Rice 1996

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

TOC Electronic Journal: To print this article select pages 50-57.

Late Adolescent and Parent Perceptions


of Attachment: An Exploratory Study of
Personal and Social Well-Being
KENNETH G. RICE AND PAIGE N. CUMMINS

Research in the area of late adolescent attachment and adjustment has focused almost exclusively on adolescent reports of the
quality of their relationships with parents. The purpose of this study was to extend the attachment literature through examining
the correspondence between late adolescent children and their parents when assessing aspects of parental bonds with children.
After completing a set of questionnaires, a sample of older undergraduate students (mean age = 21 years) identified parent
figures who subsequently were sent a similar packet of questionnaires. Parents were asked to complete measures of parental bonds
as they believed their child would complete the same measure. Regression analyses revealed that parent reports of attachment
added nonsignificant portions of unique variance to the prediction of student self-esteem and social self-efficacy. Student perceptions
of attachment accounted for significant unique variation in self-esteem but not in social self-efficacy.

here has been increasing conceptual and empir- conceptual challenge in the study of attachment in late

T ical interest during the past several years in the


relevance of attachment theory for understand-
ing the development and well-being of late ado-
lescents and young adults (see Kenny & Rice,
1995, for a recent review). Several themes have emerged
in this literature. One theme has been concerned with the
development and validation of assessments that are suitable
adolescents.
Ainsworth (1969, 1989; Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, &
Wall, 1978; Ainsworth & Bowlby, 1991), Bowlby (1982,
1988), and others (e.g., Sroufe & Waters, 1977) defined
an attachment as a significant and enduring emotional bond
between two people. A secure attachment bond is impor-
tant because it facilitates a child’s affect regulation and
for measuring attachment beyond infancy and childhood other coping skills with the ultimate aim of less dependence
(Lopez & Gover, 1993; Lyddon, Bradford, & Nelson, on an actual attachment figure and more reliance on the
1993). Another theme has been the systematic study of internalized aspects of the attachment bond. Historically,
the association between attachment and numerous dimen- the emphasis in describing and studying this bond has been
sions of emotional and social well-being and development on the observable interactions within the mother–child
(Kenny, 1994; Kenny & Donaldson, 1991; Kenny & Hart, dyad. In this context, attachment behavior has been
1992; Lapsley, Rice, & FitzGerald, 1990; Mallinckrodt, described as the means by which the child maintains prox-
1992; Rice, FitzGerald, Whaley, & Gibbs, 1995; Rice & imity to mother. Such behavior is observed primarily when
Whaley, 1994; Schultheiss & Blustein, 1994). A third the child is concerned with his or her safety or when she
theme concerns the study of internal working models and or he wants to feel more secure (Sroufe & Waters, 1977).
their role in perceptions of social support (Bartholomew Ainsworth et al. (1978; see also Rheingold, 1969) devel-
& Horowitz, 1991; Blain, Thompson, & Whiffen, 1993; oped a method for classifying various behaviors as indicative
Sarason, Pierce, Shearin, Sarason, & Waltz, 1991). A final of secure or insecure (anxious, avoidant) attachment in
theme includes the application of attachment theory, more children, a method that is based on reactions of children
specifically internal working models of self and others, to in different “strange situations.” Furthermore, the quality
the practice of counseling (Pistole, 1989; Pistole & Watkins, of attachment is linked intrinsically to the quality of the
1995). The purpose of the present study was to draw on responsiveness of the child’s mother or other important
these themes in an effort to meet a methodological and attachment figures. Securely attached children tend to have

Portions of this article were completed while Kenneth G. Rice was an associate professor in counseling and development at Purdue University. Currently, he
is an associate professor in the Counseling Psychology Program at Michigan State University. Paige N. Cummins is a doctoral candidate in counselor education
at Purdue University. This research was supported in part by the Purdue Research Foundation and by an Undergraduate Research Training Grant. The
assistance of Teddi Whaley, Rachel Sterlett, and Janet Oberholtzer is gratefully acknowledged. Correspondence regarding this article should be sent to Kenneth
G. Rice, Department of Counseling, Educational Psychology and Special Education, 440 Erickson Hall, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI 48824.

50 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SEPTEMBER/OCTOBER 1996 • VOLUME 75


Late Adolescent and Parent Perceptions of Attachment

caretakers who are reliable and responsive to their needs, describe themselves as more caring and less overprotective
whereas insecurely attached children experience caretakers than their adult children described them. He also found
as unreliable sources of support or encouragement (Ains- moderate correlations (ranging from .44 to .55) between
worth et al., 1978; Rice, 1990; Sroufe, 1979). Hence, an mother and child reports of attachment (fathers were not
understanding of the meaning of attachment behavior is included in his study). These findings suggest that there
facilitated by the inclusion of an attachment figure during may be some differences in perceptions of attachment
assessment of childhood attachment. depending on the person (parent or child) responding to
Although early work in the area of attachment postulated the query. Perhaps more intriguing and unexplored, how-
the persistence or enduring nature of attachment bonds as ever, are the possible effects that such differences in percep-
well as the possibility of attachments in addition to the tion may have on the child’s adjustment (e.g., self-esteem
mother–child dyad, only recently have studies been con- and social self-efficacy).
ducted on attachment beyond the early childhood years The developing research literature in the area of attach-
and beyond attachment to mother. An initial challenge in ment beyond childhood has demonstrated that secure
this line of inquiry has been the development of assessment attachment, as perceived by late adolescents and young
strategies and research designs that would be more suitable adults, is associated with personal and interpersonal well-
for older participants (e.g., adolescents, young adults) than being, whereas insecure attachment is associated with per-
the Strange Situation (Ainsworth et al., 1978). As Bowlby sonal and interpersonal problems such as depression (Rich-
(1988) pointed out, attachment behavior is most evident in man & Flaherty, 1987), eating disorders (Kenny & Hart,
childhood and less apparent at other times in development, 1992), lack of assertiveness (Kenny, 1987), lower self-effi-
although the attachment bond is presumed to persist cacy (Mallinckrodt, 1992), and college maladjustment
throughout the life cycle. (Rice & Whaley, 1994). Nevertheless, the effect sizes in
Many of the more commonly used assessment strategies these studies tend to be moderate, at best, leaving consider-
have been reviewed recently by Lopez and Gover (1993) able variance in adjustment unaccounted for. There also
and Lyddon et al. (1993). These measures tap respondent has been a ceiling effect in studies of attachment, with
perceptions of the current or past quality of their attach- most participants reporting secure attachment with their
ment relationships with mother, father, both parents, and/ parents (Armsden & Greenberg, 1987; Mallinckrodt, 1992;
or friends. Indeed, the late adolescent’s perception of the Rice et al., 1995). Closer inspection of the parent–child
parent–child relationship is foundational in the develop- attachment bond in late adolescence, and specifically the
ment of working models of self and others (Blain et al., degree of correspondence between parents and their chil-
1993). Sarason et al. (1991) suggested that the late adoles- dren, may yield a clearer understanding of the influence of
cent uses working models of self and others as filters attachment on adjustment.
through which self-perceptions are developed and level of The general research question addressed in this study was
social support from other relationships is judged. Students whether child perceptions of attachment mattered more,
who perceive a high level of support from their parents in terms of individual and interpersonal functioning, than
and are satisfied with the level of support they currently parents’ understanding of the attachment relationship. We
receive are better able to cope with life transitions (Bartho- hypothesized that late adolescent reports of their attach-
lomew & Horowitz, 1991). Late adolescents with high ment with parents would correlate positively and signifi-
perceived support may cope more effectively with change cantly with parent reports of attachment. However, consis-
because they are able to access social support should they tent with a cognitive mediational hypothesis, it was
need it. Finally, internal representations of the family expected that child reports and parent reports would
attachment relationship serve to mediate the late adoles- account for significant variability in self-esteem and social
cent’s development of relationships with peers by providing self-efficacy after controlling for the shared variance
a template through which self and others are perceived as between child and parent perceptions. In other words, late
lovable/unlovable or trustworthy/untrustworthy (Bartho- adolescent child perceptions of attachment were expected
lomew & Horowitz, 1991; Sarason et al., 1991). Young to contribute unique and significant variance in the expla-
adults’ perceptions of the parent–child relationship are, nation of self and social dimensions of adjustment after
therefore, extremely important to the development of accounting for the influence of parent reports.
accurate self-perceptions and supportive peer relationships.
Despite the relevance of self-perceptions of attachment,
it is surprising that very little research and measurement METHOD
development have been directed toward examining the
parent side of the parent–child attachment bond. To our Participants
knowledge, there has been only one study in which parent A total of 140 undergraduate students (32 men and 108
reports on a measure of attachment were compared with women) from a large, public university in the Midwest
the reports of their children on the same measure. Using participated in the study. The students were recruited from
the Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI; Parker, Tupling, & a multisection course on personal growth and development.
Brown, 1979), Parker (1981) found that mothers tended to Although more women than men participated in this study,

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SEPTEMBER/OCTOBER 1996 • VOLUME 75 51


Kenneth G. Rice and Paige N. Cummins

the sample distribution approximated the gender distribu- Items on the PBI measure two dimensions of parent-
tion of students in the course (70% of all students in the child attachment: care and protection. The Care subscale
course were female, 30% were male). Ninety-five percent consists of 12 items designed to assess “care/involvement
of the sample were White. Eighty-one percent of the sam- versus indifference/rejection” (Parker et al., 1979, pp. 2–3)
ple had completed four or more semesters at the university, and consists of items such as, “spoke to me with a warm
and the average age of the participants was 21.0 years (SD and friendly voice” and “did not help me as much as I
= 2.2). The largest number of majors represented in the needed” (reverse-scored). The Protection subscale, pre-
sample was within the liberal arts area (55%), with the viously referred to as Overprotection (Parker, 1989) con-
second largest concentration being students pursuing sists of 13 items assessing “control/overprotection versus
majors in consumer and family sciences (14%). The grade encouragement of independence” (Parker et al., 1979,
point average of the sample was 2.1 (SD = 1.0) on a 4- p. 3). Sample items from the Protection subscale include,
point scale. Students received credit for participating in this “let me make my own decisions” (reverse-scored) and
research but also could choose other nonresearch options in “tried to control everything I did.”
order to achieve the same credit. Evidence for reliability and validity of the PBI is summa-
Of these students, 79% reported that their parents were rized by Parker (1989). In the initial study, Parker et al.
married (never divorced) and living together, whereas 7% (1979) reported test–retest reliability over a 3-week period
reported that one or both of their parents had remarried. to be r = .76 (Care) and r = .63 (Protection). Subsequent
Eight percent of the sample reported that their parents had studies have found test-retest coefficients to exceed .87
divorced or separated, and another 5% indicated that one for all subscales of the PBI (see Parker, 1989). Split-half
of their parents was deceased. reliability was r = .88 (Care) and r = .74 (Protection) in
A total of 117 of the students (84%) volunteered the the original report (Parker et al., 1979). Parker (1989)
names and addresses of their parents. Students were asked reported that concurrent and predictive validity have been
specifically to identify “your mother/father or the person demonstrated in several studies in which PBI scores corre-
who acts most like a mother/father to you.” Parents were lated in expected directions with measures tapping depres-
sent a letter describing the study, an informed consent sion, neuroticism, life satisfaction, social desirability, and
form, and a questionnaire packet. A total of 77 mothers other relationship dimensions (e.g., marital adjustment of
and 66 fathers returned survey packets. When completed parents and social support).
questionnaires from parents were matched with student Self-esteem. Self-esteem was measured with the Rosen-
questionnaires, it was determined that 50 of the female berg (1965) Self-Esteem Scale. Rosenberg’s measure con-
students had matching data from two parents, whereas 13 sists of 10 statements responded to on a scale from 1 =
of the male students had matching data from both parents. strongly agree through 4 = strongly disagree. The scale mea-
Another 9 women had data from mothers but not fathers, sures general self-esteem (e.g., “I feel I have a number of
whereas 2 women had data from fathers and not mothers. good qualities” and “I feel I do not have much to be proud
Three men had data from mothers but not fathers, and of”), with half of the items worded positively. Internal
there were no men in the sample with data from fathers consistency reliability ranges from .86 to .93 (Goldsmith,
only. 1986). Test–retest reliability for the measure has been
reported as r = .85 over a 2-week period of time (Crandall,
Variables 1973). Goldsmith (1986) and Rosenberg (1965, 1979)
reported adequate evidence for the construct validity of the
Attachment. The Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI; Parker Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale in that the measure correlated
et al., 1979) is a 50-item self-report questionnaire with 25 with other scales in expected directions (e.g., negative cor-
items assessing relations with mother and the other 25 relations with dogmatism, depression, and anxiety). Gold-
items assessing relations with father. The items are identical smith (1986) questioned the unidimensional nature of the
for mothers and fathers. Following standard PBI directions, scale and determined, after confirmatory factor analyses,
students were instructed to recall their impressions of their that the Rosenberg measure “when used with some popula-
parents as formed during the first 16 years of their lives. tions [in which age did not vary substantially] . . . seems
To make parent and child reports consistent, we asked to measure a single dimension of global attitude toward
students to think about people who acted most like parents the self” (p. 263).
to them. To assess parent attunement with their child’s Social self-efficacy. The Social Self-Efficacy subscale of
responses (i.e., sensitivity to their child’s perceptions of the the Self-Efficacy Scale (SES; Sherer et al., 1982) was used
child–parent bond), we gave parents these instructions: to assess late adolescent perceptions of their efficacy in
“Please fill in the blank with the response that comes closest social situations. The SES is a 23-item (6 items for social
to how you think your child experienced you, as a parent, self-efficacy) self-report questionnaire that assesses expec-
in his/her first 16 years of life.” Both sets of participants tations in terms of willingness to initiate behaviors, willing-
then responded to items that reflected their impressions, ness to expend effort in completing behaviors, and persis-
using a 4-point scale from 1 = very like my mother/father tence despite difficulties. Sample items include, “It is diffi-
through 4 = very unlike my mother/father. cult for me to make new friends” and “If I see someone I

52 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SEPTEMBER/OCTOBER 1996 • VOLUME 75


Late Adolescent and Parent Perceptions of Attachment

would like to meet, I go to that person instead of waiting coefficient alpha and were calculated for each group of
for him or her to come to me.” Although the original SES participants. Coefficient alphas ranged from .77 (for stu-
used a 14-point Likert scale, the present study, following dent reports of social self-efficacy) to .95 (for student
Mallinckrodt (1992), used an 8-point response option for- reports of care by fathers) and appeared to be adequate
mat with 1 = strongly disagree through 8 = strongly agree. for research purposes. These estimates also were similar to
Adequate reliability and validity for the SES has been and, in the case of social self-efficacy, somewhat better
reported. Although the Social Self-Efficacy subscale con- than, prior results published by the scale authors.
sists of 6 items, internal consistency (Cronbach’s coefficient The subscale means suggested that, on average, there
alpha) was reported as .71 (Sherer et al., 1982). The Social was agreement on the PBI subscales between students and
Self-Efficacy subscale has correlated in expected directions their parents (see Table 1). The correlations between dif-
with measures of social introversion (Sherer & Adams, ferent respondents assessing the same relationship dimen-
1983), social skill and past employment success (Sherer et sion were in the moderate range, suggesting some degree
al., 1982), and locus of control and social support (Mallin- of consistency, though certainly not complete agreement,
ckrodt, 1992). between parents and children.
Parent and Late Adolescent Reports of Attachment
RESULTS We conducted a set of regression (commonality) analyses
(Pedhazur, 1982) to determine the unique and shared con-
Preliminary Analyses tributions of parent and child ratings of attachment in the
Although attachment theory indicates that the attachment prediction of self and social functioning. Statistically, these
bond is equally important for both genders, some studies analyses assess the reduction in explained variance after
of adolescents and young adults have found gender differ- removal of each attachment measure. This reduction yields
ences in ratings of attachment (e.g., Schultheiss & Blustein, an index of the unique contribution of each measure to
1994) whereas others have not (e.g., Armsden & Green- the criterion. That is, this analysis provides the proportion
berg, 1987). Therefore, one preliminary analysis consisted of the explained variance that can be attributed to one or
of a multivariate analysis of variance with gender as the another individual predictor. In more practical terms, an
factor and the four PBI subscales (late adolescent reports) extrapolation can then be made regarding shared variance
and self-esteem and social self-efficacy as the dependent (in this case, the relative importance of child–parent agree-
variables. Listwise deletion of missing data was used in this ment on attachment) versus unique variance (the impor-
and in all subsequent analyses. The multivariate test of tance of idiosyncratic attachment appraisals). (For another
gender differences was not significant, F(6, 69) = 1.75, example of this strategy, see Cutrona, 1989.) In one set of
p < .123. As a result, data for both genders were combined equations, self-esteem served as the criterion, whereas in
for all subsequent analyses. another set, social self-efficacy was the criterion. Separate
Because not all of the late adolescents in this study had equations were constructed for attachment to mother and
parents (or parent figures) participating, it was important attachment to father, and the same data-analytic strategy
to determine if there was a priori bias in the parent–child was used in each regression equation. Table 2 presents a
sample. A total of 78 students had at least one parent summary of these analyses.
participating, whereas 62 students had no parent participat- In the prediction of self-esteem, significant variability
ing. Analyses were conducted to examine possible demo- was accounted for by the combined effects of attachment
graphic differences between late adolescents with parent to mother scores (R2 = .14). The relative importance of
data and adolescents without parent data. The two groups informant ratings differed when comparing the unique vari-
also were compared on each of the PBI subscales and on the ance attributed to adolescents’ versus their mothers’ reports
self-esteem and social efficacy measures. T tests revealed no of the same measures. For example, approximately 43% of
significant age or grade point average differences between the explained variance could be attributed to adolescent
the groups, and chi-square analyses revealed no significant reports of attachment (R2 change to remove divided by
differences in the frequency distributions for gender, race/ total R2), whereas 31% of explained variance was attributed
ethnicity, semester status, college major, or marital status to mother reports of attachment. Approximately 26% of
of parents. Similarly, the results indicated that there were the variance in self-esteem was shared by adolescent and
no significant differences between the two groups of late mother reports of attachment. Therefore, the unique per-
adolescents on the six subscales, F(6, 129) = 0.92, p > spective of the attachment bond as perceived by adoles-
.481. Therefore, in terms of demographic and measure- cents surpassed the relative importance (in terms of pre-
ment concerns, the subsample of late adolescents in this dicting self-esteem) of mother sensitivity to the bond as
study, for whom parent data were available, can be consid- well as the importance of the shared perception of the
ered representative of the initial larger sample. attachment relationship.
The means, standard deviations, scale ranges, reliability Attachment to father accounted for 15% of the variance
results, and correlations for the measures appear in Table 1. in self-esteem scores. Adolescent reports contributed a
Reliability estimates were determined using Cronbach’s unique and significant 87% of the total variance, whereas

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SEPTEMBER/OCTOBER 1996 • VOLUME 75 53


Kenneth G. Rice and Paige N. Cummins

TABLE 1

Means, Standard Deviations, and Cronbach’s Coefficient Alpha Estimates for Measures
Completed by Late Adolescents and Parents

Variable/Group Scale Range M SD α r a

Care–Mother
Late adolescents (n = 75)
Mothers (n = 75)
Protection–Mother
Late adolescents
Mothers
Care–Father
Late adolescents (n = 65)
Fathers (n = 65)
Protection–Father
Late adolescents
Fathers
Self-Esteem
Late adolescents (n = 78)
Social Self-Efficacy
Late adolescents
a
Correlation of adolescent and parent.

father reports only accounted for 7% of the unique variance outcome-dependent associations between attachment and
in self-esteem. Shared variance between fathers and chil- adjustment.
dren in the prediction of self-esteem scores was 6%. Thus, Consistent with prior studies in the area of late adoles-
in determining self-esteem, adolescent perceptions of the cent and young adult attachment, perceptions that mothers
child–father attachment bond considerably exceeded and fathers were caring and encouraging tended to predict
either the importance of fathers’ unique assessment of the current self-esteem. In general, this finding and the amount
bond or the importance of the perspective shared by fathers of explained variance in esteem concur with other results
and their children. in which dimensions of self-reported attachment have been
Attachment to mother scores did not account for signifi- associated with emotional adjustment; greater attachment
cant variability in social self-efficacy. Likewise, the com- security (care and encouragement of autonomy) is associ-
bined effects of attachment to father scores did not account ated with enhanced esteem. For example, in a meta-analysis
for significant variance in social self-efficacy. Therefore, of adolescent attachment-adjustment studies, Rice (1990)
the relative unique contributions of the different sets of found an average effect size (r) of .40 for studies examining
predictors were not explored. the association between attachment and self-esteem or self-
concept (i.e., about 16% of the variance in esteem could
DISCUSSION be attributed to attachment).
A primary purpose of the present study was to extend the Closer inspection of the unique influence of parent versus
late adolescent and young adult attachment literature by child reports of attachment on self-esteem revealed a some-
examining the degree to which reports of children and their what surprising gender-of-parent difference. For example,
parents tell a unique story about late adolescent self-esteem both mother and child reports of attachment contributed
and social efficacy. Prior research in this area has relied about the same amount to the total variance in self-esteem
almost exclusively on adolescent self-report and has not scores (31% and 43%, respectively), with the combined
examined the responsiveness, sensitivity, or concordance of effects of adolescent reports seeming to be slightly more
parents to parent–child relationship dynamics. This study important. When attachment to father was the predictor,
sought to explain additional variance in personal and inter- child reports were considerably more important (i.e.,
personal adjustment over and above what is typically found accounted for almost all of the variance) for self-esteem.
in studies of adolescent attachment. To this end, late ado- Father sensitivity to the attachment bond mattered little
lescents and their parents completed retrospective accounts in the prediction of self-esteem, whereas mother reports
of mother–child and father–child relations. Late adoles- and mother–child attunement to attachment seemed to be
cents completed additional measures of self-esteem and almost as important as child reports. In another study of
social self-efficacy. The results revealed some findings con- young adults using the PBI, Richman and Flaherty (1987)
sistent with other studies in this area. In addition, respon- found that paternal bonding was more strongly associated
dents and their uniquely more or less important influence with self-esteem (and other outcomes) than was maternal
on well-being revealed an interesting pattern of gender and attachment. In contrast, other studies using different mea-

54 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SEPTEMBER/OCTOBER 1996 • VOLUME 75


Late Adolescent and Parent Perceptions of Attachment

TABLE 2

Multiple Regression Results for the Prediction


of Self-Esteem and Social Efficacy

Criterion (Predictor — Respondent) Standardized Beta R2 Change to Remove


Self-Esteem (n = 75)
Care–Mother — Adolescent
Protection–Mother — Adolescent
Care–Mother — Mother
Protection–Mother — Mother

Self-Esteem (n = 65)
Care–Father — Adolescent
Protection–Father — Adolescent
Care–Father — Father
Protection–Father — Father

Social Self-Efficacy (n = 75)


Care–Mother — Adolescent
Protection–Mother — Adolescent
Care–Mother — Mother
Protection–Mother — Mother

Social Self-Efficacy (n = 65)


Care–Father — Adolescent
Protection–Father — Adolescent
Care–Father — Father
Protection–Father — Father

sures have found that maternal attachment is a stronger tions of their relationships with their parents, particularly
predictor of emotional well-being than paternal attachment their fathers, when the clients have issues related to self-
(e.g., Schultheiss & Blustein, 1994) and that both maternal esteem.
and paternal attachment exert similar influences on adjust- The finding regarding the nonsignificant association
ment (Holmbeck & Wandrei, 1993). Different measures between attachment and social self-efficacy is in contrast
of attachment, which assess different dimensions of attach- to a report by Collins and Read (1990) in which perceptions
ment bonds or behavior, may yield differential sensitivity to of mother were small but significantly associated with social
the unique effects of mothers and fathers on their children’s adjustment whereas perceptions of father were not. Like-
self-esteem. Put differently, it may become important to wise, Mallinckrodt (1992) found significant associations
use multiple measures (and informants) of attachment in between both mother and father care and social self-effi-
older samples to tease apart the effects of attachment to cacy. He suggested, following object relations theories, that
mother and to father. A caveat to this recommendation is viewing oneself as an “effective actor” (i.e., high social self-
that not all current assessments may adequately capture efficacy) in a social world “may depend on how responsive
differences between paternal and maternal attachment . . . early caregivers have been” (p. 459). Some possible
bonds with children. For example, “healthy” father–child explanations for these different results may be linked to
relationships may mean that fathers are dependable, consis- sample size, measurement, and analysis strategies (e.g.,
tent, and active with their children, elements of attachment small sample size would preclude some associations from
bonds that are not assessed by the PBI. reaching statistical significance). Certainly, the results of
As Bowlby (1988) and Main and Weston (1981) this study should be replicated with larger samples and
observed in studies of toddlers, it is quite possible to witness other measures of attachment. Moreover, the present sam-
a secure attachment with one parent and an insecure attach- ple of college students represents only one element of the
ment with another parent for the same child and with larger late adolescent and young adult population; an addi-
potentially different adjustment outcomes as a result. As tional avenue for future research on adolescent–parent
studies of attachment beyond early childhood increase, at attachment could incorporate noncollege participants.
a minimum it would seem important to attend both to Likewise, it should be noted that parents volunteered to
maternal and paternal dimensions, and perhaps reports, participate and returned surveys that were sent to them.
of the bond. Furthermore, counselors may need to gain These parents may not represent the typical parents of late
information from their clients about the clients’ percep- adolescents (although, in the present study, there were no

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SEPTEMBER/OCTOBER 1996 • VOLUME 75 55


Kenneth G. Rice and Paige N. Cummins

significant differences between adolescents whose parents control Jayne’s daily activities. She described her father as
participated and those whose parents did not). Parents were not being involved in her life. She indicated that she could
asked to report their assessment of the parent–adolescent not recall her father ever telling her that he loved her or
bond as they believed they were experienced by their child, showing her any affection.
as “through the child’s eyes.” The sample of parents who After working with Jayne individually for several ses-
participated may have been more or less capable of that sions, the counselor and Jayne discussed the possibility of
degree of perspective taking when compared with parents involving Jayne’s parents in her counseling. Despite some
in general. Because the PBI assesses retrospective impres- initial hesitancy, Jayne decided that she would like her
sions of relationships that likely are influenced by current parents involved in at least one counseling session, if not
perceptions (see Mallinckrodt, 1992, and Parker, 1989, for more, and subsequent contact with the parents revealed
further discussion of this point), it would be important the same degree of interest. During the first family session,
in future studies to assess the stability or consistency of it became quite obvious that Jayne’s mother was not sensi-
adolescent–parent attachment over time. It should be tive to the dynamics of her relationship with her daughter.
added, however, that no other report to date has involved Jayne’s mother admitted that she made decisions for her
both parent figures and their adolescent child in a study of daughter but insisted this was necessary as her daughter
attachment and adjustment. had difficulty making decisions on her own. Jayne’s father,
Implications for practice should be tempered by the limi- however, seemed more involved and supportive of his
tations of this research, as noted in preceding paragraphs. daughter than Jayne indicated initially. Using the informa-
One possible application of the present study would be to tion gained from Jayne and her family, the counselor
attend to participant perceptions of both parents in the worked with Jayne to help her verbalize how she experi-
development and implementation of campus self-esteem enced her relationship with her mother and that her moth-
workshops or other similar outreach activities. Develop- er’s behaviors made it difficult for Jayne to make her own
ment of outreach activities that address family dynamics decisions. The counselor also helped Jayne’s father under-
also may be helpful in facilitating the late adolescent’s stand that Jayne perceived him as being distant and uncar-
understanding of how past and current perceptions of their ing and that this perception affected how she felt about
relationships with their parents may affect their ability to herself. In addition, the counselor helped Jayne recognize
gain social support and to develop relationships with peers. that her father was more supportive than she thought.
Another extension of this study would be to consider the As a result of increased understanding of these relational
importance of counselor sensitivity to attachment-relevant concerns, the family began to make changes in their rela-
concerns when working with clients who are experiencing tionships that helped Jayne to develop a more positive
low self-esteem. Bowlby (1988) described how counseling image of herself.
relationships can parallel attachment relationships. Accord- In other words, a specific focus of counseling could be
ing to Bowlby (1988), counselors help clients by providing on the perceived agreement between the client and his or
a secure base for exploration of difficult situations; by her parents, particularly when self-esteem is a concern.
encouraging exploration of current relationships, including Counseling could provide an opportunity to help the late
the counseling relationship, to understand the links adolescent confront relational distortions or misperceptions
between earlier attachments and current social and per- that may be affecting self-perceptions as well as the devel-
sonal functioning; and by helping clients to achieve a more opment of supportive relationships with others (Pistole,
accurate and adequate understanding of self and others. 1989). Attachment style also may affect the ability of the
late adolescent to be emotionally expressive, intimate, and
Case Example independent. These behaviors are necessary for the devel-
The following illustration exemplifies how attachment opment of successful relationships. Thus, counseling may
issues might present themselves in counseling with an ado- help teach important relationship skills to the late adoles-
lescent client. In addition, this example highlights how this cent whose parental attachment is not, or was not, secure.
information might be used by the counselor to work with
self-esteem issues. Jayne, an 18-year-old woman, came to
counseling because she was experiencing difficulty adjust- REFERENCES
ing to the local community college she began attending just Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1969). Object relations, dependency, and attach-
after graduation from high school. She expressed difficulty ment: A theoretical review of the infant–mother relationship. Child
Development, 40, 969-1025.
making new friends and some minor discomfort with con- Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American
tinuing to live at home. Further exploration revealed that Psychologist, 44, 709-716.
she often felt depressed and that she found it difficult to Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns
think positively about herself. When asked about her rela- of attachment: A psychological study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale,
tionships with her parents, Jayne disclosed that she experi- NJ: Erlbaum.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Bowlby, J. (1991). An ethological approach to
enced both of her parents as being distant, though in differ- personality development. American Psychologist, 46, 333-341.
ent ways. She indicated that her mother did not let her Armsden, G. C., & Greenberg, M. T. (1987). The inventory of parent
make her own decisions to the point that she wanted to and peer attachment: Individual differences and their relationship to

56 JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SEPTEMBER/OCTOBER 1996 • VOLUME 75


Late Adolescent and Parent Perceptions of Attachment

psychological well-being in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adoles- Main, M., & Weston, D. (1981). Quality of attachment to mother and
cence, 16, 427-453. to father: Related to conflict behavior and the readiness for establishing
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among new relationships. Child Development, 52, 932-940.
young adults: A test of a four-category model. Journal of Personality Mallinckrodt, B. (1992). Childhood emotional bonds with parents, devel-
and Social Psychology, 61, 226-244. opment of adult social competencies, and availability of social support.
Blain, M. D., Thompson, J, M., & Whiffen, V. E. (1993). Attachment and Journal of Counseling Psychology, 39, 453-461.
perceived social support in late adolescence: The interaction between Parker, G. (1981). Parental reports of depressives: An investigation of
working models of self and others. Journal of Adolescent Research, 8, several explanations. Journal of Affective Disorders, 3, 131-140.
226-241. Parker, G. (1989). The parental bonding instrument: Psychometric prop-
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York: erties reviewed. Psychiatric Development, 4, 317-335.
Basic Books. (Original work published 1969) Parker, G., Tupling, H., & Brown, L. B. (1979). A parental bonding
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent–child attachment and healthy instrument. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 52, 1-10.
human development. New York: Basic Books. Pedhazur, E. J. (1982). Multiple regression in behavioral research. New
Collins, N. L., & Read, S. J. (1990). Adult attachment, working models, York: CBS College Publishing.
and relationship quality in dating couples. Journal of Personality and Pistole, M. C. (1989). Attachment: Implications for counselors. Journal
Social Psychology, 58, 644-663. of Counseling & Development, 68, 190-193.
Crandall, R. (1973). The measurement of self-esteem and related con- Pistole, M. C., & Watkins, C. E. (1995). Attachment theory, counseling
cepts. In J. P. Robinson & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Measures of social psycho- process, and supervision. The Counseling Psychologist, 23, 457-478.
logical attitudes (pp. 45-167). Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press. Rheingold, H. (1969). The effect of a strange environment on the behavior
Cutrona, C. E. (1989). Ratings of social support by adolescents and adult of infants. In B. M. Foss (Ed.), Determinants of infant behavior (Vol. 4).
informants: Degree of correspondence and prediction of depressive New York: Barnes & Noble.
symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 723-730. Rice, K. G. (1990). Attachment in adolescence: A narrative and meta-
Goldsmith, R. E. (1986). Dimensionality of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem analytic review. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 19, 511-538.
Scale. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 1, 253-264. Rice, K. G., FitzGerald, D. P., Whaley, T. J., & Gibbs, C. L. (1995).
Holmbeck, G. N., & Wandrei, M. L. (1993). Individual and relational Cross-sectional and longitudinal examination of attachment, separa-
predictors of adjustment in first-year college students. Journal of Coun- tion–individuation, and college student adjustment. Journal of Counsel-
seling Psychology, 40, 73-78. ing & Development, 73, 463-474.
Kenny, M. E. (1987). The extent and function of parental attachment Rice, K. G., & Whaley, T. J. (1994). A short-term longitudinal study of
among first-year college students. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, within-semester stability and change in attachment and college student
16, 17-27. adjustment. Journal of College Student Development, 35, 324-330.
Kenny, M. E. (1994). Quality and correlates of parental attachment among Richman, J. A., & Flaherty, J. A. (1987). Adult psychosocial assets and
late adolescents. Journal of Counseling & Development, 72, 399-403. depressive mood over time: Effects of internalized childhood attach-
Kenny, M. E., & Donaldson, G. (1991). Contributions of parental attach- ments. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 703-712.
ment and family structure to the social and psychological functioning Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ:
of first-year college students. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38, Princeton University Press.
479-486. Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. New York: Basic Books.
Kenny, M. E., & Hart, K. (1992). Relationship between parental attach- Sarason, B. R., Pierce, G. R., Shearin, E. N., Sarason, I. G., & Waltz, J. A.
ment and eating disorders in an inpatient and a college sample. Journal (1991). Perceived social support and working models of self and actual
of Counseling Psychology, 39, 521-526. others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 273-287.
Kenny, M. E., & Rice, K. G. (1995). Attachment to parents and adjustment Sherer, M., & Adams, C. H. (1983). Construct validation of the Self-
in late adolescent college students: Current status, applications, and Efficacy Scale. Psychological Reports, 53, 899-902.
future considerations. The Counseling Psychologist, 23, 433-456. Sherer, M., Maddux, J. E., Mercandante, B., Prentice-Dunn, S., Jacobs,
Lapsley, D. K., Rice, K. G., & FitzGerald, D. (1990). Adolescent attach- B., & Rogers, R. W. (1982). The Self-Efficacy Scale: Construction and
ment, identity, and adjustment to college: Implications for the continu- validation. Psychological Reports, 51, 663-671.
ity of adaptation hypothesis. Journal of Counseling and Development, Schultheiss, D. E. P., & Blustein, D. L. (1994). Role of adolescent–parent
68, 561-565. relationships in college student development and adjustment. Journal
Lopez, F. G., & Gover, M. R. (1993). Self-report measures of parent-ado- of Counseling Psychology, 41, 248-255.
lescent attachment and separation–individuation: A selective review. Sroufe, L. A. (1979). The coherence of individual development: Early
Journal of Counseling & Development, 71, 560-569. care, attachment, and subsequent developmental issues. American Psy-
Lyddon, W. J., Bradford, E., & Nelson, J. P. (1993). Assessing adolescent chologist, 34, 834-841.
and adult attachment: A review of current self-report measures. Journal Sroufe, L. A., & Waters, E. (1977). Attachment as an organizational
of Counseling & Development, 71, 390-395. perspective. Child Development, 48, 1184-1199.

JOURNAL OF COUNSELING & DEVELOPMENT • SEPTEMBER/OCTOBER 1996 • VOLUME 75 57

You might also like