Tanjitpiyanondetal 2022

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 18

1074550

research-article2022
GPI0010.1177/13684302221074550Group Processes & Intergroup RelationsTanjitpiyanond et al.

G
Group Processes & P
Intergroup Relations I
Article R

Group Processes & Intergroup Relations

A social identity analysis of how pay 1–18


© The Author(s) 2022
Article reuse guidelines:
inequality divides the workplace sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/13684302221074550
https://doi.org/10.1177/13684302221074550
journals.sagepub.com/home/gpi

Porntida Tanjitpiyanond,1
Jolanda Jetten1 and Kim Peters1,2

Abstract
The present research examines why organizations with more unequal pay structures have been
found to be characterized by a range of negative workplace outcomes. Drawing on the social identity
approach, we propose that higher pay disparity can increase the comparative fit of pay categories
whereby the organizational “haves” (the highest paid employees) and “have nots” (the lowest paid
employees) are more likely to be categorized into distinct social groups. In turn, this can lead to poorer
organizational functioning. In two studies, a field survey (N = 413) and an experiment (N = 286),
we found that higher pay inequality increased the comparative fit of pay categories, which, in turn,
was associated with lower superordinate (organizational) identification, higher perceived workplace
conflict, higher leader toxicity, and lower perceptions of identity leadership (i.e., a leader who creates
a sense of shared identity in the organization). Our research provides novel insights into how higher
inequality affects employees’ categorization processes, thereby creating a psychological divide and
contributing to organizational dysfunction.

Keywords
leadership, organizational identification, pay inequality, self-categorization, social identity approach

Paper received 8 August 2021; revised version accepted 3 January 2022.

In 2011, the wave of Occupy Wall Street move- because there is evidence that inequality is associ-
ments drew public attention to the role of large ated with a range of negative workplace outcomes,
corporations in perpetuating economic inequality. including reduced organizational identification
The iconic slogan “We are the 99%” captured the and job satisfaction, less effective leadership and
social identity of the protestors who were direct-
ing their discontentment towards the top 1%, the 1The University of Queensland, Australia
leaders of large corporations (CEOs and top 2University of Exeter, UK
executives) whose salaries had increased over
Corresponding author:
1000% (i.e., from 1978 to 2019) whilst wages of
Porntida Tanjitpiyanond, School of Psychology, The
the typical worker grew by only 10% (Mishel & University of Queensland, St Lucia, Brisbane QLD 4072,
Kandra, 2020). This rise in pay inequality between Australia.
the highest and lowest paid workers is concerning Email: uqptanji@uq.edu.au
2 Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 00(0)

collaboration, and higher turnover intentions category are more similar to one another (i.e.,
(Bloom, 1999; Connelly et al., 2016; Peters et al., high intragroup similarity) while they perceive
2019; Pfeffer & Langton, 1993; Shaw et al., 2002; members of a different social category as more
Steffens et al., 2020). Yet, to date, there is little distinct (i.e., high intergroup difference, also
understanding of why higher organizational pay known as the metacontrast principle; Brown &
inequality may have these negative effects (Shaw, Turner, 2002). In this way, comparative fit allows
2014). individuals to understand their social world by
In the present research, we draw on the social mapping preexisting social categories (e.g.,
identity approach—which is comprised of self- wealthy or poor) onto properties of the social
categorization theory (Turner et al., 1987) and context (Hogg & Terry, 2000).
social identity theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1979)—to When applied to organizational pay inequality,
propose that higher pay disparity may do harm by we can expect that as inequality grows and the
creating a psychological divide within the organi- difference between high and low earners becomes
zation. Specifically, we propose that higher pay more marked, then pay-based categories will
disparity introduces divisions by making “pay” a become a more fitting basis for organizing the
fitting basis for social categorization whereby the work environment (for a theoretical overview, see
highest paid (e.g., the top 1%; CEOs, top manag- Jetten et al., 2017; Platow et al., 2008; Putnam-
ers) and the lowest paid employees are seen as Farr & Morewedge, 2020). In particular, with
distinct social groups (i.e., enhanced perceptions higher pay inequality, people will come to per-
of intergroup differences and intragroup similari- ceive employees from the highest and lowest paid
ties). We argue that such pay-based fragmentation categories as more different while perceiving
can reinforce an “us” versus “them” dynamic and greater similarity between employees within the
corrode a sense of unity and oneness within the same pay category, thus reinforcing the category
larger organization. As a result, employees may distinction between the organizational “haves”
identify less with the superordinate organiza- and “have nots.” There is a growing body of
tional group, experience higher levels of work- work that supports this possibility (Connor et al.,
place conflict, and perceive their (generally higher 2020; for reviews, see Jetten et al., 2021; Platow
paid) leaders more negatively (e.g., as engaging in et al., 2008; Tanjitpiyanond et al., 2022). For
more toxic behaviors and unable to create a sense instance, in a series of archival and experimental
of “us” in the organization). In the following sec- studies, Peters et al. (2021) found that higher ine-
tions, we will elaborate on our reasoning. quality was associated with the increased ten-
dency for people to use wealth categories in
language (e.g., books published in years of higher
Self-Categorization Theory and Pay-Based income inequality in the US and UK had more
Division wealth-related words). This suggests that when
According to self-categorization theory, features income inequality is higher, it increases the
of the broader social context shape the way peo- importance of wealth in guiding how people cat-
ple organize and categorize their social world egorize and understand their social world.
(Turner et al., 1987). Of particular relevance is
the principle of comparative fit, which posits that How Pay-Based Division Creates
categorization—the process of grouping people
Organizational Dysfunction
based on similarities or shared characteristics—is
affected by the extent to which salient cues in the There is reason to expect that pay-based social
social context fit with known categories. categorization would come at a cost to workplace
Specifically, people are more likely to utilize a functioning by undermining a sense of unity and
given category in a particular context if they per- oneness within the organization (Akerlof &
ceive that individuals who are members of that Kranton, 2010; Steffens et al., 2020). According
Tanjitpiyanond et al. 3

to social identity theorizing, when pay becomes a induce conflict between coworkers. In particular,
core basis for social categorization, not only does an experiment by Cheng et al. (2021) found that
it highlight employees’ perceived differences participants who were assigned to imagine working
along pay lines (between colleagues, different in an organization with high (vs. low) income ine-
departmental teams, or professional roles) but it quality were more vigilant of colleagues whom they
also diminishes their perceived similarities based suspected would undermine their work perfor-
on shared superordinate—organizational—iden- mance. These findings may reflect that employees
tity (whether with colleagues or leaders; Peters in more unequal work settings are focused on
et al., 2019; Platow et al., 2008; Steffens et al., improving their own individual pay and prospects
2020). Such categorization, in turn, can reduce rather than cooperating with colleagues to meet
the tendency for employees to identify as mem- shared goals (Bratanova et al., 2019).
bers of the organization or perceive one another Although the social identity perspective can
as fellow in-group members who share the same explain the observed deficit in cooperation and
identity and goals (Haslam et al., 2000). Instead, greater proneness to conflict, there is limited
the categorization of colleagues into the “haves” work that has directly tested this hypothesis in the
(winners) and “have nots” (losers)—based on context of pay inequality. Here, we aim to test,
their level of pay and seniority—can trigger for the first time, the role of comparative fit—the
unhealthy social comparison and workplace com- extent to which pay is used as a basis for social
petition on the basis of pay (Bratanova et al., categorization—in explaining how higher pay
2019; Cheung & Lucas, 2016; Jetten et al., 2017; inequality can create organizational dysfunction
Kraus et al., 2017; Walasek & Brown, 2015, 2016, (see also Platow et al., 2008). In particular, we aim
2019). Importantly, such intergroup dynamics to unpack the proposition that the observed
within the workplace are likely to undermine undermined sense of oneness (i.e., reduced
organizational performance as it pertains to the superordinate organizational identification) and
ability for employees to work collaboratively collaboration (i.e., increased proneness to con-
towards shared organizational goals. flict) in more unequal workplaces are due to
Consistent with this reasoning, there is a body higher inequality introducing divisions via pay-
of work showing that higher pay inequality can fuel based social categorization.
uncooperative workplace dynamics among lower Social identity theorizing also leads to the pre-
paid employees (e.g., production workers; Bloom, diction that the comparative fit of pay would
1999; Pfeffer & Langton, 1993; Shaw, 2014; Siegel have significant implications for leadership. Top
& Hambrick, 2005). For instance, Shaw et al. (2002) leaders, those who represent the organization and
studied pipeline production workers in the US, and are in the position to set its goals, are likely to be
found that higher pay inequality (e.g., greater differ- categorized as the “other” who belong in a higher
ences in hourly pay rate of starting, middle, and paid category (e.g., the 1%, as indicated by the
senior employees in the organization) was detri- Occupy movement). Such perceptions would be
mental to employees’ performance on collaborative consequential for their ability to steer the organi-
tasks that required face-to-face interactions and zation. Specifically, according to social identity
joint participation. Along the same lines, Breza theory, leadership behavior is appraised differ-
et al. (2018) found that workers who were randomly ently depending on whether the leader is per-
assigned to an unequal (vs. equal) pay unit were less ceived as an in-group or an out-group member
collaborative with their coworkers on cooperative (e.g., Ellemers et al., 2004; Haslam et al., 2020).
games. In a subsequent survey, the workers also Leaders who are perceived to share the same
reported reduced social connection and willingness identity with their followers—that is, perceived as
to engage in prosocial behaviors (e.g., to borrow, an in-group member—are seen as more positive
lend, or give advice to others in general). There is and charismatic, and are more likely to be
also initial evidence that higher inequality can endorsed and trusted by their followers (Hains
4 Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 00(0)

et al., 1997; Hogg et al., 1998; Platow & van whether inequality is associated with more dys-
Knippenberg, 2001; Platow et al., 2006). In other functional workplace outcomes (as measured by
words, shared organizational identity breeds a participants’ workplace perceptions, in Study 1,
better leader in the eyes of followers. In contrast, and expectations, in Study 2) because it makes pay
when leaders are perceived to be out-group mem- a fitting basis for organizing the work environ-
bers, their actions may be parsed as malign and ment. We also examine whether this, in turn,
not in the best interests of the group. impairs superordinate identification, raises per-
Consistent with this, a study by Steffens et al. ceptions of workplace conflict, and increases
(2020) provided evidence that when CEO pay was negative perceptions of leaders (e.g., perceptions
relatively high (vs. low), people perceived the CEO of leader toxicity).1
as less charismatic and as unable to create a sense We conducted two studies to test the role of
of “we” and “us” in the organization (i.e., identity the comparative fit of pay categories in explain-
leadership; Steffens et al., 2014). Importantly, their ing the effect of higher inequality on poorer
research also found that the negative impact of workplace outcomes.2 Study 1 was a field survey
higher CEO pay on leadership perceptions was of participants’ perceptions of pay inequality
driven by the lack of shared identity between lead- within their current workplaces. Study 2 was an
ers and followers. However, it is important to note experimental study where participants were ran-
that Steffens et al. (2020) did not examine the domly assigned to imagine working in a fictitious
degree of pay inequality or comparative fit of pay organization with a high or low level of pay ine-
categories. Rather, they examined how well CEOs quality. In this way, Study 2 builds upon Study 1
were paid, rather than how well they were paid by unpacking whether manipulating levels of
relative to other members of their organizations objective inequality (high or low in the fictional
(i.e., the degree of pay inequality). As such, this organization) would affect participants’ percep-
leaves us with some unanswered questions. In par- tions of inequality and their expectations of dif-
ticular, from this research, it is not clear how higher ferent workplace outcomes.
pay inequality may shape categorization processes
that give rise to employees’ negative perceptions Study 1
of their leaders. Is it the case that higher pay ine-
quality increases the fitness of pay as a basis of Method
social categorization (i.e., comparative fit of pay Participants. We recruited 465 U.K. participants
categories) and thus creates perceptions of leaders from Prolific.co with custom screening for par-
as a separate out-group in the organization? And, ticipants who were working in large organizations
does this, in turn, create negative perceptions of (i.e., large private or publicly listed) and working
leaders as more toxic and less able to create a sense full time. We chose large organizations to control
of shared identity in the organization? Answering for firm size as there is evidence that pay inequal-
this question is important to understand the appli- ity increases with the size of the organization
cability of the social identity approach to the study (Henderson & Fredrickson, 2001). A total of 52
of pay inequality, and to advance our current participants were excluded for failing the com-
understanding of how a specific psychological prehension/attention checks (N = 30) or having
mechanism—that is, categorization—may con- incomplete responses (N = 1) on the main vari-
tribute to higher pay inequality creating organiza- ables of interest or both (N = 21).3 The study
tional dysfunction. took 10–15 minutes, and participants were paid
£1.66 for their participation.
The Present Research The final sample included 413 participants
who were on average 36.89 (SD = 10.32) years
In this paper, we aim to build on the social identity old (male: n = 177, female: n = 235, other or
approach to organizational inequality. We examine prefer not to say: n = 1). Participants were mostly
Tanjitpiyanond et al. 5

White (n = 356), with a postsecondary qualifica- top 20% are to one another?” and “Thinking
tion (n = 348), working in associate (n = 128) or about employees within the bottom 20%: How
managerial level positions (n = 130), with more similar do you think that people within the bot-
than 5 years of working experience (n = 207). tom 20% are to one another?”). All responses
This study was approved by the Human Research were recorded on 7-point scales (1 = very different,
Ethics Committee at the first author’s 7 = very similar). To compute a comparative fit
institution. index, we averaged the intragroup similarity score
for each pay category and divided it by the inter-
Procedure. The study was completed online on group similarity score. A higher ratio of intra-
Qualtrics. Participants first read a short instruc- group to intergroup similarity indicated a greater
tion on what the study involved before being comparative fit of pay, such that people perceived
asked to answer two comprehension checks to the highest and lower paid employees as distinct
make sure they understood what they had to do social categories within the organization.
in the study. Subsequently, participants completed
the following measures. Superordinate identification. Participants com-
pleted a four-item Social Identification Scale
Pay inequality. We measured income inequal- adapted to an organizational context (α = .93;
ity in participants’ organizations using two meas- Jetten et al., 2002): “I feel committed to this
ures.4 organization,” “I am glad to be a part of this
organization,” “Being an employee at this organi-
Ratio inequality zation is an important part of how I see myself,”
Participants first completed a measure which “I identify with being an employee at this organi-
asked them to estimate the actual average salaries zation.” All responses were recorded on a 7-point
of employees within the top 20% and bottom scale (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree).
20% of their organizations. The final index was
calculated by dividing the top 20% salary by the Workplace conflict. Participants indicated how
bottom 20% salary. Higher index values reflect often they experienced conflict in the workplace
greater estimates of income inequality. using a four-item scale (α = .87; Penney & Spec-
tor, 2005): At work, how often do others: “. . . get
Perception of inequality into arguments at work?”; “. . . yell at each other
We measured perception of income inequality at work?”; “. . . behave rudely to each other at
using a single item (i.e., “Overall, please rate how work?”; and “. . . do nasty things to each other at
equal or unequal you think the pay distribution is work?” All responses were recorded on a 5-point
between the top and bottom 20% at your organi- scale (1 = never, 5 = very often).
zation”). This item was recorded on a 10-point
scale (1 = very equal, 10 = very unequal).5 Leadership perceptions. We measured partici-
pants’ perceptions of (a) toxic leadership behav-
Pay categorization. We used a comparative fit iors and (b) the leader’s ability to create a sense of
index to capture pay-based social categoriza- shared identity in the organization.6
tion (adapted from Tanjitpiyanond et al., 2022).
Participants were asked to evaluate intergroup Leader toxicity
similarity (i.e., “Thinking about people in the top We measured perceived leadership toxicity using
and bottom 20%: How similar do you think that 15 of Schmidt’s (2008) 30 items (α = .97).
people in the top 20% are to people in the bot- Participants rated the extent to which they
tom 20%?”) as well as intragroup similarity (i.e., thought a typical employee from the top 20%
“Thinking about employees within the top 20%: would engage in a range of toxic behaviors (i.e.,
How similar do you think that people within the “Tells subordinates they are incompetent,”
6 Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 00(0)

“Publicly ridicules subordinates,” “Speaks poorly measures of organizational functioning, we first


about subordinates to other people in the work- conducted a series of multiple linear regressions
place,” “Likes to control how subordinates com- examining models with each inequality measure
plete their tasks,” “Ignore ideas that are contrary (i.e., ratio and perceived inequality) as predictors
to their own,” “Invades the privacy of subordi- of our dependent measures (i.e., pay-based cate-
nates,” “Feels entitled to be at the highest rank of gorization, superordinate identification, and per-
the organization,” “Thinks that they are more ceptions of conflict as well as leader toxicity and
capable than others,” “Believes that they are identity leadership), controlling for perceived sta-
extraordinary people,” “Likes to deny responsi- tus and their interactions. The results were the
bility for mistakes made in the unit,” “Would same with or without controlling for participant
accept credit for successes that do not belong to status, thus we report the results excluding status
them,” “Acts only in the best interest of their from the analysis. See Table 1 for means, standard
next promotion,” “Expresses anger at subordi- deviations, and correlations.
nates for unknown reasons,” “Allows their cur-
rent mood to define the climate of the workplace,” Pay inequality, categorization, and organizational func-
and “Has explosive outburst”). All responses tioning. Both ratio and perceived inequality meas-
were recorded on a 7-point scale (1 = strongly disa- ures were mean centered before the analysis.
gree, 7 = strongly agree).
Ratio inequality. The analysis revealed that a
Identity Leadership Inventory higher ratio estimate of inequality was signifi-
We used a four-item scale to measure partici- cantly associated with greater comparative fit
pants’ perceptions that the typical highly paid (B = 0.02, p = .007, f = .13), suggesting that in
employee (i.e., from the top 20%) is an effective more unequal organizations, employees perceived
social identity leader (α = .87; Steffens et al., pay as a more fitting basis for categorizing their
2014): “The employee is a model member of the work environment. Further, higher inequality was
organization,” “The employee acts as a champion significantly associated with lower superordinate
for the organization,” “The employee creates a identification (B = −0.01, p = .021, f = .11). The
sense of cohesion within the organization,” and association between inequality and perception of
“The employee creates structures that are useful workplace conflict was nonsignificant (B = 0.01,
for other employees in the organization.” All p = .106, f = .08). There was a weak but signifi-
responses were recorded on a 7-point scale (1 = cant association between higher inequality and
strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree). perception of greater leader toxicity (B = 0.01,
p = .023, f = .11), but the association between
Organizational status. In addition to these inequality and perception of social identity lead-
measures, we also controlled for participants’ per- ership was not significant (B = 0.00, p = .663, f
ceptions of their own status in the organization = .02).
using the MacArthur Scale of Subjective Social
Status (Adler & Stewart, 2007). Participants indi- Perceived inequality. Higher perceived inequal-
cated where they perceived themselves along the ity was positively associated with greater com-
organizational hierarchy on a scale from 1 (one of parative fit (B = 0.11, p < .001, f = .17) and
the lowest paid employees in the organization) to 10 (one with greater dysfunction on all organizational
of the highest paid employees). outcomes: lower superordinate identification (B
= −0.11, p < .001, f = .20), higher perceptions
of workplace conflict (B = 0.05, p < .001, f =
Results
.18), leader toxicity (B = 0.19, p < .001, f = .35),
To test whether higher income inequality and lower perceptions of identity leadership (B =
impacted pay-based categorization as well as −0.08, p < .001, f = .18).
Tanjitpiyanond et al. 7

Table 1. Means, standard deviations, and correlations of inequality, comparative fit, and organizational
outcomes.

Variables M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6
1. Ratio inequality 9.17 14.57
2. Perceived inequality 6.44 2.43 .15**
3. Comparative fit 2.57 1.66 .18** .18**
4. Identification 4.84 1.44 −.09 −.20** −.19**
5. Conflict 2.06 0.72 .05 .17** .29** −.38**
6. Toxic leadership 3.80 1.41 .07 .33** .39** −.41** .55**
7. Identity leadership 5.05 1.15 −.03 −.18** −.27** .47** −.22** −.47**

Note. Actual means of ratio and perception of inequality measures prior to mean centering.
*p < .05. **p < .01.

Figure 1. Model with comparative fit as mediator between the effect of ratio and perceived inequality on
superordinate identification.

Ratio: 0.02, < .007 Comparative


Perceived: 0.11, = .001 0 16, < .001
fit

Superordinate
Inequality
Ratio: C -0.01, = .020 Identification
0.01, .063
Perceived: C: 0.12, < .001
= -0.10, < .001

Pay categorization and organizational functioning. To Ratio inequality. Based on Monte Carlo power
unpack whether pay categorization may in part analysis for indirect effects (using Schoemann
underlie the relationship between inequality and et al., 2017), we had 92–95% power to detect the
superordinate identification, perceptions of con- indirect effects of ratio inequality via comparative
flict, identity leadership, and leader toxicity, we fit on the organizational outcomes with a sample
examined the explanatory role of comparative fit size of 413.
using the lavaan pacakage on R with 95% bias
corrected confidence intervals based on 5,000 Superordinate identification
bootstrap samples. Although ratio inequality did Comparative fit (B = −0.16, p < .001) signifi-
not have a significant total effect on some organi- cantly predicted identification. This suggests that
zational outcomes (e.g., identity leadership), the higher pay categorization was associated with the
absence of a direct effect does not mean the weakening of superordinate organizational iden-
absence of an indirect effect (Zhao et al., 2010), tification. The total effect of inequality was sig-
and we therefore proceeded to conduct media- nificant (B = −0.01, p = .020), and this effect
tion analyses with our measures of primary inter- became marginally significant after accounting
est (see Figures 1–4). In all mediation models, for comparative fit (B = −0.01, p = .063).
ratio and perceived inequality were significantly However, there was a significant indirect effect of
associated with comparative fit (ratio: B = 0.02, p inequality via comparative fit (B = −0.006, SE =
= .007; perceived: B = 0.11, p = .001).7 0.00, 95% CI [−0.006, −0.000]).
8 Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 00(0)

Figure 2. Model with comparative fit as mediator between the effect of ratio and perceived inequality on
workplace conflict.

Ratio: 0.02, < .007 Comparative


Perceived: 0.11, = .001 0.12, < .001
fit

Workplace
Inequality
Ratio: C 0.01, = .104 Conflict
0.00, 377
Perceived: C: 0 05, < .001
= -0.04, .006

Figure 3. Model with comparative fit as mediator between the effect of ratio and perceived inequality on toxic
leadership.

Ratio: 0.02, < .007 Comparative


Perceived: 0.11, = .001 0.34, < .001
fit

Toxic
Inequality
Ratio: C 0.01, = .186 Leadership
0.01, .186
Perceived: C: 0.19, < .001
= 0.16, < .001

Figure 4. Model with comparative fit as mediator between the effect of ratio and perceived inequality on
identity leadership.

Ratio: 0.02, < .007 Comparative


Perceived: 0.11, = .001 -0.17, < .001
fit

Identity
Inequality
Ratio: C 0.00, = .662 Leadership
0.01, .237
Perceived: C: 0.08, < .001
= -0.06, .005

Workplace conflict workplace conflict. The total effect of inequality


Comparative fit (B = 0.12, p < .001) significantly was nonsignificant (B = 0.01, p = .104). This
predicted perception of workplace conflict. This effect remained nonsignificant after accounting
suggests that greater pay-based categorization for comparative fit (B = 0.00, p = .377). There
was associated with higher perception of was a significant indirect effect of inequality via
Tanjitpiyanond et al. 9

comparative fit (B = 0.002, SE = 0.001, 95% CI There was a significant total effect of inequality
[0.000, 0.004]). (B = −0.12, p < .001). This effect remained sig-
nificant after accounting for comparative fit (B =
Leader toxicity −0.10, p < .001). The indirect effect of inequality
Comparative fit (B = 0.34, p < .001) significantly via comparative fit was significant (B = −0.02,
predicted perception of toxic leadership. SE = 0.01, 95% CI [−0.03, −0.00]).
Consistent with our arguments, higher pay-based
categorization was associated with a negative per- Workplace conflict
ception of the leader as more toxic. The total Comparative fit (B = 0.12, p < .001) significantly
effect of inequality was nonsignificant (B = 0.01, predicted perceived workplace conflict. There
p = .186). This effect remained nonsignificant was a significant total effect of inequality (B =
after accounting for comparative fit (B = 0.01, p 0.05, p < .001). This effect remained significant
= .186). However, there was a significant indirect after accounting for comparative fit (B = 0.04, p
effect of inequality via comparative fit (B = = .006). The indirect effect of inequality via
0.006, SE = 0.001, 95% CI [0.002, 0.011]). comparative fit was significant (B = 0.01, SE =
0.01, 95% CI [0.01, 0.02]).
Identity leadership
Comparative fit (B = −0.17, p < .001) signifi- Leader toxicity
cantly predicted perception of identity leader- Comparative fit (B = 0.34, p < .001) significantly
ship, which suggests that increased pay predicted perceived toxic leadership. The total
categorization was associated with the perception effect of inequality was significant (B = 0.19, p <
of leaders as less able to create a shared social .001). This effect remained significant after
identity in the organization. The total effect of accounting for comparative fit (B = 0.16, p <
inequality was nonsignificant (B = 0.00, p = .001). There was a significant indirect effect of
.662). This effect remained nonsignificant after inequality via comparative fit (B = 0.04, SE =
accounting for comparative fit (B = 0.01, p = 0.01, 95% CI [0.01, 0.06]).
.237). However, there was a significant indirect
effect of inequality via comparative fit (B = Identity leadership
−0.004, SE = 0.001, 95% CI [−0.006, −0.001]). Comparative fit (B = −0.17, p < .001) signifi-
Although the effects were weak, the results cantly predicted perceived identity leadership.
from the ratio measure provide some evidence to The total effect of inequality was significant (B =
suggest that categorization on the basis of pay −0.08, p < .001). This effect remained significant
may in part influence the impact of inequality on after accounting for comparative fit (B = −0.06,
superordinate identification, perception of con- p = .005). There was a significant indirect effect
flict, and identity leadership as well as leader tox- of inequality via comparative fit (B = −0.02, SE
icity in the organization. = 0.01, 95% CI [−0.03, −0.01]).

Perceived inequality. Based on Monte Carlo power


Discussion
analysis for indirect effects (using Schoemann et al.,
2017), we had 86–95% power to detect the indirect We found evidence that both inequality measures
effects of perceived inequality via comparative fit (ratio and perceived inequality measures) were
on the organizational outcomes with a sample size associated with greater comparative fit—the cat-
of 413. egorization of the highest and lowest paid
employees as distinct groups in the organiza-
Superordinate identification tion—and that this, in turn, was associated with
Comparative fit (B = −0.16, p < .001) signifi- lower superordinate organizational identification,
cantly predicted organizational identification. higher perceptions of workplace conflict and
10 Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 00(0)

leader toxicity as well as perception of lower condition and 144 were in the high inequality
identity leadership. Thus, our findings suggest condition.
that employees who work in highly unequal Participants were on average 30 years old (SD
organizations may be less likely to feel a sense of = 10.45; male: n = 140, female: n = 141, other or
“we,” and more likely to not get along or collabo- prefer not to say: n = 5). Most had completed
rate with their colleagues. Importantly, they may postsecondary education (n = 188), and all were
also hold negative perceptions of their current Australian residents. Participants were mostly
leaders as toxic and lacking the ability to steer a entry-level (n = 82) or associate-level employees
cohesive ship within the organization. It is impor- (n = 73) and had 1–5 years of working experi-
tant to note that the effect of the ratio measure ence (n = 121). This study was approved by the
of inequality was smaller compared to the effects Human Research Ethics Committee at the first
for perceived inequality. This suggests that per- author’s institution.
haps the perceived measure may better capture
people’s subjective evaluation of inequality com- Procedure. Participants completed the experiment
pared to the ratio measure. online on the Qualtrics platform, where they
Nevertheless, the correlational nature of this were introduced to a fictional organization called
study poses the main limitation. In particular, we JAI and were asked to read a general description
have not yet answered the question of whether about what JAI was like as an organization (e.g.,
higher pay inequality causes greater pay-based number of employees, franchises, etc.).
categorization, and whether inequality leads to Next, participants were randomly assigned to
lower superordinate organizational identification, either the high or low inequality condition,
heightened perceptions of conflict and leader where they learnt that the pay distribution in JAI
toxicity as well as reduced perception of identity was highly unequal or relatively equal. In the
leadership. To overcome this limitation, Study 2 high inequality condition, participants saw a lop-
was an experimental investigation where partici- sided scale representing the state of inequality
pants were randomly assigned to imagine work- within the organization while learning that the
ing in an organization with either a high or low top 5% of the company (i.e., the highest paid
pay inequality. We examined whether the inequal- employees) earned 150 times more than the bot-
ity manipulation would influence participants’ tom 5% (i.e., the lowest paid employees; see
pay-based categorization as well as their expecta- Figure 5a). In contrast, in the low inequality
tions of a range of organizational outcomes. condition, participants saw a balanced scale and
learnt that the top 5% only earned 1.5 times
more than the bottom 5% (see Figure 5b).
Study 2 Subsequently, participants were asked to imag-
ine being an average-paid employee from the
Method middle 5% of JAI and to describe their experi-
Participants. We recruited 314 participants from ence working at the organization (e.g., “What
the first-year student pool at the first author’s would be your daily routine?”). The task aimed
institution (N = 47) and from an online research to get participants engaged with the paradigm.
recruitment website (Prolific.co; N = 267). A Subsequently, to examine whether participants
total of 28 participants were excluded due to fail- perceived different levels of inequality between
ing the attention/comprehension (N = 20) our experimental manipulations, they also com-
checks, having incomplete responses (N = 1), or pleted a manipulation check (e.g., “Please rate
both (N = 7; 0.3% of participants in the high the extent to which you think the income distri-
inequality condition and 0.5% of participants in bution is equal or unequal in JAI”) with a 9-point
the low inequality condition were in this exclu- response scale (1 = very unequal, 9 = very equal).
sion). The final sample included 286 participants; They also completed a range of measures of
142 were randomly allocated to the low inequality interest.
Tanjitpiyanond et al. 11

Figure 5. Infographic for the high and low inequality conditions.

The measures were, with two exceptions, low inequality condition (M = 6.81, SD = 1.86),
the same as in Study 1 and were found to be t(251.53) = 26.13, p < .001.
reliable (all αs > .70). There were two differ-
ences from Study 1: (a) rather than asking Pay inequality, categorization, and organizational function-
about the top and bottom 20%, participants ing. To test whether high (vs. low) organizational
were asked about the top and bottom 5% of pay inequality impacted comparative fit and the
their organizations, and (b) leadership percep- organizational outcomes, we first conducted a
tions were only measured using the Leader series of independent samples t tests. Means, mean
Toxicity Scale.8 difference, and standard deviations are reported in
Table 2 (please note that confidence intervals
reported reflect the mean difference between the
Results
low inequality and high inequality conditions).
Manipulation check. Our manipulation was suc- In line with the expectation that greater
cessful: participants in the high inequality condi- income inequality would increase the fit of wealth
tion perceived the pay distribution to be less categories, participants in the high (vs. low) ine-
equal (M = 1.84, SD = 1.30) than those in the quality condition expected significantly greater
12 Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 00(0)

Table 2. Means and standard deviations of organizational outcomes between experimental conditions.

Low inequality High inequality Mean difference


Comparative Fit 2.11 (1.39)a 3.47 (1.88)b −1.36
Identification 4.68 (1.22)a 3.27 (1.41)b 1.41
Conflict 2.19 (0.55)a 2.80 (0.72)b −0.61
Toxic Leadership 3.27 (1.22)a 4.81 (1.06)b −1.54

Note. Different superscripts indicate significant difference between conditions.

comparative fit, t(263.73) = −7.01, p < .001, Workplace conflict. Comparative fit (B = 0.11, p <
95% CI [−1.75, −0.98], d = 0.86. .001) significantly predicted expectations of
Moreover, in line with findings that higher workplace conflict. There was a significant total
inequality would be associated with negative effect of inequality (B = 0.61, p < .001). This
organizational outcomes, we found that partici- effect remained significant (B = 0.46, p < .001)
pants in the high (vs. low) inequality condition after controlling for comparative fit. The indirect
expected to have lower superordinate identifica- effect of inequality through comparative fit was
tion, t(279.45) = 9.03, p < .001, 95% CI [1.10, significant (B = 0.15, SE = 0.05, 95% CI [0.08,
1.71], d = 1.08, and greater workplace conflict, 0.23]).
t(267.62) = −8.09, p < .001, 95% CI [−0.76,
−0.46], d = 0.99, as well as greater leader toxicity, Leader toxicity. Comparative fit (B = 0.25, p <
t(276.94) = −11.43, p < .001, 95% CI [−1.81, .001) significantly predicted expectations of toxic
−1.28], d = 1.36. leadership. There was a significant total effect of
inequality (B = 1.54, p < .001). This effect
Pay categorization and organizational function- remained significant (B = 1.20, p < .001) after
ing. Similar to Study 1, we conducted mediation controlling for comparative fit. The indirect
analyses to test the explanatory role of compar- effect of inequality through comparative fit was
ative fit in explaining the effect of high (vs. low) significant (B = 0.35, SE = 0.07, 95% CI [0.21,
pay inequality on organizational outcomes. In 0.49]).
these mediation models, inequality significantly
predicted comparative fit (B = 1.37, p < .001).
Based on Monte Carlo power analysis for indi-
Discussion
rect effects (using Schoemann et al., 2017), we In line with findings in Study 1, we found that an
had 98–100% power to detect the indirect organization characterized by high (vs. low) pay
effects of perceived inequality via comparative inequality is likely to be seen as more dysfunc-
fit on the organizational outcomes with a sam- tional because it causes employees to perceive
ple size of 286. higher levels of inequality and, in turn, anticipate
a lower sense of superordinate identification,
Superordinate identification. Comparative fit (B = expect greater workplace conflict as well as more
−0.19, p < .001) significantly predicted superor- toxic leadership behaviors. Importantly, there was
dinate organizational identification. There was a evidence that the effects of inequality on these
significant total effect of inequality (B = −1.41, p organizational outcomes may be in part mediated
< .001). This effect remained significant (B = by the fragmentation of the organization on the
1.14, p < .001) after controlling for comparative basis of pay, such that participants in the more
fit. The indirect effect of inequality through com- unequal organization showed a tendency to cate-
parative fit was significant (B = −0.26, SE = gorize the highest and lowest paid employees as
0.07, 95% CI [−0.41, −0.12]). distinct social groups.
Tanjitpiyanond et al. 13

General Discussion of higher pay disparity (Bloom, 1999; Shaw et al.,


2002), there is little empirical evidence explaining
At present, the gap between the highest paid and why higher pay disparity has these negative out-
the lowest paid employees continues to widen. comes (Shaw, 2014). As such, the current research
Specifically, in 2020, 100 top leaders (e.g., CEOs, offers a social psychological explanation for why
top management team) within S&P 500 firms saw higher pay inequality is detrimental to organiza-
their pay increased by 29%, while average work- tional life (e.g., reduced cooperation). Moreover,
ers experienced a 2% decrease (Anderson & and in relation to this, we bring in compelling
Pizzigati, 2021). Although there is evidence that theorizing (i.e., the social identity approach) to
suggests that this rise in pay inequality has the understanding these processes. We show that the
potential to produce negative organizational out- negative outcomes associated with higher pay
comes (e.g., lower collaboration), the existing inequality may be due to inequality increasing the
organizational literature does not provide clear comparative fit of wealth categories which, in
explanations as to why or how inequality may turn, may propel employees to categorize their
have these effects (Shaw, 2014). Using the social organizations into the “haves” and “have nots.”
identity approach, the present research suggests As we have shown, such categorization, in turn,
that higher pay inequality may lead to organiza- can reinforce perceptions of division and cor-
tional dysfunction because it enhances the com- rode a sense of unity in the workplace (e.g.,
parative fit of pay as a basis for social reduced organizational identification). In this
categorization, and thus psychologically divides way, the current research not only builds on pre-
and undermines a sense of oneness in the vious work (i.e., explaining why higher pay ine-
workplace. quality is detrimental) but also lends support to
Specifically, across two studies (i.e., a field sur-
social identity theorizing and highlights its appli-
vey and an experiment), we found consistent evi-
cability to the pay inequality literature (Peters
dence that higher inequality increased the
et al., 2019; Steffens et al., 2020).
tendency for participants to perceive greater dis-
Our research also has implications for the
similarity between employees from the highest
design of organizational reward structures that
and lowest paid categories, and greater similarity
are likely to maximize the level of cooperation
between employees within the same pay category
(i.e., greater comparative fit to categorize the and efficiency of their workers. Although extrav-
organizational “haves” and “have nots” as dis- agant pay to top leaders is often seen as a reward
tinct social groups). Such categorization, in turn, for their value and performance (i.e., the value
was associated with participants reporting they generate for the organization), our results
reduced identification as members of the organi- suggest that this reward structure may backfire by
zation and increased expectations of conflict introducing division among employees. In par-
among coworkers. We also found that pay-based ticular, our results suggest that the more top lead-
categorization contributed to perceptions of the ers are paid relative to other employees (especially
current leaders as more toxic (measured in both when employees perceive this pay inequality), the
Study 1 and Study 2) and as unable to create a more likely they are to be categorized as “not one
sense of “we” and “us” (i.e., poorer identity lead- of us” by other organizational members (Steffens
ership measured in Study 1). et al., 2020). To the extent this occurs, this is likely
to be consequential for organizational perfor-
mance as there is abundant evidence showing
Implications that leaders are most able to steer the organiza-
Taken together, the present research provides a tion and guide their followers if they are per-
unique contribution to the study of organiza- ceived as in-group members (e.g., Ellemers et al.,
tional pay inequality. First, although previous 2004; Haslam et al., 2020). Indeed, given the cur-
studies have focused on the negative outcomes rent trend towards greater pay transparency in
14 Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 00(0)

many countries across the world (i.e., regulations Along similar lines, it is plausible that as inequality
for companies to make their pay structure acces- increases pay categorization, it may increase con-
sible to the public; see Dodd–Frank Act in the flict at an intergroup level (between higher and
US), which may increase perceptions of pay ine- lower paid work groups) but greater solidarity at
quality, it behoves companies to be mindful of an intragroup level (among employees in the
the way in which pay structure may have signifi- same work team). As our measure assessed gen-
cant psychological and behavioral impacts on eral conflict within the organization, it would be
their workforce. interesting for future studies to include a wider
range of measures to explore this possibility.
Moreover, it would be interesting to explore
Limitations and Future Directions
how other sociostructural features within organi-
The present research has a number of strengths. zations, such as legitimacy (or perceived fairness
In particular, our multimethod approach (i.e., of pay distribution), can interact with inequality to
data from both a field survey and an experiment) shape organizational outcomes. For instance, is it
has allowed us to provide novel evidence that the case that the negative impact of pay inequality
higher (vs. lower) pay inequality may create on organizational outcomes is weakened if ine-
organizational dysfunction because it causes pay- quality is perceived as legitimate (or fair) whereby
based categorization and division in the work- the higher paid employees are seen as more com-
place. At the same time, our studies are not petent and indispensable to organizational perfor-
without limitations. In particular, due to our study mance? Indeed, according to social identity
design, we cannot provide causal evidence that theorizing, legitimacy might impact how people
greater pay-based categorization leads to more make sense of inequality by changing the content
negative organizational outcomes (e.g., increased or stereotypes that people have of these pay cate-
perception of leader toxicity). For instance, in gories (i.e., normative fit; see Tanjitpiyanond et al.,
relation to leadership, it is also possible that the 2022). In this paper, we focused on how inequality
perception of leaders as more toxic serves to divides organizations via the principle of com-
increase the comparative fit of pay categories— parative fit, however, future studies should extend
that is, it leads to the categorization of leaders as our findings to how normative fit or stereotype
more different from lower paid employees. content of the wealthy and poor may impact how
Future research should focus on establishing the inequality shapes organizational outcomes.
directionality of these effects (i.e., manipulation Lastly, there would be value in examining how
of the mediator variable) to further test our theo- the effects we observed change across different
retical approach. industries and organizational structures, sizes,
The present research also included a limited and cultures. Specifically, it is possible, for
number of variables. For instance, we did not instance, that the strength of our effects may
include other measures of identification (except change in organizations situated in cultures that
superordinate organizational identification) and highly value seniority and hierarchy. Such values
thus were unable to pinpoint how higher pay ine- may moderate how the separation of organiza-
quality (and the subsequent fitness of pay catego- tional “haves” and “have nots” (i.e., pay categori-
ries) may influence other work-related identities zation) influences employees’ sense of shared
(e.g., based on seniority, department, or team) to identity and collaboration. In light of this, it is
reinforce a specific “us” versus “them” distinc- important to note that the data from the present
tion in the workplace. This is an important point research were collected from participants in
because research suggests that different levels of Western countries (i.e., the UK and Australia).
work-related identities (e.g., occupation, team) Thus, we acknowledge that it is possible that our
may shape perceptions and workplace coopera- results may not reflect the psychological experi-
tion in varying ways (e.g., Haslam et al., 2000). ences of employees working in non-Western
Tanjitpiyanond et al. 15

organizations. This acknowledgement is impor- 2. This research was part of a larger project examining
tant because, as alluded to before, norms that are the effect of income inequality and stereotyping in
culture-specific may interact with inequality and organizations. Thus, we also included measures of
change how pay categorization influences differ- other work-related outcomes (e.g., job satisfaction).
The research questions in the current study were
ent organizational outcomes. Future research
exploratory and none were preregistered (for a full
may address these questions.
list of measures and preregistration for the larger
project, see https://osf.io/hp4dv/?view_only=2e
Conclusion 944b9ede5d4258ab6109b0a21e36b1).
3. We also analyzed the data for both Study 1 and
In sum, the current research provides a novel lens Study 2 including these excluded participants. The
to understanding how higher pay inequality may results were generally identical, except in Study 1
harm organizational functioning. In particular, it where ratio inequality no longer predicted organi-
extends the applicability of the social identity zational identification (became nonsignificant,
approach to show that higher pay inequality may B = 0.00, p = .554) and leader toxicity (became
lead people to map wealth categories to their marginally significant, B = 0.00, p = .068), but
now did predict conflict (B = 0.00, p = .019).
work environment and introduce division to the
4. We also included another measure of inequality
organization such that employees identify less
asking participants to indicate how many times
with the superordinate organizational group, (from 0 to 100) the highest paid employees earn
experience higher levels of workplace conflict, more than the lowest paid employees. However,
and perceive their leaders more negatively. In this due to the low correlation between this meas-
way, the present paper sheds light on the way ure and the reported ratio measure (r = .20, p
higher pay inequality shapes employees’ categori- < .001), we decided to use the ratio measure as
zation processes, which, in turn, may partly our main index of inequality. We did not find any
explain why it can contribute to a state of organi- effect of the previous measure on organizational
zational dysfunction. outcomes.
5. This item was not included in the preregistra-
tion and was added just prior to data collection
Funding
to check whether participants’ perception of ine-
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following finan- quality corresponded to the ratio measure.
cial support for the research, authorship, and/or publi- 6. Please note that although we argue that reduced
cation of this article: This research contribution was leader toxicity and higher identity leadership
supported by an Australian Research Council together would increase the effectiveness of lead-
Discovery Grant (DP170101008). ers in creating a sense of “we” and “us” in the
organization, these qualities may not always guar-
ORCID iD antee the effectiveness of leaders in meeting all
concrete organizational goals (e.g., increasing the
Porntida Tanjitpiyanond https://orcid.org/0000-
company’s profit).
0003-4144-8816
7. To check the robustness of our findings, we also
combined the ratio and perceived inequality meas-
Notes ures into a single inequality index and ran media-
1. Here, we acknowledge that the stark contrast tion analyses with this index as a predictor. The
between the organization’s “haves” and “have results were the same as the findings reported in
nots” can also evoke a sense of injustice which this paper.
then reduces the likelihood of organizational 8. This study was conducted prior to Study 1. We
identification. However, the theoretical focus asked about the top and bottom 5% as it was
of this paper is not on “fairness” (which has the original design of this inequality paradigm.
been widely studied in organizational literature), However, in Study 1, we asked about the top
but on how inequality influences categorization and bottom 20% to include a larger segment
processes. of employees within participants’ organizations.
16 Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 00(0)

The Identity Leadership Scale was not included Journal of Management, 42, 862–885. https://doi.
because we only added it in Study 1 (conducted org/10.1177/0149206313503015
after Study 2). Connor, P., Varney, J., Keltner, D., & Chen, S.
(2020). Social class competence stereotypes
References are amplified by socially signaled economic
inequality. Personality and Social Psychology Bul-
Adler, N., & Stewart, J. (2007). The MacArthur Scale of
letin. Advance online publication. https://doi.
Subjective Social Status. MacArthur Research Net-
org/10.1177/0146167220916640
work on SES & Health.
Ellemers, N., De Gilder, D., & Haslam, A. S. (2004).
Akerlof, G. A., & Kranton, R. E. (2010). Identity econom-
Motivating individuals and groups at work: A
ics. Princeton University Press.
social identity perspective on leadership and
Anderson, S., & Pizzigati, S. (2021). Pandemic pay
group performance. Academy of Management
plunder: Low-wage workers lost hours, jobs, and lives.
Review, 29, 459–478. https://doi.org/10.5465/
Their employers bent rules to pump up CEO paychecks.
amr.2004.13670967
Institute for Policy Studies. https://ips-dc.org/
Hains, S. C., Hogg, M. A., & Duck, J. M. (1997).
report-executive-excess-2021
Self-categorization and leadership: Effects
Bloom, M. (1999). The performance effects of pay dis-
of group prototypicality and leader ste-
persion on individuals and organizations. Acad-
reotypicality. Personality and Social Psychol-
emy of Management Journal, 42, 25–40. https://doi.
ogy Bulletin, 23, 1087–1099. https://doi.
org/10.5465/256872
org/10.1177/01461672972310009
Bratanova, B., Summers, J., Liu, S., & Vauclair, M. C.
Haslam, A. S., Powell, C., & Turner, J. (2000). Social
(2019). A rising tide lifts some boats but leaves
identity, self-categorization, and work motiva-
many others behind: The harms of inequality-
tion: Rethinking the contribution of the group
induced status seeking and the remedial effects
to positive and sustainable organisational out-
of employee ownership. In J. Jetten & K. Peters
comes. Applied Psychology, 49, 319–339. https://
(Eds.), The social psychology of inequality (pp. 67–83).
doi.org/10.1111/1464-0597.00018
Springer.
Haslam, A. S., Reicher, S. D., & Platow, M. J. (2020).
Breza, E., Kaur, S., & Shamdasani, Y. (2018). The
The new psychology of leadership: Identity, influence and
morale effects of pay inequality. The Quarterly
power. Routledge.
Journal of Economics, 133, 611–663. https://doi.
Henderson, A. D., & Fredrickson, J. W. (2001). Top
org/10.1093/qje/qjx041
management team coordination needs and the
Brown, P. M., & Turner, J. C. (2002). The role of theo-
CEO pay gap: A competitive test of economic and
ries in the formation of stereotype content. In C.
behavioral views. Academy of Management Journal,
McGarty, V. Y. Yzerbyt & R. Spears (Eds.), Stereo-
44, 96–117. https://doi.org/10.5465/3069339
types as explanations: The formation of meaningful beliefs
Hogg, M. A., Hains, S. C., & Mason, I. (1998). Identi-
about social groups (pp. 67–89). Cambridge Uni-
fication and leadership in small groups: Salience,
versity Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO978
0511489877.005 frame of reference, and leader stereotypicality
Cheng, L., Hao, M., & Wang, F. (2021). Beware of the effects on leader evaluations. Journal of Personality
“bad guys”: Economic inequality, perceived com- and Social Psychology, 75, 1248–1263. https://doi.
petition, and social vigilance. International Review org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.5.1248
of Social Psychology, 34, 9. http://doi.org/10.5334/ Hogg, M., & Terry, D. J. (2000). Social identity and
irsp.497 self-categorization processes in organizational
Cheung, F., & Lucas, R. E. (2016). Income inequality is context. Academy of Management Review, 25, 121–
associated with stronger social comparison effects: 140. https://doi.org/10.2307/259266
The effect of relative income on life satisfaction. Jetten, J., Peters, K., Álvarez, B., Casara, B. G. S.,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110, 332– Dare, M., Kirkland, K., Sánchez-Rodríguez, Á.,
341. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000059 Selvanathan, H. P., Sprong, S., Tanjitpiyanond,
Connelly, B. L., Haynes, K. T., Tihanyi, L., P., Wang, Z., & Mols, F. (2021). Consequences of
Gamache, D. L., & Devers, C. E. (2016). Mind- economic inequality for societies’ social and political vital-
ing the gap: Antecedents and consequences ity: A social identity analysis. Political Psychology, 42,
of top management-to-worker pay dispersion. 241–266. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12800
Tanjitpiyanond et al. 17

Jetten, J., Postmes, T., & McAuliffe, B. J. (2002). Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1508–1519. https://doi.
“We’re all individuals”: Group norms of individu- org/10.1177/01461672012711011
alism and collectivism, levels of identification and Platow, M. J., van Knippenberg, D., Haslam, S. A.,
identity threat. European Journal of Social Psychology, van Knippenberg, B., & Spears, R. (2006). A spe-
32, 189–207. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.65 cial gift we bestow on you for being representa-
Jetten, J., Wang, Z., Steffens, N. K., Mols, F., Peters, tive of us: Considering leader charisma from a
K., & Verkuyten, M. (2017). A social identity self-categorization perspective. British Journal
analysis of responses to economic inequality. of Social Psychology, 45, 303–320. https://doi.
Current Opinion in Psychology, 18, 1–5. https://doi. org/10.1348/014466605X41986
org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.05.011 Putnam-Farr, E., & Morewedge, C. K. (2020). Which
Kraus, M. W., Park, J. W., & Tan, J. J. (2017). Signs social comparisons influence happiness with
of social class: The experience of economic unequal pay? Journal of Experimental Psychology:
inequality in everyday life. Perspectives on Psy- General, 150, 570–582. https://doi.org/10.1037/
chological Science, 12, 422–435. https://doi. xge0000965
org/10.1177/1745691616673192 Schmidt, A. A. (2008). Development and validation of the
Mishel, L., & Kandra, J. (2020). CEO compensation Toxic Leadership Scale (Publication No. 1453699)
surged 14% in 2019 to $21.3 million. Economic Pol- [Doctoral dissertation, University of Maryland].
icy Institute. https://www.epi.org/publication/ ProQuest Dissertations.
ceo-compensation-surged-14-in-2019-to-21-3- Schoemann, A. M., Boulton, A. J., & Short, S. D.
million-ceos-now-earn-320-times-as-much-as-a- (2017). Determining power and sample size for
typical-worker/ simple and complex mediation models. Social
Penney, L. M., & Spector, P. E. (2005). Job stress, Psychological and Personality Science, 8(4), 379–386.
incivility, and counterproductive work behavior https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617715068
(CWB): The moderating role of negative affectiv- Shaw, J. D. (2014). Pay dispersion. Annual Review of
ity. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26, 777–796. Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behav-
https://doi.org/10.1002/job.336 iour, 1, 521–544. https://doi.org/10.1146/
Peters, K., Fonseca, M. A., Haslam, S. A., Steffens, N. annurev-orgpsych-031413-091253
K., & Quiggin, J. (2019). Fat cats and thin fol- Shaw, J. D., Gupta, N., & Delery, J. E. (2002). Pay dis-
lowers: Excessive CEO pay may reduce ability to persion and workforce performance: Moderating
lead. In J. Jetten & K. Peters (Eds.), The social psy- effects of incentives and interdependence. Strate-
chology of inequality (pp. 21–34). Springer. gic Management Journal, 23, 491–512. https://doi.
Peters, K., Jetten, J., Tanjitpiyanond, P., Zhechen, W., org/10.1002/smj.235
Mols, F., & Verkuyten, M. (2021). The language of Siegel, P. A., & Hambrick, D. C. (2005). Pay dispari-
inequality: Evidence economic inequality increases ties within top management groups: Evidence
wealth category salience. Personality and Social of harmful effects on performance of high-tech-
Psychology Bulletin. Advance online publication. nology firms. Organization Science, 16, 259–274.
https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672211036627 https://doi.org/10.1287/orsc.1050.0128
Pfeffer, J., & Langton, N. (1993). The effect of wage Steffens, N. K., Haslam, S. A., Peters, K., & Quig-
dispersion on satisfaction, productivity, and work- gin, J. (2020). Identity economics meets iden-
ing collaboratively: Evidence from college and tity leadership: Exploring the consequences of
university faculty. Administrative Science Quarterly, elevated CEO pay. The Leadership Quarterly, 31,
38, 382–407. https://doi.org/10.2307/2393373 Article 101269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lea-
Platow, M. J., Grace, D. M., Wilson, N., Burton, D., qua.2018.10.001
& Wilson, A. (2008). Psychological group mem- Steffens, N. K., Haslam, S. A., Reicher, S. D., Platow,
berships as outcomes of resource distributions. M. J., Fransen, K., Yang, J., Ryan, M. K., Jetten,
European Journal of Social Psychology, 38(5), 836–851. J., & Boen, F. (2014). Leadership as social iden-
https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.48 tity management: Introducing the Identity Lead-
Platow, M. J., & van Knippenberg, D. (2001). A social ership Inventory (ILI) to assess and validate a
identity analysis of leadership endorsement: The four-dimensional model. The Leadership Quarterly,
effects of leader ingroup prototypicality and dis- 25, 1001–1024. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lea-
tributive intergroup fairness. Personality and Social qua.2014.05.002
18 Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 00(0)

Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative the- Walasek, L., & Brown, G. D. (2016). Income inequal-
ory of intergroup conflict. In W. G. Austin & S. ity, income, and internet searches for status goods:
Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup rela- A cross-national study of the association between
tions (pp. 33–37). Brooks/Cole. inequality and well-being. Social Indicators Research,
Tanjitpiyanond, P., Jetten, J., & Peters, K. (2022). 129, 1001–1014. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-
How economic inequality shapes social class ste- 015-1158-4
reotyping. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, Walasek, L., & Brown, G. D. (2019). Income inequal-
98, Article 104248. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. ity and social status: The social rank and material
jesp.2021.104248 rank hypotheses. In J. Jetten & K. Peters (Eds.),
Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. The social psychology of inequality (pp. 235–248).
D., & Wetherell, M. S. (1987). Rediscovering the social Springer.
group: A self-categorization theory. Basil Blackwell. Zhao, X., Lynch, J. G., Jr., & Chen, Q. (2010).
Walasek, L., & Brown, G. D. (2015). Income inequality Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: Myths and
and status seeking: Searching for positional goods truths about mediation analysis. Journal of
in unequal U.S. states. Psychological Science, 26, 527– Consumer Research, 37, 197–206. https://doi.
533. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614567511 org/10.1086/651257

You might also like