Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 18

Progress in Retinal and Eye Research 33 (2013) 67e84

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Progress in Retinal and Eye Research


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/prer

Amblyopia and binocular visionq


Eileen E. Birch a, b, *,1
a
Pediatric Laboratory, Retina Foundation of the Southwest, Dallas, TX, USA2
b
Department of Ophthalmology, UT Southwestern Medical Center, Dallas, TX, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Amblyopia is the most common cause of monocular visual loss in children, affecting 1.3%e3.6% of children.
Available online 29 November 2012 Current treatments are effective in reducing the visual acuity deficit but many amblyopic individuals are
left with residual visual acuity deficits, ocular motor abnormalities, deficient fine motor skills, and risk for
Keywords: recurrent amblyopia. Using a combination of psychophysical, electrophysiological, imaging, risk factor
Amblyopia analysis, and fine motor skill assessment, the primary role of binocular dysfunction in the genesis of
Binocular vision
amblyopia and the constellation of visual and motor deficits that accompany the visual acuity deficit
Stereoacuity
has been identified. These findings motivated us to evaluate a new, binocular approach to amblyopia
Treatment
Anisometropia
treatment with the goals of reducing or eliminating residual and recurrent amblyopia and of improving
Strabismus the deficient ocular motor function and fine motor skills that accompany amblyopia.
Ó 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Contents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 68
1.1. Amblyopia . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 68
1.2. Clinical profile of amblyopia . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 68
1.3. Amblyopia treatment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 68
1.4. Failures of amblyopia treatment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 69
1.5. Insights from treatment failures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71
2. Amblyopia and binocular vision . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .71
2.1. Stereoacuity and amblyopia . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71
2.2. Risk factors for persistent amblyopia . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 73
2.3. Fusional suppression . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 74
2.4. Gaze instability, binocular vision, and amblyopia . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 75
2.5. Motor skills in amblyopia . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77
3. Binocular treatment of amblyopia . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .77
3.1. Perceptual learning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 78
3.2. Treating binocular dysfunction amblyopia . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 78
4. Future directions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 80
4.1. Screening for amblyopia . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 80
4.2. Diagnosis of amblyopia . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 80
4.3. Optimizing treatment for amblyopia . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 81
5. Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 81
Conflicts of interest . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 81
Acknowledgments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 81
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 81

q The research was conducted at the Retina Foundation of the Southwest.


* Pediatric Laboratory, Retina Foundation of the Southwest, 9900 North Central Expressway, Suite 400, Dallas, TX 75231, USA. Tel.: þ1 214 363 3911; fax: þ1 214 363 4538.
E-mail address: ebirch@retinafoundation.org.
1
Percentage of work contributed by each author in the production of the manuscript is as follows: Birch 100%.
2
Please note that the Retina Foundation of the Southwest will have a new address as of December 15, 2012: 9600 North Central Expressway, Suite 100, Dallas, TX 75231.

1350-9462/$ e see front matter Ó 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.preteyeres.2012.11.001
68 E.E. Birch / Progress in Retinal and Eye Research 33 (2013) 67e84

1. Introduction Group, 2002a). Both studies included children with strabismic,


anisometropic, or combined-mechanism amblyopia referred by
1.1. Amblyopia multiple community- and university-based pediatric ophthalmolo-
gists. In the Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group (PEDIG) study,
Amblyopia is diminished vision that results from inadequate we assessed the clinical characteristics of 409 children age 3e
visual experience during the first years of life. Typically, amblyopia 6.9 years with moderate amblyopia who were enrolled in a multi-
is clinically defined as reduced visual acuity accompanied by one or center randomized study of amblyopia treatment (Pediatric Eye
more known amblyogenic factors, such as strabismus, anisome- Disease Investigator Group, 2002a). Strabismus or anisometropia
tropia, high refractive error, and cataract. Amblyogenic factors each accounted for about 40% of amblyopia, with just over 20% of
interfere with normal development of the visual pathways during amblyopia in children with both strabismus and anisometropia
a critical period of maturation. The result is structural and func- (Fig. 1). Overall, mean refractive error in the amblyopic eye
tional impairment of the visual cortex, and impaired form vision. was þ4.52 D and in the fellow eye was þ2.83 D, but fellow eye
Although amblyopia can be bilateral, it most commonly affects refractive error was higher in the strabismic children (þ3.54 D) and
one eye of children with strabismus, anisometropia, or both. The lower in the anisometropic children (þ1.95 D).
prevalence of strabismus, anisometropia, and amblyopia has been In a second study, we examined the clinical profile of 250
reported in a number of recent population-based studies of amblyopic children less than 3 years old in the Dallas area (Birch
preschool children and school children in the United States, the and Holmes, 2010); 82% of amblyopia was associated with stra-
United Kingdom, Netherlands, Sweden, and Australia (Table 1). bismus, 5% with anisometropia, and 13% with both (Fig. 1), a strik-
Overall, results of the studies are consistent, with a mean ingly different distribution of amblyogenic factors compared with
prevalence of 2.8% for strabismus, 3.5% for anisometropia, and 2.4% the older cohort. Mean refractive error in the amblyopic eye
for amblyopia. With 625 million children under the age of 5 years was þ2.63 D, substantially lower than in the older cohort. Fellow
worldwide, more than 15 million may have amblyopia, and more eye refractive error averaged þ2.60 D, similar to the overall mean
than half of them will not be identified before they reach school age for the older cohort and, as with the older cohort, fellow eye
(Wu and Hunter, 2006). Many affected children will suffer irre- refractive error was higher in the strabismic children (þ2.59 D) and
versible vision loss that could have been prevented. The conse- lower in the anisometropic children (þ0.94 D). Our studies, along
quences of not identifying and treating strabismus and amblyopia with two additional studies from the UK (Shaw et al., 1988;
early include permanent visual impairment, adverse effects on Woodruff et al., 1994b), suggest that the factors responsible for
school performance, poor fine motor skills, social interactions, and amblyopia vary with age (Fig. 2). Strabismus is the overwhelmingly
self-image (Choong et al., 2004; Chua and Mitchell, 2004; Horwood the factor most associated with amblyopia during the first year.
et al., 2005; O’Connor et al., 2010b; O’Connor et al., 2009; Packwood Anisometropia, either alone or in combination with strabismus,
et al., 1999; Rahi et al., 2006; Webber et al., 2008a,b). Importantly, becomes equally prominent as a cause of amblyopia by the third
permanent monocular visual impairment due to amblyopia is a risk year and, by the fifth year, is the causative factor in nearly two-
factor for total blindness if the better eye is injured or if the fellow thirds of amblyopic children.
eye is affected by disease later in life (Harrad and Williams, 2003). The low percentage of amblyopia attributable to anisometropia
The predominant theory is that amblyopia results when there is in the <3 year cohort is unlikely to be the result of marked under-
a mismatch between the images to each eye; one eye is favored referral of anisometropia in this age range. Overall, in the parent
while the information from the other eye is suppressed (Harrad study from which the amblyopic children <3 years old were drawn,
et al., 1996). Because amblyopia typically affects visual acuity in only 18% of the 67 of anisometropic children were diagnosed with
one eye, amblyopia is often considered to be a monocular disease. amblyopia (Birch and Holmes, 2010). In contrast, almost 50% of the
Thus, the mainstay of treatment for amblyopia has been patching of 396 children with strabismus or strabismus with anisometropia
the normal fellow eye to improve the monocular function of the were diagnosed with amblyopia. This suggests that anisometropia
amblyopic eye. However, there are significant shortcomings to this may develop later, and become an etiologic factor for amblyopia
approach to understanding and treating amblyopia. Our research primarily after 3 years of age. Another alternative is that aniso-
studies on amblyopia have revealed that binocular dysfunction plays metropia may be present early but requires a longer duration than
an important role in amblyopia and has laid the groundwork for strabismus to cause amblyopia.
a new approach for the development of more effective treatment.
1.3. Amblyopia treatment
1.2. Clinical profile of amblyopia
Current treatments for amblyopia rely on depriving the healthy,
Two large studies have defined the clinical profile of amblyopic fellow eye of vision to force use of the amblyopic eye. The
children (Birch and Holmes, 2010; Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator assumption is that the primary deficit in amblyopia is a monocular

Table 1
Prevalence of strabismus, anisometropia, and amblyopia in recent (2001e2012) population-based studies of children.

Site N Age Strabismus Anisometropia Amblyopia


BPEDS (Friedman et al., 2009; Giordano et al., 2009) US 2546 0.5e6 yr 2.2% 4.5% 1.3%
MEPEDS (Multi-Ethnic Pediatric Eye Disease Study, 2008) US 6014 0.5e6 yr 2.4% 4.1% 2.0%
NICHS (Donnelly et al., 2005) UK 1582 8e9 yr 4.0% e 2.0%
ALSPAC (Williams et al., 2008) UK 7825 7 yr 2.3% e 3.6%
RAMSES (Groenewoud et al., 2010) N 2964 7 yr e e 3.4%
SCHC (Kvarnstrom et al., 2001) S 3126 10 yr 3.1% e
SPEDS (Pai et al., 2012) A 1422 2.5e6 yr e e 1.9%
SMS (Huynh et al., 2006; Robaei et al., 2006) A 1765 5e8 yr 2.8% 2.6% 1.8%
Mean 2.8% 3.8% 2.4%

US ¼ United States, UK ¼ United Kingdom, N ¼ Netherlands, S ¼ Sweden, A ¼ Australia.


E.E. Birch / Progress in Retinal and Eye Research 33 (2013) 67e84 69

define optimal treatment protocols. The Pediatric Eye Disease


Investigator Group (PEDIG) has conducted a series of randomized
clinical trials of amblyopic treatment for children ages 3e17 years
old. To date, our major results are:

 A period of 16e22 weeks of treatment with optical correction


alone leads to an improvement of 0.2 logMAR (2 lines) in both
children with anisometropic amblyopia (Cotter et al., 2006)
and children strabismic or combined mechanism amblyopia
(Cotter et al., 2012). In nearly one-third of amblyopic children
treated with optical correction, amblyopia completed resolved.
Similar results have been reported by the Monitored Occlusion
Treatment of Amblyopia Study (MOTAS) Cooperative Group,
and the Randomized Occlusion Treatment of Amblyopia
(ROTAS) Cooperative Group studies (Stewart et al., 2011). Thus,
refractive correction as the sole initial treatment for amblyopia
is a viable option.
 Patching is an effective treatment for amblyopia. After stable
Fig. 1. In the <3-year-old cohort, (Birch and Holmes, 2010) 82% of amblyopia was visual acuity was achieved with spectacle wear, 3- to 7-year-old
associated with strabismus, 5% with anisometropia, and 13% with combined mecha- amblyopic children who were randomized to patching treat-
nisms. This finding is strikingly different from the PEDIG 3- to 6-year-old cohort,
ment had further visual acuity improvement of 0.22 logMAR
(Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group, 2002a) where only 38% of amblyopia was
associated with strabismus, 37% with anisometropia, and 24% with combined mech-
(2.2 lines) (Wallace et al., 2006).
anism (p < 0.001 for each of the 3 paired comparisons). Among children with stra-  For moderate amblyopia, a prescribed patching dose of 2 h/day
bismus in the <3-year-old cohort, 62% had infantile esotropia (onset 6 months of results in the same visual acuity outcome as a prescribed
age), 22% had accommodative esotropia, 10% had acquired nonaccommodative eso- patching dose of 6 h/day (Repka et al., 2003) and, for severe
tropia (onset 7 months of age), and 6% had other types of strabismus.
amblyopia, 6 h/day yields similar results to 12 h/day (Holmes
et al., 2003). Objective monitoring of compliance with
visual acuity deficit caused by preference for fixation by the fellow prescribed patching in a similar UK study suggests that the
eye. By depriving the fellow eye of vision, suppression of the similar visual acuity outcomes may have been due to lack of
amblyopic eye is eliminated and visual experience will promote compliance with the higher prescribed doses; i.e., the two
development or recovery of visual acuity. Patching, atropine, and treatment groups may have actually received similar doses
filters that penalize the fellow eye have been used to treat ambly- despite assignment to different doses (Stewart et al., 2007b).
opia for hundreds of years (Loudon and Simonsz, 2005). Only in the  Atropine and patching result in similar improvements in visual
last 15 years have randomized clinical trials been conducted to acuity when used as the initial treatment of moderate ambly-
evaluate the effectiveness of amblyopia treatment and to begin to opia in children aged 3e6 years (Pediatric Eye Disease
Investigator Group, 2002b). Most children had 0.3 logMAR
(3 lines) improvement in visual acuity and about 75% achieved
20/30 or better visual acuity within 6 months (Pediatric Eye
Disease Investigator Group, 2002b).
 Among children with severe amblyopia (>0.7 logMAR), treat-
ment on weekends with atropine led to an average improve-
ment of 0.45 logMAR (4.5 lines) (Repka et al., 2009).
 Treatment of amblyopia can be effective beyond 7 years of age,
although the rate of response to treatment may be slower, may
require a higher dose of patching, and the extent of recovery
may be less complete (Holmes et al., 2011; Pediatric Eye
Disease Investigator Group, 2003, 2004).

The PEDIG results, along with results from MOTAS and ROTAS
are summarized in Fig. 3.

1.4. Failures of amblyopia treatment

Not all amblyopic children achieve normal visual acuity despite


treatment. Although 73e90% have improvements in visual acuity
Fig. 2. Strabismic amblyopia was diagnosed much more commonly than anisome- with various treatment modalities alone or in combination, (Repka
tropic or combined-mechanism amblyopia in children <3 years of age (Birch and et al., 2003; Repka et al., 2004; Repka et al., 2005; Rutstein et al.,
Holmes, 2010). This finding is in sharp contrast to the PEDIG report of approxi-
2010; Stewart et al., 2004b; Wallace et al., 2006) 15e50% fail to
mately equal proportions of patients with amblyopia attributable to strabismus and
anisometropia, and approximately a quarter of the amblyopic patients exhibiting both achieve normal visual acuity even after extended periods of treat-
(Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group, 2002a). Two UK studies of amblyopic chil- ment (Birch and Stager, 2006; Birch et al., 2004; Repka et al., 2003;
dren, which included younger children than the PEDIG study, found the cause to be Repka et al., 2004; Repka et al., 2005; Rutstein et al., 2010; Stewart
strabismus in a greater percentage than the PEDIG study (45%e57%), a lower et al., 2004b; Wallace et al., 2006; Woodruff et al., 1994a). One
percentage to be anisometropic (17%), and about the same percentage to be combined
mechanism (27%e35%) (Shaw et al., 1988; Woodruff et al., 1994b). This summary of all
possible explanation for the failure to achieve normal visual acuity
4 studies suggests that one source of the differences in the proportion of amblyopia is that the treatment was started too late. Both experimental and
attributable to strabismus may be age. clinical data support the hypothesis that there is an early sensitive
70 E.E. Birch / Progress in Retinal and Eye Research 33 (2013) 67e84

Fig. 3. Summary of results from recent randomized clinical trials for the treatment of amblyopia conducted by the Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group (PEDIG) (Holmes et al.,
2006; Quinn et al., 2004; Repka and Holmes, 2012), the Monitored Occlusion Treatment of Amblyopia Study (MOTAS) Cooperative Group, (Stewart et al., 2002, 2005; Stewart et al.,
2004a; Stewart et al., 2004b; Stewart et al., 2007a) and the Randomized Occlusion Treatment of Amblyopia (ROTAS) Cooperative Group (Stewart et al., 2007b). Values represent
mean improvement of visual acuity (logMAR) with 3e6 months of treatment. When more than one randomized clinical trial evaluated similar treatment plans, the range of means
reported in the various studies is provided.

period during which strabismus and anisometropia can lead to and that visual acuity outcome is related to compliance (Loudon
abnormal visual development, conventionally considered to be the et al., 2002, 2003; Stewart et al., 2004b; Stewart et al., 2007a,b).
first 7 years of life. As a consequence, there is also a widely held Randomized clinical trials have supported the use of educational
assumption that treatment during this early period is critical for and motivational material for the children and the parents to
obtaining optimal visual acuity outcome. A recent meta-analysis of improve compliance (Loudon et al., 2006; Tjiam et al., 2012).
4 multi-center amblyopia treatment studies (Holmes et al., 2011) However, none of these studies has yet demonstrated that
reported that 7- to 13-year-old children were significantly less improved compliance reduces the proportion of children who fail
responsive to amblyopia treatment than children less than 7 years to achieve normal visual acuity with amblyopia treatment.
old. However, the meta-analysis failed to find a significant differ- A third possible reason for failure to achieve normal visual
ence in treatment response between the 3- and 4-year olds and the acuity with standard treatments for amblyopia is that current
5- and 6-years olds. On the one hand, these results appear to treatments are inadequate. In other words, perhaps more intensive
support the concept of a critical period for treatment that coincides treatment is needed as a “final push” to overcome residual
with the critical period for visual development. On the other hand, amblyopia. Recently, the Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group
poor response to treatment among older children could simply conducted a multi-center randomized clinical trial for children with
reflect poor compliance with treatment; none of these treatment residual amblyopia do 0.2e0.5 logMAR who had stopped
studies included an objective measure of compliance with patching improving with 6 h of prescribed daily patching or daily atropine
and glasses. In fact, we have observed approximately the same (Wallace et al., 2011a). We found that an intensive combined
prevalence of unresolved amblyopia in children whose amblyopia treatment of patching and atropine did not result in better visual
treatment was initiated during the first year of life (Birch and acuity outcomes after 10 weeks compared with a control group in
Stager, 2006; Birch et al., 1990; Birch et al., 2004) as we and which treatment was gradually discontinued.
others have observed when treatment is not initiated until 3e Finally, it has been suggested that children who fail to recover
6 years of age (Birch and Stager, 2006; Birch et al., 2004; Repka normal visual acuity with standard amblyopia treatments may have
et al., 2003; Repka et al., 2004; Repka et al., 2005; Rutstein et al., subtle retinal, optic nerve, or gaze control abnormalities that limit
2010; Stewart et al., 2004b; Wallace et al., 2006; Woodruff et al., their potential for visual acuity recovery (Giaschi et al., 1992;
1994a). This suggests that delay in treatment is not the sole Lempert, 2000, 2003, 2004, 2008a,b). Abnormal macular thickness
reason for failure to recover normal visual acuity. (Al-Haddad et al., 2011; Altintas et al., 2005; Dickmann et al., 2009;
An alternative explanation for failure to achieve normal visual Huynh et al., 2009; Walker et al., 2011), optic disc dysversion
acuity is lack of compliance with treatment. Using objective (Lempert and Porter, 1998), optic nerve hypoplasia (Lempert,
occlusion dose monitoring devices, it has been demonstrated that 2000), and gaze instability (Regan et al., 1992) have all been
compliance varies widely among children treated for amblyopia described in subsets of patients with amblyopia. To evaluate
E.E. Birch / Progress in Retinal and Eye Research 33 (2013) 67e84 71

whether changes in the retina, optic nerve, or gaze control may 1.5. Insights from treatment failures
limit some children’s response to amblyopia treatment, we evalu-
ated each of these aspects of the visual system in 26 children with Residual and recurrent amblyopia remain unexplained. The
persistent residual strabismic, anisometropic, or combined mech- studies reviewed in Sections 1.3 and 1.4 all have one important
anism amblyopia. We compared the retina, optic nerve, and gaze feature in common; amblyopia treatment was monocular. Yet the
control characteristics of their amblyopic eyes with those of the predominant theory is that amblyopia arises when there is
fellow eyes, with age-matched nonamblyopic children who had a mismatch between the images to each eye. In other words,
strabismus or anisometropia (n ¼ 12), and with age-matched abnormal binocular experience is the primary cause. This prompted
normal controls (n ¼ 48) (Subramanian et al., in press). All us to investigate the link between amblyopia and binocular func-
amblyopic children had been treated with glasses, patching, and/or tion from a number of different approaches. Our investigations
atropine for 0.8e5 years, had a residual visual acuity deficit, and no have evaluated the role of binocular dysfunction in the dispropor-
improvement in visual acuity despite treatment and excellent tionate loss of optotype visual acuity in strabismic amblyopia
compliance for at least 6 months prior to enrollment. (Section 2.1), in the risk for persistent, residual amblyopia (Section
Images of the macula were obtained with the Spectralis 2.2), in asymmetric fusional suppression (Section 2.3), in the gaze
(Heidelberg Engineering) spectral domain OCT (SD-OCT) using the instabilities associated with amblyopia (Section 2.4), and in the fine
eye-tracking feature (ART). Each child had one to three line scans motor skill deficits that are present in amblyopic children and
centered on the optic disc to measure horizontal and vertical disc adults (Section 2.5). As evidence about the primary role of binoc-
diameter, and one to three mm high-resolution peripapillary RNFL ular dysfunction in amblyopia has accumulated, we have worked to
circular scans, and one to three high-resolution macular volume develop approaches to amblyopia treatment (Section 3).
scans. Optic disc dysversion (tilt) was quantified by calculating the
ratio of vertical to horizontal disc diameters. Optic nerve hypo- 2. Amblyopia and binocular vision
plasia was quantified by calculating the area of the ellipse specified
by the vertical and horizontal diameters. In order to assess possible 2.1. Stereoacuity and amblyopia
sectoral hypoplasia, (Lee and Kee, 2009) RNFL thickness was
automatically segmented using the Spectralis software that Strabismic and anisometropic amblyopia differ in the spectrum
provided average thickness measurements for each of 6 RNFL of associated visual deficits despite their common effect on visual
sectors centered on the optic disc [temporal superior (TS), acuity. Strabismic amblyopia is associated with disproportionately
temporal (T), temporal inferior (TI), nasal inferior (NI), nasal (N), greater deficits in optotype visual acuity and vernier acuity
nasal superior (NS)] together with a global average (G). Total compared with grating acuity, but anisometropic amblyopia is
macular thickness and sectional volumes were automatically associated with proportional deficits in optotype, vernier, and
determined by software provided by Heidelberg Engineering for grating acuity (Levi and Klein, 1982; Levi and Klein, 1985). There
the Spectralis SD-OCT, using a modified ETDRS circle grid (center, are two hypotheses regarding the source of differences in the
middle and outer rings: 1, 2, and 3 mm). To assess gaze control, eye pattern of visual deficits between anisometropic and strabismic
movements during attempted steady fixation in primary gaze amblyopia. First, the differences may reflect fundamentally
(binocular and monocular) were recorded using an infrared eye different pathophysiological processes (etiology hypothesis) (Levi,
tracker. 1990; Levi and Carkeet, 1993). For example, sparse/irregular
As shown in Fig. 4, no abnormalities were apparent in macular sampling may be associated with binocular competition between
structure, and there were no significant differences in macular two discordant images in strabismus but not between the sharp
thickness between the amblyopic and fellow eyes, nonamblyopic vs. defocused images in anisometropia. The second hypothesis is
eyes, or normal control eyes. Nor were there significant differences that the different constellations of spatial deficits in anisome-
among these groups in optic disc diameter or shape (dysversion), or tropic and strabismic amblyopia reflect the degree of visual
in peripapillary RNFL thickness (Fig. 4). Children with persistent maturation present at the onset of amblyopia (effective age
amblyopia had eye movement abnormalities in both amblyopic and hypothesis); (Levi, 1990; Levi and Carkeet, 1993) that is, aniso-
fellow eyes, including infantile nystagmus (13%), fusion maldevel- metropic amblyopia may arise at an age where visual maturation
opment nystagmus (47.8%), and saccadic oscillations (39.1%). Some is more complete. The low prevalence of anisometropic amblyopic
non-amblyopic eyes of children with strabismus or anisometropia in children <3 years old (Section 1.2) is consistent with the
also had saccadic oscillations (44.4%) or fusion maldevelopment effective age hypothesis. The effective age hypothesis predicts that
nystagmus (11.1%). Normal controls showed no gaze abnormalities. visual functions that mature earliest will be less susceptible to
Thus, retinal and optic nerve abnormalities cannot explain persis- disruption by abnormal visual experience. Much of the data from
tent amblyopia. Gaze instabilities are typically bilateral, but may be adult with amblyopia is consistent with both of these ideas but
a plausible explanation of persistent amblyopia in a subset of direct tests of the alternative hypotheses were not possible in
children who have asymmetric gaze instability (more instability in adults because complete clinical histories are rarely available to
the amblyopic eye). precisely document etiology and the time course over which
Even among the 50e85% of children who do achieve normal amblyopia developed.
visual acuity with amblyopia treatment, the risk for recurrence of We designed a study to overcome this limitation by evaluating
amblyopia is high. In a series of prospective, longitudinal studies of vernier, resolution and recognition acuities of amblyopic children
infantile and accommodative esotropia conducted in our laboratory who were enrolled in a prospective study of visual maturation
over the past 22 years, (Birch, 2003; Birch et al., 2005; Birch and during infancy and early childhood, i.e., amblyopic children with
Stager, 2006; Birch et al., 1990; Birch et al., 2004; Birch et al., known age of onset and etiology (Birch and Swanson, 2000). We
2010) 80% of esotropic children were treated for amblyopia at found that strabismic amblyopia, whether it had infantile onset or
least once during the first 5 years of life; 60% had recurrent preschool age onset, was associated with disproportionately larger
amblyopia that required re-treatment during the 5-year follow-up. optotype and vernier acuity deficits compared to grating acuity
PEDIG reported that 25% of successfully treated amblyopic children deficits (Fig. 5). Moreover, looking only at the subgroup of ambly-
experience a recurrence within the first year off treatment (Holmes opic children with infantile onset (Fig. 5), we observed differences
et al., 2004). in the pattern of visual deficits. Children with infantile strabismic
72 E.E. Birch / Progress in Retinal and Eye Research 33 (2013) 67e84

Fig. 4. Spectral domain optical coherence tomography (SD-OCT) scans of the amblyopic (top left) and non-amblyopic (top right) eyes of a child with persistent amblyopia
(Subramanian et al., in press). Overall (n ¼ 26), there were no significant differences in total macular thickness or in any of the ETDRS macular sectors (second panel) between
amblyopic and fellow eyes (p > 0.2 for all paired t-tests). The third panel shows a sample SD-OCT scan of the RNFL obtained by SD-OCT and means for each RNFL sector; there were
no significant differences in global RFNL thickness nor in any sector between amblyopic and fellow eyes (p > 0.4 for all paired t-tests). The bottom panel illustrates the method used
to quantify optic disc dysversion (Horiz:Vert disc diameter ratio). No significant differences were found for amblyopic vs. fellow eyes (H/Vambly ¼ 1.15  0.18; H/Vfellow ¼ 1.14  0.17;
p ¼ 0.87). Hypoplasia (assessed by area of the optic disc, mm2) did not differ for amblyopic vs. fellow eyes (Areaambly ¼ 1.88  0.44; Areafellow ¼ 2.10  0.61; p ¼ 0.26).

amblyopia had disproportionately larger optotype and vernier to the development of a fixation preference for one eye and
acuity deficits compared to grating acuity deficits but children with a constellation of monocular visual deficits in the non-preferred eye
infantile anisometropic amblyopia had proportional deficits in in which optotype and vernier acuity are especially compromised
optotype, vernier, and grating acuity. These data support the (Kiorpes et al., 1998; Kiorpes and McKee, 1999). However, it is
etiology hypothesis. Infantile anisometropic amblyopia results important to note that anisometropic amblyopia, whose eyes are
from blurred vision in one eye during the first years of life; in infant aligned but who lack central binocular function, resemble those
monkeys, experimentally induced blur leads to a selective loss of with strabismic amblyopia in their pattern of visual deficits (Levi
neurons tuned to high spatial frequencies (Kiorpes et al., 1998; et al., 2011). Thus, it appears that the presence or absence of
Kiorpes and McKee, 1999). On the other hand, infantile strabismus binocular function drives the pattern of visual deficits, not the
disrupts the binocular connections of cortical neurons and can lead presence or absence of strabismus.
E.E. Birch / Progress in Retinal and Eye Research 33 (2013) 67e84 73

Fig. 6. Grating and optotype acuities for 106 children with mild-to-moderate ambly-
opia (Bosworth and Birch, 2003). Amblyopic children with preserved stereopsis (gray
symbols) fall near the 1:1 line (gray line), consistent with similar loss of grating acuity
and optotype acuity. Amblyopic children with nil stereoacuity (yellow symbols) fall to
Fig. 5. Grating acuity and vernier acuity at 6e9 years of age for 20 amblyopic children the right of the 1:1 line, consistent with similar greater loss optotype acuity of opto-
enrolled in a prospective study at the time of diagnosis during infancy (Birch and type acuity than grating acuity. The mean optotype grating acuity difference was
Swanson, 2000). The solid line shows the predicted relationship if comparable defi- 0.13  0.13 logMAR for amblyopic children with preserved stereopsis and
cits were present for both vernier and grating acuity so that the normal grating:vernier 0.22  0.16 logMAR for amblyopic children with nil stereoacuity (F1,104 ¼ 10.4;
acuity ratio of 8:1 was maintained. Data from amblyopic children with infantile p ¼ 0.0016).
anisometropia fall near the prediction line. Data from amblyopic children with
infantile esotropia fall above the prediction line, i.e., there is a disproportionally larger
loss in vernier acuity. age of onset of esotropia, delay in referral to an ophthalmologist,
age at surgery, and initial refractive error.
Early onset of esotropia might be a risk factor for persistent
amblyopia because it may represent more severe disease (Tychsen,
We tested the hypothesis that regardless of the etiology of
2005) or because early onset may result in a longer duration of
amblyopia, those who have nil stereoacuity experience a dispro-
abnormal visual experience (Birch, 2003; Birch et al., 2000;
portionate loss in optotype acuity, and this loss exceeds what is
Tychsen, 2005, 2007; Tychsen et al., 2004) The incidence of
seen in amblyopia with preserved stereopsis (Bosworth and Birch,
amblyopia was high in children diagnosed with esotropia; 80% of
2003). Optotype acuity, Teller grating acuity, and stereoacuity were
the children with infantile esotropia and 74% of the children with
evaluated in 106 children with moderate amblyopia (0.3e
accommodative esotropia developed amblyopia. In neither the
0.7 logMAR) due to anisometropia, strabismus, or both. Overall,
infantile esotropia cohort nor the accommodative esotropia cohort,
amblyopic children had 2 times worse optotype acuity than grating
was there an association between age at onset of esotropia and the
acuity. Regardless of age at onset or etiology, amblyopic children
prevalence of persistent amblyopia (Fig. 7).
with nil stereoacuity had large optotype-grating acuity discrep-
Delay in referral was evaluated as a risk factor for persistent
ancies (Fig. 6), Our data suggest that the residual monocular
amblyopia because it is known that a shorter duration of esotropia
sampling mosaic provides adequate information for grating
optimizes stereoacuity outcomes (Birch, 2003; Birch et al., 2000;
detection but is inadequate to represent the local spatial relation-
Birch et al., 2005; Fawcett et al., 2000; Fawcett and Birch, 2003) and
ships needed for optotypes. These data support the hypothesized
stereopsis may reduce the risk for moderate to severe amblyopia
link between impaired binocular function and the neural under-
(Bosworth and Birch, 2003; McKee et al., 2003). Among children
sampling/disarray associated with amblyopia.
who were referred for treatment within 3 months of onset of
esotropia, only 12% of the infantile esotropia cohort and 14% of the
2.2. Risk factors for persistent amblyopia accommodative esotropia cohort developed persistent amblyopia.
Among those who were not referred for treatment for more than
Independent support for a strong link between binocular 3 months post-onset, 42% of the infantile esotropia cohort and 56%
dysfunction and amblyopia has come from our studies of persistent of the accommodative esotropia cohort developed persistent
residual amblyopia. To evaluate potential risk factors for persistent amblyopia; i.e., a 3.5e4 times elevated risk for persistent amblyopia
amblyopia, (Birch et al., 2007) we enrolled 130 consecutive children was associated with delayed referral (Fig. 7). Within the infantile
with infantile esotropia or accommodative esotropia in a prospec- esotropia cohort, we were also able to evaluate whether age at
tive, longitudinal study at the time of their initial diagnosis, and surgery was a risk factor for persistent amblyopia (surgical treat-
conducted regular follow-up visits to assess visual acuity and ment in the accommodative esotropia cohort was rare). Very early
stereoacuity through visual maturity (mean age ¼ 9.5 years). On the surgery, by 6 months of age, may preserve stereopsis in children
basis of the longitudinal data set, the children were classified as: with infantile esotropia (Birch et al., 1998; Birch et al., 2000; Ing,
never amblyopic, recovered (treated for amblyopia and currently 1991; Ing and Okino, 2002; Wright et al., 1994). Nonetheless, we
0.1 logMAR or better visual acuity and 0.1 logMAR or less inter- found no significant differences in age at surgery among those who
ocular difference in visual acuity), or persistent amblyopic (lengthy never had amblyopia, those who recovered, and those who devel-
and/or repeated attempts to treat amblyopia with one or more oped persistent amblyopia (Fig. 7).
treatment modalities but visual acuity remained 0.2 logMAR or Initial refractive error was evaluated as a risk factor for persis-
poorer and 0.2 logMAR or more interocular difference in visual tent amblyopia because amblyopia may be more prevalent in
acuity persisted). Four risk factors for amblyopia were evaluated: children with moderate to high hyperopia (American Academy of
74 E.E. Birch / Progress in Retinal and Eye Research 33 (2013) 67e84

Fig. 7. Risk factors for amblyopia in 130 consecutive children with infantile esotropia or accommodative esotropia enrolled in a prospective longitudinal study at the time of initial
diagnosis with follow-up to a mean age of 9.5 years (Birch et al., 2007). No significant differences between nonamblyopic, recovered amblyopic, and persistent amblyopic groups
were found for age of onset (upper left) or age at surgery (lower left). Delay in referral for treatment was associated with risk for amblyopia and persistent amblyopia (F ¼ 12.6,
p < 0.001). Anisometropia 1.00 D on the initial visit was also associated with elevated risk for amblyopia and persistent amblyopia (F ¼ 11.1, p ¼ 0.002).

Ophthalmology, 2011) and because spherical anisometropia of 1.00 2.3. Fusional suppression
D or more had been shown previously to be associated with
increased risk for amblyopia in hyperopic children with no stra- When information from the two eyes is combined and binocular
bismus (Weakley, 1999, 2001) Neither the infantile esotropia cohort disparity is used to decode depth, monocular position information is
nor the accommodative esotropia cohort exhibited an association lost. “Fusional suppression” is the term coined by McKee and Harrad
between initial spherical equivalent refractive error and the prev- (1993) to describe the suppression of monocular position informa-
alence of persistent amblyopia. On the other hand, initial aniso- tion in the presence of a standing non-zero binocular disparity. They
metropia was a strong risk factor for persistent amblyopia. Among were able to measure the suppression of monocular position infor-
children with less than 1.00 D initial anisometropia, only 12% of the mation in terms of diminished visual evoked potential (VEP)
infantile esotropia cohort and 16% of the accommodative esotropia amplitude in response to vernier offsets when a standing non-zero
cohort developed persistent amblyopia. Among those with 1.00 D disparity was introduced. We used this paradigm to investigate the
or more initial anisometropia, 39% of the infantile esotropia link between the fusional suppression that occurs in normal binoc-
cohort and 50% of the accommodative esotropia cohort developed ular vision and the suppression of the amblyopic eye in accommo-
persistent amblyopia; i.e., a 3.1e3.3 times elevated risk for persis- dative esotropia (Fu et al., 2006). We chose to study children with
tent amblyopia was associated when anisometropia of at least 1.00 accommodative esotropia have a wide range of stereoacuity, from
D was present at the initial visit (Fig. 7). normal to nil, which makes it easier to observe the relationships
In a related study, we determined whether nil stereoacuity was among stereoacuity, fusional suppression, and amblyopia.
associated with risk for poor response to amblyopia treatment The stimulus paradigm was to record VEPs in response to
(Bosworth and Birch, 2003). Seventy-one children were enrolled oscillating vernier positional offsets of a stripe pattern presented to
in a prospective study of amblyopia treatment when they were one eye (Fig. 8). What was varied between the test two conditions
4e6 years old. At enrollment 66% had nil stereoacuity; following was the pattern in the other eye, which could either have 0 min
two years of treatment, 55% remained amblyopic. In contrast, binocular disparity or 5 min binocular disparity. Since it was
among children who had measurable stereoacuity, only 25% a standing (static) disparity, it could only influence VEP amplitude
remained amblyopic. Thus the risk for persistent amblyopia was 2.2 through its indirect effect on the vernier positional response
times greater among children with nil stereoacuity. generated by the other eye; i.e., fusional suppression. In a cohort of
Taken together, these data demonstrate that risk for persistent 37 children with accommodative esotropia, we found that all 8
amblyopia is elevated in infantile esotropia and accommodative children with normal stereoacuity and 10 children with deficient
esotropia if there is a long delay between onset and treatment and stereoacuity had normal fusion suppression (see Fig. 9 for a repre-
by anisometropia. The finding that the same two risk factors greatly sentative set of data from a normal control child). Amplitudes of
elevate the risk for abnormal stereoacuity and persistent amblyopia responses to vernier offsets were diminished in the presence of
supports the hypothesis that there is a link between binocular a standing binocular disparity. The vernier responses were similar
function and persistent amblyopia. In individuals with preserved in each eye and the degree of fusional suppression was similar
binocular function, the fellow eye may maintain a binocular grid of regardless of which eye viewed the vernier stimulus and which eye
fine receptive fields so that input to the weaker eye is not severely viewed the static disparity stimulus. Children with accommodative
undersampled. Treatment plans designed to preserve stereoacuity ET and nil stereoacuity but no amblyopia (n ¼ 11) showed little if
may help to minimize the additional loss in optotype acuity asso- any fusional suppression; VEP amplitudes were similar whether or
ciated with neural undersampling. not a standing binocular disparity was present (not shown). Results
E.E. Birch / Progress in Retinal and Eye Research 33 (2013) 67e84 75

fusion in normal binocular vision suggests that restoration of


binocular function may be possible in amblyopia.

2.4. Gaze instability, binocular vision, and amblyopia

Abnormal visual experience during infancy or early childhood


that interferes with binocular fusion is invariably associated with
gaze instability, most often fusion maldevelopment nystagmus
syndrome (FMNS) (Tychsen et al., 2010). FMNS is a commonly
associated with strabismus and amblyopia. It is characterized by
a conjugate horizontal slow-phase nasalward drift of eye position,
with temporalward corrective flicks. Because the slow drift is
always nasalward, a switch in fixation eye results in an immediate
reversal of the direction of the drift. This feature makes it easily
distinguishable from infantile nystagmus (INS). Studies of non-
human primates demonstrated that FMNS can result from the
loss of binocular connections in the striate cortex that occurs in
experimental models of strabismus and anisometropia. In a series
Fig. 8. Schematic of the stimuli used to assess fusional suppression (Fu et al., 2006). of elegant experiments, Tychsen et al. (2010) have demonstrated
Stimuli were dichoptic (anaglyphic) multi-bar vernier targets. When there was no
standing binocular disparity present (left), 5 static segments interspersed with 4
that the prevalence and severity of FMNS increases as the period of
oscillating segments that aligned and misaligned at 2 Hz. The magnitude of the decorrelated abnormal binocular experience is lengthened, with
misalignment offset was swept in 7 steps from 10 to 1 min over each 7 s trial while the a duration in primates that is equivalent to 3 months in humans
other eye viewed a static multi-bar target with 0 min standing binocular disparity. resulting in 100% prevalence. Tychsen et al. (2010) propose that the
The VEP was elicited by the making and breaking of co-linearity in the eye that viewed
binocular maldevelopment of the striate cortex is passed on to
the vernier offsets. The standing disparity condition was similar except that the other
eye viewed static multi-bar target with 5 min standing disparity. As in the other downstream extrastriate regions of cerebral cortex that drive
condition, the VEP was elicited by the making and breaking of co-linearity in the eye conjugate gaze, including medial superior temporal area and that
that viewed the vernier offsets; any difference in VEP amplitude between the two unbalanced monocular activity may result in one hemisphere
conditions is due to a suppressive effect of the static binocular disparity on the becoming more active than the other, resulting in nasalward drift
monocular vernier position response.
bias that typifies FMNS.
We evaluated the link between binocular vision, gaze instability,
from the 8 amblyopic children had a distinct pattern; fusional and amblyopia in a group of 33 children 5 years of age with
suppression was noted when the standing disparity was presented hyperopic anisometropia (Birch et al., 2012). Hyperopic anisome-
to the fellow eye but no suppression was observed when the tropia places the child at risk for abnormal stereoacuity and
standing disparity was presented to the amblyopic eye (Fig. 9). microstrabismus, which is noted clinically as a temporalward “flick”
These data from children with accommodative esotropia support as each assumes fixation on cover testing. The dominant hypothesis
an association between asymmetric fusional suppression and is that abnormal sensory experience due to anisometropia leads
amblyopia. Moreover, the evidence that suppression is present and, to foveal suppression, which in turn leads to microstrabismus
at least in part, involves the same mechanisms responsible for (American Academy of Ophthalmology, 2011). We proposed an

Fig. 9. VEP responses to monocular oscillating vernier offsets recorded from a child with normal vision (left) and an amblyopic child (right) with 0 or 5 min standing binocular
disparity (Fu et al., 2006). For the child with normal vision, vernier responses are similar for each eye (gray symbols). The addition of a standing binocular disparity results in
decreased VEP amplitude, consistent with fusional suppression. The amount of suppression is similar regardless of which eye views the vernier target and which views the static,
disparate stimulus (yellow symbols). For the amblyopic child (right), the amblyopic eye has slightly, but consistently lower, amplitude vernier responses than the fellow eye. When
the standing disparity is introduced to the fellow eye, fusion suppression is observed (yellow circles). However, when the standing disparity is introduced in the amblyopic eye, no
fusional suppression is observed (yellow triangles). Fusion suppression is asymmetric in the amblyopic child.
76 E.E. Birch / Progress in Retinal and Eye Research 33 (2013) 67e84

Fig. 10. Examples of fixation stability obtained from 33, 5- to 9-year-old children with hyperopic anisometropia using the Nidek MP-1 microperimeter (Birch et al., 2012). On each
image, the small blue dots mark the location of the 750 fixation points acquired during the 30 s test period. The Nidek MP-1 calculated three ellipses to describe the areas that
enclose 68%, 95%, and 99% of the fixation points; the 95% BCEA ellipse is highlighted in yellow. On the left is a child with steady fixation; only small amplitude drifts and
microsaccades were present and nearly all of the fixation points fell inside of the 1 deg fixation circle shown in red (BCEA ¼ 0.8 deg2). In the center is a more typical anisometropic
child, who shows moderate fixation instability (BCEA ¼ 5.9 deg2). On the right, a child with substantial fixation instability is shown (BCEA ¼ 10.8 deg2).

alternative hypothesis; namely, that disruption of bifoveal fusion by (Fig. 11). Although there was moderate variability among children,
anisometropia has a direct effect on ocular motor function. Fixation there was a strong correlation between stereoacuity and fixation
stability was measured using the Nidek MP-1 microperimeter, instability (BCEA area); rs ¼ 0.54, p ¼ 0.002. We were also able to
which combines a non-mydriatic infrared fundus camera to obtain export the raw eye movement data to analyze waveforms and
real time retinal images and a liquid crystal display (LCD) to present categorize eye movements as normal (with or without square-wave
a fixation target. The fixation target was a 1 radius red circular ring, oscillations), FMNS, or INS (Fig. 12).
displayed in the center of the LCD screen. Children were provided None of the children with normal stereoacuity had FMNS
a chin rest and forehead rest and were instructed to fixate steadily at waveforms, 67% of children with abnormal stereoacuity had FMNS
the center of the ring. Initially, a reference fundus image with 1 pixel waveforms, and nearly all of the children with nil stereoacuity had
(0.1 deg) resolution was captured, and a high contrast retinal FMNS waveforms (Fig. 13). Thus, the temporalward flick that is seen
landmark in the frozen image was identified by the examiner. during cover testing in children with hyperopic anisometropia is
During the 30 s test period, software calculated shifts in horizontal likely evidence of a direct effect of disruption of bifoveal fusion by
and vertical eye position between the reference image and the real- anisometropia on ocular motor function; i.e., the decorrelation of
time fundus image at 25 Hz. A scattergraph of the fixation points retinal images caused by anisometropia results in FMNS. Note that
was displayed and fixation stability was quantified as the area of the this FMNS is often very small in amplitude, making it difficult to
95% bivariate contour ellipse (BCEA); i.e., an ellipse that provided the appreciate clinically except with close observation, like during
best fit to the boundary of 95% of the 750 fixation points acquired cover testing. More importantly, in most children with anisome-
during the 30 s test interval. Stereoacuity was measured using the tropia, we observed FMNS only in intermittent, small amplitude
Preschool Randot Stereoacuity Test. Examples of the fixation bursts, so it may not always be obvious during a brief clinical
stability data obtained from 3 children with hyperopic anisome- examination. Our data are consistent with the model of FMNS
tropia are shown in Fig. 10. proposed by Tychsen, Wong, and colleagues in which correlated
All children with normal stereoacuity had normal fixation visual experience is necessary for the development of stereopsis
stability (Fig. 11). Children with abnormal stereoacuity had unstable and balanced gaze (Tychsen et al., 2010). Decorrelated visual
fixation, and children with nil stereoacuity had the most instability experience, due to anisometropia can lead to stereoacuity deficits

Fig. 11. Fixation instability during 30 s of attempted steady fixation for 33 children
with hyperopic anisometropia (Birch et al., 2012). Age-matched normal controls had Fig. 12. Eye position records derived from the Nidek MP-1 for the 3 anisometropic
a mean BCEA ¼ 2.1  0.9 deg2 (yellow line), with a 95% tolerance range of 0.2e3.8 deg2 children shown in Fig. 10 (Birch et al., 2012). The top trace shows eye position for the
(yellow shaded area). Anisometropic children with normal stereoacuity (bifoveal; child with normal fixation stability (Fig. 10, left); overall, fixation is accurate, with only
20e60 arcsecs) had normal fixation stability. Children with reduced stereoacuity of microsaccades and a brief saccadic oscillation. The middle trace shows the typical
100e800 arcsecs (monofixation) had significantly more fixation instability than anisometropic child (Fig. 10, middle); it shows the classic waveform of FMNS with slow
normal (p ¼ 0.008) and those with nil stereoacuity were significantly more unstable drifts nasalward, and rapid re-fixating temporalward saccades. The bottom trace
than those with normal stereoacuity and those with reduced stereoacuity (p < 0.001 shows the anisometropic child with extreme fixation instability (Fig. 10, right); high
for both comparisons). frequency, large amplitude FMNS is evident.
E.E. Birch / Progress in Retinal and Eye Research 33 (2013) 67e84 77

Fig. 14. Mean fixation areas (95% BCEA) for amblyopic, non-amblyopic and normal
Fig. 13. Prevalence of FMNS among 33 children with hyperopic anisometropia divided
control children (Subramanian et al., 2012). Compared with children with strabismus,
into 3 groups based on their stereoacuity (Birch et al., 2012).
anisometropia, or both who were nonamblyopic, amblyopic children had significantly
greater fixation instability; mean BCEA for amblyopic eyes was significantly larger than
and the gaze asymmetry of FMNS, a disruption of ocular motor for non-amblyopic right or left eyes (p  0.02). Mean BCEA when fixating with the
development. We propose that the temporalward re-foveating amblyopic eye was significantly larger than for normal controls’ right or left eyes
(p  0.01) left eyes. Amblyopic eyes also had significantly more fixation instability than
FMNS “flicks” may mimic microstrabismus during cover testing
their fellow eyes (p ¼ 0.01). Fixation stability of fellow eyes did not differ significantly
but are actually an ocular motor abnormality that directly results from right or left eyes of normal controls.
from binocularly decorrelated visual experience. If this hypothesis
is correct, our proposal that binocular dysfunction plays a primary
role in amblyopia leads us to also expect a relationship between of these studies aimed to evaluate the utility and value of rehabil-
amblyopia and fixation instability. itation of the amblyopic eye as a “spare eye” and so relied on testing
We investigated the relationship between fixation instability amblyopic individuals when they are forced use the amblyopic eye
and amblyopic visual acuity deficits. Carpineto et al. (2007) re- alone. However, if amblyopia is primarily the result of binocular
ported that fixation instability was present in some children with dysfunction, we would also expect to see such deficits in binocular
microstrabismus and amblyopia. More recently, Gonzalez et al. task performance. We evaluated the effects of amblyopia on motor
(2012) reported significantly larger fixation areas (more insta- skills under binocular viewing conditions (O’Connor et al.,
bility) in amblyopic eyes of 13 adults compared with fellow eyes 2010a,b); specifically we evaluated performance on the Purdue
and control eye. We quantified fixation stability in 89 amblyopic pegboard (number of pegs placed in 30 s) and bead threading (time
and nonamblyopic children with strabismus, anisometropia, or to thread a set number of large and small beads in 143 individuals,
both and an age-matched normal control group using the Nidek 47 of whom had manifest strabismus and 30 of whom had
MP-1 microperimeter to acquire eye position and BCEA to quantify amblyopia (21 strabismic amblyopia; 9 anisometropic amblyopia).
fixation instability (Subramanian et al., 2012). During attempted Performance on the fine motor skills tasks was related to stereoa-
steady fixation with their amblyopic eyes, children had 3e5 cuity, with subjects with normal stereoacuity performing best on
greater fixation instability (larger BCEA areas) than when fixing all tests (Fig. 15). Individuals with amblyopia were significantly
with their fellow eyes and compared with normal controls fixing slower on both bead tasks than were those without amblyopia.
monocularly (Fig. 14). However, when the statistical analysis was repeated with stereoa-
There was a statistically significant positive correlation between cuity as a covariate, the difference between amblyopic and non-
visual acuity and BCEA (r ¼ 0.58; p < 0.0001) for amblyopic eyes; amblyopic individuals on the bead task was no longer statistically
amblyopic eyes with poorer visual acuity had greater fixation significant. This result is in agreement with a previous report that
instability and, thus, larger and wider bivariate contour ellipse fine motor skill performance was related to the level of stereoacuity
areas. Categorical severity of fixation instability has been previously but not to the presence of unilateral vision impairment in 3- to 4-
reported to be associated with depth of amblyopia (Carpineto et al., year-old children (Grant et al., 2007). Webber et al. (2008a) have
2007). On the other hand, BCEA in 13 adults with amblyopia was also reported that both the amblyopia and stereoacuity deficits
not correlated with amblyopic eye visual acuity, although inter- were associated with reduced performance on fine motor skill
ocular differences in visual acuity were correlated with BCEA tasks. Taken together, these results confirm that children with
(Gonzalez et al., 2012). Additionally, we found that amblyopic eyes binocular dysfunction have impaired fine motor skills and that this
had highly elliptical BCEAs, with a much longer horizontal than impairment is more closely related to the severity of binocular
vertical axis, consistent with abnormal development of the ocular dysfunction than to the severity of amblyopia.
motor system. Fixation instability along both the horizontal and
vertical axes was about 1.5e2 larger for amblyopic eyes than 3. Binocular treatment of amblyopia
normal control eyes. This finding is consistent reports of predom-
inantly horizontal square-wave oscillations and FMNS in amblyopic As noted above, current treatments for amblyopia are not
children (Felius et al., 2011; Felius et al., 2012). sufficient to achieve normal visual acuity for 15e50% of amblyopic
children. Even when normal visual acuity is achieved, amblyopia
2.5. Motor skills in amblyopia often recurs. Older children and adults with persistent amblyopia
are rarely treated because the predominant mindset is that decor-
Deficits in fine motor skills have been widely reported to be related binocular visual experience and habitual suppression of the
present in amblyopic individuals (Grant and Moseley, 2011). Most amblyopic eye has caused permanent loss of binocular cells and
78 E.E. Birch / Progress in Retinal and Eye Research 33 (2013) 67e84

in visual acuity (1e2 lines) (Levi, 2012). The limited improvement in


visual acuity, the dedicated time required for treatment, and the
boredom/compliance issues associated with repetition of a percep-
tual task, have limited the application of perceptual learning as
a routine treatment for amblyopia. To overcome these limitations,
research has turned toward the evaluation of video games as
a potential treatment for amblyopia in adults. The rationale is that
action video games, in particular, engage visual attention and
require demanding visual discriminations in a similar way to the
more standard perceptual learning paradigms, but is able to sustain
prolonged participation because of the story lines, varied visual
tasks, and rewards provided by the games for making correct
discriminations. A recent evaluation of an of-the-shelf action game
(Medal of Honor: Pacific Assault) found that just 20 h of play with
the fellow eye patched resulted in a mean improvement of
0.15 logMAR and that additional small gains in visual acuity
continued in some amblyopic individuals through 80 h of play
(Fig. 16) (Li et al., 2011). Interestingly, though, similar gains in visual
Fig. 15. Performance on fine motor tasks by amblyopic and nonamblyopic individuals acuity were experienced by amblyopic adults playing a non-action
with three levels of binocular function (O’Connor et al., 2010a,b). There was a signifi- video game (Sim City) or performing a simple perceptual learning
cant difference in performance on the Purdue Pegboard and the large and small bead grating acuity task while patched. Patching alone was not sufficient
tasks (p < 0.03 in all cases). Post-hoc analyses demonstrated that those with normal
stereoacuity performed significantly better than those with nil stereoacuity. When
to improve visual acuity (Fig. 16). Of course, the idea of visual
comparing those with reduced stereoacuity with those with normal stereoacuity, stimulation as a part of amblyopia is not new. For many years, eye
individuals with reduced stereoacuity were quicker than those with nil stereoacuity. care professionals have placed an emphasis on the child performing
Analysis of covariance between individuals with and without amblyopia found that “near activities while patched”; i.e., the role of active, attentive
amblyopia was associated with slower completion of both bead tasks (p < 0.04) but,
vision in amblyopia recovery has been appreciated for many years.
when the analysis was repeated with stereoacuity as a covariate the difference in
speed on the bead tasks was no longer statistically significant.
3.2. Treating binocular dysfunction amblyopia

changed the functional profile of cortical cells responsive to the


In most studies to date, perceptual learning has been applied
amblyopic eye. However, the evidence discussed in Section 2
monocularly with the same aim as patching therapy, to improve
suggests, although there is binocular dysfunction, the capacity
visual acuity of the amblyopic eye.
for binocular interaction is not absent. Moreover, interocular
Some improvements in stereoacuity have been found to
suppression of the amblyopic eye may be preventing binocular
accompany the amblyopic eye visual acuity improvement, (Levi,
interaction. Taken together, these data suggest that there is a need
2012) similar to the modest gains in stereoacuity that have been
for a new, binocular approach to amblyopia treatment. The goal is
reported to accompany successful patching therapy (Wallace et al.,
to reduce the prevalence of residual amblyopia and prevent
2011b). However, a growing appreciation of the role of decorrela-
amblyopia recurrence.
tion of binocular stimulation in suppression of the amblyopic eye
has motivated a reformulation of amblyopia treatment. Decorre-
3.1. Perceptual learning lated binocular experience may not only compromise binocular
function but also contribute to or cause the monocular deficits. In
One new approach is perceptual learning. The effect of repeated other words, we have come to view binocular dysfunction as the
practice on perceptual performance has become a focus of ambly-
opia treatment, particularly for adults and older children who have
abandoned traditional approaches to treatment. It is clear that
normal controls in the research laboratory who repeatedly practice
on a demanding visual task achieve significant and durable
improvement in their visual performance on that specific task (Levi,
2012). More relevant, is the evidence that amblyopic individuals
who practice a perceptual task improve not only on that specific
task, but also have generalized improvement when tested with
other perceptual tasks (Levi, 2012).
Perceptual learning as a treatment for adult amblyopia has
typically been conducted with the fellow eye patched. The visual
task for training is designed to engage the amblyopic individual in
making fine discriminations and to provide repeated exposure over
many hours. It has been suggested that perceptual learning is simply
an extension of the traditional effects of patching therapy, where the
amblyopic individual must accomplish many visual discriminations
in everyday life. The argument that perceptual learning succeeds
where everyday experience fails in amblyopic adults is that the less
plastic brain of the adult requires “attention and action using the
Fig. 16. Improvement in visual acuity with patching in 18 amblyopic adults while
amblyopic eye, supervised with feedback” in order to provide playing an action video game or a non-action video game for 20 h. Also shown is the
effective treatment (Levi, 2012). Even with 40e50 h of perceptual visual acuity improvement achieved with 20 h of patching while practicing a grating
learning, most adults achieve only 0.1e0.2 logMAR improvements acuity task (perceptual learning; PL) and with patching alone for 20 h (Li et al., 2011).
E.E. Birch / Progress in Retinal and Eye Research 33 (2013) 67e84 79

primary deficit and the monocular visual acuity deficit as Tetris game to an iPod platform to allow for more comfortable play
secondary. In this framework, patching, with or without perceptual and play at home. They evaluated the effects of dichoptic Tetris
learning treatment, treats the secondary monocular visual deficits practice (with low contrast to the fellow eye) in 10 amblyopic adults
but does not directly address the primary binocular deficit. We and found found a median visual acuity improvement of 0.2 log-
wanted to find a new approach to treatment for amblyopia that MAR after 10e19 one hour sessions (Fig. 17); 7 of the 10 adults also
would not only improve visual acuity of the amblyopic eye but also had improvements in stereoacuity. Taken together, these data
provide repeated, correlated binocular visual experience. Unless support the hypothesis that a binocular approach to treatment of
the binocular dysfunction is treated, abnormal binocular vision amblyopia can be successful. Repeated experience with dichoptic
may result in residual amblyopia, and may trigger recurrence of perceptual tasks led to modest improvements in monocular visual
amblyopia. acuity and, in some cases, improved stereopsis.
Reducing contrast to the fellow eye to equate the visibility To date, this approach largely has been limited to the laboratory,
between amblyopic and fellow eyes can allow binocular contrast with the use of video game goggles and the need for administration
summation (Baker et al., 2007), and suprathreshold binocular of practice sessions by trained staff, and limited to adults and
interactions in motion coherence and orientation discrimination school-age children. We wanted to extend binocular treatment to
tasks (Mansouri et al., 2008) to occur in at least some amblyopic the preschool age range, to be used as an adjunct treatment along
individuals. This suggests that the absence of binocular interactions with patching (at separate times of day) to reduce residual
in amblyopia reported in the literature may have been due to the amblyopia and to reduce risk for recurrence of amblyopia. The
imbalance in the monocular signals rather than to an absence of extension to an iPod platform and lenticular display was a step
capacity for binocular interaction. Hess et al. (2010) reported toward incorporating dichoptic training into clinical settings for
improvement in visual acuity and binocular vision following adults and school-age children. However, it is difficult to extend the
several weeks of practice with a dichoptic motion coherence task lenticular iPod to preschool children because the iPod requires fine
presented via video gaming goggles. For the initial practice session, motor skills and the lenticular display requires precise and steady
the contrast of the randomly moving dots in the fellow eye was head position to provide dichoptic stimulation. We worked with
reduced to about 10% while the amblyopic eye coherent dots were Hess and To to adapt the dichoptic Tetris game for use on the larger
presented at 100% contrast. This allowed the amblyopic eye to iPad format, which is more compatible with the immature fine
“break-through” and observers were able to experience binocular motor skills of young children, and anaglyphic dichoptic separation,
vision. As the 10 amblyopic adults began to experience binocular which is not dependent on head position. We are currently
vision, the contrast in the fellow eye was gradually increased during studying the effects of anaglyphic dichoptic training on amblyopia
3 or 4 sessions per week for 3e5 weeks until the two eyes worked and risk for recurrence in 3- to 5-, 6- to 8-, and 9- to 11-year old
together with more evenly matched contrasts. In addition to children with strabismic, anisometropic, or combined mechanism
improvements in binocular combination, visual acuity improved amblyopia (Birch et al., 2013). Many of the children we are studying
0.1e0.7 logMAR (Fig. 17) and stereoacuity improved in 6 of the 10 were enrolled in our prospective longitudinal study of visual
adults. Knox et al. (2012) used the same device to administer one development at time of initial diagnosis of strabismus or aniso-
week of dichoptic treatment but used a more interesting video metropia. For these children we have detailed histories of visual
game task (Tetris). As with the motion coherence task, the contrast acuity, treatment regimens, and response to treatment. An example
of the fellow eye image was reduced to allow binocular vision to of a child’s visual acuity history and visual acuity improvement
occur. Five 1-h practice sessions during a one week were provided. with the anaglyphic dichoptic training are shown in Fig. 18. While
The 14 amblyopic children (5e11 years old) has a median
improvement of 0.09 logMAR in visual acuity (about 1 line) and 9 of
the 14 also had improvements in stereoacuity (Fig. 17). Most
recently Hess et al. (2012) and To et al. (2011) moved the dichoptic

Fig. 18. Visual acuity data from a child with anisometropic amblyopia before and after
dichoptic training (Birch et al., 2013). On the initial visit at 4.4 years old, best corrected
visual acuity for the amblyopic eye was 0.9 logMAR. Treatment with glasses, patching,
atropine, and Bangerter filter resulted in improvement to 0.5e0.6 logMAR. Slight
Fig. 17. Visual acuity improvement following 5e20 h of dichoptic training to ambly- regression to 0.6e0.7 logMAR occurred, and visual acuity remained in this range for
opic individuals using video game goggles to make motion coherence judgments (Hess the next 3 years with continued spectacle wear. At 9.9 years, the child enrolled in the
et al., 2010) or play Tetris (Knox et al., 2012) or using a lenticular overlay on an iPod to dichoptic treatment study and showed 0.2 logMAR improvement to visual acuity of
play Tetris (Hess et al., 2012; To et al., 2011). Despite the pilot studies having relatively 0.4 logMAR over the next 8 weeks. Three months after discontinuing dichoptic
short durations of dichoptic training and small cohorts, all 3 studies showed significant treatment, visual acuity for the amblyopic eye remained at 0.4 logMAR. Visual acuity of
improvement in visual acuity and stereoacuity. the fellow eye remained at 0.1 to 0.0 logMAR from age 7.4 yearse10.4 years.
80 E.E. Birch / Progress in Retinal and Eye Research 33 (2013) 67e84

still preliminary, our results from 23 children are consistent with Table 2
the data from the published laboratory studies of small cohorts of Reported success rate for vision screening of preschool children by pediatricians and
family care practitioners.
amblyopic adults and school-age children. Many of the children
have improved stereoacuity after only 15e20 h of dichoptic Study 2-Year-olds 3-Year-olds 4-Year-olds 5-Year-olds
practice, including some who achieve coarse stereopsis for the Pediatric Research in 0% 38% 71% 81%
first time. In addition, there is an improvement in visual acuity of Office Settings
(Wasserman et al.,
0.1e0.4 logMAR. That rehabilitation of binocular interaction is
1992)
accompanied by improved monocular visual acuity supports the Kemper and Clark (2006) 0% 38% 56% 73%
hypothesis that binocular dysfunction plays a pivotal role in Wall et al. (2002) 0% 37% 79% 91%
amblyopia, an appreciation of which is not currently reflected in Marsh-Tootle et al. (2008) 0% 12% 25% 48%
clinical practice. Kemper et al. (2011) 0% 43% 64% 73%
Range 0% 12e43% 25e79% 48e91%
Mean 0% 34% 59% 73%
4. Future directions

As summarized above, there is accumulating evidence that Study (MEPEDS) and the Baltimore Pediatric Eye Study (BPEDS)
amblyopia and binocular function are linked and several note- (N ¼ 5710), only 14% of children with 1.00 D anisometropia had
worthy clues that binocular dysfunction is primary and monocular amblyopia and only 60% of children with 2.00 D anisometropia
visual acuity loss is secondary. This new understanding of ambly- had amblyopia; (Cotter et al., 2011) i.e., screening for anisometropia
opia provides a foundation for a broadened scope of research and and will have low specificity for amblyopia. In addition, amblyopia
for crafting more effective treatments. The last 15 years has seen an risk factor analysis failed to identify hyperopia a significant risk
explosion of randomized clinical trials to evaluate treatments for factor for unilateral amblyopia (Tarczy-Hornoch et al., 2011).
children with amblyopia, and a shift from treatment based on Because refraction screening devices cannot directly detect stra-
consensus to evidence-based treatment. Nonetheless, there remain bismus or amblyopia, many “at risk” children are referred for
many unanswered questions about amblyopia screening, diagnosis, comprehensive ophthalmic examinations at considerable cost in
and treatment. time and money to the family. Once these “at risk” children are
referred, they often are “monitored” with regularly scheduled
4.1. Screening for amblyopia ophthalmic examinations for years, even though the data suggest
that only 9%e18% of the children “at risk” will ever develop
Screening for amblyopia is a part of the regular well-child visit amblyopia.
recommended by the American Academy of Pediatrics. Screening Recently, Hunter and colleagues developed an alternative
by a pediatrician or family care practitioner can identify amblyopia approach to screening for amblyopia, a binocular birefringence
at a time when treatment is most effective. Screening improves scanner called the “Pediatric Vision Scanner” or “PVS” (Hunter
vision outcomes. In a population-based, randomized longitudinal et al., 2004; Loudon et al., 2011; Nassif et al., 2004; Nassif et al.,
study, repeated early screening resulted in a 60% decreased prev- 2006). Simultaneous retinal birefringence scans of both eyes
alence of amblyopia and improved visual acuity outcome at age detect whether fixation of a target is foveal (central), steady, and
7 years compared with surveillance only until school entry maintained by identifying the unique polarization signal created by
(Williams and Harrad, 2006; Williams et al., 2001; Williams et al., the radially arranged Henle fibers (photoreceptor axons) that
2008). Amblyopia can be treated more effectively when treated emanate from the fovea(Hunter et al., 2004; Loudon et al., 2011;
early; the same study found a 70% lower prevalence of residual Nassif et al., 2004). In a pilot study of the PVS, Loudon et al. (2011)
amblyopia after treatment when therapy was initiated before age reported that all normal control children had at least 4 of 5 scans
3 years (Williams et al., 2001; Williams et al., 2002, 2003) However, consistent with bifoveal fixation, and that 62 of 64 amblyopic
pediatricians and family care practitioners experience considerable children failed to exhibit birefringence (97% sensitivity to ambly-
difficulties in screening for amblyopia (Tingley, 2007). Most opia). The PVS was able to detect amblyopia even in the 22 children
primary care providers in the United States still use visual acuity with anisometropic amblyopia and no strabismus (20/22;
charts as their primary approach to vision screening for amblyopia sensitivity ¼ 91%) and birefringence was normal in children with
(Wall et al., 2002). Unfortunately, reading letters or numbers on anisometropia without amblyopia (11/12; specificity ¼ 92%). We
a visual acuity chart requires a verbal, cooperative child and is not have independently confirmed these PVS results in anisometropic
routinely successful in children under 5 years old, too late for the children with no strabismus (Yanni et al., 2012). Moreover, these
most effective therapy. When the testing is conducted earlier, data are consistent with the association between fixation instability
nonstandard symbols or chart formats are used and often they are and amblyopia in both anisometropic and strabismic individuals
not presented in the linear or crowded formats needed to detect (Section 2.4) (Birch et al., 2012; Subramanian et al., 2012). Taken
mild amblyopia. Table 2 summarizes recent studies that document together, the pilot data suggest that binocular birefringence
how these difficulties impact the vision screening provided by screening could provide more accurate identification of children
primary care physicians to preschool children. who need ophthalmological intervention.
Automated devices, like the SureSightÒ and PlusOptixÒ have
been developed to try to improve upon visual acuity testing, by 4.2. Diagnosis of amblyopia
providing a simple approach to screening for refractive error risk
factors, rather than screening for amblyopia directly. These devices Diagnosis of amblyopia relies on the measurement of visual
are attractive because they are designed for use by pediatric nurses, acuity. Reduced best corrected visual acuity in presence of an
pediatric medical assistants, and even lay people. However, these amblyogenic factor (usually strabismus or anisometropia) with no
automated devices miss many children with amblyopia and many other ocular or visual cortical abnormality is currently the critical
normal children are sent to the ophthalmologist for an unnecessary component of amblyopia diagnosis. However, most children
eye examination, further increasing the cost of health care. For <3 years old and some older children cannot complete a visual
example, in the combined population-based cohorts of 2- to 6-year acuity test. Instead, the clinical diagnosis of amblyopia in children
old children from the NIH-sponsored Multi-Ethnic Pediatric Eye <3 years old must rely on fixation preference.
E.E. Birch / Progress in Retinal and Eye Research 33 (2013) 67e84 81

Recently, fixation preference has been disparaged as a method centuries, have traditionally focused on forcing use of the ambly-
for assessing amblyopia. The basis for questioning the value of opic eye to recover monocular visual acuity.
fixation preference is found in two large (n ¼ 9970) population- Although this approach has had substantial success, and is
based screening studies, MEPEDS and BPEDS (Cotter et al., 2009; supported by recent evidence from randomized clinical trials of
Friedman et al., 2008). Both included primarily normal children; patching and atropine treatment, many amblyopic individuals are
only 44 had 2 lines interocular difference in visual acuity. With left with residual visual acuity deficits, ocular motor abnormalities,
such a small sample of primarily mild amblyopia, it is not surprising deficient fine motor skills, and high risk for recurrent amblyopia.
that they reported low sensitivity of fixation preference. Only Converging evidence points to the pivotal role of decorrelated
a handful had strabismus, so most fixation preference testing relied binocular experience in the genesis of amblyopia and the associ-
on the more difficult induced tropia (prism) test. Another limitation ated residual deficits. These findings suggest that a new treatment
was that fixation preference was tested without optical correction approach designed to treat the binocular dysfunction as the
while visual acuity was tested with optical correction. Given these primary deficit in amblyopia may be needed.
limitations, it is not surprising that they reported low specificity.
Data collected from clinical cohorts support much higher sensi- Conflicts of interest
tivity and specificity for detection amblyopia by fixation preference
in cohorts of children with strabismus (sensitivity 73%e93%; None.
specificity 78%e97%) (Birch and Holmes, 2010; Kothari et al.,
2009; Procianoy and Procianoy, 2010; Sener et al., 2002; Wright Acknowledgments
et al., 1986) As noted in Section 1.2, 95% of amblyopic children
<3 years old have strabismus, (Birch and Holmes, 2010) so we This work was supported by grants from the National Eye
anticipate high sensitivity and specificity of fixation preference in Institute (EY05236 and EY022313), Thrasher Research Fund, Pearle
this age range where visual acuity cannot be tested with standard Vision Foundation, One Sight Foundation, Knights Templar, and
optotypes. Whether small changes in fixation preference can be Fight for Sight. The research depended on many important
used to track treatment response remains an open question. Given contributions by past and present post-doctoral fellows, the Dallas-
the scarcity of evidence for treatment effects and risks of amblyopia area pediatric ophthalmologists who referred patients to the
treatment (e.g., reverse amblyopia) in the <3 year age range, it is research projects and collaborated in data collection and analysis,
imperative to answer this question before we can begin to build an research assistants, and interns. This research relied on the many
evidence base for treatment of amblyopia in children <3 years old. contributions of the families of amblyopic children who have
participated in these studies.
4.3. Optimizing treatment for amblyopia
References
The cornerstone of amblyopia treatment is patching the “fellow”
Al-Haddad, C.E., Mollayess, G.M., Cherfan, C.G., Jaafar, D.F., Bashshur, Z.F., 2011.
eye to force use of the amblyopic eye. Recent randomized clinical Retinal nerve fibre layer and macular thickness in amblyopia as measured by
trials have provided and extensive knowledge base for treatment of spectral-domain optical coherence tomography. Br. J. Ophthalmol. 95, 1696e
amblyopia in children over 3 years old (Holmes et al., 2006; Repka 1699.
Altintas, O., Yuksel, N., Ozkan, B., Caglar, Y., 2005. Thickness of the retinal nerve fiber
and Holmes, 2012; Stewart et al., 2011). Nonetheless, the high layer, macular thickness, and macular volume in patients with strabismic
prevalence of persistent residual amblyopia and recurrent ambly- amblyopia. J. Pediatr. Ophthalmol. Strabismus 42, 216e221.
opia underscore the need for more effective treatments. Our American Academy of Ophthalmology, 2011. Pediatric Ophthalmology and Stra-
bismus. American Academy of Ophthalmology, San Francisco, CA.
current focus is to provide regular binocular experience to treat Baker, D.H., Meese, T.S., Mansouri, B., Hess, R.F., 2007. Binocular summation of
binocular dysfunction by adjusting the contrast of dichoptic images contrast remains intact in strabismic amblyopia. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 48,
to balance sensitivity differences between the amblyopic and 5332e5338.
Birch, E.E., Holmes, J.M., 2010. The clinical profile of amblyopia in children younger
fellow eyes and allow binocular interaction (Section 3.2). However,
than 3 years of age. J. AAPOS 14, 494e497.
it is improbable that sensitivity differences are the only limiting Birch, E.E., Stager Sr., D.R., 2006. Long-term motor and sensory outcomes after early
factor. Other approaches to treating binocular dysfunction, tailored surgery for infantile esotropia. J. AAPOS 10, 409e413.
Birch, E.E., Swanson, W.H., 2000. Hyperacuity deficits in anisometropic and stra-
to the specific constellation of deficits present in the amblyopic
bismic amblyopes with known ages of onset. Vis. Res. 40, 1035e1040.
individual may be more effective. In addition, it may prove Birch, E.E., Stager, D.R., Berry, P., Everett, M.E., 1990. Prospective assessment of
impossible to fully restore binocular interaction. Such an outcome acuity and stereopsis in amblyopic infantile esotropes following early surgery.
would shift the emphasis toward amblyopia prevention. We know, Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 31, 758e765.
Birch, E., Stager, D., Wright, K., Beck, R., 1998. The natural history of infantile eso-
for example, that early surgery for infantile esotropia can preserve tropia during the first six months of life. Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator
binocular function and reduce the risk for persistent amblyopia Group. J. AAPOS 2, 325e328. discussion 329.
(Birch and Stager, 2006; Birch et al., 2004). Clinical pearls, such as Birch, E.E., Fawcett, S., Stager, D.R., 2000. Why does early surgical alignment
improve stereoacuity outcomes in infantile esotropia? J. AAPOS 4, 10e14.
the reduced risk for recurrent amblyopia when patching therapy is Birch, E.E., Stager Sr., D.R., Berry, P., Leffler, J., 2004. Stereopsis and long-term
tapered rather simply halted when the child attains equal visual stability of alignment in esotropia. J. AAPOS 8, 146e150.
acuities, may also point to new insights for the design of effective Birch, E.E., Fawcett, S.L., Morale, S.E., Weakley Jr., D.R., Wheaton, D.H., 2005. Risk
factors for accommodative esotropia among hypermetropic children. Invest.
binocular treatments. Clearly, there is a need for a broader under- Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 46, 526e529.
standing of the effects of decorrelated binocular visual experience Birch, E.E., Stager Sr., D.R., Stager Jr., D.R., Berry, P.M., 2007. Risk factors for esotropic
on the developing sensory and motor systems, the role of amblyopia. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 48, e1108.
Birch, E.E., Stager Sr., D.R., Wang, J., O’Connor, A., 2010. Longitudinal changes in
suppression in various forms of amblyopia, and the contribution of
refractive error of children with infantile esotropia. Eye (Lond.) 24, 1814e1821.
binocular dysfunction to residual and recurrent esotropia. Birch, E.E., Subramanian, V., Weakley, D.R., 2012. Is the “flick” in anisometropia
a sign of microstrabismus or fixation instability? J. AAPOS 16, e2.
Birch, E.E., Subramanian, V., Jost, S., Jost, R., Stager Sr., D.R., Stager Jr., D.R., Dao, L.,
5. Summary 2013. Binocular iPad treatment for amblyopia in children. J. AAPOS 17, e2.
Birch, E.E., 2003. Marshall Parks lecture. Binocular sensory outcomes in accom-
modative ET. J. AAPOS 7, 369e373.
Amblyopia is widely viewed as a monocular disorder and Bosworth, R.G., Birch, E.E., 2003. Binocular function and optotype-grating acuity
treatments for amblyopia, many of which have been used for discrepancies in amblyopic children. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 44, e3183.
82 E.E. Birch / Progress in Retinal and Eye Research 33 (2013) 67e84

Carpineto, P., Ciancaglini, M., Nubile, M., Di Marzio, G., Toto, L., Di Antonio, L., Holmes, J.M., Beck, R.W., Kraker, R.T., Astle, W.F., Birch, E.E., Cole, S.R., Cotter, S.A.,
Mastropasqua, L., 2007. Fixation patterns evaluation by means of MP-1 micro- Donahue, S., Everett, D.F., Hertle, R.W., Keech, R.V., Paysse, E., Quinn, G.F.,
perimeter in microstrabismic children treated for unilateral amblyopia. Eur. J. Repka, M.X., Scheiman, M.M., 2004. Risk of amblyopia recurrence after cessa-
Ophthalmol. 17, 885e890. tion of treatment. J. AAPOS 8, 420e428.
Choong, Y.F., Lukman, H., Martin, S., Laws, D.E., 2004. Childhood amblyopia treat- Holmes, J.M., Repka, M.X., Kraker, R.T., Clarke, M.P., 2006. The treatment of
ment: psychosocial implications for patients and primary carers. Eye (Lond.) 18, amblyopia. Strabismus 14, 37e42.
369e375. Holmes, J.M., Lazar, E.L., Melia, B.M., Astle, W.F., Dagi, L.R., Donahue, S.P.,
Chua, B., Mitchell, P., 2004. Consequences of amblyopia on education, occupation, Frazier, M.G., Hertle, R.W., Repka, M.X., Quinn, G.E., Weise, K.K., 2011. Effect of
and long term vision loss. Br. J. Ophthalmol. 88, 1119e1121. age on response to amblyopia treatment in children. Arch. Ophthalmol. 129,
Cotter, S.A., Edwards, A.R., Wallace, D.K., Beck, R.W., Arnold, R.W., Astle, W.F., 1451e1457.
Barnhardt, C.N., Birch, E.E., Donahue, S.P., Everett, D.F., Felius, J., Holmes, J.M., Horwood, J., Waylen, A., Herrick, D., Williams, C., Wolke, D., 2005. Common visual
Kraker, R.T., Melia, M., Repka, M.X., Sala, N.A., Silbert, D.I., Weise, K.K., 2006. defects and peer victimization in children. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 46,
Treatment of anisometropic amblyopia in children with refractive correction. 1177e1181.
Ophthalmology 113, 895e903. Hunter, D.G., Nassif, D.S., Piskun, N.V., Winsor, R., Gramatikov, B.I., Guyton, D.L.,
Cotter, S.A., Tarczy-Hornoch, K., Song, E., Lin, J., Borchert, M., Azen, S.P., Varma, R., 2004. Pediatric Vision Screener 1: instrument design and operation. J. Biomed.
2009. Fixation preference and visual acuity testing in a population-based Opt. 9, 1363e1368.
cohort of preschool children with amblyopia risk factors. Ophthalmology Huynh, S.C., Wang, X.Y., Ip, J., Robaei, D., Kifley, A., Rose, K.A., Mitchell, P., 2006.
116, 145e153. Prevalence and associations of anisometropia and aniso-astigmatism in a pop-
Cotter, S.A., Varma, R., Tarczy-Hornoch, K., McKean-Cowdin, R., Lin, J., Wen, G., ulation based sample of 6 year old children. Br. J. Ophthalmol. 90, 597e601.
Wei, J., Borchert, M., Azen, S.P., Torres, M., Tielsch, J.M., Friedman, D.S., Huynh, S.C., Samarawickrama, C., Wang, X.Y., Rochtchina, E., Wong, T.Y., Gole, G.A.,
Repka, M.X., Katz, J., Ibironke, J., Giordano, L., 2011. Risk factors associated with Rose, K.A., Mitchell, P., 2009. Macular and nerve fiber layer thickness in
childhood strabismus: the multi-ethnic pediatric eye disease and Baltimore amblyopia: the Sydney Childhood Eye Study. Ophthalmology 116, 1604e1609.
pediatric eye disease studies. Ophthalmology 118, 2251e2261. Ing, M.R., Okino, L.M., 2002. Outcome study of stereopsis in relation to duration of
Cotter, S.A., Foster, N.C., Holmes, J.M., Melia, B.M., Wallace, D.K., Repka, M.X., misalignment in congenital esotropia. J. AAPOS 6, 3e8.
Tamkins, S.M., Kraker, R.T., Beck, R.W., Hoover, D.L., Crouch 3rd, E.R., Ing, M.R., 1991. Early surgery for essential infantile esotropia. J. Pediatr. Ophthalmol.
Miller, A.M., Morse, C.L., Suh, D.W., 2012. Optical treatment of strabismic and Strabismus 28, 119.
combined strabismic-anisometropic amblyopia. Ophthalmology 119, 150e158. Kemper, A.R., Clark, S.J., 2006. Preschool vision screening in pediatric practices. Clin.
Dickmann, A., Petroni, S., Salerni, A., Dell’Omo, R., Balestrazzi, E., 2009. Unilateral Pediatr. (Phila) 45, 263e266.
amblyopia: an optical coherence tomography study. J. AAPOS 13, 148e150. Kemper, A.R., Wallace, D.K., Patel, N., Crews, J.E., 2011. Preschool vision testing by
Donnelly, U.M., Stewart, N.M., Hollinger, M., 2005. Prevalence and outcomes of health providers in the United States: findings from the 2006e2007 Medical
childhood visual disorders. Ophthalmic Epidemiol. 12, 243e250. Expenditure Panel Survey. J. AAPOS 15, 480e483.
Fawcett, S.L., Birch, E.E., 2003. Risk factors for abnormal binocular vision after Kiorpes, L., McKee, S.P., 1999. Neural mechanisms underlying amblyopia. Curr. Opin.
successful alignment of accommodative esotropia. J. AAPOS 7, 256e262. Neurobiol. 9, 480e486.
Fawcett, S., Leffler, J., Birch, E.E., 2000. Factors influencing stereoacuity in accom- Kiorpes, L., Kiper, D.C., O’Keefe, L.P., Cavanaugh, J.R., Movshon, J.A., 1998. Neuronal
modative esotropia. J. AAPOS 4, 15e20. correlates of amblyopia in the visual cortex of macaque monkeys with exper-
Felius, J., Hertle, R.W., Wang, J., Subramanian, V., Jost, R., Berry, P.M., Birch, E.E., 2011. imental strabismus and anisometropia. J. Neurosci. 18, 6411e6424.
Ocular motor analysis of monocular visual preference in children with ambly- Knox, P.J., Simmers, A.J., Gray, L.S., Cleary, M., 2012. An exploratory study: prolonged
opia. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 52, e4690. periods of binocular stimulation can provide an effective treatment for child-
Felius, J., Jost, R., Stager Sr., D.R., 2012. Interocular asymmetries in square-wave hood amblyopia. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 53, 817e824.
oscillations in children with amblyopia. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 53, e3898. Kothari, M., Bhaskare, A., Mete, D., Toshniwal, S., Doshi, P., Kaul, S., 2009. Evaluation
Friedman, D.S., Katz, J., Repka, M.X., Giordano, L., Ibironke, J., Hawse, P., Tielsch, J.M., of central, steady, maintained fixation grading for predicting inter-eye visual
2008. Lack of concordance between fixation preference and HOTV optotype acuity difference to diagnose and treat amblyopia in strabismic patients. Indian
visual acuity in preschool children: the Baltimore Pediatric Eye Disease Study. J. Ophthalmol. 57, 281e284.
Ophthalmology 115, 1796e1799. Kvarnstrom, G., Jakobsson, P., Lennerstrand, G., 2001. Visual screening of Swedish
Friedman, D.S., Repka, M.X., Katz, J., Giordano, L., Ibironke, J., Hawse, P., Tielsch, J.M., children: an ophthalmological evaluation. Acta Ophthalmol. Scand. 79, 240e
2009. Prevalence of amblyopia and strabismus in white and African American 244.
children aged 6 through 71 months the Baltimore Pediatric Eye Disease Study. Lee, H.J., Kee, C., 2009. Optical coherence tomography and Heidelberg retina
Ophthalmology 116, 2128e2134. e2121e2122. tomography for superior segmental optic hypoplasia. Br. J. Ophthalmol. 93,
Fu, V.L., Norcia, A.M., Birch, E.E., et al., 2006. Fusional suppression in accommodative 1468e1473.
and infantile esotropia. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 47, e2450. Lempert, P., Porter, L., 1998. Dysversion of the optic disc and axial length
Giaschi, D.E., Regan, D., Kraft, S.P., Hong, X.H., 1992. Defective processing of motion- measurements in a presumed amblyopic population. J. AAPOS 2, 207e213.
defined form in the fellow eye of patients with unilateral amblyopia. Invest. Lempert, P., 2000. Optic nerve hypoplasia and small eyes in presumed amblyopia.
Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 33, 2483e2489. J. AAPOS 4, 258e266.
Giordano, L., Friedman, D.S., Repka, M.X., Katz, J., Ibironke, J., Hawes, P., Tielsch, J.M., Lempert, P., 2003. Axial length-disc area ratio in esotropic amblyopia. Arch. Oph-
2009. Prevalence of refractive error among preschool children in an urban thalmol. 121, 821e824.
population: the Baltimore Pediatric Eye Disease Study. Ophthalmology 116, Lempert, P., 2004. The axial length/disc area ratio in anisometropic hyperopic
739e746. 746 e731e734. amblyopia: a hypothesis for decreased unilateral vision associated with
Gonzalez, E.G., Wong, A.M., Niechwiej-Szwedo, E., Tarita-Nistor, L., Steinbach, M.J., hyperopic anisometropia. Ophthalmology 111, 304e308.
2012. Eye position stability in amblyopia and in normal binocular vision. Invest. Lempert, P., 2008a. Optic disc area and retinal area in amblyopia. Semin. Oph-
Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 53, 5386e5394. thalmol. 23, 302e306.
Grant, S., Moseley, M.J., 2011. Amblyopia and real-world visuomotor tasks. Stra- Lempert, P., 2008b. Retinal area and optic disc rim area in amblyopic, fellow, and
bismus 19, 119e128. normal hyperopic eyes: a hypothesis for decreased acuity in amblyopia.
Grant, S., Melmoth, D.R., Morgan, M.J., Finlay, A.L., 2007. Prehension deficits in Ophthalmology 115, 2259e2261.
amblyopia. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 48, 1139e1148. Levi, D.M., Carkeet, A., 1993. Amblyopia: a consequence of abnormal visual devel-
Groenewoud, J.H., Tjiam, A.M., Lantau, V.K., Hoogeveen, W.C., de Faber, J.T., opment. In: Simons, K. (Ed.), Early Visual Development: Normal and Abnormal.
Juttmann, R.E., de Koning, H.J., Simonsz, H.J., 2010. Rotterdam AMblyopia Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 391e405.
screening effectiveness study: detection and causes of amblyopia in a large Levi, D.M., Klein, S., 1982. Hyperacuity and amblyopia. Nature 298, 268e270.
birth cohort. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 51, 3476e3484. Levi, D.M., Klein, S.A., 1985. Vernier acuity, crowding and amblyopia. Vis. Res. 25,
Harrad, R., Williams, C., 2003. Risk, causes and outcomes of visual impairment after 979e991.
loss of vision in the non-amblyopic eye, a population-based study, by J. S. Rahi, Levi, D.M., McKee, S.P., Movshon, J.A., 2011. Visual deficits in anisometropia. Vis. Res.
S. Logan, C. Timms, I. Russel-Eggitt, and D. Taylor. Lancet 360:597e602, 2002. 51, 48e57.
Surv. Ophthalmol. 48, 235. Levi, D., 1990. Visual acuity in strabismic and anisometropic amblyopia. Oph-
Harrad, R., Sengpiel, F., Blakemore, C., 1996. Physiology of suppression in strabismic thalmol. Clin. North America 3, 289e301.
amblyopia. Br. J. Ophthalmol. 80, 373e377. Levi, D.M., 2012. Prentice award lecture 2011: removing the brakes on plasticity in
Hess, R.F., Mansouri, B., Thompson, B., 2010. A new binocular approach to the the amblyopic brain. Optom. Vis. Sci. 89, 827e838.
treatment of amblyopia in adults well beyond the critical period of visual Li, R.W., Ngo, C., Nguyen, J., Levi, D.M., 2011. Video-game play induces plasticity in
development. Restore Neurol. Neurosci. 28, 793e802. the visual system of adults with amblyopia. PLoS Biol. 9, e1001135.
Hess, R.F., Thompson, B., Black, J.M., Maehara, G., Zhang, P., Bobier, W.R., To, L., Loudon, S.E., Simonsz, H.J., 2005. The history of the treatment of amblyopia. Stra-
Cooperstock, J., February 2012. An iPod treatment of amblyopia: an updated bismus 13, 93e106.
binocular approach. Optometry. http://viewer.zmags.com/publication/ Loudon, S.E., Polling, J.R., Simonsz, H.J., 2002. A preliminary report about the rela-
95f199fd-/95f199fd/28. tion between visual acuity increase and compliance in patching therapy for
Holmes, J.M., Kraker, R.T., Beck, R.W., Birch, E.E., Cotter, S.A., Everett, D.F., amblyopia. Strabismus 10, 79e82.
Hertle, R.W., Quinn, G.E., Repka, M.X., Scheiman, M.M., Wallace, D.K., 2003. Loudon, S.E., Polling, J.R., Simonsz, H.J., 2003. Electronically measured compliance
A randomized trial of prescribed patching regimens for treatment of severe with occlusion therapy for amblyopia is related to visual acuity increase.
amblyopia in children. Ophthalmology 110, 2075e2087. Graefes Arch. Clin. Exp. Ophthalmol. 241, 176e180.
E.E. Birch / Progress in Retinal and Eye Research 33 (2013) 67e84 83

Loudon, S.E., Fronius, M., Looman, C.W., Awan, M., Simonsz, B., van der Maas, P.J., Sener, E.C., Mocan, M.C., Gedik, S., Ergin, A., Sanac, A.S., 2002. The reliability of
Simonsz, H.J., 2006. Predictors and a remedy for noncompliance with ambly- grading the fixation preference test for the assessment of interocular visual
opia therapy in children measured with the occlusion dose monitor. Invest. acuity differences in patients with strabismus. J. AAPOS 6, 191e194.
Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 47, 4393e4400. Shaw, D.E., Fielder, A.R., Minshull, C., Rosenthal, A.R., 1988. Amblyopiaefactors
Loudon, S.E., Rook, C.A., Nassif, D.S., Piskun, N.V., Hunter, D.G., 2011. Rapid, high- influencing age of presentation. Lancet 2, 207e209.
accuracy detection of strabismus and amblyopia using the pediatric vision Stewart, C.E., Fielder, A.R., Stephens, D.A., Moseley, M.J., 2002. Design of the
scanner. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 52, 5043e5048. Monitored Occlusion Treatment of Amblyopia Study (MOTAS). Br. J. Oph-
Mansouri, B., Thompson, B., Hess, R.F., 2008. Measurement of suprathreshold thalmol. 86, 915e919.
binocular interactions in amblyopia. Vis. Res. 48, 2775e2784. Stewart, C.E., Moseley, M.J., Fielder, A.R., Stephens, D.A., 2004a. Refractive adapta-
Marsh-Tootle, W.L., Wall, T.C., Tootle, J.S., Person, S.D., Kristofco, R.E., 2008. Quan- tion in amblyopia: quantification of effect and implications for practice. Br. J.
titative pediatric vision screening in primary care settings in Alabama. Optom. Ophthalmol. 88, 1552e1556.
Vis. Sci. 85, 849e856. Stewart, C.E., Moseley, M.J., Stephens, D.A., Fielder, A.R., 2004b. Treatment dose-
McKee, S.P., Harrad, R.A., 1993. Fusional suppression in normal and stereo- response in amblyopia therapy: the Monitored Occlusion Treatment of
anomalous observers. Vis. Res. 33, 1645e1658. Amblyopia Study (MOTAS). Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 45, 3048e3054.
McKee, S.P., Levi, D.M., Movshon, J.A., 2003. The pattern of visual deficits in Stewart, C.E., Fielder, A.R., Stephens, D.A., Moseley, M.J., 2005. Treatment of
amblyopia. J. Vis. 3, 380e405. unilateral amblyopia: factors influencing visual outcome. Invest. Ophthalmol.
Multi-Ethnic Pediatric Eye Disease Study, 2008. Prevalence of amblyopia and stra- Vis. Sci. 46, 3152e3160.
bismus in African American and Hispanic children ages 6 to 72 months the Stewart, C.E., Stephens, D.A., Fielder, A.R., Moseley, M.J., 2007a. Modeling dose-
multi-ethnic pediatric eye disease study. Ophthalmology 115, 1229e1236. response in amblyopia: toward a child-specific treatment plan. Invest. Oph-
e1221. thalmol. Vis. Sci. 48, 2589e2594.
Nassif, D.S., Piskun, N.V., Gramatikov, B.I., Guyton, D.L., Hunter, D.G., 2004. Pediatric Stewart, C.E., Stephens, D.A., Fielder, A.R., Moseley, M.J., 2007b. Objectively monitored
Vision Screener 2: pilot study in adults. J. Biomed. Opt. 9, 1369e1374. patching regimens for treatment of amblyopia: randomised trial. BMJ 335, 707.
Nassif, D.S., Piskun, N.V., Hunter, D.G., 2006. The Pediatric Vision Screener III: Stewart, C.E., Moseley, M.J., Fielder, A.R., 2011. Amblyopia therapy: an update.
detection of strabismus in children. Arch. Ophthalmol. 124, 509e513. Strabismus 19, 91e98.
O’Connor, A.R., Birch, E.E., Spencer, R., 2009. Factors affecting development of motor Subramanian, V., Jost, R., Birch, E.E., 2012. A quantitative study of fixation stability in
skills in extremely low birth weight children. Strabismus 17, 20e23. amblyopia. J. AAPOS 15, e30.
O’Connor, A.R., Birch, E.E., Anderson, S., Draper, H., 2010a. The functional signifi- Subramanian, S., Wang, J., Hertle, R.W., Felius, J., Birch, E.E. Eye Movement, optic
cance of stereopsis. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 51, 2019e2023. nerve, and macular characteristics of children with persistent amblyopia, in press.
O’Connor, A.R., Birch, E.E., Anderson, S., Draper, H., 2010b. Relationship between Tarczy-Hornoch, K., Varma, R., Cotter, S.A., McKean-Cowdin, R., Lin, J.H.,
binocular vision, visual acuity, and fine motor skills. Optom. Vis. Sci. 87, 942e Borchert, M.S., Torres, M., Wen, G., Azen, S.P., Tielsch, J.M., Friedman, D.S.,
947. Repka, M.X., Katz, J., Ibironke, J., Giordano, L., 2011. Risk factors for decreased
Packwood, E.A., Cruz, O.A., Rychwalski, P.J., Keech, R.V., 1999. The psychosocial visual acuity in preschool children: the multi-ethnic pediatric eye disease and
effects of amblyopia study. J. AAPOS 3, 15e17. Baltimore pediatric eye disease studies. Ophthalmology 118, 2262e2273.
Pai, A.S., Rose, K.A., Leone, J.F., Sharbini, S., Burlutsky, G., Varma, R., Wong, T.Y., Tingley, D.H., 2007. Vision screening essentials: screening today for eye disorders in
Mitchell, P., 2012. Amblyopia prevalence and risk factors in Australian preschool the pediatric patient. Pediatr. Rev. 28, 54e61.
children. Ophthalmology 119, 138e144. Tjiam, A.M., Holtslag, G., Van Minderhout, H.M., Simonsz-Toth, B., Vermeulen-
Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group, 2002a. The clinical profile of moderate Jong, M.H., Borsboom, G.J., Loudon, S.E., Simonsz, H.J., 2012. Randomised
amblyopia in children younger than 7 years. Arch. Ophthalmol. 120, 281e comparison of three tools for improving compliance with occlusion therapy: an
287. educational cartoon story, a reward calendar, and an information leaflet for
Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group, 2002b. A randomized trial of atropine vs. parents. Graefes Arch. Clin. Exp. Ophthalmol. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/500417-
patching for treatment of moderate amblyopia in children. Arch. Ophthalmol. 012-2107-4 [published online 22 July 2012].
120, 268e278. To, L., Thompson, B., Blum, J.R., Maehara, G., Hess, R.F., Cooperstock, J.R., 2011.
Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group, 2003. A comparison of atropine and A game platform for treatment of amblyopia. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil.
patching treatments for moderate amblyopia by patient age, cause of ambly- Eng. 19, 280e289.
opia, depth of amblyopia, and other factors. Ophthalmology 110, 1632e1637. Tychsen, L., Wong, A.M., Foeller, P., Bradley, D., 2004. Early versus delayed repair of
discussion 1637e1638. infantile strabismus in macaque monkeys: II. Effects on motion visually evoked
Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group, 2004. A prospective, pilot study of treatment responses. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 45, 821e827.
of amblyopia in children 10 to <18 years old. Am. J. Ophthalmol. 137, 581e583. Tychsen, L., Richards, M., Wong, A., Foeller, P., Bradley, D., Burkhalter, A., 2010. The
Procianoy, L., Procianoy, E., 2010. The accuracy of binocular fixation preference for neural mechanism for latent (fusion maldevelopment) nystagmus.
the diagnosis of strabismic amblyopia. J. AAPOS 14, 205e210. J. Neuroophthalmol. 30, 276e283.
Quinn, G.E., Beck, R.W., Holmes, J.M., Repka, M.X., 2004. Recent advances in the Tychsen, L., 2005. Can ophthalmologists repair the brain in infantile esotropia?
treatment of amblyopia. Pediatrics 113, 1800e1802. Early surgery, stereopsis, monofixation syndrome, and the legacy of Marshall
Rahi, J.S., Cumberland, P.M., Peckham, C.S., 2006. Does amblyopia affect educational, Parks. J. AAPOS 9, 510e521.
health, and social outcomes? Findings from 1958 British birth cohort. BMJ 332, Tychsen, L., 2007. Causing and Curing Infantile Esotropia in Primates: the Role of
820e825. Decorrelated Binocular Input (an American Ophthalmological Society thesis).
Regan, D., Giaschi, D.E., Kraft, S.P., Kothe, A.C., 1992. Method for identifying Trans. Am. Ophthalmol. Soc. 105, 564e593.
amblyopes whose reduced line acuity is caused by defective selection and/or Walker, R.A., Rubab, S., Voll, A.R., Erraguntla, V., Murphy, P.H., 2011. Macular and
control of gaze. Ophthalmic Physiol. Opt. 12, 425e432. peripapillary retinal nerve fibre layer thickness in adults with amblyopia. Can. J.
Repka, M.X., Holmes, J.M., 2012. Lessons from the amblyopia treatment studies. Ophthalmol. 46, 425e427.
Ophthalmology 119, 657e658. Wall, T.C., Marsh-Tootle, W., Evans, H.H., Fargason, C.A., Ashworth, C.S., Hardin, J.M.,
Repka, M.X., Beck, R.W., Holmes, J.M., Birch, E.E., Chandler, D.L., Cotter, S.A., 2002. Compliance with vision-screening guidelines among a national sample of
Hertle, R.W., Kraker, R.T., Moke, P.S., Quinn, G.E., Scheiman, M.M., 2003. pediatricians. Ambul. Pediatr. 2, 449e455.
A randomized trial of patching regimens for treatment of moderate amblyopia Wallace, D.K., Edwards, A.R., Cotter, S.A., Beck, R.W., Arnold, R.W., Astle, W.F.,
in children. Arch. Ophthalmol. 121, 603e611. Barnhardt, C.N., Birch, E.E., Donahue, S.P., Everett, D.F., Felius, J., Holmes, J.M.,
Repka, M.X., Cotter, S.A., Beck, R.W., Kraker, R.T., Birch, E.E., Everett, D.F., Kraker, R.T., Melia, M., Repka, M.X., Sala, N.A., Silbert, D.I., Weise, K.K., 2006.
Hertle, R.W., Holmes, J.M., Quinn, G.E., Sala, N.A., Scheiman, M.M., A randomized trial to evaluate 2 hours of daily patching for strabismic and
Stager Sr., D.R., Wallace, D.K., 2004. A randomized trial of atropine regimens for anisometropic amblyopia in children. Ophthalmology 113, 904e912.
treatment of moderate amblyopia in children. Ophthalmology 111, 2076e2085. Wallace, D.K., Kraker, R.T., Beck, R.W., Cotter, S.A., Davis, P.L., Holmes, J.M.,
Repka, M.X., Wallace, D.K., Beck, R.W., Kraker, R.T., Birch, E.E., Cotter, S.A., Repka, M.X., Suh, D.W., 2011a. Randomized trial to evaluate combined patching
Donahue, S., Everett, D.F., Hertle, R.W., Holmes, J.M., Quinn, G.E., and atropine for residual amblyopia. Arch. Ophthalmol. 129, 960e962.
Scheiman, M.M., Weakley, D.R., 2005. Two-year follow-up of a 6-month Wallace, D.K., Lazar, E.L., Melia, M., Birch, E.E., Holmes, J.M., Hopkins, K.B., Kraker, R.T.,
randomized trial of atropine vs patching for treatment of moderate ambly- Kulp, M.T., Pang, Y., Repka, M.X., Tamkins, S.M., Weise, K.K., 2011b. Stereoacuity in
opia in children. Arch. Ophthalmol. 123, 149e157. children with anisometropic amblyopia. J. AAPOS 15, 455e461.
Repka, M.X., Kraker, R.T., Beck, R.W., Birch, E., Cotter, S.A., Holmes, J.M., Hertle, R.W., Wasserman, R.C., Croft, C.A., Brotherton, S.E., 1992. Preschool vision screening in
Hoover, D.L., Klimek, D.L., Marsh-Tootle, W., Scheiman, M.M., Suh, D.W., pediatric practice: a study from the Pediatric Research in Office Settings (PROS)
Weakley, D.R., 2009. Treatment of severe amblyopia with weekend atropine: Network. American Academy of Pediatrics. Pediatrics 89, 834e838.
results from 2 randomized clinical trials. J. AAPOS 13, 258e263. Weakley, D.R., 1999. The association between anisometropia, amblyopia, and
Robaei, D., Huynh, S.C., Kifley, A., Mitchell, P., 2006. Correctable and non-correctable binocularity in the absence of strabismus. Trans. Am. Ophthalmol. Soc. 97, 987e
visual impairment in a population-based sample of 12-year-old Australian 1021.
children. Am. J. Ophthalmol. 142, 112e118. Weakley Jr., D.R., 2001. The association between nonstrabismic anisometropia,
Rutstein, R.P., Quinn, G.E., Lazar, E.L., Beck, R.W., Bonsall, D.J., Cotter, S.A., amblyopia, and subnormal binocularity. Ophthalmology 108, 163e171.
Crouch, E.R., Holmes, J.M., Hoover, D.L., Leske, D.A., Lorenzana, I.J., Repka, M.X., Webber, A.L., Wood, J.M., Gole, G.A., Brown, B., 2008a. The effect of amblyopia on
Suh, D.W., 2010. A randomized trial comparing Bangerter filters and patching fine motor skills in children. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 49, 594e603.
for the treatment of moderate amblyopia in children. Ophthalmology 117, 998e Webber, A.L., Wood, J.M., Gole, G.A., Brown, B., 2008b. Effect of amblyopia on self-
1004. e1006. esteem in children. Optom. Vis. Sci. 85, 1074e1081.
84 E.E. Birch / Progress in Retinal and Eye Research 33 (2013) 67e84

Williams, C., Harrad, R., 2006. Amblyopia: contemporary clinical issues. Strabismus Woodruff, G., Hiscox, F., Thompson, J.R., Smith, L.K., 1994a. Factors affecting the
14, 43e50. outcome of children treated for amblyopia. Eye (Lond.) 8 (Pt 6), 627e631.
Williams, C., Harrad, R.A., Harvey, I., Sparrow, J.M., 2001. Screening for amblyopia in Woodruff, G., Hiscox, F., Thompson, J.R., Smith, L.K., 1994b. The presentation of
preschool children: results of a population-based, randomised controlled trial. children with amblyopia. Eye (Lond.) 8 (Pt 6), 623e626.
ALSPAC Study Team. Avon Longitudinal Study of Pregnancy and Childhood. Wright, K.W., Edelman, P.M., Walonker, F., Yiu, S., 1986. Reliability of fixation
Ophthalmic Epidemiol. 8, 279e295. preference testing in diagnosing amblyopia. Arch. Ophthalmol. 104, 549e
Williams, C., Northstone, K., Harrad, R.A., Sparrow, J.M., Harvey, I., 2002. Amblyopia 553.
treatment outcomes after screening before or at age 3 years: follow up from Wright, K.W., Edelman, P.M., McVey, J.H., Terry, A.P., Lin, M., 1994. High-grade stereo
randomised trial. BMJ 324, 1549. acuity after early surgery for congenital esotropia. Arch. Ophthalmol. 112, 913e
Williams, C., Northstone, K., Harrad, R.A., Sparrow, J.M., Harvey, I., 2003. Amblyopia 919.
treatment outcomes after preschool screening v school entry screening: obser- Wu, C., Hunter, D.G., 2006. Amblyopia: diagnostic and therapeutic options. Am. J.
vational data from a prospective cohort study. Br. J. Ophthalmol. 87, 988e993. Ophthalmol. 141, 175e184.
Williams, C., Northstone, K., Howard, M., Harvey, I., Harrad, R.A., Sparrow, J.M., Yanni, S.E., Jost, R., Beauchamp, C.L., Stager Sr., D.R., Birch, E.E., 2012. Objec-
2008. Prevalence and risk factors for common vision problems in children: data tive detection of amblyopia and strabismus not just risk factors. J. AAPOS
from the ALSPAC study. Br. J. Ophthalmol. 92, 959e964. 16, e9.

You might also like