Age Sex Taiwan 2014

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 5

Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 60 (2015) 196–200

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/archger

Cognitive function and associated factors among older people


in Taiwan: Age and sex differences
Cheng-Lun Li a, Hui-Chuan Hsu a,b,*
a
Department of Health Care Administration, Research Center on Health Policy and Management, Asia University, No. 500, Lioufeng Road, Wufeng,
Taichung 41354, Taiwan
b
Department of Medical Research, China Medical University Hospital, China Medical University

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Article history: Purpose: The aim of this study was to examine cognitive function and the risk and the protective factors
Received 31 October 2013 by age and sex among Taiwanese older people.
Received in revised form 6 October 2014 Methods: The data were from a nation-representative panel of older people in Taiwan. The participants
Accepted 7 October 2014
completing both the 2003 and 2007 waves were included for analysis in this study (n = 3228).
Available online 17 October 2014
Descriptive analysis and generalized linear model were applied, and the samples were stratified by age
groups and by sex.
Keywords:
Results: The factors related to higher cognitive function at the intercept included being younger, male,
Cognitive function
higher education, and doing unpaid work. At the time slope, the age effect and physical function
Longitudinal study
Older adults difficulties would reduce the cognitive function across time, while education and providing
Age difference informational support would increase the cognitive function across time. There were age- and sex-
Gender difference differences in the factors related to cognitive function, particularly on the working status and social
participation.
Conclusion: Different health promotion strategies to target these populations should be accordingly
developed.
ß 2014 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction cognitive function (Fernandez Martinez et al., 2008; Matthews


et al., 2012; Piccinin et al., 2012; Proust-Lima et al., 2008; Yen et al.,
Cognitive impairment has significant negative impacts for older 2010). The patterns and factors related to the cognitive function
people. Dementia or severe cognitive impairment not only affects over time in different age and sex groups have not been compared
the quality of life for older people but also causes a heavy burden thoroughly in the existing literature.
for their families and increases medical costs for society. Only Previous studies have found factors related to cognitive
when the cognitive impairment is mild is it possible to prevent impairment among older people. The incidence of cognitive
worsening of the progression (Weimer & Sager, 2009). Previous impairment is related to being older, being female, and having a
studies have explored the risk factors for cognitive function lower education level (Kawas, Gray, Brookmeyer, Fozard, &
impairment and the incidence of dementia (Burton, Campbell, Zonderman, 2000; Miech et al., 2002; Sabia et al., 2009; Shadlen
Jordan, Strauss, & Mallen, 2013; Loef & Walach, 2013; Teixeira et al., 2006; Yen et al., 2010). Certain chronic diseases are
et al., 2012; Wang et al., 2010; Yen et al., 2010), and some studies associated with cognitive impairment, including diabetes, chronic
have tried to explored the changes in the cognitive function of kidney disease, vascular diseases, and stroke (Duron & Hanon,
older people (Eisele et al., 2012; Matthews, Marioni, & Brayne, 2008; Etgen, Chonchol, Forstl, & Sander, 2012; Gorelick, 2005;
2012; Piccinin et al., 2012; Yen et al., 2010). Furthermore, age and Kilander, Nyman, Boberg, Hansson, & Lithell, 1998; Mok et al.,
sex have been demonstrated to be significant risk factors for 2004; Muller, Grobbee, Aleman, Bots, & Van Der Schouw, 2007;
Okereke et al., 2008; Tzourio, 2007). Cognitive impairment is
associated with frailty (Auyeung, Lee, Kwok, & Woo, 2011; Boyle,
Buchman, Wilson, Leurgans, & Bennett, 2010; Curcio, Henao, &
* Corresponding author at: Department of Health Care Administration,
Asia University, No. 500, Lioufeng Road, Wufeng, Taichung 41354, Taiwan.
Gomez, 2014; Faria Cde, Lourenco, Ribeiro, & Lopes, 2013; Han, Lee,
Tel.: +886 4 23323456x1827; fax: +886 4 23332970. & Kim, 2014; Kilgour, Todd, & Starr, 2014; Shimada et al., 2013;
E-mail address: gingerhsu@seed.net.tw (H.-C. Hsu). Samper-Ternent, Al Snih, Raji, Markides, & Ottenbacher, 2008). In

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2014.10.007
0167-4943/ß 2014 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
C.-L. Li, H.-C. Hsu / Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 60 (2015) 196–200 197

addition, social support and social engagement have been found to coded as having low physical activity. The respondents who met
influence cognitive function and dementia in old age (Zunzunegui, three or more of the five criteria above were considered to be frail;
Alvarado, Del Ser, & Otero, 2003), and social support may be a those who met one or two criteria were considered pre-frail; and
stronger protective predictor for women than for men (Baumeister those with none of the criteria were considered non-frail.
& Sommer, 1997; Fratiglioni, Paillard-Borg, & Winblad, 2004).
In this study, we aimed to explore the cognitive function and 2.2.2.2. Repeatedly measured variables. Marital status was defined
the factors related across time by age and sex in older Taiwanese as having a spouse or not. Chronic diseases were selected from
people. This study is expected to provide information about the those related to higher cognitive impairment risks, including
factors related to the cognitive function in older adults in different hypertension, diabetes mellitus, heart disease, and stroke. The
groups and to enable the development of prevention/intervention cumulative number of the four chronic diseases was used for
strategies. analysis.
Physical function difficulty was assessed by the activities of
2. Methods daily living (ADLs), including following items: eating, clothing,
transferring (getting up from a chair or from bed), using the toilet,
2.1. Data and samples taking a bath, and walking indoors. Each item was scored 0–3,
ranging from no difficulty at all to unable to perform the activity.
The data were from the ‘‘Taiwan Longitudinal Study of Aging The physical function difficulty scores ranged from 0 to 18.
(TLSA)’’. The samples of the TLSA were drawn from the elderly The respondent’s social support was assessed by measuring
population with multi-stage proportional probability sampling, their received emotional support, received instrumental support,
initiated with a sample of 4049 older people aged 60 years or more and provided information support. Emotional support was
in 1989. The samples were face-to-face interviewed from 1989 to measured as the degree of received care from their family
2007, every 3 or 4 years. Supplemental samples aged 50–66 years members, relatives, or friends. Each item was scored from 1 to
were added to the cohort in 1996 and in 2003. In this study, the 5, and the total scores ranged from 0 to 20. Higher scores indicated
data from 2003 to 2007 were used. Only those participants who that more support was received. Received instrumental support
completed both the 2003 and 2007 waves were included in the items were determined by asking ‘‘when you need someone to
analysis. Overall, the sample size was 3226 individuals. help, is there anyone who can help you or not’’ and were coded yes
or no. Providing informational support was assessed by the
2.2. Measures questions ‘‘do you feel the help of your family is useful or not’’ and
‘‘do your family consult your opinion when they make decisions’’.
2.2.1. Dependent variable Each item was scored from 1 to 3, and the total scores ranged from
Cognitive function was measured by the Short Portable Mental 2 to 6.
Status Questionnaire (SPMSQ) (Pfeiffer, 1975) in both 2003 and Social participation included working status and participating
2007. There were only eight items available in both waves, and the in volunteer or social groups. Working status was coded as doing
8-item scale has been applied and validated (Purser, Fillenbaum, paid work, unpaid work, and no work. Social group participation
Pieper, & Wallace, 2005; Yen et al., 2010). Thus, the 8 items as used was coded as yes/no, which was defined as participating in any
as the measures of cognitive function. The questions included the group of the following: volunteering in social services, religious
following: what is your home address; what is today’s date; what group, union or occupational organization, social service or
day is today; how old are you; what is your mother’s surname; who volunteer work, political group, community action or service
is the current president and the last president; and subtract 3 from group, clan association, and elderly club.
20 for a total of four consecutive times. Each item was scored as
0 or 1. If the item was answered as ‘‘unknown’’, the item was scored 2.3. Analysis
as 0. The scores ranged from 0 to 8. The scores in 2003 and
2007 were used as repeated measures of the outcome variable. Descriptive analysis and the generalized linear model of a time-
linear function with normal distribution were applied for the
2.2.2. Independent variables analysis. The entire sample was first analyzed, and then the
The independent variables included the fixed variables and the samples were stratified by age groups (aged less than 65 or more)
repeatedly measured variables. The fixed independent variables and by sex and analyzed accordingly. The analysis was conducted
included age, sex, educational years, and frailty. Other independent using SPSS version 19.0 for Windows (IBM Inc., New York,
variables (marital status, chronic diseases, physical function Armonk).
difficulties, social support, and social participation) were repeated
measures in both waves. 3. Results

2.2.2.1. Fixed variables. The demographic variables included sex, The descriptive data of the samples in the two waves are
age and educational years. The definition of physical frailty described in Table 1. The average of the cognitive function score
referred to the study of Fried et al. (2001). Because some of the reduced a little from 2003 (mean = 7.76, SD = 0.59) to 2007
variables were unavailable in the data, proxy variables were used. (mean = 7.53, SD = 0.96). In fact, about 22.43% of the respondents
(1) Weight loss: The unintentional loss of more than 10 pounds declined, 68.46% of them remained constant, and 9.11% improved.
between 2003 and 2007. (2) Poor grip strength: To perform The results of the generalized linear model are shown in
grasping objects with the fingers with any difficulty. (3) Exhaus- Table 2. The dependent variable was defined as the cognitive
tion: Two items from the CES-D scale were used (Kohout, Berkman, function scores in 2 waves. Model 1 indicates the results of the
Evans, & Cornoni-Huntley, 1993), asking ‘‘I felt that everything I did entire sample. The time slope was negative but not significant,
was an effort’’ and ‘‘I could not get going’’ in the last week, giving a indicating that the cognitive function did not reduce significantly
score of 0–3. Persons answering ‘2’ or ‘3’ to either question were across time. The factors related to higher cognitive function at the
categorized as frail. (4) Walking 200–300 m with any difficulty. (5) intercept included being younger (b = 0.009, p < 0.001), male
Low physical activity: Defined as not performing regular exercise. (b = 0.128, p < 0.001), higher education (b = 0.025, p < 0.001), and
The respondents who exercised two times per week or less were doing unpaid work (b = 0.091, p < 0.001). At the time slope, the age
198 C.-L. Li, H.-C. Hsu / Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 60 (2015) 196–200

Table 1 the female respondents, being younger, having higher education


Sample characteristics.
being non-frail, working on paid work or unpaid work, having less
Characteristics Mean (SD) or % physical difficulties at the intercept would be related to higher
2003 2007 cognitive function; while higher education, having less physical
functional difficulties, and providing informational support would
Age 62.73 (9.64) –
Years of education 7.13 (4.50) –
be related to increased cognitive function over time.
Sex
Men 46.40% – 4. Discussion
Women 53.60% –
Physical frailty
In this study, the cognitive function of two waves among the
Frail 3.66% –
Pre-frail 57.37% – Taiwanese older people was examined, and the differences
Non-frail 38.97% – associated with age and sex were compared. In general, being
Cognitive function score 7.76 (0.59) 7.53 (0.96) younger, males, having higher education, still working, having less
Physical function difficulties 0.07 (0.64) 0.21 (1.38) physical function difficulties, and providing more informational
Marital status
support, were related to higher cognitive function. There were age-
No spouse 21.73% 26.04%
Having a spouse 78.27% 73.96% and sex-differences in the factors related to cognitive function.
Working status Past studies have only found a relationship between physical
Paid work 38.3% 32.6% function difficulty and cognitive impairment (Dodge et al., 2005;
Unpaid work 33.9% 31.5%
Fauth et al., 2013). Our study found the associations of physical
No work 27.7% 35.9%
Social group or volunteering function difficulties with cognitive function particularly for males
participation and females, and particularly significant for the older group. The
Yes 46.5% 48.0% mechanism of cognitive function improvement may vary in different
No 53.5% 52.0% ages, or there may be confounding factors related to age and sex with
Emotional support 16.64 (2.82) 16.66 (2.66)
respect to cognitive function. Further research about the age effect
Received instrumental support 0.91 (0.28) 0.90 (0.29)
Providing informational support 5.07 (1.06) 4.94 (1.10) on the improvement of cognitive function is suggested.
Hypertension Providing more informational support was significant with
Yes 30.29% 37.79% cognitive function. Providing informational support or advice for
No 69.71% 62.21%
their family/friends may increase the stimulation of cognitive
Diabetes
Yes 11.25% 15.16%
function (Bassuk, Glass, & Berkman, 1999), and then shows a
No 88.75% 84.84% protective effect, particularly for the people with less social
Heart disease connection such as the older and female elderly. Another
Yes 15.07% 17.39% explanation is that the providing of informational support was
No 84.93% 82.61%
concurrent with the cognitive function trajectory in the models,
Stroke
Yes 2.23% 3.10% and thus, those who had better cognitive function were able to
No 97.77% 96.90% provide more informational support for their family/friends. When
Note. n = 3226.
they received good feedback from the social network, their
cognitive function would increase over time.
The effects of working status and participating in social groups
effect (b = 0.002, p < 0.01) and physical function difficulties on cognitive function found in previous studies (Hsu, 2007;
(b = 0.028, p < 0.001) would reduce the cognitive function across Winningham & Pike, 2007; Zunzunegui et al., 2003) were also
time, while education (b = 0.004, p < 0.001) and providing replicated in this study. However, the effect of working status and
informational support (b = 0.023, p < 0.001) would increase the social participation showed different effects by age and gender.
cognitive function across time. Doing paid or unpaid work was only significant for females, and
Because age and sex were significant to cognitive function, doing unpaid work was significant for the older group only.
Model 2 shows the results of the age stratification, and Model Participating in volunteering/social groups was significantly
3 shows the results of the sex stratification. Model 2A shows the beneficial only for the males but not significant for the females.
results of those younger who were aged less than 65 years old. The The younger participants may still have had a job during the
younger respondents who were male, having higher education, survey, but the older participants may have retired. Thus, doing
having a spouse, and providing more informational support at the unpaid work (such as taking care of grandchildren or doing
intercept, were more likely to have higher cognitive function housework) still make the older people living actively, and further
across time. Education and providing informational support even being beneficial for the cognitive function. The gender difference
increase a little bit cognitive function across time. Model 2B shows may be related to the gender inequality of the older cohorts in
the results of the older samples (aged 65 years old or more). For the Taiwanese society. For the older cohorts of Taiwanese elderly, the
older respondents, being younger, being male, having higher females did not have an equal chance to obtain education or were
education, doing unpaid work at the intercept were more likely to not allowed to work, other than being housewives. Thus, for those
have higher cognitive function; while having less physical function females who were doing paid work must be the ones with higher
difficulties and providing more informational support at the time socioeconomic status or having great working ability, and their
slope were more likely to increased higher cognitive function cognitive function was more likely to be higher and maintained.
across time. The relationship between physical frailty and cognitive
Model 3A and 3B are the results for the male and female function in the elderly was found in previous research (Auyeung
respondents, respectively. For the male respondents, being et al., 2011; Boyle et al., 2010; Buchman, Boyle, Wilson, Tang, &
younger, having higher education, participating in volunteering Bennett, 2007; Curcio et al., 2014; Kilgour et al., 2014), but the
or social groups at the intercept were more likely to have higher effect of frailty only showed on females only in this study. One
cognitive function. At the time slope, age and physical function explanation was that frailty was an essential health problem for
difficulties would reduce the cognitive function while providing the female elderly in Taiwan. Another possible reason was that
informational support would increase it across time. Regarding to frailty was defined as the fixed variable in this study (due to weight
C.-L. Li, H.-C. Hsu / Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 60 (2015) 196–200 199

Table 2
Trajectory of cognitive function among Taiwanese older people by generalized linear model, 2003–2007.

Variable Model 1 Model 2A Model 2B Model 3A Model 3B


Total samples Younger (age < 65) Older (age  65) Male Female

Intercept 7.895 (0.150)*** 7.598 (0.194)*** 8.501 (0.394)*** 8.065 (0.168)*** 7.778 (0.266)***
Age 0.009 (0.002)*** 0.002 (0.003) 0.018 (0.005)*** 0.008 (0.019)*** 0.009 (0.003)**
Gender (male) 0.128 (0.030)*** 0.082 (0.027)** 0.168 (0.062)** – –
Years of education 0.025 (0.003)*** 0.016 (0.003)*** 0.038 (0.006)*** 0.014 (0.004)*** 0.038 (0.005)***
Frail 0.111 (0.068) 0.007 (0.075) 0.175 (0.117) <0.001 (0.087) 0.215 (0.102)*
Pre-frail 0.020 (0.030) 0.008 (0.025) 0.089 (0.071) 0.011 (0.035) 0.047 (0.050)
Marital status (no spouse) 0.014 (0.034) 0.067 (0.034)* 0.050 (0.059) 0.029 (0.045) 0.021 (0.050)
Working status (paid work) 0.043 (0.038) 0.031 (0.036) 0.093 (0.084) 0.004 (0.040) 0.176 (0.077)*
Working status (unpaid work) 0.091 (0.036)* 0.029 (0.041) 0.132 (0.059)* 0.021 (0.044) 0.197 (0.064)**
Social participation (yes) 0.028 (0.026) 0.016 (0.023) 0.051 (0.050) 0.059 (0.029)* 0.011 (0.043)
Physical function difficulties 0.025 (0.020) 0.006 (0.023) 0.025 (0.032) 0.013 (0.023) 0.071 (0.035)*
Received emotional support 0.003 (0.005) 0.005 (0.005) 0.005 (0.010) 0.003 (0.006) 0.005 (0.009)
Received instrumental support 0.022 (0.048) 0.043 (0.045) 0.021 (0.088) 0.002 (0.053) 0.041 (0.084)
Providing informational supports 0.018 (0.015) 0.031 (0.015)* 0.008 (0.026) 0.011 (0.017) 0.016 (0.025)
Chronic disease numbers 0.019 (0.016) 0.014 (0.015) 0.044 (0.027) 0.007 (0.018) 0.049 (0.026)
Time 0.091 (0.053) 0.051 (0.083) 0.102 (0.129) 0.020 (0.061) 0.163 (0.093)
Time * age 0.002 (0.001)** 0.001 (0.001) 0.002 (0.002) 0.002 (0.001)** 0.001 (0.001)
Time * gender (male) 0.020 (0.011) 0.010 (0.010) 0.016 (0.021) – –
Time * years of education 0.004 (0.001)** 0.004 (0.001)*** 0.003 (0.002) 0.001 (0.001) 0.006 (0.002)**
Time * frail 0.014 (0.024) 0.025 (0.028) 0.034 (0.039) 0.035 (0.030) 0.042 (0.036)
Time * prefrail 0.007 (0.011) 0.007 (0.009) 0.036 (0.023) 0.000 (0.012) 0.012 (0.018)
Time * Martial status 0.000 (0.012) 0.001 (0.012) 0.005 (0.020) 0.005 (0.015) 0.001 (0.018)
Time * working status (paid work) 0.004 (0.013) 0.004 (0.013) 0.003 (0.028) 0.005 (0.014) 0.001 (0.027)
Time * working status (unpaid work) 0.020 (0.013) 0.009 (0.015) 0.017 (0.020) 0.016 (0.016) 0.008 (0.022)
Time * social participation (yes) 0.014 (0.009) 0.001 (0.009) 0.021 (0.017) 0.003 (0.010) 0.022 (0.015)
Time * physical function difficulties 0.028 (0.056)*** 0.005 (0.007) 0.031 (0.009)*** 0.015 (0.007)* 0.031 (0.009)**
Time * received emotional support 0.001 (0.019) 0.001 (0.002) 0.002 (0.003) <0.001 (0.002) 0.003 (0.003)
Time * received instrumental support 0.020 (0.017) 0.008 (0.016) 0.048 (0.030) 0.024 (0.018) 0.011 (0.029)
Time * providing informational support 0.023 (0.005)*** 0.011 (0.005)* 0.033 (0.009)*** 0.025 (0.006)*** 0.022 (0.008)*
Time * chronic disease numbers 0.004 (0.005) 0.007 (0.006) 0.008 (0.009) 0.004 (0.006) 0.015 (0.009)

AIC 14,014.997 5017.280 7777.704 6339.035 7271.953


BIC 14,224.615 5208.238 7963.402 6517.323 7446.080
*
p < 0.05.
**
p < 0.01.
***
p < 0.001.
The reference group of the dependent variables was scores of cognitive function (SPMSQ scale). The reference group of the independent variables included: sex (female),
marital status (having a spouse), working status (no), participation in volunteering or social groups (no), frailty (non-frail). The number of chronic diseases (include:
hypertension, diabetes, heart disease and stroke).

loss was defined as the changes between these two waves), and the constructed in the policy. The dynamic relationship of cognitive
change of frailty was not measured. Another Taiwanese study has function and related factors are suggested be evaluated in future
shown that cognitive function and frailty may concurrently research.
associated, and previous frailty may also predict later cognitive
function decline (Hsu, 2014). Conflict of interests
There are some limitations to this study. First, there were
missing cases because of deaths or loss to follow-up. The missing None.
cases may be relatively healthier, and thus, the conclusion of this
study should be considered when transferred to other populations.
Acknowledgments
Second, only two waves of data were applied in this study. The
longer longitudinal dynamics of cognitive function related to
This work was supported by grants from the National Science
factors over the long-term were not examined. Third, only the
Council, Taiwan, Republic of China (NSC 101-2410-H-468-008-
SPMSQ scale was available for the measure of cognitive function.
MY2). This study had obtained approval of Research Ethics
The measure of cognitive function may not be comprehensive
Committee of Central Regional Research Ethics Center, Taiwan,
enough to measure different dimensions of cognitive function. A
R.O.C. (No. CRREC-101-062).
better measurement tool is suggested in future research.
The cognitive function of older adults may be dynamic, and the
factors associated with its decline or improvement is different by References
age and by sex. As the population ages, the number of older people
Auyeung, T. W., Lee, J. S., Kwok, T., & Woo, J. (2011). Physical frailty predicts future
with cognitive impairment may increase rapidly in the future. cognitive decline – a four-year prospective study in 2737 cognitively normal older
Focusing on the effectiveness of preventive interventions to adults. Journal of Nutrition, Health & Aging, 15(8), 690–694. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1007/s12603-011-0110-9
promote cognitive health for the older population is necessary.
Bassuk, S. S., Glass, T. A., & Berkman, L. F. (1999). Social disengagement and incident
Different risks and health promotion strategies to target popula- cognitive decline in community-dwelling elderly persons. Annals of Internal Medi-
tions should be age- and gender-sensitively developed. The older cine, 131(3), 165–173. http://dx.doi.org/10.7326/0003-4819-131-3-199908030-
people and females should be encouraged to engage with social 00002
Baumeister, R. F., & Sommer, K. L. (1997). What do men want? Gender differences and
participation or work (whether paid or unpaid); the age-friendly two spheres of belongingness: Comment on Cross and Madson (1997). Psychologi-
environment to active aging and life-long education should be cal Bulletin, 122(1), 38–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.122.1.38
200 C.-L. Li, H.-C. Hsu / Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 60 (2015) 196–200

Boyle, P. A., Buchman, A. S., Wilson, R. S., Leurgans, S. E., & Bennett, D. A. (2010). Physical Matthews, F., Marioni, R., & Brayne, C. (2012). Medical Research Council Cognitive
frailty is associated with incident mild cognitive impairment in community-based Function and Ageing Study. Examining the influence of gender, education, social
older persons. American Geriatrics Society, 58(2), 248–255. http://dx.doi.org/ class, and birth cohort on MMSE tracking over time: A population-based prospec-
10.1111/j.1532-5415.2009.02671.x tive cohort study. BMC Geriatrics, 12, 45. http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-
Buchman, A. S., Boyle, P. A., Wilson, R. S., Tang, Y., & Bennett, D. A. (2007). Frailty is 218-12/45
associated with incident Alzheimer’s disease and cognitive decline in the elderly. Miech, R., Breitner, J., Zandi, P., Khachaturian, A., Anthony, J., & Mayer, L. (2002).
Psychosomatic Medicine, 69(5), 483–489. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/psy.0- Incidence of AD may decline in the early 90s for men, later for women: The Cache
b013e318068de1d County study. Neurology, 58(2), 209–218. http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/WNL.58.2.209
Burton, C., Campbell, P., Jordan, K., Strauss, V., & Mallen, C. (2013). The association of Mok, V. C., Wong, A., Lam, W. W., Fan, Y. H., Tang, W. K., Kwok, T., et al. (2004). Cognitive
anxiety and depression with future dementia diagnosis: A case-control study in impairment and functional outcome after stroke associated with small vessel
primary care. Family Practice, 30(1), 25–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/fampra/ disease. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 75(4), 560–566. http://
cms044 dx.doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.2003.015107
Curcio, C.-L., Henao, G.-M., & Gomez, F. (2014). Frailty among rural elderly adults. BMC Muller, M., Grobbee, D., Aleman, A., Bots, M., & Van Der Schouw, Y. (2007). Cardiovas-
Geriatrics, 14, 2. Available at: http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2318-14/2 cular disease and cognitive performance in middle-aged and elderly men. Athero-
Dodge, H. H., Kadowaki, T., Hayakawa, T., Yamakawa, M., Sekikawa, A., & Ueshima, H. sclerosis, 190(1), 143–149. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.atherosclerosis.2006.01.005
(2005). Cognitive impairment as a strong predictor of incident disability in specific Okereke, O. I., Kang, J. H., Cook, N. R., Gaziano, J. M., Manson, J. A. E., Buring, J. E., et al.
ADL-IADL tasks among community-dwelling elders: The Azuchi Study. Gerontolo- (2008). Type 2 diabetes mellitus and cognitive decline in two large cohorts of
gist, 45(2), 222–230. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geront/45.2.222 community-dwelling older adults. American Geriatrics Society, 56(6), 1028–1036.
Duron, E., & Hanon, O. (2008). Hypertension, cognitive decline and dementia. Archives http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01686.x
of Cardiovascular Diseases, 101(3), 181–189. ISSN: 1875-2136. Pfeiffer, E. (1975). A short portable mental status questionnaire for the assessment of
Eisele, M., Zimmermann, T., Köhler, M., Wiese, B., Heser, K., Tebarth, F., et al. (2012). organic brain deficit in elderly patients. American Geriatrics Society, 23(10), 433–
Influence of social support on cognitive change and mortality in old age: results 441. ISSN: 1532-5415.
from the prospective multicenter cohort study AgeCoDe. BMC Geriatrics, 12(9) Piccinin, A. M., Muniz-Terrera, G., Clouston, S., Reynolds, C. A., Thorvaldsson, V., Deary,
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2318-12-9 I. J., et al. (2012). Coordinated analysis of age, sex and education effects on change
Etgen, T., Chonchol, M., Forstl, H., & Sander, D. (2012). Chronic kidney disease and in MMSE scores. Journal of Gerontology: Series B Psychological Sciences and Social
cognitive impairment: A systematic review and meta-analysis. American Journal of Sciences, 68(3), 374–390. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbs077
Nephrology, 35(5), 474–482. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000338135 Proust-Lima, C., Amieva, H., Letenneur, L., Orgogozo, J.-M., Jacqmin-Gadda, H., &
Faria Cde, A., Lourenco, R. A., Ribeiro, P. C., & Lopes, C. S. (2013). Cognitive performance Dartigues, J.-F. (2008). Gender and education impact on brain aging: A general
and frailty in older adults clients of a private health care plan. Revista de Saude cognitive factor approach. Psychology and Aging, 23(3), 608–620.
Publica, 47(5), 923–930. Purser, J. L., Fillenbaum, G. G., Pieper, C. F., & Wallace, R. B. (2005). Mild cognitive
Fauth, E. B., Schwartz, S., Tschanz, J. T., Ostbye, T., Corcoran, C., & Norton, M. C. (2013). impairment and 10-year trajectories of disability in the Iowa established popula-
Baseline disability in activities of daily living predicts dementia risk even after tions for epidemiologic studies of the elderly cohort. American Geriatrics Society,
controlling for baseline global cognitive ability and depressive symptoms. Inter- 53(11), 1966–1972. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53566.x
national Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 28(6), 597–606. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ Sabia, S., Nabi, H., Kivimaki, M., Shipley, M. J., Marmot, M. G., & Singh-Manoux, A.
gps.3865 (2009). Health behaviors from early to late mid-life as predictors of cognitive
Fernandez Martinez, M., Castro Flores, J., Perez de Las Heras, S., Mandaluniz Lekum- function. American Journal of Epidemiology, 170(4), 428–437. http://dx.doi.org/
berri, A., Gordejuela Menocal, M., & Zarranz Imirizaldu, J. J. (2008). Risk factors for 10.1093/aje/kwp161
dementia in the epidemiological study of Munguialde County (Basque Country- Samper-Ternent, R., Al Snih, S., Raji, M. A., Markides, K. S., & Ottenbacher, K. J. (2008).
Spain). BMC Neurology, 8, 39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2377-8-39 Relationship between frailty and cognitive decline in older Mexican Americans.
Fratiglioni, L., Paillard-Borg, S., & Winblad, B. (2004). An active and socially integrated American Geriatrics Society, 56(10), 1845–1852. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-
lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia. Lancet Neurology, 3(6), 343– 5415.2008.01947.x
353. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(04)00767-7 Shadlen, M. F., Siscovick, D., Fitzpatrick, A. L., Dulberg, C., Kuller, L. H., & Jackson, S.
Fried, L. P., Tangen, C. M., Walston, J., Newman, A. B., Hirsch, C., Gottdiener, J., et al. (2006). Education, cognitive test scores, and black-white differences in dementia
(2001). Frailty in older adults evidence for a phenotype. Journals of Gerontology risk. American Geriatrics Society, 54(6), 898–905. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-
Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 56(3), 146–157. http://dx.doi.org/ 5415.2006.00747.x
10.1093/gerona/56.3.M146 Shimada, H., Makizako, H., Doi, T., Yoshida, D., Tsutsumimoto, K., Anan, Y., et al. (2013).
Gorelick, P. B. (2005). William M. Feinberg Lecture: Cognitive vitality and the role of Combined prevalence of frailty and mild cognitive impairment in a population of
stroke and cardiovascular disease risk factors. Stroke, 36(4), 875–879. http:// elderly Japanese people. Journal of the American Medical Directors Association, 14(7),
dx.doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.0000158916.59742.7c 518–524. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jamda.2013.03.010
Han, E. S., Lee, Y., & Kim, J. (2014). Association of cognitive impairment with frailty in Teixeira, C. V., Gobbi, L. T., Corazza, D. I., Stella, F., Costa, J. L., & Gobbi, S. (2012). Non-
community-dwelling older adults. International Psychogeriatrics, 26(1), 155–163. pharmacological interventions on cognitive functions in older people with mild
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1041610213001841 cognitive impairment (MCI). Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 54(1), 175–180.
Hsu, H. (2007). Does social participation by the elderly reduce mortality and cognitive http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2011.02.014
impairment? Aging & Mental Health, 11(6), 699–707. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ Tzourio, C. (2007). Hypertension, cognitive decline, and dementia: An epidemiological
13607860701366335 perspective. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 9(1), 61–70. ISSN: 1294-8322.
Hsu, H. C. (2014). Relationship between frailty and cognitive function among older Wang, C.-C., Lu, T.-H., Liao, W.-C., Yuan, S.-C., Kuo, P.-C., Chuang, H.-L., et al. (2010).
adults in Taiwan. Journal of Frailty & Aging, 3(3), 153–157. Available at http:// Cigarette smoking and cognitive impairment: A 10-year cohort study in Taiwan.
www.jfrailtyaging.com/all-issues.html?article=191 Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 51(2), 143–148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
Kawas, C., Gray, S., Brookmeyer, R., Fozard, J., & Zonderman, A. (2000). Age-specific j.archger.2009.09.041
incidence rates of Alzheimer’s disease The Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Weimer, D. L., & Sager, M. A. (2009). Early identification and treatment of Alzheimer?s
Neurology, 54(11), 2072–2077. http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/WNL.54.11.2072 disease: social and fiscal outcomes. Alzheimer?s & Dementia, 5(3), 215–226. http://
Kilander, L., Nyman, H., Boberg, M., Hansson, L., & Lithell, H. (1998). Hypertension is dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2009.01.028
related to cognitive impairment a 20-year follow-up of 999 men. Hypertension, Winningham, R. G., & Pike, N. L. (2007). A cognitive intervention to enhance institu-
31(3), 780–786. http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/01.HYP.31.3.780 tionalized older adults’ social support networks and decrease loneliness. Aging &
Kilgour, A. H. M., Todd, O. M., & Starr, J. M. (2014). A systematic review of the evidence Mental Health, 11(6), 716–721. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607860701366228
that brain structure is related to muscle structure and their relationship to brain Yen, C. H., Yeh, C. J., Wang, C. C., Liao, W. C., Chen, S. C., Chen, C. C., et al. (2010).
and muscle function in human over the life course. BMC Geriatrics, 14, 85 Available Determinants of cognitive impairment over time among the elderly in Taiwan:
at http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2218-14-85 Results of the national longitudinal study. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 50,
Kohout, F. J., Berkman, L. F., Evans, D. A., & Cornoni-Huntley, J. (1993). Two shorter 53–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0167-4943(10)70014-5
forms of the CES-D depression symptoms index. Journal of Aging and Health, 5(2), Zunzunegui, M. V., Alvarado, B. E., Del Ser, T., & Otero, A. (2003). Social networks, social
179–193. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/089826439300500202 integration, and social engagement determine cognitive decline in community-
Loef, M., & Walach, H. (2013). Midlife obesity and dementia: Meta-analysis and dwelling Spanish older adults. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological
adjusted forecast of dementia prevalence in the United States and China. Obesity, Sciences and Social Sciences, 58(2), 93–100. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/
(1), 51–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/oby.20037 58.2.S93

You might also like