Undetected Delincvent Behaviour

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 20

Undetected Delinquent Behavior

MARTIN GOLD
*
Assistant Program Director, Research Center for Group Dynamics,
Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan; Assistant Professor,
Department of Psychology, University of Michigan
A.B. (Psychology), 1953, Dartmouth College; M.A. (Psychology), 1955,
Ph.D. (Social Psychology), 1961, University of Michigan

The article discusses an interview method designed to measure


delinquent behavior directly from the confidential confessions of
teen-agers. The method is described and results are presented
from a validation study of 125 undetected delinquents. Applica-
tion of the measure is illustrated with findings from a study of
522 randomly selected boys and girls, thirteen to sixteen years
old, residing in Flint, Mich. Conclusions are drawn about the
relationship between delinquency and social status and sex, and
about the validity ofofficial records.

TUDENTS OF JUVENILE DELINQUENCY upon which many theories of delinquen-


and practitioners who explore the cy have been built.
research literature for help in treating This article introduces an interview
delinquents have found that measure- method designed to find out from teen-
ments of delinquency have been griev- agers how many delinquent acts they
ously inadequate. A youngster generally have committed in the recent past and to
has been labeled &dquo;delinquent&dquo; either discover other pertinent information
because he has been caught by the police about this behavior.t
or because his answers to questions
about himself are similar to those of WHY COLLECT SUCH DATA?
youngsters caught by the police. Almost all of the research on delin-
Social scientists alert to this problem quency begins in the official records of
have developed various methods to mea- police, courts, and institutions. A large
sure delinquency directly from young- number of delinquent acts and the iden-
sters themselves, including field observa- tities of children who committed them
tion, self-administered questionnaires, are unrecorded in these sources. In ad-
and interviews. Results have been dis- dition, they may not accurately reflect
crepant, as, for example, in studies of the distribution of delinquency by sex,
the relationship between juvenile delin- social status, race, and other variables.
quency and social status, a relationship
t This study was part of the Inter-Center
* The author wishes to thank Bert Greene, Program on Children, Youth, and Family Life.
Donald Halsted, Safia Mohsen, and llsargaret which was directed by Ronald Lippitt and
Simberg, who worked on various phases of this Stephen Withey. The research was supported
research; the Juvenile Division of the Flint Po- by Grant M-09-109 from the National Institute
lice Department and the Pupil Personnel Office of Mental Health.
of the Flint Public Schools, who gave invaluable 1 Fred
J. Murphy, Mary W. Shirley, and Helen
assistance; and Dorwin Cartwright, Elizabeth L. Witmer, "The Incidence of Hidden Delin-
Douvan, Ronald Lippitt, and Alvin Zander, who quency," American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,
helped improve the manuscript. October 1946, pp. 686-96.

27
28

Murphy, Shirley, and Witmer,i for ex- tion, so their files do not adequately
ample, report that social agency records sample the population of American ado-
reveal what everyone suspected-that po- lescents. Other investigators have ad-
lice never learn who committed most de- ministered anonymous questionnaires to
linquent acts. samples of adolescentS5 and have pro-
Furthermore, as demonstrated in one duced findings which challenge the real-
the supposed relationship between
Michigan city, boys who live in poorer ity of
socio-economic status and delinquency.
parts of town and are apprehended by
Clark and Wenninger report:
police for delinquency are four to five
times more likely to appear in some offi- Our findings are similar to those of Nye-
cial record than boys from wealthier sec- Short and Dentler-Monroe in that we failed
to detect differences in
any significant illegal
tions who commit the same kinds of of-
behavior rates among the social classes of
fenses.2 These same data show that, at
rural and small urban areas
each stage in the legal process from
On the other hand, Reiss and Rhodes7
charging a boy with an offense to some conducted personal interviews with
sort of disposition in court, boys from
different socio-economic backgrounds are boys in Nashville, Tenn., asking them if
treated differently, so that those eventu- they had ever done something at one
time or another for which they would
ally incarcerated in public institutions, have been arrested if they had been
that site of most of the research on de-
linquency, are selectively poorer boys. caught, and found that &dquo;delinquency
rates, in general, vary inversely with ...

Many well-known social-psychological the ascribed social status of the boy.&dquo;


and sociological theories of delinquency
Because no investigation of undetected
are grounded in data abstracted from
official records.3 These theories are built delinquency so far has sampled a large
fundamentally on the finding that de- representative group of teen-age boys
and girls and because results thus far
linquency is related to socio-economic have been so mixed, further work along
status, although the theoreticians recog-
these lines is indicated.
nize that this relationship may arise
from the method by which the data are Beyond the need simply for accurate
data on the extent and distribution of
compiled. We need better data if we
are to build and test theories more con- delinquencv, descriptions of a large
number of representative delinquent
fidently. acts should give us further insight into
Attempts have been made to collect
the etiology of delinquency. For exam-
data on delinquency independently of
official records. Case histories in the ple, if we were able to distinguish the
files of social agencies have been one person who commits delinquent acts in
source of data,4 but social agencies them- 5
John P. Clark and Eugene P. Wenninger,
selves contact a highly selective popula- "Socio-Economic Class and Area as Correlates of
Illegal Behavior among Juveniles," American
2
Martin Gold, Status Forces in Delinquent Sociological Review, December 1962, pp. 826-34;
Boys (Ann Arbor: University of Michigan, In- Robert Dentler and Lawrence J. Monroe, "Early
stitute for Social Research, 1963). Adolescent Theft," American Sociological Re-
3 Albert K. Cohen,
Delinquent Boys (New view, October 1961, pp. 733-43; Ivan E. Nye,
York: Free Press, 1955) ; Richard Cloward and Family Relationships and Delinquent Behavior
Lloyd Ohlin, Delinquency and Opportunity (New York: Wiley, 1958); Austin L. Porterfield
(New York: Free Press, 1960). and Stanley C. Clifton, Youth in Trouble (Fort
4
Murphy et al., supra note 1; Sophia M. Worth, Tex.: Leo Potishman Foundation, 1946) .
Robison, Can Delinquency Be Measured? (New 6 Clark and Wenninger,
supra note 5.
York: Columbia University Press, 1936) ; Clif- 7 Albert
J. Reiss, Jr. and Albert L. Rhodes,
ford R. Shaw and Henry D. McKay, Juvenile "The Distribution of Juvenile Delinquency in
Delinquency and Urban Areas (Chicago: Uni- the Social Class Structure," American Sociologi-
versity of Chicago Press, 1942). ral Review, October 1961, pp. 720-32.
29

the company of others from the one was not diminished by our inability to
whose delinquency is solo behavior, we interview them. Table 1 describes the
might be in a better position to sort out distribution of the sample by sex, race,
various motivations behind delinquen- and the occupation of the chief bread-
cy. Or, if we were able to determine winner in the family.
precisely what it is that youngsters steal
and what they do with the stolen goods, Introductory Procedure
this information would help us distin- We trained local college students in
guish between &dquo;utilitarian&dquo; and &dquo;mali- field interviewing techniques. Each in-
cious&dquo; forms of delinquency. terviewer was assigned to interview
MEASUREMENT TECHNIQUE youngsters of the same sex and race as
himself. Interviewers were instructed to
The Sample turn back an assignment if they or mem-
We aimed to study a representative set bers of their immediate family knew the
of teen-agers in Flint, Mich., an indus- youngster or the youngster’s family.
trial city of 200,000 people. With the After sending an initial letter to par-
cooperation of the public school system, ents and youngsters announcing a study
we selected at random a sample of six of what teen-agers do in their spare time,
hundred from a list of almost all boys the interviewer arranged by telephone
and girls thirteen through sixteen years to drive the youngster to a community
old living in the school district, regard- center, firehouse, or similar facility near
less of whether they were attending pub- his home so that all interviews could be
lic or private schools or had dropped taken under standardized conditions. We
out of school altogether. We eventually suspect that almost all of the thirty-eight
interviewed 522 of them, or 87 per cent refusals were attributable to parental ob-
of those originally selected. A look at jection to their son’s or daughter’s leav-
the available demographic data on the 6 ing the house with the interviewers, a
per cent who refused to be interviewed practice we required so the youngster
and the 7 per cent who had moved from could be interviewed out of earshot of
Flint indicated that representativeness his family.

Table I
Random Sample
By Sex, Race, and Occupation of Chief Breadwinner in Youth’s Family

0
Less than .5%.
30

During the drive to the interviewing The personal interview has an appar-
station, the interviewer revealed that the ent disadvantage, however. It is reason-
study was about behavior
delinquent able to suppose that offenses would be
which may or may have been de-
not concealed from an interviewer more fre-
tected. He assured the youngster of quently than from an anonymous, self-
confidentiality and anonymity, and he administered checklist. Later in this
stressed the importance of truthfulness. article we report a validity study of the
Then he explained the randomness of personal interview method we used. It
sample selection and offered the young- throws some light on concealment.
ster an opportunity to withdraw from
the study. The interview began with the inter-
One boy asked to be returned home at viewer setting up a large sortboard in
this point. Of course, this does not front of the respondent. Across the top
necessarily mean that the rest were com- was written &dquo;How often have you ... ?&dquo;

pletely honest. Indeed, we know now Under this heading were slots marked
that some were not. &dquo;Never,&dquo; &dquo;More than three years ago,&dquo;
We elected to employ the method of &dquo;In the last three years ... once, ...
personal interview for two reasons. twice, ... three or more times.&dquo; The
First, we wanted to obtain detailed de- slots were large enough to accept a 3 x 5
scriptions of delinquent acts,to find out card.
the who, what, when, where, and how of The interviewer placed a packet of
them. Such data are too complicated to
fifty-one 3 x 5 cards in front of the re-
get in a self-administered questionnaire. spondent : &dquo;First of all, I’m going to ask
Second, we suspected that, given a you to sort this pack of cards for me.
checklist, some youngsters might admit On each card is a statement describing
delinquent acts which would turn out
not to be offenses at all, while others
something a fellow or girl might have
done, like the first one....&dquo;
might overlook actual offenses. Prob- Almost all
ing by an interviewer on the spot could respondents were able to
winnow out the misunderstandings and sort their cards without further instruc-
tions. The fifty-one questions inquired
identify and draw out omissions. In- about such activities as truancy or dis-
deed, our subsequent analysis of data
barment from school, trespass, damage,
shows, for example, that half of the acts
of property destruction, one-fourth of hitting father, lying, stealing, drinking
the confidence games, and one-fifth of beer, fighting, arson, smoking, taking a
the personal assaults to which our sam- car, fornication, and carrying weapons.

ple initially admitted could not con- Questions on approved activities were
also asked. These included mention on
ceivably be called chargeable offenses. the school honor roll, helping charity
We found that such overreporting was
sometimes related to other variables in drives, and working on the school news-
which we were interested; for example, paper.
the proportion of confessions of acci- The interviewer questioned the young-
dental or trivial acts of property destruc- ster about those offenses he admitted
tion was significantly higher among committing in the last three years. If
wealthier than among poorer white boys. the youngster indicated he had com-
We also found that confession of one mitted any particular offense more than
act sometimes led an interviewer to dis- once in that time, he was asked about
cover other related each of the two most recent offenses of
delinquencies; for
example, about one-third of the thefts that kind. A standard form, adminis-
involved at least one other chargeable tered to every respondent, elicited demo-
offense, as did 10 per cent of the assaults. graphic and other data.
31

The questions asked about admitted The interview lasted from thirty-five
offenses were essentially the same for all minutes to over two hours, depending
offenses, with only minor variations on the extent of the youngster’s confes-
tailored for the specific kind of offense. sions. Average duration was about an
Figure I reproduces the form appropri- hour and a half.
ate to &dquo;taking some part of a car or gaso-
line.&dquo; VALIDATION
It should be pointed out that, in A central problem in this sort of re-

Questions 5 and 5a (&dquo;Were you with search is the extent of concealment by


anyone? Who were they?&dquo;), the respon- respondents.
dent was not asked for the names of his Our interviewers were carefully se-
delinquent companions, but only their lected and trained to gain the confidence
age and sex and their relationship (close of their respondents; our entire proce-
friends, acquaintances, etc.) . dure was built around convincing young-

Figure I
One Section of the Interview Schedule
32

sters of the scientific nature of the study; about thedelinquencies they revealed to
and we emphasized in many ways that us, as weaccepted no second-hand testi-
confidentiality was assured. Neverthe- mony. The only facts counted as reli-
less we expected that some proportion of able were those which the informants
youngsters would conceal offenses from had witnessed themselves or which the
us. Certain of them felt apprehensive delinquent himself had told them.
about telling the complete truth. After Over fifty potential informants were
all, said one boy, the interviewer would obtained, and more than forty of them
&dquo;have enough on me to send me up for agreed to cooperate. They were not all
thirty years.&dquo; We did not know what equally helpful. Some could not or
proportion of our sample would conceal would not give us any reliable informa-
offenses; we did not know whether con- tion ; others supplied us with as many as
cealment would vary with factors like seven names along with complete de-
social status, race, and sex; and we did scriptions of the delinquencies, often
not know whether certain offenses would based on participant observation.
be concealed more often than others. Table 2 describes the validation sam-
To study concealment we found it ple by categories of race, sex, and social
necessary to interview a criterion group, status. The status distinction is primar-
youngsters about whose delinquency we ily between blue-collar and white-collar
already had reliable information but workers. Because higher status Negroes
who were not aware we had it. We man- are rare in Flint, few fell into our ran-

aged eventually to interview 125 young- dom sample of 522 teen-agers, so we did
sters under these conditions. not at this time make any attempt to ob-
tain a validating set of higher status Ne-
Procedure
gro teen-agers.
Our strategy was first to contact teen- An interviewer was assigned to a
agers who seemed likely to have infor- youngster in the validation sample with-
mation about the delinquency of other out any prior knowledge of what offenses
boys and girls. We were introduced to his respondent had committed; indeed,
these potential informants by teachers, in most cases he was unaware that his
youth workers, other interested adults, respondent was not among the random
and by some informants themselves. sample. The interviewing procedure was
We explained the study to potential the same as the one used with the ran-
informants and asked them to help us by dom sample.
supplying names of boys and girls who Our data on concealment came from
they knew had committed delinquent comparisons of the responses of the 125
acts for which they had not been caught, validating respondents with what our
together with as much as they could tell informants had already told us they had
us about these acts. We also asked them done. We considered a youngster a
to tell us how they happened to know truthteller if he told us what our rele-

Table 2
Validation Sample
33

vant informant had told us, or if he told Specificoffenses most often concealed
us about more recent offenses of the by the male validity sample were break-
same type, or if he told us about more ing and entering, property destruction,
serious offenses. A respondent was con- and carrying concealed weapons. Al-
sidered a concealer if he did not confess though many of each of these offenses
to an offense about which an informant were confessed, almost as many of those
had told us, or to any more recent simi- we had been told about were concealed
lar offense, or to any more serious of- as were confessed. For example, boys
fense. Youngsters were categorized as confessed to eighteen acts of breaking
questionables when they told us about and entering about which we had no
offenses which were similar to but did prior knowledge; but of the ten offenses
not exactly match offenses about which of this 1’= ’d about which we were quite
we already had information. In such certain, twur were concealed.
cases we were not certain whether some- Girls most frequently concealed break-
thing was being deliberately concealed ing and entering, property destruction,
or distorted or whether ’lie memories of unauthorized driving away, gangfighting,
informants and respondents merely dif- miscellaneous theft, and fornication.
fered.
Limitations
Results The validation study is far from fool-
Table 3presents the findings of the proof. It is vulnerable in at least two
validity check by the sex, race, and social respects:
status of respondents. Overall, 72 per First of all, the validation study, which
cent of the youngsters seemed to tell us is especially concerned with the problem
everything which informants had told of concealment, does not resolve the
us; 17 per cent appear to be outright problem of exaggeration. We do not
concealers; the rest are questionables. know to what extent teen-agers, and
While the proportion of truthtellers boys especially, want to project an im-
differs somewhat from one category to age of at least moderate delinquency
another, no difference falls below the .20 as a demonstration of daring and manli-
chance probability level by the chi- ness. To check would require a criterion

square or Fisher Exact Test. We also group for which we were certain not
wondered whether the more delinquent only of some of the offenses they had
youngsters were more frequently the con- committed but also of some which they
cealers. But a comparative check re- had not. We learned early in our study
vealed no reliable differences. that teen-agers could not vouchsafe that

Table 3
Proportions of Truthtellers, Questionables, and Concealers,
by Sex, Race, and Sodal Status
34

even their closest friends had not com- mother, drinking alcoholic beverages
mitted any particular kind of offense. without parental knowledge or permis-
One safeguard against exaggeration sion, running away from home, gang-
was the set of detailed questions we fighting, shoplifting, larceny, and forni-
asked about each offense. It would have cation. This list covers a wide range of
offenses, including offenses against per-
required an especially creative and sons (e.g., assault) and against property
quick-thinking youngster to fabricate offenses generally believed
offenses during his interview. In a few (e.g., theft) ;
to be moretypical of boys (stealing a
cases, interviewers did catch respondents
car part or gasoline) and more typical
in what may have been exaggeration:
some concealed weapons turned out to
of girls (shoplifting) ; and offenses gen-
be Boy Scout pocket knives; some gang- erally thought trivial (trespassing) as
well as serious (hitting mother) .
fights were nothing more than minor Since interviewers questioned respon-
playground scuffles; and some instances dents closely on no more than the two
of auto theft were only quick spins
around the block in the family car. most recent offenses of each type, Index
F is not as sensitive as other indices
Although some exaggerations must
still be distorting our data, we believe might be to the frequency of delinquent
the distortion is quite minor. activity, but its correlations with indices
Our use of informants-youngsters which include a wider range of offenses
who are aware of the delinquencies of and more information on frequency are
others-leads to a second possible .87 and higher.
source of vulnerability in the validation
The major advantage of Index F is
that detailed information on the rele-
design, which is that we have no infor- vant offenses permitted exclusion of all
mation about loners. Youngsters who
commit their delinquent acts alone and those &dquo;offenses&dquo; which coders judged
not to be chargeable by the police; that
tell no other youngster about them could
not fall into our criterion group. We is, Index F includes only those offenses
do not know how much loners are likely which in themselves would clearly have
to conceal or how much they would
warranted police action if they had been
detected. We noted in the first part of
gladly unburden, for the sake of sci- this paper that not all of the &dquo;offenses&dquo;
ence, to an accepting, interested young
to which youngsters confessed could rea-
interviewer.
sonably be considered &dquo;delinquent
INDICES OF DELINQUENT BEHAVIOR acts&dquo;; 28 per cent were not.
Measures of delinquent behavior are
Index S
based on the confessions of the random
This index is based on a delinquency
sample of boys and girls, aged thirteen index devised by Sellin and Wolfgang,8
to sixteen, who lived in the Flint school
district in 1960. Of the several indices which takes into account the seriousness.
of delinquency which have so far been of an offense as rated by university stu-
constructed from these data, two-Index dents, police ofhcers, juvenile aid work-
F and Index S-are currently employed ers, and juvenile court judges. Each
in our analyses. offense is weighted by some factor which
reflects the seriousness with which that
Index F offense was regarded by the raters.
Index F draws data only from the Our data were not collected in a way
detailed descriptions of delinquent acts which allows precise application of the
which youngsters most seldom conceal- 8Thorsten Sellin and Marvin E. Wolfgang,
trespassing, assault, stealing a part of a The Measurement of Delinquency (New York:
car or gasoline, hitting father, hitting Wiley, 1964) .
35

weights prescribed by Sellin and Wolf- the absolute level of delinquency and
gang, for data collection had begun
our the relative delinquency of youngsters
before they had published their index. in the sample.
However, we found it possible to use an
approximation of their index, assign- Comparisons
ing the weights to a set of nine offenses, Figure 3 presents the distributions of
as described in Figure 2. the two delinquency indices, each one a
different way of organizing the young-
Only those offenses were included for
which interviewers had obtained detailed
sters’ responses. A reader familiar with
social scientific data will recognize the
information because descriptions of the
familiar J-shape of the curves generated
offenses, their chargeability, and the na-
ture of items stolen or damaged were re- by the distributions of the delinquency
indices. Allport9 has observed that this
quired in order to assign weights for is just the shape of curve one should ex-
seriousness.
Because the validation data show that pect from data on deviations from recog-
nized norms. That is, most people stick
youngsters fairly frequently conceal in-
stances of property destruction and un- closely to the rules, and the curve drops
off sharply and bottoms out at its more
authorized driving away of an automo-
deviant end. In this case, most youngsters
bile, Index S in this respect may distort
are not very delinquent, either in the

Figure 2 frequency or seriousness of their delin-


Offenses and Weights Assigned for Serious- quent behaviour, and there are relative-
ness on Index S* ly few youngsters at the more delinquent
ends of the curves.
The rank order (rho) correlation be-
tween these two indices is .68, a statisti-

cally reliable correlation (p<.001). These


indices are highly correlated with one
another as one might expect since they
are both based largely on the same set
of responses. However, correlations
among several indices reveal that Index
S, which takes into account a judgment
of seriousness of the offense, stands some-
what apart from the others. Material
presented later will demonstrate that
the data on official records and the data
collected from Negro boys turn out
somewhat differently when seriousness is
taken into account from when it is not.
This difference makes Index S of spe-
cial interest.
Illustrative Cases
The meaning of a score on a delin-
quency index will be more clear from
the following examples of youngsters
with different scores:

Floyd H. Allport, "The J-curve Hypothesis


9
*
Modification of delinquency index by Sellin of Conforming Behavior," Journal of Social Psy-
and Wolfgang. chology, May 1934, pp. 141-83.
36

Figure 3
Frequency by Distribution of Scores on Delinquency Indices

Case 115 (Index F=00; Index S=00). On December 6, 1958, her birthday, she
-The respondent was a thirteen-year- ran away from home because her parents
were fighting so much, and she said, &dquo;I got
old boy in the ninth grade. His mother
real nervous and scared.&dquo; After wandering
and father are teachers. The boy is an
about Flint for hours, she went to her ma-
active Boy Scout and an honor roll stu-
ternal grandmother’s house, where her moth-
dent. He was interviewed in October
er came to fetch her home.
1961. Sometime in the summer of 1960 she and
He had played truant once, more than a friend went exploring in a house under
three years ago, and currently lies about his construction. They found a case of beer,
age when he goes to the movies so that he probably cached there by the workmen, and
can get in for the children’s
price. &dquo;But the took it and some nails. They brought the
pnce is so high, I seldom go to the movies loot to her stepfather, who spanked her
anymore,&dquo; he said. &dquo;When I do, I save soundly and made her take it all back.
money on the ticket so I can buy popcorn In January 1961 she stole a phonograph
and candy.&dquo; record from a department store. Explaining
her reason for this she said, &dquo;I wanted it and
Case 297 (Index F=04; Index S-06) . didn’t have enough money with me.&dquo;
-The subject here is a fifteen-year-old In October 1961 she took some doll’s
girl in the eighth grade. Her stepfather clothes and three little bells from a depart-
is a foreman for a beer distributor. She ment store. &dquo;I gave them to my little sister
was interviewed in December 1961. for a surprise,&dquo; she reported.
37

Case 026 (Index F=14; Index S=1) . SOCIAL STATUS AND JUVENILE
-This fifteen-year-old boy is in the DELINQUENCY
eighth grade. His stepfather runs a bal- We noted earlier that social scientists
ing machine in a container manufactur- have long maintained that more lower-
ing plant; his mother works part-time as class youngsters are delinquent than are
a salesclerk in a large department store
downtown. He is on a basketball team their middle-class peers. This relation-
in his junior high school and has been a ship and the theories built from it have
class officer. He was interviewed in guided the major efforts to combat de-
August 1962. linquency in action programs such as
the current Federal War on Poverty,
In May 1961 he and a friend entered a New York City’s Mobilization for Youth,
barn on a farm outside of Flint, played
and the Chicago Boys Clubs Youth De-
around in the hayloft awhile, then left.
In July he and some friends stole two hub- velopment Project, all designed to alle-
viate in some way the condition of lower
caps from cars parked outside a Flint dance
hall and sold them. status life. There has, however, always
In August he stole the net from a basket- been a persistent set of competent re-
ball hoop on a school playground and gave searchers who question the belief that
it to a friend for the hoop over his garage lower status youngsters are the most
door. serious and frequent offenders because
On a Friday night that August he and of their lower status. They maintain
some friends got into a fight with &dquo;some
that this relationship is only an arti-
northside niggers.&dquo;
fact of the way data on delinquency are
Again in August he and some friends stole
an air mattress, a portable icebox, a six-pack gathered. Probably a substantial portion
of beer, and the spark from the outboard of the American public shares these re-
motor of a boat left unattended at a pier. searchers’ suspicions that the rich kids
The boys drank the beer and divided the get away with delinquency and the poor
rest. kids get records.
In September, he pulled his switchblade
Our measurement of undetected de-
knife on &dquo;a big Italian boy&dquo; who had taken
his notebook. linquency is free from the selectivity
Later in September he and his friends which exists in the records of police,
pried open trunks of cars at the Armory courts, and social agencies. It allows us
to examine the relationship between so-
parking lot and stole two tires, which were
given to one of the group who had been cial status and delinquency; to see to
driving them around in his car for several what extent this selectivity does exist
months. in the official records; and to determine
In December he and some friends stole to what extent official records truly re-
two cases of soda pop from a laundromat,
flect the actual distribution of delin-
drank what they wanted, and discarded the
rest. quency among different social strata.
In January 1962 he shoplifted fishhooks, Our findings indicate that social status
gloves, and two friendship rings from stores is indeed inversely related to juvenile
in a shopping center. &dquo;We had a kind of delinquency, that more lower status
club like,&dquo; he said, &dquo;and we decided to get
youngsters commit delinquent acts more
this stuff.&dquo;
frequently than do higher status young-
During January he participated in two sters. However, the data allow us to be
successive Saturday night drinking parties.
more specific about who among lower
At the first, he drank liquor at a friend’s
house when the friend’s parents were not at status youngsters are more likely to be
home; at the second, he drank beer, bought delinquent and enable us to place im-
bv an older boy, in a car parked behind a portant qualifications on the general
sch ool. statement of the relationship.
38

Table 4

The Meaning of &dquo;Social Status&dquo;


Police Referral to Court of Offenses Com-
&dquo;Social status,&dquo; as we use the term
mitted by Lower Status Youngsters Compared
here, refers to the prestige hierarchy of with Offenses Committed by Middle Status
occupations in our society. There is a Youngsters
great deal of agreement in the United
States about the relative prestige of oc-
cupations10 and a great deal of stability
to the prestige hierarchy.&dquo; In this pa-
per, a youngster’s &dquo;status,&dquo; unless other-
wise qualified, refers to social status
based on father’s occupation.
The specific measure of status used
here is O. D. Duncan’s scale of occupa-
tions,12 which is based on national stud-
ies of the prestige of occupations in the
United States. These studies demon-
strated that income and education are
the two most important determinants of
the prestige of an occupation. By com-
puting the average income and level of
education achieved by persons in each
reported by the youngsters in the sam-
occupational category and making some ple resulted in police apprehension of
adjustments for their ages, Duncan as- the offender, and, if the offender came
signed a status score to a large number from a higher status family, police were
of occupations.
more likely to handle the matter them-

Selectivity in the Official Records selves without referring it to the court.


Some judgment by the police about
These data demonstrate that official
the ability of a family to control its son’s
records exaggerate the delinquent behav-
ior of boys from lower status homes rela- behavior is likely to be a major factor in
tive to their higher status peers. (Since deteri-nining whether official action will
be taken. Lower status families as a
only five of the 264 girls in the sample
reported being caught by the police, the
analyses of these data refer mainly to the Table 5
boys.) Relationship between Delinquency and Social
Police are more likely to record offi- Status (Boys Only) Demonstrated by Court
Records
cially those offenses committed by lower
status youngsters, the children of semi-
skilled and unskilled men. Table 4
documents this statement. Only about 3
per cent of all the chargeable offenses
10 Wellman L. Warner, M. Meeker, and K.
Eels, Social Class in America (Chicago: Science
Research Associates, 1949) .
11 Robert W.
Hodge, Paul M. Siegel. and
Peter H. Rossi, "Occupational Prestige in the
United States, 1952-63," American Journal of
Sociology, November 1964, pp. 286-302.
12 Albert
J. Reiss, Jr., Otis D. Duncan, Paul
K. Hatt, and C. C. North, Occupations and So-
cial Status (New York: Free Press, 1961).
39

group arejudged less able to keep their delinquent behavior. For example, even
sons out of trouble, so official action is though most juvenile offenses do not re-
more often taken. sult in the apprehension of the offender
Partly as a result of this procedure, and few juvenile offenders are on record
court records in Flint demonstrate the with the authorities, the more delin-
usual relationship between delinquency quent youngsters may have been detect-
and social status: greater proportions of ed and recorded.
boys are adjudged delinquent as one Only boys are included here in the
goes from higher to lower status cate- analysis of the data since only five girls
gories. The proportion listed in Table in the sample have official records, and
5, showing that four to five times more only six report being caught by the
lower status boys are delinquent than police.
higher status boys, is a common ratio in It is clear from the data in Tables 6
the delinquency literature. and 7 that the more delinquent boys are
more likely to be caught by the police.
Do Official Records Reflect the Amount Sixteen per cent of the boys report being
of Delinquent Behavior? caught at least once; but, compared with
While official records are selective in a the least delinquent boys, about four
way which exaggerates the relative de- times as many of the most delinquent
linquency of lower status youngsters, boys on Index S are caught, and about
they may nevertheless approximate real seventeen times as many when Index F
is the measure of delinquency. Since In-
Table 6
dex F emphasizes frequency of offenses,
Proportion of
Boys at Each Level of Delin- while Index S emphasizes seriousness,
quency (Index S) Who Report Being Caught these data suggest that frequency of of-
by Police at Least Once fenses is a greater determinant of being
caught than their seriousness. Erickson
and Empeyl3 come to the same conclusion
on the basis of their data on Utah boys.
Do the figures on boys &dquo;booked&dquo; pre-
sented in Table 10 reflect degree of de-
linquency as the contact records do?
The data show that about five times
more of the most delinquent boys are
booked than the least delinquent boys.
Furthermore, it seems that the serious-
ness of an offense is taken into account
Table 7
of in the decision to book a boy. The data
Proportion Boys at Each Level of Delin- have shown that frequency is a greater
quency (Index F) Who Report Being Caught
by Police at Least Once determinant of apprehension than seri-
ousness ; data in Table 8 show that the
most serious offenders, high on Index S,
are about as likely to be booked as the
most frequent offenders, high on Index
F. Of course, most of the boys high on
one index are also high on the other.

13 L. Erickson and LaMar T. Em-


Maynard
pey, "Court Records, Undetected Delinquency
and Decision-Making," Journal of Criminal Law,
Criminology and Police Science, December 1963,
pp. 456-69.
40

Table 8
Proportion of Boys at Each Level of Delinquency Who Have Been Booked

But although more of the frequent of- of all the boys caught are not by any
fenders are caught, no more of them are means equally delinquent or representa-
booked. Seriousness of the offense en- tive of delinquent boys. Researchers who
ters the decision to book. generalize about delinquents from ap-
It should be borne in mind that the prehended or adjudged delinquents
majority of even the most delinquent should be cautioned by these data.
boys are unknown to the police and the Behavior and Social Status
courts. This comes as no surprise to the Delinquent
police. The point is that the one-third The data to be presented now demon-
or less of the most
delinquent boys who strate that there is indeed an inverse re-
are caught may be a highly selected lationship between delinquent behavior
group of youngsters; and the 16 per cent and social status. However, this rela-

Table 9
Relationship between Delinquency (Index F) and Social Status by Race and Sex
41

Table 10
Relationship between Delinquency (Index S) and Social Status by Race and Sex

tionship exists only among boys. Tables dex F, compared with 52 per cent of the
9 and 10 and Figures 4 and 5 present highest status girls. Similarly, almost
these data. equal proportions of girls fall into the
Both indices have been divided into lower half of the sample on Index S.
four levels ’of delinquency for the pur- An inspection of the data on nonwhite,
poses of presenting the data as graphs. mostly Negro girls shows the same lack
The lowest level of delinquency on both of relationship between delinquency and
indices includes about 53 per cent of the social status, although the comparison
youngsters; the next to lowest, about 20 here is limited to the two lowest social
per cent; the next to highest, about 15 status levels. These data contradict the
per cent; and the highest, about 12 per relationship between girls’ delinquency
cen t. and their social status which emerges
Also, to make the data more clear, the from official records.14
measure of social status has been divided The pattern in the data on white boys!I
into four levels described in Table 5. is quite different from that for the girls.
While the presentation of data is aid- The proportion of lowest status boys
ed by so categorizing the measures, a climbs from 8 per cent in the lowest de-
more sensitive test of the relationship linquency category of Index F to 36 per
between social status and delinquency cent in the highest; the proportion of
considers the complete order from high highest status boys falls from 35 per cent
to low in both sets of measures. Ken- in the lowest category to 11 per cent in
clall’s rank order correlation is used here
to measure the degree of their covaria- 14 William I.
Thomas, The Unadjusted Girl
tion. (Boston, Little, Brown, 1937) ; William W. Wat
The data reveal no reliable relation- tenberg. "Differences between Girl and Boy ’Re-
ship between delinquency and social sta- peaters,"’ Journal of Educational Psychology.
March 1953, pp. 137-46; William W. Wattenberg
tus among girls. Among white girls, 47 and Frank Saunders, "Recidiv ism among Girls,"
per cent of the lowest status girls are in Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,
about the lower half of delinquency in- May 1955. pp. 405-06.
42

Figure 4
Percentage of Youngsters at Four Levels of Delinquency (Index F)
by Social Status and by Race and Sex

the highest category. The two middle make clear that most of the relationship
status categories of white boys occupy between social status and delinquency
intermediate positions on delinquency among white boys is accounted for by
index F. The rank order correlation the greater delinquency of the lowest
between social status and delinquency status white boys.

among white boys indicates that this re- Among the nonwhite boys, the two
lationship could have occurred merely delinquency indices produce somewhat
by chance less than once in 100 times. different results. Xeither index is cor-
White boys show the same pattern on ! dated reliably with social status, but
Index S (Figure 5) . Figiures 4 and 5 the range of social status among non-
43

Figure 5
Percentage of Youngsters at Four Levels of Delinquency (Index S)
by Social Status and by Race and Sex

white boys is so truncated, effectively en- tween higher and lower status nonwhite
compassing only the lowest three scores boys than Index F does. Index S puts
on the Duncan scale, that any rank cor- the proportions of lower status boys be-
relation is limited by the data them- low the proportions of higher status
selves. A comparison of the graphs in boys at the less delinquent end of the
Figures 4 and5 demonstrates that Index index and above the higher status boys
S, which takes seriousness of the offense at the more delinquent end. Two-by-two
into account, better discriminates be- chi-square analyses bear out the different
44

Table 11I
Relationship between Delinquency and Social Status among Nonwhite
Males: Comparing Index F and Index S Measures

patterns obtained by the two indices 1.5:1 against the lowest status boys; that
(Table 11) : the chi-square result from is, the official records come closer to a
Index F is reliable only at about the .40 valid picture than does the estimate of
level; from Index S, it is reliable at less unselective records. The data in Figures
than .05, more lower status boys being 4 and 5 show that three to four times
more delinquent. So it seems that, in more lowest status boys than highest

frequency of delinquency, the sons of status boys behave at the highest delin-
nonwhite unskilled workers do not differ quency level on either index. If we con-
much from the sons of nonwhite, semi- sider these boys to be the ones who rep-
skilled workers; but, in seriousness of of- resent the most pressing social problem
fense-as defined by white, middle-class and therefore should be apprehended
judges-the former commit more serious and given attention, then the official
offenses than the latter. booking rates do ncrt depart so far from
So we have found that delinquent be- truly representing differential delinquen-
havior among boys is related to social cy among social status levels.
status, just as the much criticized official On the other hand, if we define the
records have demonstrated over and over social problem to include the top two
gain. It seems reasonable, then, to levels of delinquency, then the ratio of
raise the question: why continue to
not delinquents is only about 1.5:1I or 2:11
employ official records, at least to ex- against the lowest status boys.
plore this relationship, rather than col- This kind of discussion exposes the
lect more expensive data? greatest source of invalidity inherent in
One reason is that the official records official records: youngsters are categor-
exaggerate the differences in delinquen- ized as &dquo;delinquent&dquo; or not categorized
at all. Some researchers have found this
cy among boys of different status levels.
They make social status, in the sense of distinction too limiting, so they have dis-
the breadwinner’s occupation, seem tinguished between &dquo;sometime delin-
more important than it really is as far quents,&dquo; who appear in the official rec-
as researchers and practitioners are con- ords only once, and &dquo;repeaters.&dquo; 15 We
cerned. About five times more lowest share the view of other researchers in
than highest status boys appear in the this field that it is more useful to think
official records; if records were complete
and unselective, we estimate that the 15 Gold, op. cit. supra note 2; William W.
ratio would be closer to 1.5:1. Wattenberg, "A Comparison of
Repeaters and
However, there is a sense in which the Nonrepeaters among Boys in Trouble with Po-
lice in Detroit in 1946 and 1947," in Michigan
actual ratio of delinquent behavior spe-
Academy of Science, Arts, and Letters, Papers
cifically among boys is closer to 5:1 than 35, 1949.
45

of delinquency as a continuous rather Perhaps the tendency we have found


than as a discrete variable. One of the here for higher status youngsters more
major advantages of our method of gath- often to report nonchargeable behavior
ering data is that it permits us to mea- as delinquent obscures the
relationship
sure delinquency in this way. with social status in checklist data. Per-
Finally, we should point out again haps, in addition, social status itself is
that, according to our findings, official too roughly ascertained in a self-admin-
records are altogether invalid when they istered questionnaire.
reflect a difference in degree of delin- Our data are limited to one city. How-
quency among social status levels for ever, we suspect that, in regard to delin-
girls. It is true that our validation study quency, Flint is not different from any
revealed that girls tend to conceal forni- other community encompassing a fairly
cation, on offense thought to account for broad range of social status categories.
a substantial portion of girls’ delinquen- We suspect that these same findings will
cy. Nevertheless, the validation data hold not only for other urban communi-
give us no reason to believe that there ties but also for rural communities. We
are differences in concealment among so- intend to do research on this problem.
cial status levels. As far as we can tell Data presented here do not permit us
from our data, delinquency among girls to choose among the various theories of
is not related to their social status.
delinquency which are based on its re-
Theoretical Implications lationship to social status.18 The low
correlation of delinquency with social
We noted near the beginning of this
status, however reliable that relation-
paper that major theories of delinquency ship is, suggests that it is time to ex-
are based on the inverse relationship
amine empirically the links between so-
between the degree of delinquency and cial status and delinquency to see wheth-
social status. We noted, too, that the er we can discover the more
potent de-
reality of this relationship has been terminants to which social status is but
questioned and that data have been pro- a scant clue.
duced to refute it. These data are only a small part of
On the basis of the data reported here, the total collected on this project. Data
we conclude that the inverse relation-
are now being organized around such
ship is indeed a fact among boys. (The topics as differences in sex, age, and race,
theories of delinquency to which we the composition of delinquent groups,
have alluded have been implicitly, if not
delinquent &dquo;loners,&dquo; seasonal variations,
explicitly, limited to delinquency among the location of delinquent acts, types of
boys.) delinquents, educational and vocational
It that studies of undetected de-
seems
aspirations of youngsters related to their
linquency by interview methods consis- delinquency, school achievement, and so
tently find a relationship with social on.
status among boys,16 while those which
use self-administered checklists do not.17
note 5; Gerald J. Pine, The Significance of the
Relationships between Social Class Status, So-
16
Maynard L. Erickson and LaMar T. Em- cial Mobility, and Delinquent Behavior, unpub-
pey, "Class Position, Peers, and Delinquency," lished doctoral dissertation, Boston University
Sociology and Social Research, April 1965, pp. School of Education, 1963; Porterfield and Clif-
268-82; Reiss and Rhodes, supra note 7. ton, op. cit. supra note 5.
18 Cohen,
17 Clark and
Wenninger, supra note 5; Dent- op. cit. supra note 3; Gold, op. cit.
ler and Monroe, supra note 5; Jerome Himel- supra note 2; Cloward and Ohlin, op. cit. supra
hoch, "Socio-Economic Status and Delinquency note 3; William C. Kvaraceus and Walter B.
in Rural New England" (paper read at the an- Miller, eds., Delinquent Behavior: Culture and
nual meeting of the American Sociological As- the Individual (Washington, D.C.: National
sociation, Montreal, 1964) ; Nye, op. cit. supra Education Association, 1959) .
46

As for girls, it seems that social scien- emotional disturbance and family rela-
tists will have to search for theoretical tionships. While this study has only a
bases different from a relationship with little to say about thes.e factors, it will
social status. Relatively little systematic perhaps turn up enough new informa-
research has been done on delinquency tion about the nature of delinquency
among girlsl9; the emphasis has been on among girls to throw some light on the
19 Ruth R. Morris, "Female Delinquency and
problem. Our data here already indi-
cate that girls are far less delinquent
Relational Problems," Social Forces, October
than boys. Further analyses of data are
1964, pp. 82-89; Thomas, supra note 14; Watten-
berg, supra note 14. proceeding.

You might also like