Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/338409260

Social Dominance Orientation (SDO)

Chapter · January 2020


DOI: 10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_2602-1

CITATIONS READS

8 2,161

3 authors, including:

Thomas Haarklau Kleppestø Lotte Thomsen


University of Oslo University of Oslo
17 PUBLICATIONS   65 CITATIONS    54 PUBLICATIONS   2,086 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

PREPRINT: Toddlers prefer those who win, but not when they win by force View project

Acknowledging common Abrahamic heritage View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Thomas Haarklau Kleppestø on 06 January 2020.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


S

Social Dominance Orientation resources such as prestige, wealth, social status,


(SDO) healthcare, food, homes, mates, and so on. Social
dominance theory (SDT, Sidanius and Pratto
Thomas Haarklau Kleppestø1, 1999) asks the questions why and how group-
Nikolai Haahjem Eftedal1 and Lotte Thomsen1,2 based hierarchies are continuously reproduced,
1
Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, at least among surplus-producing societies. The
Oslo, Norway theoretical framework spans macrostructural,
2
Department of Political Science, Aarhus institutional, ideological, social role, individual,
University, Aarhus, Denmark and behavioral genetic levels of analysis to
address this question and postulates that humans
have a predisposition to navigate group-based
Synonyms social structures (Kleppestø et al. 2019; Kunst
et al. 2017; Pratto et al. 2006; Sidanius et al.
Between-group hierarchy; Dominance; 2016; Sidanius and Pratto 1999).
Evolutionary political psychology; Political Conflicts between groups are ubiquitous across
attitudes; Prejudice; Sex differences; Warfare history and cultures. Ideologies that justify
beliefs about the superiority of dominating groups
might serve the function of reducing conflicts by
Definition legitimizing the hierarchical status quo (Pratto
et al. 1994; Sidanius and Pratto 1999). According
Social dominance orientation is a measurement of to SDT, when these ideologies are widely
“the general desire to establish and maintain accepted within a society, they justify discrimina-
hierarchically structured intergroup relations tion and allow some groups to dominate over
regardless of the position of one’s own group others based on their gender, age, and, especially,
(s) within this hierarchy” (Sidanius et al. 2016, socially constructed markers of group member-
p. 152). ship such as class, race, ethnicity, caste, and
religion – what SDT coins arbitrary sets. The
beliefs and ideologies that justify dominance hier-
Introduction archies are called hierarchy-enhancing legitimiz-
ing myths; examples are the “divine rights of
Human societies tend to structure themselves as kings,” sexism, and the Protestant work ethic
group-based social hierarchies such that some suggesting that you get what you personally
groups enjoy greater access to fitness-relevant deserve (myths in this context mean widely shared
© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2020
T. K. Shackelford, V. A. Weekes-Shackelford (eds.), Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_2602-1
2 Social Dominance Orientation (SDO)

ideologies, not implying that they are false or averaged to form an SDO score (with half the
true). Beliefs that justify flat or egalitarian group items being reverse scored). Below we discuss
structures are called hierarchy-attenuating legiti- the dimensionality of SDO and how SDO relates
mizing myths, examples being the universal to other variables and constructs.
rights of man, socialism, and feminism (Sidanius
et al. 2016; Sidanius and Pratto 1999). Impor- Dimensionality
tantly, SDT postulates that particular instances of SDO was previously considered a unitary con-
modern group-based discrimination such as rac- struct, but increasingly there is evidence that the
ism, classism, sexism, and nationalism are mani- construct can be divided into two: dominance
festations of more general processes where (SDO-D) and egalitarianism (SDO-E) (Ho et al.
dominant groups maintain social, economic, and 2012, 2015). SDO-D reflects preferences for
military supremacy over subordinates (Sidanius active oppression of groups and is most reflective
et al. 2016). of hostile beliefs such as old-fashioned racism,
At the individual level of analysis, endorse- dehumanization, and support for war (Sidanius
ment of such intergroup dominance hierarchies et al. 2016). SDO-E, on the other hand, reflects a
is reflected in the individual difference of preference for inequality or at least an opposition
social dominance orientation (SDO) (Ho et al. to active measures toward reducing inequality.
2015; Pratto et al. 1994) to which we now turn SDO-E is also often labeled “anti-egalitarianism.”
our attention. This general relational motive to SDO-D and SDO-E have been shown to con-
enhance versus attenuate between-group hierar- stitute separate facets of SDO in factor analyses,
chies has proven one of the most robust predictors and they also differ when used as predictors for
of intergroup attitudes (Pratto et al. 2006; Sidanius conceptually relevant variables. In the overview
et al. 2016; Sidanius and Pratto 1999), from sup- of relations between SDO and other traits below,
port for social welfare to ethnic persecution of we discuss correlations for full-scale SDO scores,
immigrants (Dunwoody and McFarland 2018; while noting differences between SDO-D and
Ho et al. 2012, 2015; Thomsen et al. 2008). SDO-E whenever relevant.
Here, we discuss the conceptual and psychometric
properties of SDO, and we highlight some key Predictive Power of SDO
evolutionary forces that might explain SDO’s SDT predicts that SDO will correlate positively
inter-individual variation and mean-level sex with support for hierarchy-enhancing and nega-
differences. tively with hierarchy-attenuating ideologies or
legitimizing myths. Consistent with this, SDO
has been tied to support for a wide range of atti-
The Construct of Social Dominance tudes about ideology and politics, including polit-
Orientation (SDO) ical and, particularly, economic conservatism and
support for social welfare and affirmative action
Sidanius et al. (2016, p. 152) define SDO as (Sidanius and Pratto 1999). And SDO continues
“the general desire to establish and maintain to predict emergent current phenomena, such as
hierarchically structured intergroup relations attitudes toward Syrian refugees and voting for
regardless of the position of one’s own group Donald Trump (Dunwoody and McFarland 2018;
(s) within this hierarchy.” Since its first introduc- Sheehy-Skeffington and Thomsen 2019). Most
tion in 1994 (Pratto et al. 1994), the SDO scale has centrally, SDO has been found to consistently
gone through several iterations. The most recent predict most kinds of prejudicial attitudes. This
version, the SDO7 (Ho et al. 2015), consists of includes prejudice against women, the poor, eth-
16 statements (such as “Some groups of people nic/racial minorities, LGBTQ people, and immi-
must be kept in their place”), which responders grants (see Sidanius et al. 2016, for an overview of
rate their levels of agreement with on 7-point studies on this). Prejudicial attitudes predicted
Likert scales. Scores on these items can be from having low scores on SDO are less studied,
Social Dominance Orientation (SDO) 3

though there are indications that they could have shifted away from this to propose that SDO
include prejudice against political conservatives reflects a person’s general orientation toward the
and against social groups deemed to be privileged idea of hierarchies between groups. Consistent
(Lucas and Kteily 2018). with SDT’s prediction of ideological asymmetry,
SDO has also been explored in relation to this suggests that a black American with a high
personality traits. In particular, SDO has been score on SDO would not desire blacks to domi-
connected to many personality traits that are nate whites, but would rather endorse the existing
socially undesirable (Ho et al. 2015), such as the hierarchical status quo, even though this implies
Dark Triad traits of Machiavellianism, narcissism, that whites continue to have more status and
and psychopathy, which are particularly strongly power than blacks (Ho et al. 2015; Sidanius and
linked to SDO-D. Relatedly, SDO-D in particular Pratto 1999). Accordingly, SDO is more posi-
is inversely correlated with the honesty/humility tively associated with ethnic identity, perceived
dimension of the HEXACO personality inven- ethnic victimization, and other variables related
tory, which reflects an unwillingness to “get to in-group favoritism among dominant than sub-
ahead” through dishonest means. Among the Big ordinate groups (Sidanius et al. 2016; Sidanius
Five traits, SDO is most strongly related to agree- and Pratto 1999; Thomsen et al. 2010).
ableness and openness to experience, with both SDO is also related to, but yet distinct from,
correlations being negative here as well. The the concept of right-wing authoritarianism
Empathic Concern scale is also negatively related (RWA). RWA is manifested in submission to tra-
to SDO (Lucas and Kteily 2018). ditional authorities and in aggressive enforcement
of established norms and customs. Thus, RWA
Distinctions Between SDO and Related and SDO are conceptually related, as they both
Constructs concern attitudes toward hierarchy. Additionally,
To further clarify what the SDO construct is SDO and RWA tend to correlate quite substan-
meant to represent, we here go through tially (usually at about r = 0.40), and they are both
three constructs that SDO is related to but impor- good predictors of many kinds of prejudicial
tantly distinct from. These are interpersonal dom- attitudes.
inance, in-group favoritism, and right-wing However, SDO and RWA also have important
authoritarianism. theoretical and empirical distinctions. The dual-
Upon hearing the name “social dominance process motivational model of ideological atti-
orientation,” a natural interpretation is that it tudes (Duckitt and Sibley 2010) proposes that
reflects how oriented someone is toward being RWA reflects needs for social order and stable
interpersonally dominant in social settings. But traditions, which result from seeing the world as
this is not what SDO is meant to measure, and a threatening and dangerous place. Thus, people
the discriminant validity of SDO from standard high in RWA should prefer authorities that empha-
personality measures of interpersonal dominance size order and law and that defend traditional
was established early on (Pratto et al. 1994; values. High SDO rather implies perceiving soci-
Sidanius and Pratto 1999): SDO concerns prefer- ety as a competitive jungle, where you need to
ences regarding dominance hierarchies between fight for power and control over others. People
groups in society, and so it is conceptually quite high in SDO therefore tend to prefer authorities
distinct from being interpersonally dominant. that favor social inequality and group dominance.
Another misinterpretation of SDO is that it While both RWA and SDO are associated with
necessarily involves a preference that one’s own prejudice toward outgroups, the particular types
group should be the one to dominate, so that it of prejudice predicted from them are importantly
would correspond to in-group favoritism. Such an different. According to Duckitt and Sibley (2010),
interpretation would indeed align with how SDO RWA predicts dislike of groups that are socially
was originally conceptualized (e.g., Pratto et al. deviant and/or norm-violating, whereas SDO
1994), but contemporary understandings of SDO predicts prejudice against groups that are low in
4 Social Dominance Orientation (SDO)

status. For example, Thomsen et al. (2008) dem- serve to uphold and enhance hierarchies, and
onstrated in Switzerland and Southwestern USA the theory predicts not only that people will self-
that for people high in RWA, negative attitudes select to hierarchy-enhancing versus hierarchy-
toward immigrants were mainly provoked by attenuating institutions depending on their SDO
immigrants’ refusal to assimilate into the domi- levels but also that membership in hierarchy-
nant culture and adhere to established norms, thus enhancing institutions will lead to corresponding
threatening cultural conventionality. In contrast, increases in SDO (Sidanius and Pratto 1999).
SDO predicted aggression toward immigrants that Examples of institutions that have been described
assimilated too much into the dominant culture to have hierarchy-enhancing elements are the
and thus threatened the existing status boundaries. criminal justice system and the police force,
housing, labor, healthcare, retail, and education
SDO Is Contingent upon Hierarchical Position (Sidanius and Pratto 1999).
but Is also a Stable Trait Consistent with this, Sidanius et al. (1994)
There are myriad studies documenting that SDO found that police officers in LA had higher mean
responds to the hierarchical nature of the context. SDO than students and jurors (who represented a
Perhaps most importantly, it has been shown random sample of Los Angeles citizens), whereas
many times over that one’s level of SDO is highly public defenders (working at the public defenders
contingent on the placing of one’s own group in office, which is a HA institution) were found to be
the hierarchy. Consistent with how SDO only significantly less social dominance-oriented than
reflects self-interest when measured in dominant both students and jurors.
groups, people from lower-ranking groups tend to SDO scores can also vary on more of a
score substantially lower on SDO. For example, moment-to-moment basis when one’s salient
the difference in mean SDO between blacks and position in the hierarchical context changes. In a
whites in the USA is substantial (e.g., Ho et al. study illustrating this, Levin (1997) divided the
2015). It has been found that as the objective, SDO scale in half and put one half alongside
salient, or perceived difference in power between questions about the tensions between Ashkenazi
groups becomes larger, so too does the difference and Mizrachi Jews in Israel and the other half
in SDO levels between the groups (e.g., Levin alongside questions about the tensions between
2004). Jews and Arabs. In the section on Jew conflict,
Consistent with this, in a meta-analysis across Ashkenazis (who are seen as higher in status)
27 countries, the average SDO level among scored higher on SDO than Mizrachis. But in the
dominant groups corresponded to the macrostruc- section on tensions between Jews and Arabs
tural level of inequality, that is, to the status (where both groups of Jews are higher in status),
gap between dominant and subordinates, consis- then both groups of Jews scored higher than in the
tent with dove-hawk type dynamics (Fischer other condition, and the difference in scores
et al. 2012). Accordingly, SDO among dominant between the groups disappeared.
groups also correlated across nations with macro- Interestingly, Levin’s (1997) study of contex-
level indicators of poor governance, corruption, tual influences on SDO also illustrates how SDO
lack of rule of law, and democracy, all consistent also can be seen as a stable trait. Specifically, as
with the notion that dominant groups are more has been shown in many studies, SDO has high
likely to coercively claim resources the greater rank-order stability. This means that people with
the macrostructural inequality. In an independent relatively high scores in one context will be likely
sample collected across 30 US states, macrostruc- to be high scorers in other contexts. Despite the
tural inequality (Gini) across states predicted SDO large shifts in mean SDO levels between condi-
and mediated the effect of SDO on attitudes tions in Levin’s study, correlations between SDO
toward minorities (Kunst et al. 2017). scores from the same participant were still sub-
Just as SDT describes hierarchy-enhancing stantial. Levin (2004) conceptually replicated
myths, it also describes hierarchy-enhancing these effects in Northern Ireland among Catholics
social institutions. These are institutions which and Protestants and further demonstrated that the
Social Dominance Orientation (SDO) 5

difference in SDO between dominant and subor- they are below the midpoint of the scale (see
dinate groups are moderated by personal percep- Lucas and Kteily 2018). Plausibly, SDO scores
tion of the nature and size of the status gap. might be shifted downward, e.g., through social
Recently, Bratt et al. (2016) demonstrated desirability bias. If so, then the “true” SDO level
the rank-order stability of SDO and its predictive of someone scoring 3.8 could be well above 4, and
power on outgroup prejudice over time using so they would have high SDO in both a relative
latent growth-curve modeling among large sam- and an absolute sense. Also, the low scores may
ples of Norwegian teenagers and American simply reflect a general human aversion to strong
university students. Thomsen et al. (2010) dem- group hierarchy or a preference for between-
onstrated the stable relations of SDO scores across group equality, relatively speaking. Indeed, SDO
more than 3 years, as well as their longitudinal, scores would presumably only be shifted down-
cross-lagged effects on perceived white victim- ward due to social desirability concerns if there is
hood and prejudice. In summary, SDO does, con- a widespread normative default expectation of
trary to the claims of some critics, constitute a equality (that makes endorsement of between-
generalized orientation toward intergroup hierar- group dominance socially undesirable). Recent
chy that holds regardless of which groups people evidence from preverbal human infants suggests
are instructed to hold in mind when they fill out this is in fact the case, at least for default expecta-
the SDO scale. tions of equal resource distribution (see Sheehy-
Skeffington and Thomsen 2019).
Interpretation of Low SDO Scores In any case, although the majority of people
SDO is often skewed toward the lower end of may disagree with strong intergroup oppression in
the scale. For instance, in Ho et al.’s (2015) vali- absolute terms, the variance between strongly and
dation study of the SDO7 scale, the average SDO less strongly disagreeing with between-group
score across six US samples on a 7-point Likert- hierarchy, as captured by the SDO scale, has
scale was 2.6, corresponding to a percentage of proven one of the most robust predictors of
maximum possible agreement (POMP) score of intergroup phenomena in political psychology.
27.2 (ranging from 0 to 100), including a repre- Social dominance theory attempts to understand
sentative population sample, the SDO mean of how this variance intersects with macrostructure
which was 3.0, corresponding to a POMP score and immediate institutional contexts and social
of 32.5. Consistent with this, meta-analytical esti- roles to explain the continuous reproduction of
mates indicated that the mean SDO POMP score the hierarchical status quo (Sidanius and Pratto
across 27 nations was at 25.8, ranging from 15.7 1999).
in Switzerland to 63.0 in Japan (Fischer
et al. 2012).
This raises issues about how scores are to be Evolution of Dominance and
categorized. An SDO score of 3.8, for example, is Egalitarianism
below the scale midpoint of 4, yet it would place
you about a standard deviation above the mean in As we have seen, social dominance orientation
Ho et al.’s samples. So, if you score at 3.8, do you predicts a plethora of political beliefs and ideolo-
then have high or low SDO? Are you predicted to gies. In the ethological literature, a concept known
want to attenuate or amplify hierarchies? Notably, as behavioral syndrome captures traits that tend to
SDO correlates very strongly with the anti- correlate across situations and contexts. Given
egalitarianism scale (r = 0.85 in Lucas and Kteily that SDO correlates with many related political
2018), which has scores more evenly distributed beliefs, can these patterns be described as a behav-
around its scale midpoint than SDO has. Thus, ioral syndrome?
it could be argued that SDO scores that are high Strategies of dominance and egalitarianism
relative to most sample means can be taken to have deep evolutionary roots and can often coex-
represent opposition to egalitarianism, even if ist within the same species. In game theoretical
6 Social Dominance Orientation (SDO)

competitions for dominance, the contests often This maintenance of genetic variation can occur
follow a hawk-dove dynamics where more formi- with personality traits because humans select
dable individuals (hawks) will prevail in such environments and alliances that match their traits
zero-sum conflicts, claiming contested resources, (niche-specialization or active gene-environment
while subordinates (doves) will yield to more correlation). Individuals will then maximize the
formidable opponents, so as to minimize costly chance of obtaining fitness-relevant resources
fighting and injury. Consistent with the notion of such as social alliances and mates.
evolved representations and motives for social The extensive evidence that people self-select
dominance, greater physical formidability is also to areas of study and professional careers that
associated with greater dominance motives and match their levels of SDO in terms of being
claiming of societal resources among humans hierarchy-enhancing or attenuating (e.g. people
(Petersen et al. 2013). Indeed, even preverbal who are high in SDO are more likely to chose to
infants use cues of formidability and predict that become police officer and people low in SDO are
the novel agents who are largest, most numerous more likely to chose to become social workers)
and have won before will prevail in zero-sum and also stay longer and do better in these educa-
conflict over smaller, less numerous, and previ- tions and jobs the better this SDO-institution
ously weaker ones (Thomsen et al. 2011). match is (e.g. students with lower SDO get better
Importantly, egalitarian strategies are likely grades in traditionally hierarchy-attenuating edu-
to have evolutionary underpinnings as well. cations), and, finally, also socialize SDO to go up
In several primate species, coalitions form or down according to the hierarchy-enhancing or
among lower-ranked males in order to gain attenuating nature of the institution (Gatto and
access to fitness-relevant resources guarded by Dambrun 2012; Sidanius and Pratto 1999) pro-
higher-ranking individuals. Furthermore, hunter- vide support for this proposal. This also suggests
gatherers do display levelling mechanisms that that any heritability of SDO must be understood
keep any one individual from gaining despotic in its broadest sense to allow for such niche-
control (Boehm 1999). building gene-environment associations, where
If both dominant and egalitarian intergroup SDO leads to the selection of environments that
strategies, as tapped by low and high SDO, are further adjust SDO levels in congruent ways.
maintained in human populations, what evolu-
tionary genetic forces maintain them? A recent
investigation using genetic sensitive data found Evolution of Sex Differences in Social
that SDO and related political beliefs were all Dominance Orientation
heritable and that the covariation between them
are genetic in origin (Kleppestø et al. 2019). Across societies men score higher on SDO than
It is possible, then, that both endorsement women (Lee et al. 2011). Typically this difference
and opposition to intergroup hierarchies and the is on the magnitude of about half a point on the
social policies that are meant to amplify or atten- scale (e.g., ~2.9 for men vs. ~2.5 for women in Ho
uate them build on universal adaptations for nav- et al. 2015). In contrast to the differences in scores
igating social hierarchies. Given the enormous between ethnic groups, which are thought to be
variation in available resources across ecological entirely due to contextual factors, this sex differ-
and social contexts throughout human history, it is ence is argued to be substantially less context
unlikely that a single optimal level of hierarchy- dependent. This claim about stable gender differ-
preference exists. Hence, it is plausible that some ences on SDO is called the “invariance hypothe-
kind of balancing selection may maintain the sis” by SDO theoreticians and has been replicated
heritable variation in SDO, for example, through across many cultures (Lee et al. 2011).
migration-selection balance, a process where Why would the sexes differ in attitudes and
genetic variation is maintained due to the chang- behavior related to intergroup aggression? Sexual
ing selection pressures over time and space. selection and parental investment theory suggest
Social Dominance Orientation (SDO) 7

that the payoff for costly intergroup aggression where coalitional aggression was much more
might be very different for men and women common than it is today (Tooby and Cosmides
(Tooby and Cosmides 2010). In mammalian spe- 2010). Indeed, high degrees of coalitional aggres-
cies, the fitness of males is constrained by access sion can be observed among current hunter-
to fertile women. For women however, fitness gatherer tribes.
payoffs are not constrained by the availability of SDT argues that men are not just the primary
fertile males, but rather by the time and resources agents of discrimination based on arbitrary-set
required for successful gestation and lactation. group differences (such as race), but also the
This pattern gives rise to strong intrasexual com- main target of it (Sidanius and Pratto 1999).
petition among men for access to fertile females. Experimental psychophysiological evidence on
This gives rise to men fighting other men individ- prepared fear-learning supports the proposition
ually, but also for intergroup competition where that we tend to primarily be wary of outgroup
men raid, or attempt to dominate, other groups for males, rather than females (Navarrete et al.
access to the reproductive resources within them. 2009). For women the argument has been made
Furthermore, coalitional aggression has also been that such outgroup bias is primarily motivated by
observed in closely related primates (Wilson and fear of sexual coercion, while for males it is based
Wrangham 2003). on motives for aggression and social dominance
Based on this, social dominance theory (Navarrete et al. 2010). In other words, sex is an
(Sidanius and Pratto 1999) predicted that important variable in both the amount of prejudice
intergroup conflict between socially constructed, held by individuals but also in who is targeted for
arbitrary-set alliances (such as race, ethnicity, cul- discrimination.
ture, class, caste, religion) will be a primarily Another crucial part of the male warrior
male-on-male phenomena. This implies that the hypothesis is that it predicts variation among
main targets of intergroup oppression will be sub- males in their desire to dominate other groups.
ordinate males (i.e., in an American context black That is, men are expected to calibrate their desire
and Hispanic males; in a European context Mus- for dominance based on their own formidability,
lim/Middle Eastern and African males), and so leadership ability, available alliances, and other
SDT coined this idea the subordinate male target environmental information (Petersen et al. 2013).
hypothesis (SMTH) (Sidanius and Pratto 1999), In fact, the association between physical formida-
described more generally elsewhere as the male bility and SDO holds even when controlling for
warrior hypothesis (McDonald et al. 2012). It the time spent lifting weights, suggesting that
postulates that men engage in coalitional aggres- physical formidability directly drives SDO, rather
sion for direct sexual access but also indirectly for than the reverse (Price et al. 2017; Sheehy-
foraging territories, social influence, power, and Skeffington and Thomsen 2019).
status, and it may explain why men are much more
likely to be both perpetrators and victims of
intergroup violence and why men tend to be less Conclusion
egalitarian in their social attitudes, such as SDO.
Indeed, the available evidence strongly suggest Human societies tend to be structured as hierar-
that men are more prejudiced, conservative, puni- chies where dominant groups enjoy a greater
tive, and racist than women are (Sidanius and access to fitness-relevant resources. Social domi-
Pratto 1999). Nevertheless, modern expressions nance orientation (SDO) measures the degree to
of prejudice are probably not adaptations in and which individuals prefer that dominant groups
of themselves (Navarrete et al. 2010). However, stay on top, or if society should be structured in
they might have important evolutionary underpin- a more egalitarian fashion. SDO predicts many
nings, such as the alliance detection system. That important attitudes, such as political beliefs, prej-
is, modern prejudice might be expressions of psy- udice, and life choices. Men tend to score higher,
chological adaptations that evolved in a context which might reflect men’s sexually selected
8 Social Dominance Orientation (SDO)

tendency to be motivated to engage in intrasexual and measuring preferences for intergroup inequality
competition for social status, which in turn give using the new SDO7 scale. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 109(6), 1003.
them access to fertile females. Humans are likely Kleppestø, T. H., Czajkowski, N. O., Vassend, O.,
to possess adaptations designed for both domi- Røysamb, E., Eftedal, N. H., Kunst, J. R., . . .
nance and egalitarianism. Thomsen, L. (2019). Correlations between social dom-
inance orientation and political attitudes reflect
common genetic underpinnings: A twin study.
https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/9bfaj
Cross-References Kunst, J. R., Fischer, R., Sidanius, J., & Thomsen,
L. (2017). Preferences for group dominance track and
▶ Context, Environment, and Learning in Evolu- mediate the effects of macro-level social inequality and
violence across societies. Proceedings of the National
tionary Psychology Academy of Sciences, 114(21), 5407–5412.
▶ Dominance in Humans Lee, I.-C., Pratto, F., & Johnson, B. T. (2011). Intergroup
▶ Evolutionary Personality Psychology consensus/disagreement in support of group-based
▶ Female Choice for Men High in SDO hierarchy: An examination of socio-structural and
psycho-cultural factors. Psychological Bulletin,
▶ Indicators and Correlates of Status and 137(6), 1029.
Dominance Levin, S. (1997). A social psychological approach to
▶ Political Affiliation understanding intergroup attitudes in the United States
▶ Primate Dominance Hierarchies and Israel (Unpublished doctoral dissertation),
University of California, Los Angeles. (Ph.D.),
▶ Self-Beneficial Political Attitudes University of California, Los Angeles
▶ Status and Dominance Hierarchies Levin, S. (2004). Perceived group status differences and
the effects of gender, ethnicity, and religion on social
dominance orientation. Political Psychology, 25(1),
31–48.
References Lucas, B. J., & Kteily, N. S. (2018). (Anti-) egalitarianism
differentially predicts empathy for members of
Boehm, C. (1999). Hierarchy in the forest: The evolution advantaged versus disadvantaged groups. Journal of
of egalitarian behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Personality and Social Psychology, 114(5), 665.
University Press. McDonald, M. M., Navarrete, C. D., &
Bratt, C., Sidanius, J., & Sheehy-Skeffington, J. (2016). Van Vugt, M. (2012). Evolution and the psychology
Shaping the development of prejudice: Latent growth of intergroup conflict: The male warrior hypothesis.
modeling of the influence of social dominance Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B,
orientation on outgroup affect in youth. Personality 367(1589), 670–679.
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42(12), 1617–1634. Navarrete, C. D., Olsson, A., Ho, A. K., Mendes, W. B.,
Duckitt, J., & Sibley, C. G. (2010). Personality, ideology, Thomsen, L., & Sidanius, J. (2009). Fear extinction to
prejudice, and politics: A dual-process motivational an out-group face: The role of target gender.
model. Journal of Personality, 78(6), 1861–1894. Psychological Science, 20(2), 155–158.
Dunwoody, P. T., & McFarland, S. G. (2018). Support for Navarrete, C. D., McDonald, M. M., Molina, L. E., &
anti-Muslim policies: The role of political traits and Sidanius, J. (2010). Prejudice at the nexus of race and
threat perception. Political Psychology, 39(1), 89–106. gender: An outgroup male target hypothesis. Journal of
Fischer, R., Hanke, K., & Sibley, C. G. (2012). Cultural Personality and Social Psychology, 98(6), 933.
and institutional determinants of social dominance Petersen, M. B., Sznycer, D., Sell, A., Cosmides, L., &
orientation: A cross-cultural meta-analysis of 27 socie- Tooby, J. (2013). The ancestral logic of politics upper-
ties. Political Psychology, 33(4), 437–467. body strength regulates men’s assertion of self-interest
Gatto, J., & Dambrun, M. (2012). Authoritarianism, social over economic redistribution. Psychological Science,
dominance, and prejudice among junior police officers. 24(7), 1098–1103.
Social Psychology, 43(2), 61–66. Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L. M., & Malle, B. F.
Ho, A. K., Sidanius, J., Pratto, F., Levin, S., Thomsen, L., (1994). Social dominance orientation: A personality
Kteily, N., & Sheehy-Skeffington, J. (2012). Social variable predicting social and political attitudes.
dominance orientation revisiting the structure and func- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(4),
tion of a variable predicting social and political atti- 741.
tudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., & Levin, S. (2006). Social domi-
38(5), 583–606. nance theory and the dynamics of intergroup relations:
Ho, A. K., Sidanius, J., Kteily, N. S., Sheehy-Skeffington, Taking stock and looking forward. European Review of
J., Pratto, F., Henkel, K. E., . . . Stewart, A. L. (2015). Social Psychology, 17(1), 271–320.
The nature of social dominance orientation: Theorizing
Social Dominance Orientation (SDO) 9

Price, M. E., Sheehy-Skeffington, J., Sidanius, J., & Thomsen, L., Green, E. G., & Sidanius, J. (2008). We will
Pound, N. (2017). Is sociopolitical egalitarianism hunt them down: How social dominance orientation
related to bodily and facial formidability in men? and right-wing authoritarianism fuel ethnic persecution
Journal of Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(5), of immigrants in fundamentally different ways. Journal
626–634. of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(6), 1455–1464.
Sheehy-Skeffington, J., & Thomsen, L. (2019). Thomsen, L., Green, E. G., Ho, A. K., Levin, S., van Laar,
Egalitarianism. Psychological and socio-ecological C., Sinclair, S., & Sidanius, J. (2010). Wolves in
foundations. Current Opinion in Psychology, 32, sheep’s clothing: SDO asymmetrically predicts per-
146–152. ceived ethnic victimization among White and Latino
Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social dominance: An students across three years. Personality and Social
intergroup theory of social hierarchy and oppression. Psychology Bulletin, 36(2), 225–238.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Thomsen, L., Frankenhuis, W. E., Ingold-Smith, M., &
Sidanius, J., Liu, J. H., Shaw, J. S., & Pratto, F. (1994). Carey, S. (2011). Big and mighty: Preverbal infants
Social dominance orientation, hierarchy attenuators mentally represent social dominance. Science,
and hierarchy enhancers: Social dominance theory 331(6016), 477–480.
and the criminal justice system. Journal of Applied Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2010). Groups in mind: The
Social Psychology, 24(4), 338–366. coalitional roots of war and morality. In H. Høgh-
Sidanius, J., Cotterill, S., Sheehy-Skeffington, J., Olesen (Ed.), Human morality and sociality: Evolu-
Kteily, N., & Carvacho, H. (2016). Social dominance tionary and comparative perspectives (pp. 191–234).
theory: Explorations in the psychology of oppression. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.
In C. G. Sibley & F. K. Barlow (Eds.), The Cambridge Wilson, M. L., & Wrangham, R. W. (2003). Intergroup
handbook of the psychology of prejudice relations in chimpanzees. Annual Review of
(pp. 149–187). Cambridge: Cambridge University Anthropology, 32(1), 363–392.
Press.

View publication stats

You might also like