Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/317137285

Literacy matters in marketing

Article  in  International Journal of Bank Marketing · July 2017


DOI: 10.1108/IJBM-12-2016-0188

CITATIONS READS

12 1,083

1 author:

Bruce Huhmann
Virginia Commonwealth University
54 PUBLICATIONS   1,395 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Direct-to-Consumer Pharmaceutical Advertising View project

International and Cross-Cultural Advertising View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Bruce Huhmann on 02 April 2021.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


International Journal of Bank Marketing
Literacy matters in marketing
Bruce A. Huhmann,
Article information:
To cite this document:
Bruce A. Huhmann, (2017) "Literacy matters in marketing", International Journal of Bank Marketing,
Vol. 35 Issue: 5, pp.750-760, https://doi.org/10.1108/IJBM-12-2016-0188
Permanent link to this document:
https://doi.org/10.1108/IJBM-12-2016-0188
Downloaded by Virginia Commonwealth University At 08:34 07 August 2017 (PT)

Downloaded on: 07 August 2017, At: 08:34 (PT)


References: this document contains references to 40 other documents.
To copy this document: permissions@emeraldinsight.com
The fulltext of this document has been downloaded 56 times since 2017*
Users who downloaded this article also downloaded:
(2017),"Clear, conspicuous, and improving: US corporate websites for critical financial literacy in
retirement", International Journal of Bank Marketing, Vol. 35 Iss 5 pp. 761-780 <a href="https://
doi.org/10.1108/IJBM-01-2016-0010">https://doi.org/10.1108/IJBM-01-2016-0010</a>
(2017),"Financial education and financial satisfaction: Financial literacy, behavior, and capability
as mediators", International Journal of Bank Marketing, Vol. 35 Iss 5 pp. 805-817 <a href="https://
doi.org/10.1108/IJBM-01-2016-0009">https://doi.org/10.1108/IJBM-01-2016-0009</a>

Access to this document was granted through an Emerald subscription provided by emerald-
srm:112114 []
For Authors
If you would like to write for this, or any other Emerald publication, then please use our Emerald
for Authors service information about how to choose which publication to write for and submission
guidelines are available for all. Please visit www.emeraldinsight.com/authors for more information.
About Emerald www.emeraldinsight.com
Emerald is a global publisher linking research and practice to the benefit of society. The company
manages a portfolio of more than 290 journals and over 2,350 books and book series volumes, as
well as providing an extensive range of online products and additional customer resources and
services.
Emerald is both COUNTER 4 and TRANSFER compliant. The organization is a partner of the
Committee on Publication Ethics (COPE) and also works with Portico and the LOCKSS initiative for
digital archive preservation.

*Related content and download information correct at time of download.


The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available on Emerald Insight at:
www.emeraldinsight.com/0265-2323.htm

IJBM
35,5 Literacy matters in marketing
Bruce A. Huhmann
Department of Marketing, Virginia Commonwealth University,
Richmond, Virginia, USA
750
Abstract
Received 9 December 2016 Purpose – Literacy represents one’s ability to process and produce materials related to a domain. One type
Accepted 9 December 2016 of this higher-order, global individual difference variable is consumer financial literacy. It stems from one’s
financial information processing capacity, prior financial knowledge, and proficiency in optimizing
financial decisions and managing financial resources. The paper aims to discuss these issues.
Design/methodology/approach – The research matching perspective theoretically explains findings related
Downloaded by Virginia Commonwealth University At 08:34 07 August 2017 (PT)

to literacy, including those in this special issue. Optimal processing arises as available and required processing
resources correspond. Thus, cognitive comprehension and behavioral application/decision-making outcomes
following financial marketing communication exposure are optimized when consumer financial literacy matches
the level needed for successful processing. Insufficient or excess available resources harm outcomes.
Findings – The resource-matching perspective clarifies consumers’ increasing financial difficulties.
Consumers limit personal finance efforts because required resources overwhelm limited financial literacy.
However, education or experience can expand consumer financial literacy. Alternatively, financial service
marketers may accommodate low consumer financial literacy by simplifying financial information
presentation. Consumers reward firms that show sensitivity to their domain-specific literacy limitations with
stronger loyalty.
Research limitations/implications – Construct definition is vital to advance research. Yet, financial
literacy has no generally accepted definition. This paper’s definitions should aid understanding of the
psychological underpinnings of financial literacy’s components.
Originality/value – Much has been written about consumers’ inability to manage personal finances.
This paper provides a unified, theoretical explanation for consumers increasing financial literacy difficulties
and suggests ways that consumers, financial service providers, and public policy makers can overcome
these difficulties.
Keywords Consumer financial literacy, Cognitive capacity, Objective prior knowledge,
Decision-making proficiency, Resource-matching perspective, Financial services marketing, Financial education,
Processing fluency, Personal relevance, Need for cognition, Heuristics, Regulatory resource depletion
Paper type Viewpoint

Literacy and its components


Literacy encompasses a range of capabilities and skills that aid the understanding of
communicated material and processing of that information. Thus, literacy is of great
interest to marketers. Yet, research applying theories and methodological tools common in
the marketing discipline remains uncommon to investigations of advertising, health,
financial and other literacies despite their relevance to marketers (e.g. Huhmann, 2014;
Jae et al., 2011). For example, marketing researchers could contribute greatly to understanding
literacy and its effects by applying information processing theories developed to understand
consumer decision making and the processing of advertising, product information displays,
internet and social media marketing, and other forms of marketing communication.
Literacy is a higher-level construct that represents one’s ability to comprehend
information and, thus, optimize decision-making related to a specific domain. Literacy in a
specific domain is function of one’s overall cognitive capacity combined with
domain-specific learnt skills and heuristics as well as objective prior knowledge that
leads to proficiency in that domain (Huhmann, 2014).
International Journal of Bank
Marketing Researchers can apply this higher-order construct model of the basic components of
Vol. 35 No. 5, 2017
pp. 750-760
literacy to many domains (see Table I). Although literacy in one domain (e.g. language
© Emerald Publishing Limited
0265-2323
literacy) may aid in another domain (e.g. advertising literacy or consumer financial literacy),
DOI 10.1108/IJBM-12-2016-0188 each domain-specific literacy has elements that make it unique.
Literacy
Learnt skills and Objective prior Decision-making
Domain Definition heuristics knowledge proficiency matters in
marketing
Consumer Ability to comprehend Capacity to Knowledge of Proficiency in saving,
financial financial information, successfully process financial concepts, budgeting, expenditure
literacya optimize financial and comprehend services and products, control, and debt
decisions and information about such as loans, credit management that
effectively manage financial concepts, cards, banking, allows one to optimize 751
financial resources services and products investments, financial decisions and
insurance, and management financial
retirement planning resources
Advertising Ability to interpret, Skills to comprehend Knowledge of Sophisticated use of
literacyb analyze, evaluate, and advertising’s complex advertising trends, advertising to
Downloaded by Virginia Commonwealth University At 08:34 07 August 2017 (PT)

produce advertising imagery, text, and use conventions, styles, construct individual
messages across a of intertextuality and vocabulary, elements and group identities,
variety of media persuasion knowledge and techniques be a competent
to gain insight into consumer, and view
advertising’s advertising messages
persuasive intent; with skepticism
objectives; tactics; and
media, positioning
and targeting issues
Health Ability to obtain, Cognitive and social Knowledge of health Proficiency in decision
literacyc comprehend, evaluate, competencies and and healthcare making and judging
act upon or deliver skills needed for terminology, health courses of action that
health-related informed health or risks, treatment promote and maintain
information from healthcare decisions options and outcomes, good health and help
physicians, by members of the possible side effects one thrive in a
caregivers; public public or personnel in and contraindications healthcare setting
health, direct-to- health-related of pharmaceutical,
consumer, or contexts over-the-counter, and
professional traditional medicines
advertising;
prescriptions;
directions; labels; or
online sites
Language Ability to effectively Capacity to Knowledge of letters, Proficiency in reading
literacya read and write printed successfully identify, language, idioms, and creating printed
or hand-written understand and vocabulary, sentence or hand-written
materials in a interpret printed and structure and grammar materials in a
particular language written materials particular language
associated with a
particular language
Visual Ability to successfully Figure-ground Knowledge of how Proficiency in
literacyd interpret images and discrimination and individual images are recognizing three-
encode images that picture perception formed and the use of dimensional subjects
others can capacity arising from cultural content or settings from two-
comprehend familiarity with visual (e.g. icons, signs, dimensional images;
conventions (e.g. depth symbols, references resolving ambiguity
cues; representational and allusions) in or indeterminate
style; cropping; visual communication meanings in images;
illumination/color and applying
indicators; shape) and arbitrary, formal Table I.
cultural cues conventions Types of literacy
a b c d
Notes: Huhmann (2014), O’Donohoe and Tynan (1998), Wright et al. (2005), Pleasant (2014), Messaris of interest to
(1997), Meyers-Levy and Malaviya (1999) marketing researchers
IJBM I have incorporated a brief introduction to the papers in this special issue on consumer
35,5 financial literacy into a demonstration of how one theory developed by researchers in
marketing (i.e. the resource-matching perspective) can expand understanding of the
processes underlying a domain-specific literacy.

Theoretical explanation of literacy’s effects: a resource-matching perspective


752 The effects of higher vs lower literacy on domain-specific information processing and
downstream cognitive (e.g. comprehension) or behavioral (e.g. application or decision-making)
outcomes may be theoretically explained via the resource-matching perspective (Anand and
Sternthal, 1990; Jae et al., 2011; Meyers-Levy and Malaviya, 1999). The resource-matching
perspective predicts that optimal processing occurs when one’s available resources equal the
resource demand associated with some form of communication. For example, when a
Downloaded by Virginia Commonwealth University At 08:34 07 August 2017 (PT)

consumer possesses a level of financial literacy that matches that required by information in
a mutual fund advertisement, then processing of that advertisement’s information and,
in turn, investment behavior based on this information is optimized.
However, when information’s resource demand exceeds a consumer’s available resources,
the consumer will feel overwhelmed to the detriment of processing and its consequences.
For example, a mutual fund advertisement may contain information that demands more
resources than the consumer is willing or able to process; thus, harming processing and its
consequent outcomes. Unfortunately, decisions regarding financial services require much
information, but providers are reluctant to include all this information to avoid negative
consumer responses to copy-heavy or lengthy presentations (Franke et al., 2004). For example,
a content analysis of mutual fund ads appearing over two years in Barron’s or Money found
88 percent lack all the requisite risk-return trade-off, principal-agent conflict, or transaction
cost information needed for high quality investment decision-making (Huhmann and
Bhattacharyya, 2005). Including all this information should be beneficial according to theories
of finance and neoclassical economics (e.g. Arya et al., 2000; Fama, 1980; Huang and
Litzenberger, 1988; Ingersoll, 1987; Jensen and Meckling, 1976; Williamson, 1981). However,
the resource-matching perspective would expect this information to overwhelm most consumers.
An experiment that presented a mutual fund ad either with or without risk-return trade-off,
principal-agent conflict, and transaction cost information supports the resource-matching
perspective. Instead of being perceived as forthcoming and informative, mutual fund advertising
incorporating this information heightens consumers’ risk perceptions (Bhattacharyya et al.,
2014). Thus, including information useful to the financially literate is actually detrimental to
responsible advertisers as it drives the less financially literate toward financial service providers
who instead advertise via emotional and other non-informative appeals, which unsurprisingly
predominate in financial service advertising (Huhmann and Bhattacharyya, 2005).
A third possibility, according to the resource-matching perspective, occurs when
resource demand is insufficient to fully engage available resources. In this case, extra
resources may be devoted to counterarguments, source derogations, or other cognitive tasks
to the detriment of processing. For example, the resource demand associated with an
insurer’s website that explains its products to novices would likely lead to
counterarguments or source derogations if processed by experts or else these experts
might quickly shift the focus of their attention to more engaging stimuli.

Willingness to devote available resources


To improve processing and its consequences, the resource-matching perspective recommends
either increasing available resources that the consumer is willing or able to devote to
processing or decreasing the resource demand of information presented to consumers.
Willingness to devote available resources depends on motivation to process. Consumers
process and retain only a fraction of the information to which they are exposed. Processing
and acquisition increase with greater extrinsic or intrinsic motivation to process. Also, higher Literacy
levels of one type of motivation may somewhat compensate for lower levels of the other. matters in
Extrinsic motivation to process is predicated on information or experience’s perceived marketing
personal relevance to a consumer’s enduring or situational involvement. Enduring
involvement is on-going interest in a product or topic, whereas situational involvement
relates to one’s current decision or circumstance (Celsi and Olson, 1988). For example,
situational involvement when one is in the market for homeowner’s insurance increases 753
motivation to process insurance-related information, but may not satisfactorily boost financial
literacy due to its temporary nature. Alternatively, enduring involvement in financial matters
should benefit financial literacy. Despite government and industry education initiatives,
declining levels of financial literacy demonstrates the important, but often ignored, role of
personal relevance. Many assume that any exposure to personal finance materials improves
Downloaded by Virginia Commonwealth University At 08:34 07 August 2017 (PT)

consumers’ financial literacy. However, familiarity aids financial literacy only with sufficient
processing effort. Financial education materials that are not personally relevant fail to
increase motivation to process and, without motivation to process, exposure is insufficient for
acquiring and retaining concepts. As Xiao and Porto demonstrate in this issue, when financial
education is unable to develop sufficient financial literacy, consumers continue to be
dissatisfied with results of their financial decisions.
With intrinsic motivation, consumers find the act of processing enjoyable or personally
rewarding. Intrinsic motivation depends on need for cognition, which is one’s propensity to
engage in and prefer activities requiring considerable thought, regardless of personal
relevance (Cacioppo and Petty, 1982). For example, higher need for cognition increases
consumers’ propensity to read investment and financial media that add to their store of
financial knowledge (Martin et al., 2003). It also increases ability to evaluate information for
inaccuracy or bias due to framing, weak arguments, or extreme statements.
Higher levels of involvement or need for cognition motivate one to make more resources
available to process, which should shift the non-monotonic function between available
resources and resource demand the right such that it will peak at a point of greater resource
demand. Research supports this view as those with higher product-category involvement
exhibited greater product category, brand, and message element recall than those with
lower involvement when exposed to a resource-demanding, complex TV commercial
script (Lowrey, 2006).

Ability to devote available resources


Ability to devote resources to processing depends on one’s innate cognitive capacity.
Although it is relatively fixed, consumers can increase its efficiency in processing domain-
specific information by cultivating the components of a domain-specific literacy through
education, experience, and consumer socialization processes. For example, as mastery of
vocabulary increases, more cognitive capacity (termed “working memory” in the reading
literature) can be devoted to text comprehension as less is needed for processing individual
word meanings ( Jae and Viswanathan, 2012). Similarly, improved financial literacy makes it
easier to apply one’s innate cognitive resources (e.g. memory, attention, processing speed,
and mathematical aptitude) to comprehending information about a financial service.

Skills and heuristics


One component of domain-specific literacy involves skills and heuristics. With financial
literacy, consumers can learn skills to more efficiently utilize their financial capacity as they
would learn communication or mathematics skills to improve language or numerical
capacities. One way to learn skills is via direct experience. With financial skills, consumers
become more capable users of financial information with experience. Similarly, practicing
IJBM mathematics or language aids numeric or verbal skills. Consumers may also learn skills
35,5 indirectly by watching and modeling others, such as family or peers (Huhmann, 2014).
Heuristics simplify complex tasks; thereby, helping consumers deal with limitations to
their cognitive capacity to store, retrieve and process information. Under conditions of
uncertainty, Tversky and Kahneman (1974) suggest that consumers specifically supplement
cognitive capacity with three heuristics: representativeness, availability, and adjustment
754 and anchoring.
The representativeness heuristic uses information stored in memory to resolve an
unfamiliar problem by applying a similar, but familiar problem’s solution. However,
choosing the most similar, familiar problem as representative of the unfamiliar problem may
introduce bias into decision-making. Lab experiments found that the representative
heuristic leads people to underutilize other relevant information that is not shared by
Downloaded by Virginia Commonwealth University At 08:34 07 August 2017 (PT)

the unfamiliar and familiar problems and to become overconfident about the strength of the
familiar and unfamiliar problem’s similarity.
Next, the availability heuristic relies on the most accessible information at the time of
decision-making. It gives more weight to information as saliency and ease of visualizing
increase due to factors such as seriousness of the consequences or recency.
Third, the adjustment and anchoring heuristic relies on trial and error. Experiments
show that the starting point for dealing with an unfamiliar situation serves as an anchor
that biases subsequent processing of new information. One manifestation of this bias is that
an incorrect first impression continues to influence decisions despite disconfirming
evidence. Other manifestations result from excessive reliance on trial and error that leads
consumers to overlook alternative information sources or over-focus on new trials rather
than carefully analyzing the results of previous trials (Tversky and Kahneman, 1974).
Researchers have tended to investigate error resulting from these systematic biases
under specific circumstances. However, the learning of heuristics generally helps routine
task efficiency by freeing up cognitive capacity for more important or unique challenges.

Objective prior knowledge


Another component of a domain-specific literacy is objective prior knowledge. Objective
prior knowledge typically includes knowledge of the terminology, lingo, jargon,
abbreviations, and conventions used in a domain, which aids ease of processing
domain-specific information. It also includes knowledge about concepts, procedures,
and facts. Financial knowledge is a consumer’s prior objective knowledge of financial
concepts, services, products, and procedures accumulated in memory as a result of direct
and indirect experience. In this special issue, Limbu found that objective prior knowledge
related to credit cards heightens perceived credit card spending self-control while lessening
propensity to engage in risky credit card behaviors and credit card mismanagement.
Domain-specific knowledge may be viewed as a specific instance of the broader
consumer objective knowledge construct (Alba and Hutchinson, 1987). Thus, greater
familiarity and more experience strengthen objective prior knowledge in a domain
and correct misconceptions or erroneous beliefs. However, correcting misconceptions and
erroneous beliefs is more difficult than initially encoding information because: new
information that does not fit with prior knowledge may be distorted to make it congruent
with existing knowledge; and prior knowledge is used to filter, comprehend, organize, and
encode new information that passes through constraints imposed by cognitive capacity.
Cognitive capacity and objective prior knowledge should interact, such that, when
encountering new information, additions or changes to knowledge depend upon both capacity
and motivation to process and learn new information. For example, despite large capacity,
consumers would make poor financial decisions if exposed primarily to inaccurate information.
In contrast, learning skills and techniques to improve available resource usage should increase
the likelihood of adding financial knowledge. Finally, greater knowledge should enhance Literacy
resources available to process new information through a feedback loop in which prior matters in
knowledge assists comprehension of new information. marketing
Decision-making proficiency
Faced with a decision or task, one should access prior knowledge and new information
readily available or located through an external search, then apply one’s innate cognitive 755
capacity supplemented with learnt skills and heuristics. Proficiency is the quality of one’s
decision-making and task management. Financial proficiency is one’s competence in
effectively managing finances and making investment, saving, budget, risk management
and spending control decisions (Huhmann, 2014; Nga et al., 2010).
Unfortunately, available resources can also be depleted by consumer choices or
Downloaded by Virginia Commonwealth University At 08:34 07 August 2017 (PT)

environmental factors. For example, lifestyle-related stress can deplete available resources
and lead to poor financial decisions. This is a promising new avenue for consumer financial
literacy research. Montoya and Scott (2013) found that regulatory resource depletion, such as
that due to family or social demands, impedes financial decision-making. It would be useful for
research to discover ways to overcome this regulatory resource depletion. Similarly, emerging
work on scarcity’s impact on proficiency shows that even those with sufficient literacy can
have their mental bandwidth so overtaxed that they are incapable of devoting their available
resources to the task at hand. For example, Mullainathan and Shafir (2013) show that poverty,
loneliness, time pressure and other stressors degrade one’s decision quality. When one is
worried about not having enough of something (e.g. money or time), it dominates one’s
thoughts to such a degree that it distracts from thinking about other issues and harms
decision making and long-range planning. Unfortunately, many programs to help those with
problems, such as programs for the poor, often impose cognitive taxes. These cognitive taxes
take many forms – completing lengthy forms, scheduling and preparing for interviews, and
deciphering rules or incentive programs – but all consume limited cognitive resources
(Mullainathan and Shafir, 2013). These stressors likely consume so many available resources
that few remain available for processing and decision-making. Future research is needed to
examine scarcity and stress as bandwidth-reducing factors that limit the outcome proficiency
or quality for those who otherwise would possess sufficient literacy.
Similar to insufficient domain-specific literacy and bandwidth-reducing stresses, research
has also identified age as impacting one’s ability to devote available resources to processing.
Resources available for processing take time to mature, and then tend to decline with advanced
age. Thus, age should affect literacy and its outcomes. For example, the young and the aged
should possess somewhat lower levels of available resources to devote to processing, which
should negatively impact processing outcomes for resource-demanding information that those
in their prime resource availability years should be better equipped to handle. Thus, age should
shift the peak of the non-monotonic function to a point at which optimal processing and literacy
outcomes are evident with less resource-demanding materials due to lower available resources.
Evidence for such a shift in optimal processing is evident in other contexts. Wohlwill (1975)
found that more resource-demanding complex shapes engaged and were positively evaluated
by sixth graders, but not first graders whose outcomes peaked with much simpler shapes.
Huhmann (2003) found that recall of banner ad images peaked with more visually complex
images for prime resource-availability age groups (16 to 50), but shifted left toward the origin of
the x and y axes (i.e. recall peaked with less complex images) for younger (under 15) and older
(over 51) groups. Similarly, prime resource-availability age UK, U.S., and German consumers are
less likely to have a negative net worth than younger consumers with fewer available resources
(Brown and Taylor, 2008). However, the effects of age and familiarity on financial literacy or its
outcomes can be difficult to untangle from other factors, such as income, without experimental
control. More experimental research is needed in this area.
IJBM Resource demand and processing fluency
35,5 To help compensate for a lower willingness or ability to devote available resource to
processing, the resource-matching perspective recommends decreasing resource demand
to match available resources. Resource demand is the cognitive load required to successfully
process information. The growing literature on the theory of processing fluency can guide
those interested in increasing literacy by lowering the resource demand of materials
756 that consumers experience difficulty processing. Processing fluency is an evaluation of the
ease with which a stimulus’ information may be processed. Processing fluency may be
affected by message elements, such as type font and size, contrast between lettering and
background, simplicity of word choice and sentence structure.
Financial industry marketing communications (e.g. mutual fund advertisements, credit
card solicitations, securities prospectuses) provide a great deal of information largely required
Downloaded by Virginia Commonwealth University At 08:34 07 August 2017 (PT)

by regulations, but consumers with less financial literacy find this information difficult to
comprehend. Increased processing fluency should reduce resource demand and overcome
some negative financial proficiency outcomes associated with lower financial literacy.
Public policy makers have begun to take to heart the need to lower financial materials’
resource demand to compensate for consumers’ continuing low financial literacy. Huhmann
and McQuitty (2009, p. 287) urged regulators to “provide a succinct and simplified version of
critical information, perhaps in the form of a simple standardized table to aid comparisons
across product choices.” The Dodd-Frank Wall Street Reform and Consumer Protection
Act, signed into law by US President Barack Obama on July 21, 2010, encouraged simpler
language and presentations of some financial product and service information. Starting in
2000, the Fair Credit and Charge Card Disclosure Act of 1988 stipulated that rates, fees,
terms, and conditions of a credit card agreement would appear in an easy-to-read table, often
known as “Schumer’s Box,” with long-term rate information in an 18-point or greater font
and remaining terms in at least 12-point font. This simplified presentation reduced resource
demand associated with combing through small print legalese and by aiding comparison
shopping with a standardized table across all credit cards. Since 1998, the Security and
Exchange Commission’s Plain English Rule 421(d) has required that investment product
prospectuses use “plain English.” The Plain English Handbook accompanying this rule
provides guidelines for diction and syntax (e.g. short, simple sentences, active words, and
simple synonyms), straightforward layouts, clear hierarchy of headings, bullet points for
tabulated information, and appropriate table and graph use. Similarly, amendments to the
Securities Exchange Act of 1934 in 2006 and the Investment Advisers Act in 2010 require
financial disclosures and narrative brochures targeted to future and prospective consumers
to also use “plain English.”
Research is also beginning to show the value of lowering the resource demand or
cognitive load of financial product or service information. Miller (2010) finds that more
complex, longer, and less readable 10-K filings reduce small investors’ trading activity
for that security. Tan et al. (2015) find that investors’ assessment is more accurate with
easier-to-read information, especially when a corporation reports actual earnings
performance that is inconsistent with prior management guidance and year-ago
quarter performance. The performance inconsistency likely increased resource demand,
because incongruent information requires more cognitive resources to process than does
congruent information (e.g. Huhmann, 2007; Mothersbaugh et al., 2002). One way to reduce
incongruity is by dual coding a message in both verbal and visual modalities. Research in
language literature suggests that a consistent verbal and visual message improves
processing of marketing communication and warning label information ( Jae et al., 2008;
Jae and Viswanathan, 2012).
Increasing messages’ processing fluency also helps those with less domain-specific
literacy. Rennekamp’s (2012) experiment finds that enhancing processing fluency with
easier-to-read disclosures increases investors’ beliefs that they can rely on the information Literacy
provided and that greater perceptions of processing fluency increase perceived matters in
management credibility. This special issue lends further support via Lee’s examination of marketing
the type of information that retirement-related financial service providers use to alter
consumers’ financial proficiency outcomes.
Education and prior experience also lower resource demand by increasing familiarity
(see Huhmann, 2007 for a review). Thus, commonly touted literacy education programs and 757
prior experience should shift the non-monotonic (inverted-U) function of peak information
processing toward a higher level of resource demand even though available resources
remain constant.
In this special issue, Xiao and Porto examine the links between financial education and
financial satisfaction, a subjective measure of financial well-being using data from the 2012
Downloaded by Virginia Commonwealth University At 08:34 07 August 2017 (PT)

National Financial Capability Study. They find that financial education increases satisfaction
with financial proficiency outcomes, or financial well-being. Although their analysis of a
secondary data set does not permit the examination of the underlying cognitive processes,
their results are entirely consistent with the view that education makes domain-specific
information more familiar; thus, lowering resource demand and optimizing literacy outcomes.
Future research should investigate how policy makers can lower resource demand of
programs designed to help those unable to devote available resources to processing
financial information or optimizing financial management decisions. Mullainathan and
Shafir (2013) summarize research that demonstrates the resource demand reduction
associated with relatively simple interventions, such as reminders, form-completion and
planning assistance, and default options (e.g. automatic enrollment in helpful programs
unless one opts out). Reducing resource demand of financial education or financial
management programs should better serve those for whom these programs are designed by
not adding cognitive taxes that further consume available resources and make successfully
manage finances that much more difficult.

Challenging the current financial literacy research paradigm


The resource-matching perspective on consumer financial literacy challenges the two
prevailing explanations of consumers’ growing financial problems: consumers should be
accountable for managing their fiscal responsibilities and responsible for their mistakes or
suboptimal decisions; and financial service marketers fail to provide sufficient information
to consumers about the consequences of inappropriate financial management and, therefore,
are responsible for many consumers’ financial difficulties (Huhmann and McQuitty, 2009).
In reality, consumers spend very little time processing financial information. Across Europe,
for example, the average amount of time spent looking at a bank or credit card statement was
four to six minutes and less than six-and-a-half minutes for tax correspondence (Greenyer, 2008).
The resource-matching perspective suggests that consumers’ limited time spent with such
financial information sources may be due to such information’s overwhelming resource demand
rather than consumers’ irresponsible attitude toward finances. Consumer will demonstrate
strong loyalty behaviors toward firms that are sensitive to their domain-specific literacy
limitations. Viswanathan, Rosa and Harris (2005) find that functionally illiterate consumers in
terms of the English language domain display great loyalty toward firms that try to
accommodate their literacy deficiencies and respond positively to loyalty programs with these
firms even when these programs do not involve price discounts.
Hope exists for consumers with lower literacy in a particular domain if accommodating
lower domain-specific literacy improves outcomes. In the language literacy domain, for
example, reading outcomes of low-literate consumers mirrored those of high-literate
consumers when advertisements were rewritten to reflect lower reading levels ( Jae et al., 2011).
This finding is fully consistent with the resource-matching perspective.
IJBM Conclusion
35,5 This special issue presents the latest research on consumer financial literacy. Consumer
financial literacy consists of three components:
• financial capacity: one’s limited, innate available resources enhanced by learnt
processing skills or heuristics related to the financial domain;
• financial knowledge: prior objective knowledge of financial concepts, services and
758 products, which may be general or specific to some financial product or service area
(e.g. loans, credit cards, banking, health expenditures, investments, insurance,
retirement planning, real estate); and
• financial proficiency: expertise in optimizing financial decisions and managing
financial resources, such as saving, budgeting, expenditure control, and debt
Downloaded by Virginia Commonwealth University At 08:34 07 August 2017 (PT)

management (Huhmann, 2014).


Although prior research in this area has often ignored consumer psychology and marketing
theories, some researchers are beginning to investigate financial decision making from a
consumer behavior perspective. In this special issue, Sivaramakrishnan, Srivastava and
Rastogi apply the Theory of Planned Behavior to predicting investment intentions and
behavior based on societal norms, attitudes, risk avoidance and financial literacy. As I have
demonstrated, the resource-matching perspective forms a robust theoretical perspective for
examining a domain-specific literacy, such as consumer financial literacy, which affects
consumers’ ability to comprehend financial information, optimize financial decisions and
effectively manage financial resources.
Consumer financial literacy has grown in importance as consumers have become more
responsible for financial decisions related to retirement planning, health insurance, and
educational spending. Unfortunately, the complexity of options and, hence resource
demand, has also grown along with increasing consumer responsibility. Efforts to improve
consumer financial literacy and other context-specific literacies should disproportionately
benefit vulnerable populations who lack cognitive capacity, have their capacity taken up by
cognitively taxing tasks and stressors, or lack skills. Due to the recognized societal
problems resulting from poor financial decisions, research such as that in this special issue
is of increasingly importance for consumer and societal well-being.

References
Alba, J.W. and Hutchinson, J.W. (1987), “Dimensions of consumer expertise”, Journal of Consumer
Research, Vol. 13 No. 4, pp. 411-454.
Anand, P. and Sternthal, B. (1990), “Ease of message processing as a moderator of repetition effects in
advertising”, Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 27 No. 3, pp. 345-353.
Arya, A., Glover, J. and Rajan, U. (2000), “Implementation in principal-agent models of adverse
selection”, Journal of Economic Theory, Vol. 93 No. 1, pp. 87-109.
Bhattacharyya, N., Huhmann, B.A. and Samu, S. (2014), “Risk perceptions of mutual funds: evidence
from an experimental approach”, Journal of Finance Issues, Vol. 13 No. 2, pp. 40-53.
Brown, S. and Taylor, K. (2008), “Household debt and financial assets: evidence from Germany, Great
Britain and the USA”, Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, , Vol. 171 No. 3, pp. 615-643.
Cacioppo, J.T. and Petty, R.E. (1982), “The need for cognition”, Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, Vol. 42 No. 1, pp. 116-131.
Celsi, R.L. and Olson, J. (1988), “The role of involvement in attention and comprehension processes”,
Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 15 No. 2, pp. 210-224.
Fama, E.F. (1980), “Agency problems and the theory of the firm”, Journal of Political Economy, Vol. 88
No. 2, pp. 288-307.
Franke, G.R., Huhmann, B.A. and Mothersbaugh, D.L. (2004), “Information content and consumer Literacy
readership of print ads: a comparison of search and experience products”, Journal of the matters in
Academy of Marketing Science, Vol. 32 No. 1, pp. 20-31.
marketing
Greenyer, A. (2008), “Are we paying attention?”, International Journal of Bank Marketing, Vol. 26 No. 3,
pp. 200-207.
Huang, C.F. and Litzenberger, R.H. (1988), Foundations for Financial Economics, North-Holland,
New York, NY. 759
Huhmann, B.A. (2014), “Social and psychological influences on financial literacy”, in Harrison, T. and
Estelami, H. (Eds), The Routledge Companion to Financial Services Marketing, Routledge,
Abingdon, pp. 45-61.
Huhmann, B.A. (2007), “A model of the cognitive and emotional processing of rhetorical works in
advertising”, in McQuarrie, E.F. and Phillips, B.J. (Eds), Go Figure: New Directions in Advertising
Downloaded by Virginia Commonwealth University At 08:34 07 August 2017 (PT)

Rhetoric, M.E. Sharpe, Armonk, NY, pp. 133-175.


Huhmann, B.A. (2003), “Visual complexity in banner ads: the role of color, photography, and
animation”, Visual Communication Quarterly, Vol. 10 No. 3, pp. 10-17.
Huhmann, B.A. and Bhattacharyya, N. (2005), “Does mutual fund advertising provide necessary
investment information?”, International Journal of Bank Marketing, Vol. 23 No. 4, pp. 296-313.
Huhmann, B.A. and McQuitty, S. (2009), “A model of consumer financial numeracy”, International
Journal of Bank Marketing, Vol. 27 No. 4, pp. 270-293.
Ingersoll, J.E. (1987), Theory of Financial Decision Making, Rowman and Littlefield, Totowa, NJ.
Jae, H., DelVecchio, D.S. and Childers, T.L. (2011), “Are low-literate and high-literate consumers
different? Applying resource-matching theory to ad processing across literacy levels ”, Journal
of Consumer Psychology, Vol. 21 No. 3, pp. 312-323.
Jae, H., DelVecchio, D.S. and Cowles, D. (2008), “Picture-text incongruency in print advertisements
among low- and high-literacy consumers”, Journal of Consumer Affairs, Vol. 42 No. 3,
pp. 439-451.
Jae, H. and Viswanathan, M. (2012), “Effects of pictorial product-warnings on low-literate consumers”,
Journal of Business Research, Vol. 65 No. 12, pp. 1674-1682.
Jensen, M. and Meckling, W. (1976), “Theory of the firm: managerial behavior, agency costs, and capital
structure”, Journal of Financial Economics, Vol. 3 No. 4, pp. 305-360.
Lowrey, T.M. (2006), “The relationship between script complexity and commercial memorability”,
Journal of Advertising, Vol. 35 No. 3, pp. 7-15.
Martin, B.A.S., Lang, B. and Wong, S. (2003), “Conclusion explicitness in advertising: the moderating
role of need for cognition (NFC) and argument quality (AQ) on persuasion”, Journal of
Advertising, Vol. 32 No. 4, pp. 57-65.
Messaris, P. (1997), Visual Persuasion: The Role of Images in Advertising, Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA.
Meyers-Levy, J. and Malaviya, P. (1999), “Consumers’ processing of persuasive advertisements: an
integrative framework of persuasion theories”, Journal of Marketing, Vol. 63 No. 4, pp. S45-S60.
Miller, B. (2010), “The effects of reporting complexity on small and large investor trading”, Accounting
Review, Vol. 85 No. 6, pp. 2107-2143.
Montoya, D.Y. and Scott, M.L. (2013), “The effect of lifestyle-based depletion on teen consumer
behavior”, Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, Vol. 32 No. 1, pp. 82-96.
Mothersbaugh, D.L., Huhmann, B.A. and Franke, G.R. (2002), “Combinatory and separative effects of
rhetorical figures on consumers’ effort and focus in ad processing”, Journal of Consumer
Research, Vol. 28 No. 4, pp. 589-602.
Mullainathan, S. and Shafir, E. (2013), Scarcity: Why Having Too Little Means So Much, Times Books,
New York, NY.
Nga, J.K.H., Yong, L.H.L. and Sellappan, R.D. (2010), “A study of financial awareness among youths”,
Young Consumers, Vol. 11 No. 4, pp. 277-290.
IJBM O’Donohoe, S. and Tynan, C. (1998), “Beyond sophistication: dimensions of advertising literacy”,
35,5 International Journal of Advertising, Vol. 17 No. 4, pp. 467-482.
Pleasant, A. (2014), “Advancing health literacy measurement: pathway to better health and health
system performance”, Journal of Health Communication, Vol. 19 No. 12, pp. 1481-1496.
Rennekamp, K. (2012), “Processing fluency and investors’ reactions to disclosure readability”, Journal
of Accounting Research, Vol. 50 No. 5, pp. 1319-1354.
760 Tan, H.-T., Wang, E.Y. and Zhou, B. (2015), “How does readability influence investors’ judgments?
Consistency of benchmark performance matters”, The Accounting Review, Vol. 90 No. 1,
pp. 371-393.
Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D. (1974), “Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases”, Science,
Vol. 185 No. 4157, pp. 1124-1131.
Viswanathan, M., Rosa, J.A. and Harris, J.E. (2005), “Decision making and coping of functionally
Downloaded by Virginia Commonwealth University At 08:34 07 August 2017 (PT)

illiterate consumers and some implications for marketing management”, Journal of Marketing,
Vol. 69 No. 1, pp. 15-31.
Williamson, O.E. (1981), “The economics of organization: the transaction cost approach”, American
Journal of Sociology, Vol. 87 No. 3, pp. 548-577.
Wohlwill, J.F. (1975), “Children’s voluntary exploration and preference for tactually presented
nonsense shapes differing in complexity”, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, Vol. 20
No. 1, pp. 159-167.
Wright, P., Friestad, M. and Boush, D.M. (2005), “The development of marketplace persuasion
knowledge in children, adolescent, and young adults”, Journal of Public Policy & Marketing,
Vol. 24 No. 2, pp. 222-233.

Further reading
Messaris, P. (1996), “‘Visual literacy’ in a cross-cultural perspective”, in Kubey, R. (Ed.), Media Literacy
in the Information Age: Current Perspectives, Transaction, New Brunswick, NJ, pp. 135-162.

Corresponding author
Bruce A. Huhmann can be contacted at: bhuhmann@nmsu.edu

For instructions on how to order reprints of this article, please visit our website:
www.emeraldgrouppublishing.com/licensing/reprints.htm
Or contact us for further details: permissions@emeraldinsight.com

View publication stats

You might also like