Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 16

Toward a Framework of Product Placement:

Theoretical Propositions

C I TAT I O N :

Cristel A. Russell (1998) ,"Toward a Framework of Product Placement: Theoretical


Propositions", in NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 25, eds. Joseph W. Alba & J.
Wesley Hutchinson, Provo, UT : Association for Consumer Research, Pages: 357-362.

Advances in Consumer Research Volume 25, 1998 Pages 357-362


TOWARD A FRAMEWORK OF PRODUCT PLACEMENT: THEORETICAL PROPOSITIONS
Cristel A. Russell, University of Arizona
Everyone remembers seeing Reese’s Pieces candies in the movie "E.T." and Audrey
Hepburn’s Givenchy apparel in "Breakfast at Tiffanys." Seinfeld’s favorite cereal brands are
well known to all regular viewers of his NBC television show. These are just a few of the
multitude of branded products that appear in movies or television programs. This practice of
product placement usually involves a fee that manufacturers pay to have their products
included as background on television and movies. Consumer marketers spend an estimated
$50 million annually in product placements in the movie industry alone (Elliott 1997).
Even though its effectiveness as an advertising tool appears accepted among practitioners,
product placement has not generated much research interest in the marketing discipline.
Previous studies of product placement have focused on brand recall or recognition (Steortz
1987, Babin and Carder 1996) or attitudes towards product placement (Gutpa and Gould
1997). Moreover, there is no apparent theoretical framework which describes this
:
phenomenon. The objective of this paper is to present theoretical support for a framework
for how product placement works. The articulation of the theoretical framework is best
summarized in terms similar to McCracken’s meaning transfer model (1988). As depicted in
Figure 1, we posit an adapted meaning transfer model in which the product meaning
associated with a popular television show or movie is ultimatey transferred to the individual
viewer.
We first differentiate between types of placement and posit a three dimensional framework
based on the information modalities of the placements. We then incorporate transformation
as a means for assessing the effectiveness of a placement. Finally, we examine the nature of
the show-product linkage in terms of learning theory, and the strength of the show-individual
linkage in terms of behavioral modeling.
PRODUCT PLACEMENT: AN INFORMATION PROCESSING PERSPECTIVE
A Three-Dimensional Framework of Product Placement
Previous research on product placement has mostly been limited to measuring brand recall
to assess effectiveness. One of the few empirical studies on product placement indicated that
viewers were able to recognize brands placed within a film (Babin and Carder 1996).
However, the study only dealt with one form of product placement: the visual appearance of
the brand on the screen. We posit three types of product placement which are categorized
according to their modality and relevance. For audio-visual media, such as television and
cinema, we first differentiate between the forms of product placement based on their
different information modalities (cf. Bettman 1979).
A brief examination of TV shows and movies reveals that placement types vary along two
main dimensions: visual and verbal or auditory. A purely visual type of placement involves
placing the brand in the background of a show in one of two ways: either through creative
placement, which insinuates the brand into the film, such as with outdoor advertisements in
street scenes; or through on-set placement, which positions the product on the film set itself,
such as food brands placed in kitchen scenes. In the remainder of this paper, this is referred
to as screen placement. Screen placement can have different degrees, depending on the
number of appearances on the screen, the style of camera shot on the product, and so on.
The second dimension is auditory or verbal. This type of placement refers to the brand being
mentioned in a dialogue. There are also varying degrees of audio placement, depending on
the context in which the product is mentioned, the frequency with which it is mentioned, and
the emphasis placed on the product name (tone of the voice, place in the dialogue, character
speaking at the time, etc.). This purely verbal type we label script placement.
In some cases, the product becomes part of the plot, taking a major place in the story line or
building the persona of a character. This type of placement, which we call plot placement,
:
constitutes the third dimension of our framework. It consists of any combination of visual and
verbal components and can be conceived as the degree of connection between the product
and the plot. A mere mention of the brand combined with a brief appearance of the product
on the screen is considered low intensity. However, in cases where the actor is clearly
identified with the brand, e.g. James Bond with his Aston Martin, then BMW Z3, or where the
brand becomes a central part of the plot, e.g. Kenny Rogers’ Roasted Chicken episode in
"Seinfeld," such cases constitute high intensity plot placement.
Figure 2 graphically illustrates this three-dimensional framework, composed of screen
placement (visual component), script placement (audio component), and plot placement
(connection to the plot).
We now describe the theoretical premises underlying expected differences in effectiveness
for each of these three types of product placement.
Different Processing Codes for Different Types of Placement
From an informationprocessing perspective, script, screen, and plot placements differ in the
types of processing they require. Perceptual encoding refers to the "process by which the
individual, having attended to some stimulus, interprets that stimulus" (Bettman 1979, p. 25).
In his study of how incoming stimulus information is transformed and elaborated within the
organism, Paivio (1971) distinguished between these imaginal and verbal coding processes.
When analyzing the recognition memory and memory storage associated with each code, he
identified imagery as a "parallel-processing system" whereas verbal processes were assumed
to specialize in "serial processing" (1971, p. 180). Hence, visual images and verbal units
involve different memory codes.
The coding redundancy hypothesis, which states that "memory increases directly with the
number of alternative memory codes available for an item" (Paivio 1971, p. 181) has clear
implications for product placement. Since visual and audio dimensions activate different
processing codes, varying combinations of screen and script placement differ in
effectiveness and brand recall (Paivio 1971). As illustrated in Figure 2, pure screen placement
would initiate visual processing, whereas pure script placement would require only verbal
processing. A plot placement that relies on placing the brand both on the screen and in the
conversation provides an opportunity for both verbal and visual encoding, whereas the other
situations would likely activate only one form of encoding. Paivio’s coding redundancy
hypothesis provides logical support for the following research proposition:
Proposition #1: Plot placements which rely on both visual and audio information will produce
higher levels of brand recall than pure screen or script placement.
FIGURE 1
AN ADAPTED MEANING-TRANSFER MODEL
:
Although not specifically identified as "plot placement", a telephone survey conducted by
Steortz (1987) found that the average recall was significantly higher when movie placements
involved both visual and verbal product identification.
Another implication of Paivio’s research pertains to the superiority of visual over verbal
mediators, when not simultaneously available (Paivio 1971). Paivio hypothesized that
information in verbal mediators would be harder to retain than information in imagery
mediators because the latter are more readily available. This finding implies that screen
placement is more valuable in terms of its mnemonic ability and decoding accuracy.
However, according to the social learning paradigm, "most of the cognitive processes that
regulate behavior are primarily verbal rather than visual" (Bandura 1971, p. 18). These
differences suggest that product placement effectiveness may well be a function of individual
processing styles. In the following section we describe how style of processing moderates
product placement effectiveness.
Individual Processing Style Differences as a Moderator
Recent research in consumer behavior has identified individual differences in processing
styles as a determining factor of the effectiveness of visual and verbal messages (for a more
extensive review, see Childers, Houston and Heckler 1985). Childers et al.’s Style of
Processing (SOP) scale can assess an individual’s "preference and propensity to engage in a
verbal and/or visual modality of processing" (1985, p.130). According to their experiments,
an individual’s style of processing will determine his/her allocation of attention to visual
and/or verbal cues. Even though their measurement solely dealt with print material, we can
anticipate similar individual differences with audio-visual material. For instance, a visual style
of processing will lead an individual to allocate more attention to the visual cues, and
therefore, be more aware of screen placement. On the other hand, script placement would
receive more attention from viewers with a more verbal or audio style of processing.
Consequently, when assessig the effectiveness of product placement types, it is necessary to
account for individual differences in processing style. This observation leads to our next
proposition:
Proposition #2: Individual differences in style of processing moderate the effectiveness of the
different types of product placement. In other words, individuals higher in visual processing
will attend more to screen placement, whereas individuals higher in audio processing will
pay more attention to script placement.
Having established a method for classifying the different types of placements and specified
differences in how each type of placement is processed, we now turn our focus to the nature
of the effectiveness of product placement. The concept of transformational advertising offers
insightful guidance for this aspect.
:
ASSESSING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF PRODUCT PLACEMENT IN TERMS OF
TRANSFORMATION
The notion of transformational advertising was first raised by Wells (1980) as a framework for
understanding how advertising works. Transformational advertising involves the association
of "the experience of using/consuming the advertised brand with a unique set of
psychological characteristics" (Puto and Wells 1984, p.638). A transformational
advertisement would therefore make the experience of using the brand richer and more
enjoyable by connecting the experience of the ad with that of using the brand in such an
intimate fashion that "consumers cannot remember the brand without recalling the
experience generated by the advertisement" (Puto and Wells 1984, p.638).
Drawing a parallel between traditional advertising and product placement enables us to
identify a successful product placement as one that transforms the experience of using the
brand to match that shown in the movie/TV show. In order to justify product placement as a
transformational experience, its host environment (television and cinema), is analyzed on
each of the four dimensions of the transformation concept: 1- personal relevance, 2-
experiential/empathy, 3- informational, 4- executional (Puto 1986). Because this technique
involves products being placed inside a TV show or movie, we can envision it as a form of
endorsement by a (several) member(s) of the cast. Drawing evidence from traditional
celebrity endorsement research as well as research in the mass communication discipline, we
now address product placement in the context of each of the four transformation dimensions.
FIGURE 2
A THREE-DIMENSIONAL CONSTRUCT OF PRODUCT PLACEMENT
Personal Relevance: An Artistic Connection
Personal relevance refers to the degree to which the viewer connects him/herself with the TV
show/movie (cf. Puto 1986). In mass communication research, relevance can be assessed as
an intersection between the textual and the social, e.g. between plot and everyday life (Fiske
1992). Since movies are often described as a form of art, we can draw evidence for the
personal relevance component from the study of art. It has been well-documented that,
through our artistic experiences, we view ourselves metaphorically as the characters in
novels, plays, or movies (Hirschman 1988). The construct of personal relevance is even more
evident in realistic settings, which tend to reproduce an ordinary context, where the setting
and story line make relevance to the viewer’s life obvious. Therefore, placing real products
inside these shows intensifies the authenticity and salience of the television show/movie for
the viewers (Shermach 1995).
Experiential / Empathy: A Projective Function
As noted by Puto (1986), the concept of personal relevance is very closely related o that of
:
empathy. Besides depicting the personal relevance dimension of TV series heroes, or movie
stars, many researchers have also accounted for a phenomenon of identification to
characters, which is directly related to the empathy dimension (Fiske 1992, Livingstone
1990). Empathy in this context is defined as "an observer’s vicarious emotional identification"
with the TV series or movie (Puto and Wells 1984, p.639). Extensive research has shown that
"popular culture narratives serve as valuable projective functions for consumers" (Hirschman
1988, p. 357). Because of their regularity, television series, and particularly soap operas,
have actually been referred to as the "safest outlets for vicarious identification" (Mitroff and
Bennis 1989, p. 80). Because it is the main ingredient of product placement’s effectiveness,
this emotional dimension will be the object of a subsequent section in this paper, where we
highlight this identification process.
Informational: A Symbolic Contribution
In traditional advertising, the informational dimension, refers to the ability to provide
consumers with factual, relevant brand data (Puto and Wells 1984). Product placement differs
from advertising in the fact that it is mostly indirect and does not usually intend to provide the
viewers with factual information about the product. Whereas functional products may allow
for more extensive information to be communicated by showing the product being used or
providing specific information about that product, symbolic products may benefit more from
the personal relevance or emotional components, described earlier. For these products,
however, we can establish the informational nature of TV shows and movies as more
ideological or symbolic. Studies of product symbolism have largely accounted for the
contribution of the media in building a product’s symbolic meaning (Levy 1959, McCracken
1988). In their semiological analysis of the movie "Out of Africa", Holbrook and Grayson
(1986) described how both major and minor consumption symbolism were used to enrich
the plot, theme, and character. Popular television series such as "Dallas" or "Dynasty" also
encode the ideology of consumption through "fantasy narratives populated by imaginary
characters confronted by make-believe crisis and choices" (Hirschman 1988, p. 357).
Products placed inside these programs become part of a story which is literally and
figuratively rich in consumption imagery (Hirschman 1988). We can then consider the
informational dimension of product placement as a symbolic one. It is widely recognized
that, movie after movie, Humphrey Bogart contributed to the symbolic image of cigarettes,
by associating smoking with a perception of masculinity, elegance, and sensuality. Since
product placement is not designed for providing factual information to the viewers, we can
anticipate that it will rely more on affective processes, rather than on the cognitive aspect of
the experience.
Executional: The Need for a Good Match
:
This last dimension of the transformational concept, executional, highlights the importance of
a well-executed ad in terms of likeability and memorability of the corresponding experience.
Traditional advertising and product placement differ greatly in terms of their execution
requirements. Whereas the advertising plot is built around the brand and limited in time,
product placement generally involves placing the brand inside an already existing plot.
Therefore this dimension does not so much refer to the execution per se, but rather to the
good fit of the product inside its host show. Since we have defined product placement as a
form of endorsement, we can relate the executional dimension to the match-up hypothesis
developed for traditional celebrity endorsement. This hypothesis suggests that the
characteristics of a product need to match-up with the image conveyed by the celebrity (see
Kamins 1990). This topic is well-documented, and practitioners do eport that a good match is
a necessary ingredient for successful product placements (Hulin-Salkin 1989, Mitchell 1996,
Shermach 1995).
We have established that product placement satisfies all of the basic requirements for a
transformational experience. This demonstration draws our next proposition, concerning the
nature of the effectiveness of product placement:
Proposition #3: A successful product placement is one that transforms the experience of
using/consuming the product to match that shown in the TV show/movie, i.e., this
experience would not occur without the placement.
This proposition implies that the experience of using/consuming the product is different
whether you have been exposed to the placement or not. We have demonstrated that the
effectiveness of a product placement is directly linked to the transformation of the experience
with the brand. In the remainder of this paper, we therefore define effectiveness as the power
or ability of a well-placed product to transform the experience in a positive manner with
respect to the brand being "promoted."
Now that the overall process of product placement has been identified as a form of
transformation, we can focus on the internal links of the framework, the show-product link
and the show-individual link (see Figure 1).
THE SHOW-PRODUCT LINKAGE IN TERMS OF LEARNING THEORY
This section focuses on the link between show and product and draws evidence from
learning theory.
The Non-Conscious Nature of Product Placement
The concept of product placement relies on the pairing of a branded product with a host
show. This pairing of stimuli directly evokes the classical conditioning paradigm. Classical
conditioning is generally accepted in the consumer behavior literature as a mechanism
relevant for understanding and producing advertising effects (Stuart, Shimp and Engle 1987).
:
The conditioning paradigm is based on the transfer of responses between stimuli. Pavlov’s
experiments led to the conclusion that the repeated pairing of a Conditioned Stimulus (CS)
with an Unconditioned Stimulus (US) will cause the CS to elicit a Conditioned Response (CR)
in an unconscious, automatic fashion.
The foundation of the classical conditioning theory lies in the non-conscious link between the
two stimuli. Supporting evidence can be found in the Internal Processing Algorithms (IPAs)
posited by Lewicki (1986). IPAs refer to "the memory representation of covariation between
two or more features or events" (Lewicki 1986, p. 29). Lewicki demonstrated that IPAs are
acquired non-consciously, that they non-consciously influence behavior, and that they cannot
be controlled consciously. Lewicki based his non-conscious processing research on the
observation that people usually cannot control such behavioral reactions as laughing or
crying when watching a movie, that they cannot "define the specificity of the situation"
(1986, p. 9). His experiments focused on influencing information processing without
affecting the perceiver’s conscious awareness: subjects were exposed to stimuli containing a
covariation that was not accessible to conscious awareness and were then asked to complete
a task related to the covariation. Results provided support for the hypothesis that humans do
acquire information non-consciously and store it in long term memory, without being aware
of the source of the covariation information.
In the case of products placed in the background of the screen, it is apparently the goal of
advertisers to rely on non-conscious linkage between stimuli, and therefore we can consider
that the "endorsement" is processed non-conciously.
Proposition #4: The show-product linkage is processed non-consciously by the viewers: it is
not necessary for the viewer to recall a specific exposure to the product for transformation to
occur.
This proposition strictly parallels one from the transformational advertising concept (Puto
1986) and underlines the need for repeated exposures to the stimuli.
Affective Transfer
Classical conditioning stresses the conditioned emotion-eliciting properties of the stimulus
(Staats 1996). For example, Staats demonstrated that the pairing of positive words such as
beauty, honest, smart, rich, and so on with a person would increase the degree of positive
reinforcement associated with that person (1996). Analogously, if the emotional response
associated with the stimulus is negative, the reinforcement can go in the other direction and
generate a negative affective transfer.
In traditional celebrity endorsed advertising, the focus has mainly been on the celebrity’s
influence on the viewer’s affective/feeling state (Petty and Cacioppo 1981, Allen and
Madden 1985). Hence, we can anticipate that affective conditioning drives most of the
:
product placement process. Empirical evidence for the affective conditioning of products
exists in the marketing literature. Even though they generated a great deal of controversy
(Allen and Madden 1985, Bierley, McSweeney and Vannieuwkerk 1985), Gorn’s results
demonstrated that product preferences can be conditioned through a single exposure to
appealing or unappealing music (1982). This finding supports Zajonc’s proposition that affect
does not require extensive cognitive processing (Zajonc and Markus 1982). From this, we
can induce that the nature of the conditioned response linked to product placement is mostly
affective. Although it is not the objective of this paper, we also recognize that this transfer of
affect is bi-directional as it can also occur from the product onto the show, if the emotional
association with the product is strong.
Proposition #5: The pairing of a product with an emotionally rich show (television or movie)
conditions a transfer of affect from the show to the product. Therefore, the nature of the
conditioned response to the product is affective rather than cognitive.
This proposition suggests that placing products within shows that elicit positive/negative
emotional responses will translate into a similar emotional response to the product.
Obviously, this observation raises the issue of a negative-paired association, such as when
products are placed in a non-flattering or disparaging situation. Many practitioners consider
this a risk inherent to the technique (Shermach 1995) and a logical consequence of the
advertisers’ lack of control on the use of their products (Hulin-Salkin 1989, Mitchell 1996).
Similarly, if the products placed in the show have a negative content, or are ethically-
charged, the positive emotional transfer may not occur as anticipated. Gutpa and Gould
(1997) noted that consumers may alienate such product placements, therefore canceling any
chance for positive reinforcement, and subsequent positive transformation.
Some Limitations to the Application of the Conditioning Paradigm
A central debate in the conditioning research with humans has revolved around the issue of
awareness and whether or not the US/CS associations are consciously processed (Allen and
Madden 1985). One could argue that, in some cases, product placement is so intense that, at
least for some individuals, its presence becomes conscious. Two elements can potentially
affect the effectiveness of classical conditioning of product placement.
One such factor concerns the potential effects of prir knowledge of or familiarization with the
product. Some have suggested that "classical conditioning would be retarded if either or
both the conditioned and unconditioned stimuli are already familiar" to viewers (Stuart,
Shimp and Engle 1987). Numerous experiments have shown that prior arousal as well as the
number of repetitions of the stimulus both influenced the clarity of a percept (Paivio 1971).
The intensity of the placement may also impair the conditioned response. For instance, in
cases where the plot is inundated with messages about the product (high intensity plot
:
placement), it would be difficult for a viewer not to be conscious of the commercial effort.
Likewise, combining product placement with a full joint-promotion campaign usually ensures
that the consciousness level is reached. This practice has been reported by practitioners as
much more effective than mere placement (Hulin-Salkin 1989), because it emphasizes the
presence of the product and creates conscious awareness.
In both cases, the commercial intent of the placement becomes clearer. It has been noted
that when the possibility of causal cognitive explanations is allowed, the traditional classical
conditioning model does not apply (Allen and Madden 1985). These limitations are
highlighted in the following proposition:
Proposition #6: The higher the level of pre-familiarization with the product and/or the
stronger the perceived presence of the product in the show, the better the clarity of the
perception of product placement. In such cases, the nature of the response to the product is
cognitive rather than affective.
THE SHOW-INDIVIDUAL LINKAGE IN TERMS OF BEHAVIORAL MODELING
The latter part of this paper focuses on the show-individual linkage and the underlying causes
of the emotional arousal in movies or television series. The ample literature on the role of the
media in our lives forms the basis for the analysis of this process in terms of the behavioral
modeling paradigm.
The Behavioral Modeling Paradigm
The modeling paradigm is conceptually linked to vicarious learning (Nord and Peter 1980). It
assumes that observational learning occurs through reinforcement of imitative behavior
(Bandura 1971). Almost any behavior can be acquired, maintained or extinguished by means
of a carefully planned social reinforcement (Zajonc 1966). Social learning theory assumes that
modeling influences operate principally through their "symbolic representations of modeled
events, rather than specific stimulus-response associations" (Bandura 1971, p. 16). For
instance, the mere observation of the behavior and attitudes exhibited by models contributes
to the social learning process. Advertising has made intensive use of this theory by depicting
positive consequences for the use of a product (Nord and Peter 1980). Similarly, the
transformational power of product placement lies in the positioning of a product in a
positively reinforcing situation.
It has been noted that the behavior modification is directly related to the value of the
reinforcing agent (Bandura 1971). Referring to the classical conditioning paradigm, the
reinforcing power of the stimulus is determined by the strength of the emotional response
that it elicits (Staats 1996). The strength of the model in modifying behavior thus lies in the
relationship between the actor/actress and the viewer. Much evidence can be drawn from
the study of social influences in the sociology and social psychology discipline.
:
The Social Influence Power of Television and Cinema
Character representation differs signifiantly between television and cinema, and assessing
product placement’s effectiveness in terms of behavioral modeling requires that a distinction
be made between TV and cinema. In motion pictures, characters may serve as role models
for the consumers who view them, but a film is usually an entity, and the character’s story
ends with the movie. Television differs because of its serial form, and its recurrence (Fiske
1992). The lead characters appear to live in similar time scales to their audience and exceed
their textual existence, so that audience members can relate to them in terms of familiarity
and identification (Fiske 1992). Although they differ in some respects, television and cinema
share the ability of engendering a process of identification.
In the field of communications, the study of television influence is often associated with the
cultivation paradigm (see Gerbner et al. 1980). The cultivation hypothesis is based on the
premise that television’s images cultivate the dominant tendencies of our culture’s beliefs,
ideologies, and world views. A noteworthy element of cultivation research is the concept of
resonance. Resonance characterizes the amplification of issues particularly salient to certain
groups of viewers (Gerbner et al. 1980). Support for the cultivation hypothesis and
particularly the resonance effect can be found in the consumer behavior literature. In their
recent study of consumer socialization through television, O’Guinn and Shrum (1997)
demonstrated that heavy viewers of soap operas and "L.A. Law" over-estimated the
prevalence of products and activities associated with affluent lifestyle.
In terms of product placement, the resonance hypothesis suggests that individuals to whom
the story line or character development is most salient would be more affected by the
content of the movie or television show. This effect corroborates the fact that it is the strength
of the connection with the model that drives the modeling effect (Bandura 1971). Therefore,
we propose that the transformational effect of a product placement will be mediated by the
level of connectedness between the individual viewer and a television program/movie.
Proposition #7: The higher the degree of connectedness/association with the show/actor,
the greater the affective transfer, and the stronger the transformational effect of product
placement.
This proposition completes the theoretical framework for the product placement process and
subsequently assessing the effectiveness of specific product placements.
CONCLUSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS
We have demonstrated through the extant literature that a successful product placement
transforms the experience of the use/consumption of the product to match that depicted in
the show and that the transformational power of product placement relies on a conditioned
transfer of affect motivated by the influential role of television and cinema. At a time when
:
traditional advertising faces the growing problems of audience fragmentation, rising rate
cards, and cynicism among consumers toward media messages, the product placement
industry is booming. However, the effectiveness of this technique still remains to be
empirically tested. The theoretical propositions presented in this paper offer potential
directions for future research on this alternative advertising and promotional medium. Future
research should focus on each of the linkages identified in this piece, the show-product
linkage, the show-individual linkage, and the product-individual linkage.
REFERENCES
Allen, Chris T. and Thomas J. Madden (1985) A Closer Look at Classical Conditioning, Journal
of Consumer Research, 12, 301-323
Babin, Laurie A., and Sheri T. Carder (1996) Viewers’ Recognition of Brands Placed Within A
Film, International Journal of Advertising, 15(2), 140-151
Bandura, Albert (1971) Psychological Modeling: Conflicting Theories, Chicago, IL: Aldine
Atherton
Bettman, James (1979) An Information Processing Theory of Consumer Choice, Menlo Park,
CA: Addison-Wesley Publishing Company
Bierley, Calvin, Frances K. McSweeney, and Renee Vannieuwkerk (1985) Classical
Conditioning of Preferences for Stimuli, Journal of Consumer Research, 12, 316-323
Childers, Terry L., Michael J. Houston, and Susan E. Heckler (1985) Measurement of
Individual Differences in Visual Versus Verbal Information Processing, Journal of Consumer
Research, 12, 125-134
Elliott, Stuart (1997) The Spot on the Cutting-Room Floor, The New York Times, February 7,
C1
Fiske, John (1992) Television Culture, London: Methuen
Gerbner, George, Larry Gross, Michael Morgan, and Nancy Signorielli (1980). The
"Mainstreaming" of America: Violence Profile No. 11. Journal of Communication, 10-29.
Gorn, Gerald J. (1982) The Effects of Music in Advertising On Choice Behavior: A Classical
Conditioning Approach, Journal of Marketing, 46, 94-101
Gutpa, Pola B. and Stephen J. Gould (1997) Consumers’ Perceptions of the Ethics and
Acceptability of Product Placements in Movies: Product Category and Individual Differences,
The Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 19 (1)
Hirschman, Elisabeth C. (1988) The Ideology of Consumption: A Structural-Syntactical
Analysis of "Dallas" and "Dynasty", Journal of Consumer Research, 15, 344-359
Holbrook, Morris B. and Mark W. Grayson (1986) The Semiology of Cinematic Consumption:
Symbolic Consumer Behavior in Out of Africa, Journal of Consumer Research, 13, 374-381.
Hulin-Salkin, Belinda (1989) Movie Tie-Ins: Hollywood Changes Its Tune, Incentive, 163 (6)
:
Kamins, Michael A. (1990) An Investigation into the "Match-Up" Hypothesis in Celebrity
Advertising: When Beauty May Be Only Skin Deep, Journal of Advertising, 19(1), 4-13
Levy, Sidney (1959) Symbols For Sale, Harvard Business Review, 37, 117-124
Lewicki, Pawel (1986) Nonconscious Social Information Processing, Orlando: Academic
Press
Livingstone, Sonia M. (1990) Making Sense Of Television: The Psychology Of Audience
Interpretation, Oxford [England] ; New York : Pergamon Press
McCracken, Grant (1988) Culture and Consumption: New Approaches To The Symbolic
Character of Consumer Goods and Activities, Bloomington, Indiana University Press
Mitchell, Alan (1996) The Power of a Plug, Management Today, Feb., 81-82
Mitroff, Ian I. and Warren Bennis (1989) The Unreality Industry : The Deliberate Manufacturing
Of Falsehood And What It Is Doing To Our Lives, New York, NY : Carol Pub. Group
Nord, Walter R. and J. Paul Peter (1980) A Behavior Modification Perspective in Marketing,
Journal of Marketing 44, 36-47
O’Guinn, Thomas C. and L. J. Shrum (1997). The Role of Television in the Construction of
Consumer Reality. Journal of Consumer Research, 23 (4), 278-294
Paivio, Allan (1971) Imagery and Verbal Processes, New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston,
Inc.
Petty, Richard E. and John T. Cacioppo (1981) Attitudes and Persuasion: Classic and
Contemporary Approaches, Dubuque, IA: William C. Brown
Puto, Christopher P. (1986) Transformational Advertisig: Just Another Name for Emotional
Advertising or A New Approach? In Proceedings Of The Division Of Consumer Psychology,
Ed. Wayne D. Hoyer, American Psychological Association, 4-6
Puto, Christopher P. and William D. Wells (1984) Informational and Transformational
Advertising The Differential Effects Of Time, in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. XI, Ed.
Thomas C. Kinnear, Association For Consumer Research, 638-643
Shermach, Kelly (1995) Casting Call Goes Out, Marketing News, 29 (16) 11
Staats, Arthur W. (1996) Behavior and Personality: Psychological Behaviorism, New York:
Springer Publishing Company, Inc.
Steortz, Eva (1987) The Cost Efficiency and Communication Effects Associated with Brand
Name Exposure within Motion Pictures, Master’s Thesis, West Virginia University,
Morgantown, WV
Stuart, Elnora W., Terence A. Shimp, and Randall W. Engle (1987) Classical Conditioning of
Consumer Attitudes: Four Experiments in an Advertising Context, Journal of Consumer
Research, 14, 334-349
Wells, William D. (1980) How Advertising Works, Mimeo, Chicago, IL: Needham Harper
:
Worldwide
Zajonc, Robert B. (1966) Social Psychology: An Experimental Approach, Belmont, CA:
Brooks/Cole Publishing Company
Zajonc, Robert B. and Hazel Markus (1982) Affective and Cognitive Factors in Preferences,
Journal of Consumer Research, 9, 123-131.
----------------------------------------

AUTHORS

Cristel A. Russell, University of Arizona

VOLUME

NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 25 | 1998

SHARE PROCEEDING

Featured papers See More


:
F E AT U R E D

Decreasing Impatience with Bundled Donations


Sachin Banker, University of Utah, USA

Read More

F E AT U R E D

Tuition Myopia: Temporal Discounting Induces a Myopic Focus on the


Costs of Higher Education
Haewon Yoon, Indiana University, USA
Yang Yang, University of Florida, USA
Carey K. Morewedge, Boston University, USA

Read More

F E AT U R E D

L7. The Joy of Shopping: Reconciling Mixed Effects of Positive Emotions


on Shopping Behavior
Kelley Gullo, Duke University, USA
Duncan Simester, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, USA
Gavan Fitzsimons, Duke University, USA

Read More

Engage with Us
:
Becoming an Association for Consumer Research member is simple. Membership in ACR is
relatively inexpensive, but brings significant benefits to its members.

Join ACR now!

© 2022 Association for Consumer Research Contact Privacy Terms of Use Proceedings
:

You might also like