
behavior. This is important in part because suicide attempts relatively small for personality research, so caution must be ex-
may exacerbate the physical morbidity (Gallo et ah, 1997; Katz, ercised, especially in interpreting negative findings. Finally, it
1996 ) and mortality risks (Gallo et ah, 1997; Penninx et ah, must be acknowledged that the results may have differed had
1999; Zubenko et ah, 1997) frequently associated with late-life other age cutoffs been used as the lower limit of study entry
depressive disorders. (e.g., 60 or 65).
P24 DUBERSTE1NETAL.

These limitations must be weighed against the study's This study uncovered the possibility that different personal-
strengths, chief of which are its public health significance and ity variables are associated with attempted suicide and suicidal
its foray into new territory. No previous study has applied a ideation, with Extraversion associated with the former and
comprehensive personality taxonomy to the study of late-life Openness more closely tied to the latter. We are not arguing for
suicidal behavior. Suicide is a major public health problem. By eliminating the severity continuum model of suicide; rather, we
attempting to identify putative risk factors for suicidal behavior, are suggesting that the categorical model has much to offer. It is
social scientists can contribute to prevention efforts by defining likely that suicide ideators, suicide attempters, and completed
groups at high risk, before the development of an acute crisis. suicides are categorically discrete groups, each characterized by
This study represents a step in that direction. Other strengths of a discrete set of risk factors, reflecting distinct underlying per-
the study include a well characterized and carefully diagnosed sonality and constituent cognitive, affective, and motivational

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/55/1/P18/533273 by guest on 26 December 2022


sample at future risk for self-harm. processes. Recognition of these differences may result in more
efficient prediction of attempted and completed suicide (Rudd
Conclusion et al., 1996).
Our findings suggest that suicidal thoughts and behavior are A promising approach to preventing suicide in older adults
rooted in longstanding patterns of behaving, thinking, and feel- involves screening for depression in primary care practices
ing, and highlight the need to consider personality traits in craft- (Unutzer, Katon, Sullivan, & Miranda, 1999). However, some
ing and targeting prevention strategies. Suicidal ideas and believe that the need for legalization of assisted suicide in cer-
behavior are not an inevitable consequence of aging, disease, tain contexts is as pressing a need as suicide prevention. This
disability, or even depression. The current findings thus dilemma is complicated by the absence of consensus regarding
challenge an ageist stereotype that has probably contributed the conceptualization, measurement, or treatment of psycholog-
to a lack of interest in preventing late-life suicide (AARP ical distress or psychiatric disorders in individuals with life-
Foundation/Center for Mental Health Services, 1997). On the threatening illnesses. Still, screening instruments have been
other hand, findings must be regarded as preliminary. Several shown to have adequate sensitivity and specificity in predicting
lines of preintervention research ought to be pursued. major depression in older primary care patients (Lyness et al.,
The "state-trait problem" potentially confounds research on 1997). If carefully conducted preintervention research contin-
psychiatric inpatients, many of whom may be in acute distress ues to implicate certain personality traits in late-life suicidal be-
while completing questionnaires or participating in interviews. havior, it may be desirable to screen for personality traits as
Although it is unlikely that our observation that suicidal behav- well. All screening and surveillance mechanisms should be
ior and ideas are associated with Extraversion, Neuroticism, linked to systems capable of providing a range of interventions
and Openness, follow-up data, collected when patients no and services.
longer meet diagnostic criteria for major depressive disorder,
would be useful (e.g., Santor, Bagby, & Joffe, 1997). Future re- ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
search may also benefit from informant reports and projective, This project was financially supported in part by Public Heakh Service
physiological, or other nonverbal sources of psychological Grants K07-MH01135, R03-MH55149, and RO1-MH51201. Nancy Talbot, Jill
information. Eichele, and anonymous reviewers provided helpful comments on previous
By collecting data on a relatively homogeneous group of drafts of the manuscript. We also wish to extend our appreciation to Andrea
DiGiorgio, Wendy Wyland, Jack Herrmann, Barbara Hughson, Megan
older persons with major depression, we sought to decrease the Cavanagh, and Tamson Kelly Noel for their assistance in data collection; to
probability that potentially confounding effects of age or major Carrie Irvine for data management; and to Josephine Lauri and Marge Roberts
psychiatric diagnosis would obscure relationships between per- for manuscript preparation. An earlier version of this article was presented at
sonality and suicide variables. Still, heterogeneity was apparent the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, Chicago,
in the analyses on previous suicide attempts. These analyses August 1997.
suggested that Neuroticism may contribute to multiple suicide Address correspondence to Paul R. Duberstein, Department of Psychiatry,
attempts in those with comorbidity. Further research on larger University of Rochester Medical Center, 300 Crittenden Blvd., Rochester, NY
14642. E-mail: Paul_Duberstein@unnc.rochester.edu
samples may be necessary to determine whether the personality
traits associated with suicidal behavior in depressed patients
with psychiatric comorbidity differ from those without comor- REFERENCES
bidity. American Association of Retired Persons Foundation/Center for Mental Health
Even in the absence of consensus concerning the ideal de- Services. (1997). Suicide prevention project: Final report summary. Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
sign, sampling strategy, and statistical analysis required to de- mental disorders (3rd ed.—revised). Washington, DC: Author.
termine whether the constructs of suicidal ideation, attempted Bagby. R. M., Rector, N. A., Bindseil, K., Dickens, S. E., Levitan. R. D., &
suicide, and completed suicide are categorically distinct (cf. Kennedy, S. H. (1998). Self-report ratings and informants' ratings of per-
Flett, Vredenburg, & Krames, 1997; Meehl, 1992), tragedies sonalities of depressed outpatients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155,
may be prevented for now simply by acknowledging that devel- 437-438.
Beautrais, A. E., Joyce, P. R., & Mulder, R. T. (1999). Personality traits and
oping risk-identification and prevention strategies based on as- cognitive styles as risk factors for serious suicide attempts among young
sumptions implicit in the severity continuum model could be people. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 29, 37^t7.
misguided. Variables associated with the absence of reported Beck, A. T., & Greenberg, R. (1971). The nosology of suicidal phenomena:
suicidal ideation, such as low Openness, may not confer de- Past and future perspectives. Bulletin of Suicidology, 8, 10-17.
Beck, A. T, Kovacs. M., & Weissman, A. (1979). Assessment of suicidal
creased risk for completed suicide. Paradoxically, in some ideation: The scale for suicidal ideation. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
patients who are low in Openness, the absence of reported suici- Psychology. 47, 343-352.
dal ideation may confer increased suicide risk. Beck, A. T., Weissman, A., Eester, D., & Trexler, L. D. (1974). The measure-
PERSONALITY AND SUICIDE P25

ment of pessimism: The Hopelessness Scale. Journal of Consulting and Hosmer, D. W., & Lemeshow, S. (1989). Applied logistic regression. New York:
Clinical Psychology, 42, 861-865. John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Blazer, D. G., Bachar, J. R., & Manton, K. G. (1986). Suicide in late life: Review John, O. P. (1990). The "Big Five" factor taxonomy: Dimensions of personality
and commentary. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 43,216-221 in the natural language and questionnaires. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook
Block, J. (1995). A contrarian view of the five-factor approach to personality of personality: Theory and research (pp. 66-100). New York: Guilford.
description. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 187-215. Kagan, J. (with Snidman, N., Arcus, D., & Reznick, J. S.). (1994). Galen's
Buie, D. H., & Maltsberger, J. T. (1989). The psychological vulnerability to sui- prophecy: Temperament in human nature. New York: Basic Books.
cide. In D. Jacobs & H. N. Brown (Eds.), Suicide: Understanding and re- Katz, I. R. (1996). On the inseparability of mental and physical health in aged
sponding (pp. 59-72). Madison, CT: International Universities Press. persons: Lessons from depression and medical comorbidity. American
Burvill, P. W. (1995). Recent progress in the epidemiology of major depression. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 4, 1-16.
Epidemiologic Reviews, 17, 21 -31. Kessler, R. C., Borges, G., & Walters, E. E. (1999). Prevalence of and risk fac-
Centers for Disease Control. (1996). Suicide among older persons—United tors for lifetime suicide attempts in the National Comorbidity Survey.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/55/1/P18/533273 by guest on 26 December 2022


States, 1980-1992. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 45,3-6. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56,617-626.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (1999). GMWK250 death rates/or Krause, N., Liang, J., & Keith, V. (1990). Personality, social support, and psy-
72 selected causes, United States, 1993, 1994, 1995, 1996 and 1997. chological distress in later life. Psychology and Aging, 5, 315-326.
Available: http://www.cdc.gov/nchswww/data/gm250_97.pdf. Lebowitz, B. D., Pearson, J. L., Schneider, L. S., Reynolds III, C. F.,
Chatterjee, S., & Hadi, A. J. (1988). Sensitivity analysis in linear regression. Alexopoulos, G. S., Bruce, M. L., Conwell, Y, Katz, I. R., Meyers, B. S.,
New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Morrison, M. F., Mossey, J., Niederehe, G., & Parmelee, P. (1997).
Clark, D. C. (1993). Narcissistic crises of aging and suicidal despair. Suicide Diagnosis and treatment of depression in late life: Consensus statement up-
and Life-Threatening Behavior, 23, 21-26. date. Journal of the American Medical Association, 278, 1186-1190.
Clark, L. A., Watson, D., & Mineka, S. (1994). Temperament, personality, and Linehan, M. M. (1986). Suicidal people: One population or two? Annals of the
the mood and anxiety disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, New York Academy of Sciences, 487, 16-33.
103-116. Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive behavioral treatment of borderline personal-
Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M., & Przybeck, T. R. (1993). A psychobiological ity disorder. New York: Guilford Press.
model of temperament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, Linehan, M. M., Chiles, J. A., Egan, K. J., Devine, R. H., & Laffaw, J. A.
975-990. (1986). Presenting problems of parasuicides versus suicide ideators and
Congressional Record. (1997). Resolution recognizing suicide as a national nonsuicidal psychiatric patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
health problem. (CR page S-4038, May 6). Psychology, 6, 880-881.
Congressional Record. (1998). Resolution recognizing suicide as a national Lyness, J. M., Duberstein, P. R., King, D. A., Cox, C., & Caine, E. D. (1998).
problem (CR page H10309, October 9). Medical illness burden, trait neuroticism, and depression in primary care el-
Conwell, Y, Duberstein, P. R., Cox, C., Herrmann, J., Forbes, N., & Caine, E. derly. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155,969-971.
D. (1996). Relationships of age and Axis I diagnosis in victims of com- Lyness, J. M., Noel, T. K., Cox, C., King, D. A., Conwell, Y, & Caine, E. D.
pleted suicide: A psychological autopsy study. American Journal of (1997). Screening for depression in elderly primary care patients. Archives
Psychiatry, 153, 1001-1008. of Internal Medicine, 757,449-454.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1987). Neuroticism, somatic complaints, and Maris, R. W. (1992). The relationship of nonfatal suicide attempts to completed
disease: Is the bark worse than the bite? Journal of Personality, 55, suicide. In R. W. Maris, A. L. Berman, J. T. Maltsberger, & R. I. Yufit
299-316. (Eds.), Assessment and prediction of suicide (pp. 362-380). New York:
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory Guilford Press.
and NEO Five Factor Inventory': Professional manual. Odessa, FL: Marshall, G. N., Wortman, C. B., Vickers, R. R., Jr., Kusulas, J. W, & Hervig,
Psychological Assessment Resources. L. K. (1994). The five-factor model of personality as a framework for
Costa, P. T, Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1995). Solid ground in the wetlands of per- personality-health research. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
sonality: A reply to Block. Psychological Bulletin, 117,216-220. 67, 278-286.
Digman, J. M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor McAdams, D. P. (1994). Can personality change? Levels of stability and growth
model. Annual Review of Psychology, 41,417^440. in personality across the life span. In T. F. Heatherton & J. L. Weinberger
Duberstein, P. R. (1995). Openness to experience and completed suicide across (Eds.), Can personality change? (pp. 299-314). Washington, DC: Ameri-
the second half of life. International Psychogeriatrics, 7,183-198. can Psychological Association.
Duberstein, P. R., Conwell, Y, & Caine, E. D. (1994). Age differences in the McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T, Jr. (1997). Personality trait structure as ahuman
personality characteristics of suicide completers: Preliminary findings from universal. American Psychologist, 52, 509-516.
a psychological autopsy study. Psychiatry, 57,1 \ 3-224.
McCullagh, P., & Nelder, J. A. (1989). Generalized linear models (2nd ed).
Durkheim, E. (1951). Suicide: A study in sociology. New York: Free Press (orig-
New York: John Wiley & Sons.
inal work published in 1897).
Flett, G. L., Vredenburg, K., & Krames, L. (1997). The continuity of depression Meehl, P. E. (1992). Factors and taxa, traits and types, differences of degree and
in clinical and nonclinical samples. Psychological Bulletin, 121,395^116. differences in kind. Journal of Personality, 60, 117-124.
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-Mental State: A Monk, M. (1987). Epidemiology of suicide. Epidemiologic Reviews, 9,51-69.
practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Moscicki, E. K. (1989). Epidemiologic surveys as tools for studying suicidal
Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189-198. behavior: A review. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 19,131-146.
Gallo, J. J., Rabins, P. V., Lyketos, C., Tien, A. Y, & Anthony, J. C. (1997). National Institutes of Health. (1998). Priorities for prevention research at
Depression without sadness: Functional outcomes of nondysphoric depres- NIMH: A report by the National Advisory Council Workgroup on Mental
sion in later life. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 45,570-578. Disorders Prevention Research. (NTH Publication No. 98-4321). Rockville,
Hendin, H. (1991). Psychodynamics of suicide, with particular reference to the MD: Author.
young. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148,1150-1158. O'Carroll, P. W., Berman, A. L., Maris, R. W., Moscicki, E. K., Tanney, B. L.,
Hill, R. D., Gallagher, D., Thompson, L. W., & Ishida, T. (1988). Hopelessness & Silverman, M. M. (1996). Beyond the Tower of Babel: A nomenclature
as a measure of suicidal intent in the depressed elderly. Psychology and for suicidology. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 26,237-252.
Aging, 3, 230-232. Penninx, B. W. J. H., Geerlings, S. W., Deeg, D. J. H., van Eijk, J. T. M.,
Hooker, K., Frazier, L. D., & Monahan, D. (1994). Personality and coping van Tilburg, W., & Beekman, A. T. F. (1999). Minor and major depression
among caregivers of spouses with dementia. Gerontologist, 34, 386-392. and the risk of death in older persons. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56,
Hooker, K., Monahan, D., Bowman, S. R., Frazier, L. D., & Shifren, K. (1998). 889-895.
Personality counts for a lot: Predictors of mental and physical health of Pfeffer, C. R., Stokes, P., & Shindledecker, R. (1991). Suicidal behavior and
spouse caregivers in two disease groups. Journal of Gerontology: hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis indices in child psychiatric inpa-
Psychological Sciences, 53B, P73-P85. tients. Biological Psychiatry, 29,909-917.
Hooker, K., Monahan, D., Shifren, K., & Hutchinson, C. (1992). Mental and Roy, A. (1998). Is introversion a risk factor for suicidal behaviour in depres-
physical health of spouse caregivers: The role of personality. Psychology sion? Psychological Medicine, 28, 1457-1461.
and Aging, 7, 367-375. Rudd, M. D., Joiner, T, & Rajab, M. H. (1996). Relationships among suicide
P26 DUBERSTEINETAL

ideators, attempters, and multiple attempters in a young adult sample. Young, M. A., Fogg, L. F, Scheftner, W., Fawcett, J., Akiskal, H., & Maser, J.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 541-550. (1996). Stable trait components of hopelessness: Baseline and sensitivity to
Santor, D. A., Bagby, R. M., & Joffe, R. T. (1997). Evaluating stability and depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology; 105,155-165.
change in personality and depression. Journal of Abnormal Psvchologv. 73, Zubenko, G. S., Mulsant, B. H., Sweet, R. A., Pasternak, R. E., & Tu, X. M.
1354-1362. (1997). Mortality of elderly patients with psychiatric disorders. American
Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B. W., & Gibbon, M. (1987). Structured clinical in- Journal of Psychiatry. 150, 1687-1692.
ten'iewfor DSM-/II-R—patient version. New York: Biometrics Research Zweig, R. A., & Hinrichsen, G. A. (1993). Factors associated with suicide at-
Department, NY State Psychiatric Institute. tempts by depressed older adults: A prospective study. American Journal of
Tellegen, A. (1985). Structures of mood and personality and their relevance to Psychiatrv, 150, 1687-1692.
assessing anxiety, with an emphasis on self-report (pp. 681-706). In A. H.
Tuma & J. D. Maser (Eds.), Anxiety and the anxiety disorders. Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/55/1/P18/533273 by guest on 26 December 2022


Unutzer, J., Katon, W., Sullivan, M., & Miranda, J. (1999). Treating depressed
older adults in primary care: Narrowing the gap between efficacy and effec-
tiveness. Milbank Quarterly, 77, 225-256.
Von Dras, D. D., & Siegler, I. C. (1997). Stability in extraversion and aspects of Received August 12. 1998
social support at midlife. Journal of Personality and Social Psvchologv, 72, Accepted August 18, 1999
233-241. Decision Editor: Toni C. Antonucci, PhD

CALL FOR PAPERS!


The 53rd Annual Scientific Meeting of
The Gerontological Society of America
November 17-21, 2000, Washington, D.C

Abstracts due April 3, 2000. See www.geron.org for details.

You might also like