Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

236 C Critical Psychology

the University of Toronto. The University of Toronto awards he has received is beyond the scope of this entry
offered him a permanent position in 1971, which he but a few noteworthy ones are the Distinguished
accepted and held until he retired from the university. Scientific Contribution Award from the Canadian
He is currently a University Professor Emeritus in the Psychological Association (1987), the William James
Department of Psychology at The University of Fellow Award from the American Psychological Society
Toronto and a senior research scientist with the (1993), the Hebb Award from the Canadian Society for
Rotman Research Institute at Baycrest Centre for Brain, Behavior and Cognitive Science (1998), and the
Geriatric Care in Toronto. Killam Prize for Science (2000). In May 2008, Craik was
elected a Fellow of the Royal Society, the national
Major Accomplishments/ science academy of the United Kingdom and
Contributions Commonwealth.
Before coming to Toronto, Craik was fascinated with
basic memory processes as well as memory across the References
adult life span. He has examined those two primary Biography: Fergus Craik (Cognitive, University of Toronto): http://
research topics throughout his career. He spent his www.psych.ualberta.ca/~gcpws/Craik/Biography/Craik_bio1.html
Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing:
early years at Toronto laying the groundwork for what
A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning
would turn out to be an extremely influential paper
and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671–684.
written with Robert Lockhart. In this paper Craik and Craik, F. I. M., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the
Lockhart (1972) outlined a new approach to studying retention of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental
memory called levels of processing (LOP). The LOP Psychology: General, 104, 268–294.
approach suggested an alternative way of examining
memory than the memory stages approaches popular
at the time. Rather than examining different memory
stages (i.e., sensory, short-term, and long-term
memory), Craik and Lockhart felt that it was more Critical Psychology
advantageous to focus on the quality of an individual’s
processing of information. They argued that “shallow” THOMAS TEO
processing of information (e.g., analysis of the physical York University, Toronto, ON, Canada
features of a stimulus) would be more likely to lead to
fleeting memories while “deeper” and, hence, more
semantic approaches (thinking of the meaning of an Introduction
item) would likely lead to longer lasting memories. Critical psychology (better: critical psychologies) has
This emphasis on quality rather than quantity of emerged using multifaceted approaches in theory and
processing had a major impact on the study of memory practice outside of the mainstream of psychology in
that continues today. Craik went on to investigate the many countries around the globe. Although critical-
LOP model with Lockhart and other investigators (e.g., psychological ideas can be found prior to the 1960s, the
Craik and Tulving 1975) in a series of experiments. He most important developments were made since that
returned to studying memory during the aging process period on the background of the rise of social episte-
as well. His contributions in the area of memory and mologies and social movements. A core goal of critical
aging have been enormously influential by providing psychologists was to transform psychology into an
a framework for investigating the conditions under emancipatory, radical, social-justice seeking, or sta-
which memory deficits will or will not occur in older tus-quo-resisting approach that understands psycho-
adults as a function of the role of the demands of the logical issues as taking place in specific political-
processing task and environmental support. economic or cultural-historical contexts. The term
Craik’s contributions to the study of memory and critical psychology was originally claimed for a German
memory and aging are impressive and consist of many school of thought, but was soon self-applied by psy-
major articles, books, and edited books. The number of chologists from the English-speaking world and from
Critical Psychology C 237

other linguistic regions who gave the term their own In modern times, René Descartes’ (1596–1650) thoughts
meanings. Critical psychology has an even longer his- on innate ideas were criticized by John Locke (1632–
tory if one considers critiques of mainstream psychol- 1704), who in turn was criticized by Gottfried Wilhelm
ogy as belonging to critical psychology (Teo 2005). Leibniz (1646–1716). Despite the importance of these
critiques and controversies, they did not systematically C
Definition challenge an entire field of research. Immanuel Kant
Critical psychology can be divided into historical and (1724–1804) provided the first systematic critique of
current critiques of psychology (general meaning) and the dominant psychologies of his time. Rational and
into theoretical and practical frameworks that are empirical psychology, a system division introduced by
identified as such (specific meaning). In the latter Christian Wolff (1679–1754), became the twofold tar-
sense, critical psychology is whatever self-identifying get of Kant’s critique. Kant (1781/1998, 1786/1970)
critical psychologists do or whatever is published in suggested that rational psychology was trapped in
journals and books that use the title critical psychology. paralogisms, and that empirical psychology could not
Beyond an empty “operational definition,” theories become a genuine natural science.
and practices that are based on social epistemologies Other critics of psychology included the Neo-
and that have an emancipatory practical intent, in the Kantian philosopher Friedrich A. Lange (1828–1875),
widest meaning, should be subsumed under critical who, in a book section originally intended as the
psychology. The term social epistemology refers to Critique of Psychology, challenged the foundations of
approaches that examine knowledge production and philosophical psychology from the perspective of the
knowledge biases and limitations from the perspective rising natural sciences. Lange (1866/1950) criticized
of social categories (e.g., class, gender, ethnicity, sexual philosophical psychology’s subject matter and method-
orientation, ability–disability) or in terms of power. ology while outlining a program for an “objective”
The first philosopher to systematically take social psychology nearly half a century before John B. Watson
knowledge biases into account, from the perspective of did (1878–1958). Auguste Comte (1798–1857) also
class, was Karl Marx (1818–1883). Following his pro- developed a critique of the philosophical psychology
gram, the critical theorist Horkheimer (1895–1973) of his time and suggested that psychology should be
proposed that critical theory should challenge the sep- excluded from the positive sciences. Comte (1896)
aration of individual and society, values and research, identified philosophical psychology as the last phase
and knowledge and action. These three issues also form of theology and suggested that mental phenomena
the historical core of critical psychological assump- could be studied sufficiently within anatomy, physiol-
tions: The individual is embedded with in society, ogy, and his own program of a positive philosophy.
progressive sociopolitical values should guide psycho- The critique of psychology has also been advanced
logical research, and what has been gained in critical in the many crisis discussions regarding the discipline
theory should be put into practice. In all three areas, and its subfields. The first systematic book on the crisis
significant tensions exist, especially between of psychology was published by Willy (1899), who
approaches that focus on theory-development and challenged the dominant research programs of his
others that focus on practice (e.g., armchair critical time. He identified speculative theory building and
psychologists versus social activists), and in articulat- an inadequate methodology as sources of psychology’s
ing these principles, which themselves have undergone crisis. The crisis literature was on the rise in the
significant changes in the last few decades. 1920–1930s and again since the 1960s and 1970s,
some of it written by critical psychologists (e.g., Parker
Historical Background 1989). A critique of psychology’s lack of political
The critique of psychological ideas has a long history. relevance can already be found in the nineteenth
Aristotle (384–322 BCE) challenged Plato’s (427–347 century, when Beneke (1845) suggested that psychol-
BCE) conceptualization of the psyche. During the ogy should aid in solving political, social, and religious
Middle Ages, there were extensive discussions on psy- tumults while he critiqued psychology’s lack of under-
chological topics such as the primacy of will or reason. standing of social reality.
238 C Critical Psychology

Wilhelm Dilthey (1833–1911) inaugurated a long Critique of Political Economy (Marx and Engel 1845/
tradition of the critique of natural-scientific psychol- 1958; Marx 1859/1961) (also the subtitle of Das
ogy from the perspective of human-scientific Kapital) that drew up the program for a critique of
approaches. Dilthey (1894/1957) argued that due to the status quo as well as a framework for critical social
the specific subject matter of psychology, it would be inquiries and practices. Marx advanced the idea that
wrong to emulate the natural sciences and that causal humans may have false understandings of social reality
explanations as provided in those sciences could not because they belong or align themselves with a social
be used satisfactorily in the domain of mental life. category that benefits from misrepresentations.
In his program for a human-scientific hermeneutic For Marx, central was the notion that the domi-
psychology, Dilthey understood the subject matter nant, most influential ideas in society are also the ideas
of psychology as experience in its totality, which of the ruling class. Critical approaches in the tradition
could not be dealt with adequately by experimentation of this stream of thought (e.g., feminist standpoint
and measurement, and not by focusing on the elements studies) analyze the role of social interest in knowledge
or isolated parts of mental life. Eduard Spranger production and dissemination and reflect on the
(1882–1963) developed a hermeneutically based psy- knowledge producer’s social existence and the potential
chology of personality and adolescence that contained ideological and practical biases that it might produce.
a critique of natural-scientific approaches to the same Admittedly, the historical genesis of critical psychology
issues. Edmund Husserl (1859–1938) critiqued the nat- is not undisputed and other researchers have claimed
uralization of the psyche, by which he meant the different sources. Billig (2008) analyzed the third Earl
unwarranted emulation of the natural sciences in of Shaftesbury (1671–1713) as a hidden root of critical
psychology. psychology because he suggested that humans establish
In the English-speaking context, psychologists such understanding through conversation, that truth is
as Gordon W. Allport (1897–1967), Abraham H. a social and dialogical matter, and that the self is
Maslow (1908–1970), and Carl Rogers (1902–1987) divided, and because of the demand for continuous
articulated the shortcomings of a natural-scientific ori- self-reflection. These topics are central to critical psy-
ented psychology. Beginning in the 1960s, Giorgi chologies following the linguistic turn in intellectual
(1970) expressed the idea that psychology should not history.
be part of the natural sciences, while at the same time Marx’s ideas also inspired critical theory, a research
he suggested that a human-scientific psychology could program developed by German philosophers and social
be scientific in character. He identified a lack of unity in scientists in the 1920s at the Institute for Social
psychology, the inability to investigate psychological Research in Frankfurt am Main (Frankfurt School).
phenomena in a meaningful way (doing justice to the Some of the important first-generation figures include
human person), and a lack of lifeworld-relevance as Max Horkheimer (1895–1973), Erich Fromm (1900–
sources for the inadequacy of the natural-scientific 1980), Theodor Wiesengrund Adorno (1903–1969),
viewpoint in psychology. More recently, Slife et al. and Herbert Marcuse (1898–1979), whereas Jürgen
(2005) have applied, embedded in hermeneutic per- Habermas (born 1929) belongs to the second genera-
spectives, critical thinking to psychology. tion of critical theorists. In his groundbreaking article,
Although the natural-scientific critique of philo- Traditional and critical theory Horkheimer (1937/1992)
sophical psychology and the human-scientific critique argued that traditional theory (positivist theory that
of natural-scientific psychology provided important applied logic, mathematics, and deduction for the
arguments that have been taken up by some critical assessment of its ideas) hides the social function of
psychologists, these critiques cannot be seen as critical science, the social formation of facts, and the historical
psychology in a specific meaning. Kant’s (1781/1998) character of research objects. In contrast, Horkheimer
Critique of Pure Reason is not the historical starting recommended the reasonable organization of society
point for critical psychology; rather it is Marx’s that would meet the needs of the whole society. Such
(1845/1958) Critique of the Latest German Philosophy a critical practice would include the struggle for the
(subtitle of the book The German Ideology) and abolition of social injustice.
Critical Psychology C 239

Critical theorists were also drawing extensively on problems of psychology by finding compromises
Sigmund Freud’s (1856–1939) work and combined between critical-theoretical reflection and traditional
psychoanalysis with Marxist theory. Freud’s ideas con- psychology, a middle ground that was intended to
tain implicit and explicit criticisms of mainstream psy- lead to an emancipatory psychology. During this
chology, while they also provided the most influential period, to be critical meant for Holzkamp to question C
alternative approach regarding human subjectivity. the relevance of psychology for practice, to identify
However, critical psychoanalysts often self-identity as problems of traditional psychological methodology,
analysts rather than as critical psychologists. The mate- and to disclose psychology’s hidden, ideological
rialization of the critical potential of psychoanalysis, assumptions. In the critical-conceptual period (1973–
which historically has also been used for oppressive 1983), Holzkamp held that psychology’s problems
purposes, depends, in part, on national contingencies. could not be solved within the framework of traditional
In Latin America, for example, psychoanalysis was well psychology, or in compromise between critical and
received in a counterculture that understood it as mainstream thinking, but rather that psychology
a liberating force against the oppressive structures in required a radically different outlook. Thus, he and
military-dominated states. More recently, in Britain, his colleagues attempted to develop a better conceptu-
Lacanian-influenced critical approaches have achieved alization of psychological objects.
more recognition (see the special issue on Lacan and Holzkamp (1973) laid out the basic methodology
Critical psychology in the 2009 Annual Review of Critical for conceptual studies in German Critical Psychology,
Psychology). according to which a real understanding of, for
Many psychologists, especially in the German- instance, perception would only be possible through
speaking context, still associate the term Critical Psy- the inclusion of the natural history, the prehistory, and
chology with the approach taken by Klaus Holzkamp the history of humanity. To accomplish these goals,
(1927–1995). This is misleading for Germany because critical psychology needed to include interdisciplinary
there was a second group of critical psychologists (low- material. The work during this phase was integrated
ercase) that opposed Critical Psychology (uppercase; the within what Holzkamp (1983) considered a foundation
group around Holzkamp). The former emerged mainly for psychology, which he deemed a new paradigm, able
from the student movement, challenging the psycho- to compete on a level playing field with mainstream
logical mainstream or even wishing to abolish psychol- psychology. Based on this framework, Holzkamp
ogy. They agreed that mainstream psychology did not worked in his later years on establishing psychology
address the burning issues of the time (Rexilius 1988), as a science of the subject (Tolman 1994).
whereas others focused on a critical history of psychol- In the English-speaking world, or specifically in
ogy (Jaeger and Staeuble 1978). North America, Marx and associated categories such
The group surrounding Holzkamp evolved in the as class did not have the same impact in psychology
context of radical political and social movements. (this might be a case of denial or repression as Harris
The concept of a group must be emphasized because 1997, shows). Categories such as gender, race, power, or
the usual focus on Holzkamp as the mastermind of community played a more important role in the devel-
Critical Psychology and as its most important writer opment of critical approaches in psychology. Feminist
is misleading. Holzkamp was the editor of the journal psychologists were critical of either the content of a
Forum Kritische Psychologie [Forum Critical Psychology] mainstream psychology that was perceived as biased or
and was the established professor at the Psychological its methods (Gilligan 1977). Yet, most feminist psy-
Institute of the Free University of Berlin, as well as its chologists, who understand gender as a significant
most visible political and academic target, but the focus social category in order to understand the knowledge
on Holzkamp neglects the major role of an interdisci- and practices of the mainstream, identify first with
plinary working group that built Critical Psychology feminism rather than with critical psychology.
collaboratively. However, there exist leading critical psychologists that
In the critical-theoretical phase (1968–1972), combine class and gender in their analyses (Walkerdine
Holzkamp (1972) attempted to formulate solutions to et al. 2001).
240 C Critical Psychology

The obvious biases of race research in psychology point of view of social justice. Fox (2008) expressed
and the social sciences lent themselves to an easy cri- concerns regarding the status-quo-stabilizing effect of
tique (see Kamin 1974). In the context of postcolonial existing research, while calling for action in critical
writings, this critique was extended to psychology in psychology (Fox 2003), which could draw on radical
general, in asking questions about the global character programs such as anarchism. Fox et al. (2009) as well as
of mainstream, especially American, psychology Sloan (2000) provide good overviews of the diversity
(Owusu-Bempah and Howitt 2000). Based on the of ideas and practices of critical psychology. Sloan
assumption that social sciences are inherently cultural, (1996) also shows how critical theory (Habermas) can
the theories, concepts, and even the methods of psy- be applied to psychological problems, and how critical
chology were assumed to reflect a Western bias. Conse- activity can be materialized, outside of professional
quently, so the argument goes, psychology cannot be associations, in organizations such as Psychologists for
globalized and exported from Europe or North Social Responsibility.
America to the rest of the world in a meaningful way Finally, a few other critical programs should be
but is rather dependent on recognition, dialogue, or mentioned. Critical historians of psychology may
mutual exchange (see Brock 2006). Many writings in be critical of the mainstream without being critical
this area are critical of dominant theories without psychologists in a narrow sense. These historians per-
belonging to critical psychology in a specific sense form critical research, in pointing to some of the blind
(e.g., Winston 2004). spots of traditional historiography as well as of main-
Postmodern or social-constructionist approaches stream psychology, while at the same time they do not
pose a special problem in a historical and theoretical need to compete with traditional psychology for “bet-
reconstruction. Many of these programs are critical of ter” theories, methods, and practices. Thus, their pri-
the mainstream (Gergen 1985), but have not grown out mary identity is that of historians of psychology
of social categories such as class, gender, or race but are (Danziger 1990; Richards 1996; Scarborough and
based on the notion that modernity itself produces Furumoto 1987). Critical movements such as anti-
biases in theory and practice. Such an assessment psychiatry have had an influential history in the
would include a critique of grand modern programs human sciences with well-known psychiatrists, such as
such as Marxism or German Critical Psychology. Social Franco Basaglia (1924–1980), Thomas Szasz (born
constructionist approaches have been very influential 1920), or Ronald D. Laing (1927–1989), spearheading
in many strands of critical psychology in the last few ideas and practices, but they are less visible in the disci-
decades (Kvale 1992). In particular, Foucault-inspired pline of psychology. The same applies to critical disabil-
studies have had significant impact in critical psychol- ity studies, critical race theory, and queer studies.
ogy, and critical discourse analyses have been very
productive (Parker 2002). Thus, following Foucault, Key Issues
Hook (2004) grounds his vision of critical psychology Subject matter of psychology: One line of critique
in an orientation that interrogates power in general. suggests that the most important models and meta-
Indeed, critical plurality is endorsed, and instead of phors representing human subjectivity or the human
advocating for one social category, recent interdisci- mind, with implications for an understanding of
plinary developments in critical psychology focus on human nature, the relationship between mind and
power and on new social categories (e.g., immigrants) body, methodology and practice, are technological
(see Papadopoulos et al. 2008). ones, and that the history of mainstream psychology
Certain subdisciplines in psychology – due to their parallels the development of technology (see Tolman
particular subject matter – offer themselves for practi- and Maiers 1991). Machine models are embedded
cal interventions. In North America, influential within a network of ontological assumptions, such as
critical-psychological projects have been grounded in that a person reacts toward an external stimulus like
community psychology (Prilleltensky and Nelson a mechanism. The machine model excludes notions of
2002). For Prilleltensky (1994), the integration of agency, or the ability to reflect, choose, and act.
theory and practice should be based on the ethical Psychology’s mainstream operates with a mechanistic
Critical Psychology C 241

and hence an atomistic and reductionistic model of conceptualization of what this idea means exactly is
human mental life (see also Teo 2009). part of ongoing research in critical psychology.
The mechanistic model is even maintained in bio- Marx promoted an understanding of the nature of
logical traditions. Despite a commitment to an evolu- human beings and of human mental life as active and
tionary perspective, the machine model is dominant in societal. Cultural-historical approaches in the tradition C
behaviorism because it is assumed that the individual of dialectical materialism have argued that the environ-
responds to stimuli. Dividing psychological life into ment, culture, and history are not just other variables.
stimulus and response (behaviorism) or into indepen- Contexts are interwoven with the very fabric of subjec-
dent and dependent variables (mainstream psychology tivity. Vygotsky (1978) and his followers have chal-
in general) is problematic because it neglects subjectiv- lenged psychology’s intrapsychological nature when it
ity and action embedded in concrete cultural-historical comes to understanding the individual mind, individ-
contexts. More recent models in psychology that are ual language, and individual activities, and have
based on evolutionary metaphors often neglect an derived new theories and practices. Holzkamp (1983)
understanding of how human mental life differs from conceptualized the person as part of a larger sociohis-
various forms of animal mental life, particularly in torical and economic web, which did not imply that the
terms of an understanding of the societal nature of subject should not be taken into account in critical
human subjectivity. psychology. Indeed, Holzkamp envisioned a psychol-
The selection of variables and the focus on isolated ogy from, literally, the standpoint of the subject (a first-
aspects of human mental life (atomism) does not do person psychology) (see Tolman 2009). More recently,
justice to the integrated character of human mental life the problem of subjectivity has been advanced in
in concrete individuals. Instead of looking at the com- English-speaking critical psychology (Walkerdine
plexity of human life, which is the source of human 2002), and the critical-psychological journal Subjectiv-
subjectivity, the mainstream in psychology assumes ity focuses on this very topic.
that it is sufficient to study parts. It is reductionistic to Within a phenomenological tradition, it was argued
assume that the parts sufficiently explain the complex- that a focus on the mental life implies a neglect of the
ity of human subjectivity, another consequence of the body (Stam 1998). This idea had a large impact on the
machine model. In reality, human subjectivity is expe- feminist literature that had already recognized the
rienced in its totality. Critics have argued, following nexus of person and society, and emphasized the con-
Dilthey, that a psychology that does justice to human cept of subjectivity in context. In addition, feminist
mental life should begin with the nexus of psychologists have emphasized embodied theories of
human experiences in order to understand the parts subjectivity, theories that do not exclude the body from
and not vice versa (see also Martin et al. 2003). the subject matter of psychology (Bayer and Malone
The machine model of human mental life has 1998). Some of these theories draw also on arguments
another consequence: Because it conceptualizes the developed by social constructionists who understand
person as individualistic and society as an external the individual as nested in society and community
variable, the individual and society are seen as separate, (Gergen 1985). Gergen (2009) rejects the individualism
yet reflecting bourgeois ideology and practices (see also of psychology and locates mental life as embedded in
Parker and Spears 1996). Accordingly, it is insufficient relations. Such ideas have led to an interest in the
to conceptualize the sociohistorical reality as a stimulus dialogical nature of personhood by hermeneutically
environment to which one reacts; the individual is not oriented psychologists (Richardson et al. 1998) but
independent of the environment and vice versa. In also by critical psychologists who pay attention to key
contrast, critical psychologists would agree that the figures such as Mikhail Bakhtin (1895–1975) (see Billig
individual is embedded in society, an insight that has 2008).
several consequences: it allows for a critique of main- The postcolonial critique argues that the main-
stream psychology; it implies that psychology must stream’s psychological subject matter is part of
draw on other disciplines for an understanding of the a wider historical and cultural context and that the
subject matter of psychology; and yet, the concrete theories that try to capture this subject matter are
242 C Critical Psychology

part of Western theorizing. Thus, the subject matter of variables such as well-being, depression, self-esteem,
psychology must be understood as part of Western personality, and so on.
ideology (Teo and Febbraro 2003). Thus, psychologists Critical psychologists argue that problems of sub-
and social scientists outside of the presumed center of ject matter and methodology as well as practice are
psychology face the task of finding psychological theo- intertwined. In contrast, the mainstream promotes
ries, concepts, and practices that work in their life- the idea that a natural-scientific methodology can and
worlds rather than importing or exporting American must be applied unquestionably to all research areas.
ideas. For instance, Freire (1997), who emphasized that The focus on methodology rather than on the subject
learners should be treated as subjects and not as objects, matter has led to an epistemological attitude that can
and Martı́n-Baró (1994), who applied Freirean ideas to be called methodologism (Teo 2005). Others have used
psychology, have developed categories to deal specifi- the terms methodolatry (Bakan 1967), the cult of
cally with psychological issues in Latin America. empiricism (Toulmin and Leary 1985), and the meth-
In order to capture the subject matter of psychol- odological imperative (Danziger 1985). Holzkamp
ogy, psychologists use concepts, and it is through those (1991) even argued that adequacy of the methodology
concepts or categories that they perceive sociopsycho- with regard to the subject matter should be a central
logical reality. The nature of concepts has been studied, scientific criterion: As long as the adequacy of
most notably by the critical historian Danziger (1997), a methodology is not known, the scientific value and
who emphasized the social construction of psycholog- all other objectification criteria are worthless.
ical ideas and practices. Mainstream psychologists Based on the assumption that the psychological
often pretend that concepts are natural kinds because subject matter (i.e., active human mental life embed-
they have empirical support. Yet, historical studies have ded in cultural-historical contexts) demands appropri-
shown that psychological concepts (human kinds) have ate methodologies, critical psychologists, although not
a different status from natural-scientific concepts (nat- a priori opposed to quantitative methods (see Martı́n-
ural kinds) and are constructed in specific cultural Baró 1994, who demonstrated that quantitative
contexts for specific purposes. Moreover, constructed methods can be critical of the status quo), favor
concepts can become a social reality and part of human a variety of qualitative methods that tend to focus on
identity (for instance, “race” or IQ). At the same time, the content of human subjectivity as well as on the
culturally embedded concepts used in psychological possibility of social action. Critical psychologists favor
theories can be understood as sources of power and studies that have practical relevance, or emancipatory
oppression when they express a certain worldview and relevance, which means that research should contribute
are ideological (see Foucault 1966/1970; Rose 1996). to overturning oppressive social situations. Critical
Methodological issues: Mainstream psychology is researchers emphasize the transformative potential of
committed to a natural-scientific, experimental- research that not only addresses the status quo but also
statistical, or empirical-statistical methodology. The provides knowledge on how to change it.
mainstream operationalizes, in empirical research, Feminist researchers have identified the ideology of
concepts as variables (independent, dependent, mod- mainstream scientific methodology as male biased. In
erating, mediating), so that traditional psychology her classic studies, Keller (1985), using psychoanalytic
can be characterized as a psychology of variables concepts, explored the association between objectivity
(Holzkamp 1991). This methodology provides an and masculinity and defended the thesis that scientific
understanding of the functional or correlational rela- research is based on masculine discourses, ideals, meta-
tionship between variables but not the why of psycho- phors, and practices. Instead, she advocated for research
logical phenomena, which is more relevant if one that emphasizes a connection with the participant and
intends to derive social activism. For instance, of inter- that does not exclude the participant’s authentic experi-
est in traditional psychology is not the why of unem- ences. In cultural-historical approaches, it has been
ployment (which would include cultural-historical and argued that all involved parties should co-construct all
political-economic analyses), but rather the relation- parts of a study, which allows for a grounding of theories
ship between the variable of unemployment and other in praxis (see Roth and Lee 2007). According to German
Critical Psychology C 243

critical psychology, research should be able to capture only is human subjectivity embedded in society; psy-
the standpoint of the subject. This means, for instance, chology as a discipline and psychologists are also
that in psychotherapy research, it is less relevant how located in the same contexts. Science is then under-
psychotherapy shapes a person than how a person con- stood as a social enterprise embedded in power, money,
tributes to his or her own change (Dreier 2007). and prestige. As Ward (2002) has shown, the reason for C
Social-constructionist or postmodern thinkers adopting a natural-scientific identity has more to do
(a problematic label) such as Michel Foucault have with the advantages of being accepted, and of being
inaugurated various methods of discourse analysis. able to profit within academia and the larger public,
Critical discourse analysis, a method that focuses on than with internal progress. Historical and social stud-
the analysis of written or spoken language, understands ies of psychology allow critical questions about the
language as a social practice that is infused with biases purpose of research, about the personal, social, and
because language is embedded in ideological, oppres- political-economic interests involved in developing
sive, or exploitative practices. Discourse analysis allows, psychological studies, and about the groups and indi-
for example, historical reconstructions of how multi- viduals that benefit from research results. In contrast,
ple personality was made into an object of academic critical researchers have asked for more self-reflexivity
discussion (Hacking 1995) as well as an analysis in psychology (Morawski 2005).
of discourses’s dependence on context (Dijk 2008). It should be pointed out that so-called objective
Foucault (1977) also provided suggestions for an methods can and have been used for oppressive purposes.
analysis of nondiscursive practices: An analysis of The hermeneutic deficit in psychology, despite
architecture allows for insight into the workings of a sophisticated methodological apparatus, appears in
power in the context of human subjectivity and inter- the context of the interpretation of group differences
personal relations. (e.g., gender or “race” differences). Epistemological vio-
In Martı́n-Baró’s (1994) approach, methodology is lence is committed when the interpretation of data (not
intertwined with critical praxis. He suggested that psy- data themselves) lead to statements that construct mar-
chology must base its knowledge production on the ginalized groups as inferior, restrict the opportunities of
liberation needs of the oppressed people of Latin marginalized groups, and lead to aversive recommenda-
America. This means that knowledge must be gener- tions for marginalized groups. For instance, if a researcher
ated by learning from the oppressed: Research should suggests that group differences are due to the lower ability
look at psychosocial processes from the perspective of the Other, then this researcher has committed episte-
of the dominated; educational psychology should mological violence – because the data do not determine
learn from the perspective of the illiterate; industrial this interpretation, because alternative interpretations
psychology should begin with the perspective of the are available, and because this interpretation has negative
unemployed; clinical psychology should start with consequences for the Other (see Teo 2008).
the perspective of the marginalized; mental health psy- Ethical-practical issues: The mainstream’s emphasis
chology should takes as its point of departure the on control and adaptation neglects psychology’s eman-
perspective of someone who lives in a town dump. cipatory potential. Psychology has been an extremely
Martı́n-Baró suggested an epistemological change successful discipline in Europe and North America in
from the powerful to the oppressed and recommends terms of academic and professional expansion. How-
participatory action research (see below). It could be ever, such success does not necessarily imply the ethi-
argued that feminist, sociohistorical, postmodern, cal-political quality of its practice. Psychological
and postcolonial ideas can be integrated into a mean- practice has often involved abuses perpetrated by the
ingful methodology of the oppressed (Sandoval 2000). powerful, from intelligence testing as a means to con-
Some critical researchers focus on meta- trol immigration into the United States (Gould 1996),
psychological issues that generate critical awareness to the application of psychological techniques to
without having a direct societal-practical impact. His- extract information from prisoners.
torical studies and self-reflexive studies look at the Many mainstream psychologists in the past and pre-
discipline of psychology and at its participants. Not sent have emphasized that fact (what is) and value (what
244 C Critical Psychology

ought to be) are two different domains that should be knowledge is considered a form of praxis (as is teach-
kept apart. The problem is that, in any social science, ing) that is not inferior to concrete community-based
these two domains are inherently intertwined. Critical interventions in the abolition of social injustice.
psychologists (see Fox et al. 2009) challenge the idea The most obvious consequences of praxis can be
that one cannot derive ought from is, and that science seen in economically less developed contexts where
should remain neutral on political issues and concerns. theorizing for the sake of theorizing and research for
Instead, most ethical-political critical psychologists the sake of research must be considered indulgent
emphasize the issues of social injustice and inequality practices. Martı́n-Baró (1942–1989) gave his own life
(when it comes to class, ethnicity, gender, sexual ori- in the political struggle for progress. He pointed out
entation, disability, globalization, etc.) and make them that an ethical-political stance, practice, and objectivity
a practical research concern. Obviously, values come do not conflict with each other. For example, when it
into play when doing psychology and a lack of reflec- comes to torture, it would be possible to be ethical-
tion on the values that guide one’s research maintains practical (thus, rejecting and working against torture)
the status quo (see Prilleltensky 1994). while at the same time maintaining objectivity (under-
The issue of praxis relates to Marx’s famous notion standing the objective consequences of torture on
of the primacy of praxis over theory: Intellectual reflec- human mental life). Martı́n-Baró (1994) argued that
tion should not be about interpreting the world so it would be insufficient to put oneself in the shoes of
much as changing it. For Marx, the final goal of all oppressed people. Instead, he pleaded for a new praxis,
praxis was to change society’s fundamental economic which he defined as an activity that transforms social
foundations, which he perceived as the source of reality and lets humans know not only about what is
inequality. Critical social research should be guided but also about what is not, and by which means they
by progressive ethical-political ideas and should gener- may try to orient themselves toward what ought to be.
ate knowledge that has relevance. Critical psychologists In consequence, for Martı́n-Baró, the psychologist
have analyzed psychology’s role in maintaining capital- is less a traditional clinician and more a resource for the
ism, patriarchy, colonialism, and Western ideology community regarding intervention and support in the
(Hook 2004). In not challenging the mainstream, psy- fields of disability, mental health, and drug use but also
chology reinforces the status quo, which also means in terms of economic development and antipoverty
performing psychology in the interest of the powerful. programs. Martı́n-Baró worked with victims of state
The embeddedness of psychology in the market econ- oppression, assumed active social roles, and worked
omy has made it difficult to promote psychology as with marginalized groups. It allowed him to under-
a critical science. Even social psychology, which has stand suffering as a shared issue rather than an indi-
a history of contributing to emancipation, has largely vidualized problem. His preferential option for the poor
been transformed into a field that produces large was influential in shaping his political-ethical ideas. His
amounts of socially irrelevant data. concrete praxis-method is labeled participatory action
It should be mentioned that cultural-historical, Neo- research, originally introduced to psychology by Lewin
Marxist, and other critical approaches in the West have (1946), who believed in the transformative power of
acknowledged the primacy of praxis but have often research in social psychology. Concrete critical psycho-
remained in the comparably safe environment of acade- logical praxis has also been addressed in the context of
mia. Thus, instead of becoming politically active outside AIDS in Africa (Hook 2004).
the political mainstream, many critical theorists have
suggested that research, if not emancipatory itself, International Perspectives
should at least have an emancipatory intention The Annual Review of Critical Psychology of 2006 pro-
(Habermas 1972). In fact, in critical thought, one can vides an overview of critical psychology in several
find ethical-political orientations that range from left- countries in Europe, North and South America, Asia,
liberal, progressive to radical. Many ivory tower critical Oceania, and Africa. Critical psychologies have devel-
psychologists also justify theoretical research as oped many branches, and thus, only a selective and
a legitimate option, because the production of limited account can be provided here. In Germany,
Critical Psychology C 245

the (West) Berlin group that formed around Klaus Graduate Center of the City University of New York
Holzkamp founded its own school while other critical (e.g., Fine 2006) and at the University of Miami.
psychologists organized around the journal Psychologie Oceania has significant enclaves of critically ori-
und Gesellschaftskritik (Psychology and Critique of Soci- ented psychologists at several universities, particularly
ety). Both groups have struggled in recent years to keep in Australia and New Zealand, with degree opportuni- C
their theories and practices alive at universities and ties and research centers (e.g., University of Adelaide,
especially in psychology departments. In Austria, University of Auckland). South Africa has become
a Society for Critical Psychology has achieved official more visible on the international scene of critical psy-
professional status recognized by mainstream associa- chology since the publication of an extensive textbook
tions and offers continuing education credits. (Hook 2004) that provides ideas on transforming psy-
English-speaking accounts of critical psychology are chology, combining Western and indigenous psychol-
internationally the most visible ones, as they are in many ogies, introducing conceptual developments within an
areas of the social and natural sciences. Yet, English- African perspective, and outlining an African-based
speaking approaches to critical psychology show large critical psychology. Perspectives outside of the
variations, depending on intellectual and practical tradi- so-called center also emphasize the need to combine
tions as well as on the national context. The United critical psychology with practical health matters – an
Kingdom has emerged as a center of critical psychology international trend that has led to various critical
in providing training, education, and research programs health psychology approaches around the world.
of critical psychology (Manchester Metropolitan Univer- Spanish-speaking initiatives promoting critical psy-
sity, Cardiff University, the University of the West of chology in Latin America draw on the potential of
England in Bristol, the University of East London, Martı́n-Baró’s liberation psychology. But they also
Loughborough University, and other institutions). In include a critique of critical psychology, which has
Britain, a critical social psychology has also been been perceived as a project from the North (Montero
maintained (e.g., Gough and McFadden 2001). Some and Christlieb 2003). Spain also provides several
of the programs, journals, book series, research centers, approaches to critical social psychology. France, usually
and so on have sometimes short turnovers, and may lack less known for critical psychology and more for critical
continuity. For instance, the International Journal of approaches in philosophy or for psychoanalytic inno-
Critical Psychology was relaunched as the journal Sub- vations, has an important history of the critique of
jectivity. In addition, research stemming from the psychology, beginning with Politzer (1928/1994) and
English-speaking context also demonstrates that suc- also advanced by Sève (1978). In East Asia as well as in
cessful critical studies on subjectivity need to be achieved India, critical psychology programs are not located in
in an interdisciplinary context, and might even require psychology departments, but critical approaches focus-
abandoning the traditional discipline of psychology. ing on subjectivity are found in cultural studies pro-
In English-speaking North America, there exist no grams with opportunities for publishing journals and
formal programs that offer an education or graduate books and for presenting at conferences.
training in critical psychology in its specific meaning,
but there are departments where critical topics can be Future Directions
studied from theoretical, historical, social-construction- Habermas (1968/1972) articulated the relationship
ist, hermeneutic, or community-psychology perspec- between knowledge and interest and identified critical
tives. In Canada, the social and theoretical psychology social sciences as one basic category of science, besides
program at the University of Calgary, the history and the natural and the human sciences. After decades of
theory of psychology program at York University, and critical inquiry, the question remains as to whether
the community psychology program at Wilfrid Laurier a critical interest is indeed necessary for human develop-
University should be mentioned. In the Unites States, ment. Posing the question also raises issues of legitimacy,
critical ideas can be studied at smaller institutions such because critical psychologies developed out of particular
as the University of West Georgia or Duquesne Univer- cultural-historical climates themselves. The partial insti-
sity. Interdisciplinary programs have been realized at the tutionalization of critical psychology (e.g., academics
246 C Critical Psychology

working at universities) raises issues regarding the value Beneke, E. (1845). Lehrbuch der Psychologie als Naturwissenschaft
of theoretical and practical renewal that does justice to (Zweite, vermehrte und verbesserte Auflage) [Textbook of psychol-
ogy as a natural science (2nd expanded and improved ed.)]. Berlin:
changing social realities on national and international
Mittler.
levels. Problems of legitimacy are also raised on an eth- Billig, M. (2008). The hidden roots of critical psychology: Understand-
ical-practical level when, in the saturated countries of the ing the impact of Locke, Shaftesbury and Reid. London: Sage.
globe, resistance is often envisioned on a subjective level Brock, A. C. (Ed.). (2006). Internationalizing the history of psychology.
as reactionary rather than as progressive or radical. New York: New York University Press.
Choudhury, S., Nagel, S. K., & Slaby, J. (2009). Critical neuroscience:
Ironically, the development of critical psychology is
Linking neuroscience and society through critical practice.
also contingent on the development of psychology BioSocieties, 4(1), 61–77.
as a discipline. Yet, the discipline of psychology is Comte, A. (1896). The positive philosophy of Auguste Comte (Freely
fragmented, and what is perceived as the academic core translated and condensed by Harriet Martineau. With an intro-
has moved to brain physiology and neuroscience. duction by Frederic Harrison). London: George Bell.
Although a critique appears necessary for such develop- Danziger, K. (1985). The methodological imperative in psychology.
Philosophy of the Social Sciences, 15, 1–13.
ments, many critical psychologists do not consider these
Danziger, K. (1990). Constructing the subject: Historical origins of psy-
developments relevant to their own research vision. chological research. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Thus, they leave psychology for other social or cultural Danziger, K. (1997). Naming the mind: How psychology found its
sciences; nevertheless, new approaches outside of critical language. London: Sage.
psychology need to be advanced, such as critical neuro- Dijk, T. A. V. (2008). Discourse and context: A sociocognitive approach.
New York: Cambridge University Press.
science, for a better understanding of the opportunities
Dilthey, W. (1957). Ideen über eine beschreibende und zergliedernde
and limitations of the latest developments. However, Psychologie (Ideas on a descriptive and analytical psychology). In
these critical developments, where they exist, have W. Dilthey (Ed.), Die geistige Welt: Einleitung in die Philosophie des
emerged often without a connection to the field of Lebens (Gesammelte Schriften V. Band) [The mental world: Intro-
critical psychology (Choudhury et al. 2009). duction to the philosophy of life (Collected writings: Volume 5)]
Critical psychologists’ move away from psychology (pp. 139–240). Stuttgart: Teubner. Original work published 1894.
Dreier, O. (2007). Psychotherapy in everyday life. Cambridge, MA:
may make them even more marginal within the main-
Cambridge University Press.
stream. Still, this development allows for interdisciplin- Fine, M. (2006). Bearing witness: Methods for researching oppression
ary and international exchange on matters such as and resistance – a textbook for critical research. Social Justice
subjectivity and for new theoretical and practical alli- Research, 19(1), 83–108.
ances and connections to occur. Certainly, such a trend Foucault, M. (1970). The order of things: An archaeology of the human
sciences. London: Tavistock. Original work published 1966.
will lead to more heterogeneity of sources and more
Foucault, M. (1977). Discipline and punish: The birth of the prison
theoretical and practical confusion about the core fea- (A. Sheridan, Trans.). London: Lane. (Original work published
tures and traditions of critical psychology. Instead of 1975).
one critical psychology there will be many, indeed as Fox, D. R. (2003). Awareness is good, but action is better. The
there already are, because human subjectivity, and the Counseling Psychologist, 31(3), 299–304.
intellectual and practical possibilities and limitations of Fox, D. (2008). Confronting psychology’s power. Journal of Commu-
nity Psychology, 36(2), 232–237.
the subject, are inherently dynamic, global, contextual,
Fox, D., Prilleltensky, I., & Austin, S. (Eds.). (2009). Critical psychol-
and historical, and any critical psychology needs to ogy: An introduction. London: Sage.
attempt to do justice to that reality. Freire, P. (1997). Pedagogy of the oppressed (New revised 20th-
anniversary ed.) (M. B. Ramos, Trans.). New York: Continuum.
References Gergen, K. (1985). The social constructionist movement in modern
Bakan, D. (1967). Idolatry in religion and science. In D. Bakan (Ed.), psychology. The American Psychologist, 40(3), 266–275.
On method: Toward a reconstruction of psychological investigation Gergen, K. J. (2009). Relational being: Beyond self and community.
(pp. 150–159). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Original work New York: Oxford University Press.
published 1961. Gilligan, C. (1977). In a different voice: Women’s conceptions of self
Bayer, B., & Malone, K. R. (1998). Feminism, psychology and matters and of morality. Harvard Educational Review, 47(4), 481–517.
of the body. In H. J. Stam (Ed.), The body and psychology Giorgi, A. (1970). Psychology as a human science:
(pp. 94–199). London: Sage. A phenomenologically based approach. New York: Harper & Row.
Critical Psychology C 247

Gough, B., & McFadden, M. (2001). Critical social psychology: Martı́n-Baró, I. (1994). Writings for a liberation psychology. Cam-
An introduction. London: Palgrave. bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Gould, S. J. (1996). The mismeasure of man (Revised and expanded). Marx, K. (1961). Zur Kritik der politischen Ökonomie [Critique of
New York: Norton. political economy]. In K. Marx & F. Engels (Eds.), Werke Band 13
Habermas, J. (1972). Knowledge and human interest (J. J. Shapiro, [Works: Vol. 13] (pp. 3–160). Berlin: Dietz. Original work
Trans.). Boston: Beacon Press. (German original published in published 1859. C
1968). Marx, K., & Engels, F. (1958). Die deutsche Ideologie [The German
Hacking, I. (1995). Rewriting the soul: Multiple personality and the ideology]. In K. Marx & F. Engels (Eds.), Werke Band 3 [Works:
sciences of memory. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Vol. 3] (pp. 9–530). Berlin: Dietz. Original work published 1932
Harris, B. (1997). Repoliticizing the history of psychology. In D. (Written 1845/46).
Fox & I. Prilleltensky (Eds.), Critical psychology: An introduction Montero, M., & Christlieb, P. F. (Eds.). (2003). Special Issue: Critical
(pp. 21–33). London: Sage. Psychology in Latin America. Critical Psychology: The Interna-
Holzkamp, K. (1972). Kritische Psychologie: Vorbereitende Arbeiten tional Journal of Critical Psychology, (9).
[Critical psychology: Preparatory works]. Frankfurt am Main: Morawski, J. G. (2005). Reflexivity and the psychologist. History of the
Fischer. Human Sciences, 18(4), 77–105.
Holzkamp, K. (1973). Sinnliche Erkenntnis: Historischer Ursprung und Owusu-Bempah, K., & Howitt, D. (2000). Psychology beyond western
gesellschaftliche Funktion der Wahrnehmung [Sensory knowledge: perspectives. Leicester: British Psychological Association.
Historical origin and the social function of perception]. Frankfurt Papadopoulos, D., Stephenson, N., & Tsianos, V. (2008). Escape
am Main: Athenäum. routes: Control and subversion in the 21st century. London: Pluto.
Holzkamp, K. (1983). Grundlegung der Psychologie [Foundation of Parker, I. (1989). The crisis in modern social psychology: And how to
psychology]. Frankfurt am Main: Campus. end it. London: Routledge.
Holzkamp, K. (1991). Experience of self and scientific objectivity. In Parker, I. (2002). Critical discursive psychology. New York: Palgrave
C. W. Tolman & W. Maiers (Eds.), Critical psychology: Contribu- Macmillan.
tions to an historical science of the subject (pp. 65–80). Cambridge, Parker, I., & Spears, R. (Eds.). (1996). Psychology and society: Radical
MA: Cambridge University Press. Original work published 1985. theory and practice. London: Pluto.
Hook, D. (Ed.). (2004). Critical psychology. Lansdowne, South Africa: Politzer, G. (1994). Critique of the foundations of psychology: The
UCT Press. psychology of psychoanalysis. Pittsburgh: Duquesne University
Horkheimer, M. (1992). Traditional and critical theory. In D. Press. Original work published 1928.
Ingram & J. Simon-Ingram (Eds.), Critical theory: The essential Prilleltensky, I. (1994). The morals and politics of psychology: Psycho-
readings (pp. 239–254). New York: Paragon House. Original logical discourse and the status quo. Albany: State University of
work published 1937. New York Press.
Jaeger, S., & Staeuble, I. (1978). Die gesellschaftliche Genese der Prilleltensky, I., & Nelson, G. (2002). Doing psychology critically: Mak-
Psychologie [The social origins of psychology]. Frankfurt am ing a difference in diverse settings. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.
Main: Campus. Rexilius, G. E. (Ed.). (1988). Psychologie als Gesellschaftswissenschaft
Kamin, L. J. (1974). The science and politics of IQ. Potomac: Lawrence [Psychology as a social science]. Opladen: Westdeutscher Verlag.
Erlbaum. Richards, G. (1996). Putting psychology in its place: An introduction
Kant, I. (1970). Metaphysical foundations of natural science. Indianap- from a critical historical perspective. New York: Routledge.
olis: Bobbs-Merrill. Original work published 1786. Richardson, F. C., Rogers, A., & McCarroll, J. (1998). Toward a dialogical
Kant, I. (1998). Critique of pure reason (P. Guyer & A. W. Wood, self. The American Behavioral Scientist, 41(4), 496–515.
Trans.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. (Original work Rose, N. (1996). Inventing our selves: Psychology, power, and person-
published 1781). hood. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Keller, E. F. (1985). Reflections on gender and science. New Haven: Yale Roth, W. M., & Lee, Y. J. (2007). “Vygotsky’s neglected legacy”:
University Press. Cultural-historical activity theory. Review of Educational
Kvale, S. (Ed.). (1992). Psychology and postmodernism. London: Sage. Research, 77(2), 186–232.
Lange, F. A. (1950). The history of materialism and criticism of its Sandoval, C. (2000). Methodology of the oppressed. Minneapolis: Uni-
present importance (E. C. Thomas, Trans.) (3rd ed., with an versity of Minnesota Press.
introduction by B. Russell) (This translation first published Scarborough, E., & Furumoto, L. (1987). Untold lives: The first gen-
in three volumes in 1877, 1890 and 1892; reissued in one volume eration of American women psychologists. New York: Columbia
in 1925; reprinted in 1950). New York: The Humanities Press. University Press.
(Original work first published 1866). Sève, L. (1978). Man in Marxist theory and the psychology of person-
Lewin, K. (1946). Action research and minority problems. Journal of ality (J. McGreal, Trans.). Atlantic Highlands: Humanities Press.
Social Issues, 2(4), 34–46. Slife, B. D., Reber, J. S., & Richardson, F. C. (2005). Critical thinking
Martin, J., Sugarman, J. H., & Thompson, J. (2003). Psychology and about psychology: Hidden assumptions and plausible alternatives.
the question of agency. Albany: Suny. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
248 C Cronbach, Lee J.

Sloan, T. (1996). Damaged life: The crisis of the modern psyche. was 4 years old. According to his sister, Lee was over-
London: Routledge. heard then calculating the unit price of potatoes. He
Sloan, T. (Ed.). (2000). Critical psychology: Voices for change.
came to the conclusion that the market his mother
New York: St. Martin’s.
Stam, H. J. (Ed.). (1998). The body and psychology. London: Sage. shopped charged more than the market he was in
Teo, T. (2005). The critique of psychology: From Kant to postcolonial with a babysitter. This feat was then reported to
theory. New York: Springer. Blanche Cummings, a school psychologist and disciple
Teo, T. (2008). From speculation to epistemological violence in psy- of Lewis Terman. After Terman gave Lee an IQ test and
chology: A critical-hermeneutic reconstruction. Theory & Psy-
found his score was 200, he enrolled Lee into his gifted
chology, 18(1), 47–67.
Teo, T. (2009). Philosophical concerns in critical psychology. In D.
program. With the push from his mother, he graduated
Fox, I. Prilleltensky, & S. Austin (Eds.), Critical psychology: An from Fresno High School at the age of 14 and Fresno
introduction (2nd ed., pp. 36–53). London: Sage. State College at 18 (Shavelson and Gleser 2002).
Teo, T., & Febbraro, A. (2003). Ethnocentrism as a form of intuition In 1938, he earned a master’s degree at the Univer-
in psychology. Theory & Psychology, 13, 673–694. sity of California-Berkeley while teaching mathematics
Tolman, C. W. (1994). Psychology, society, and subjectivity: An intro-
and chemistry at Fresno High School. Then he went on
duction to German critical psychology. London: Routledge.
Tolman, C. W. (2009). Holzkamp’s critical psychology as a science to obtain a doctor’s degree in educational psychology
from the standpoint of the human subject. Theory & Psychology, from the University of Chicago. He became an associate
19(2), 149–160. professor of psychology at State College of Washington,
Tolman, C. W., & Maiers, W. (Eds.). (1991). Critical psychology: and then moved to teach at the University of Chicago
Contributions to an historical science of the subject. Cambridge,
and University of Illinois. In 1964, he went to
MA: Cambridge University Press.
Toulmin, S., & Leary, D. E. (1985). The cult of empiricism in psy-
Stanford’s School of Education and continued his
chology, and beyond. In S. Koch & D. E. Leary (Eds.), A century of career until he retired in 1980. After his retirement, he
psychology as science (pp. 594–617). New York: McGraw-Hill. continued to focus on the debates on educational and
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological testing. He completed a book on a new
psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. theory of aptitude (Corno et al. 2001) and went on to
Walkerdine, V., Lucey, H., & Melody, J. (2001). Growing up girl:
work on a paper discussing the uses, misuses, and
Psychosocial explorations of gender and class. New York:
New York University Press. misunderstanding of the Cronbach alpha.
Walkerdine, V. (Ed.). (2002). Challenging subjects: Critical psychology
for a new millennium. New York: Palgrave. Major Contributions
Ward, S. C. (2002). Modernizing the mind: Psychological knowledge His research can be divided into three major areas:
and the remaking of society. Westport: Praeger.
measurement theory, program evaluation, and instruc-
Willy, R. (1899). Die Krisis in der Psychologie [The crisis in psychology].
Leipzig: Reisland.
tion, among which his contribution to measurement
Winston, A. S. (Ed.). (2004). Defining difference: Race and racism in issues were undoubtedly of greatest help to all educa-
the history of psychology. Washington, DC: American Psycholog- tional psychologists. The most impressive and popular
ical Association. of Cronbach’s research is the Coefficient Alpha, cur-
rently known as Cronbach’s alpha. His experience of
teaching his first courses in evaluation and measure-
ment and writing the first edition of Essentials of Psy-
Cronbach, Lee J. chological Testing (1949) contributed to the birth of this
coefficient alpha. Most researchers in the field, whether
JUN LI they are educators, psychologists, and other social
Fordham University, New York, NY, USA scientists, have had the experience of using it to provide
a measurement of reliability from a single test.
Although the Coefficient Alpha first appeared in 1951
Early Life, Education, and Professional (Cronbach 1951), however, it is still widely used today
Development because it is easily calculated and could be applied to
Lee Joseph Cronbach was born in 1916 in Fresno, dichotomously-scored multiple-choice items or
California. His talent in testing was revealed when he polytomous attitude scales.

You might also like