Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 19

Basic and Applied Social Psychology

ISSN: 0197-3533 (Print) 1532-4834 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hbas20

The Impact of Negative Gossip on Target and


Receiver. A “Big Two” Analysis

Nicole Hauke & Andrea E. Abele

To cite this article: Nicole Hauke & Andrea E. Abele (2020) The Impact of Negative Gossip on
Target and Receiver. A “Big Two” Analysis, Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 42:2, 115-132,
DOI: 10.1080/01973533.2019.1702881

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2019.1702881

View supplementary material

Published online: 20 Dec 2019.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 419

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 2 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=hbas20
BASIC AND APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
2020, VOL. 42, NO. 2, 115–132
https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2019.1702881

The Impact of Negative Gossip on Target and Receiver. A “Big Two” Analysis
Nicole Hauke and Andrea E. Abele
University of Erlangen-N€urnberg

ABSTRACT
Negative gossip can negatively influence the gossip target as well as the gossip receiver.
Building on the “Big Two” of agency and communion and their facets of assertiveness and
competence (agency) and warmth and morality (communion), we show in three studies
that negative gossip based on these four types of content differentially affect targets’ and
receivers’ reactions. Targets’ identity threat was particularly high after negative assertiveness
and warmth gossip, their reputation threat and their negative affect were especially high
after negative morality gossip. Receivers’ impressions of and negative affect toward the tar-
get were most negative after negative morality gossip. Findings are discussed with respect
to the Big Two framework and with respect to the target versus receiver perspective in
social cognition.

People spend between 65% (Dunbar, Marriott, & The impact of negative gossip on targets
Duncan, 1997) and 80–90% (Emler, 1994) of their and receivers
day-to-day conversations gossiping (see also
Gossip
Goldsmith & Baxter, 1996). Gossiping is considered to
be a type of speech that transmits information about Gossip always involves a social triad: a gossiper (the
the behaviors and attributes of absent third parties person telling the gossip), a target (the person the gos-
(Foster, 2004). Gossip can have beneficial effects on, sip is about), and a receiver/audience (the person/peo-
for instance, group cohesion (Fonseca & Peters, 2018; ple the gossip is transmitted to; Peters & Kashima,
Feinberg, Willer, Stellar, & Keltner, 2012; Wu, Balliet, 2015). Gossip can both be accurate or inaccurate
& Van Lange, 2015). But it can also have detrimental (through error or deliberate distortion), positive about
effects on the gossip target’s well-being, especially if the target or negative, and veracity of the gossip is dif-
the content of the gossip is negative (Peters & ficult to assess. Even when inaccurate, gossip will
Kashima, 2014). In this paper, we are concerned with always have some impact (Sommerfeld, Krambeck,
negative gossip containing false accusations and its Semmann, & Milinski, 2007). Gossip has a number of
impact on both the target (in situations in which the positive functions for the gossiper and the audience –
target is aware of the negative gossip) and the receiver to inform, maintain group norms, build trust, or sim-
of the gossip. More specifically, we apply the “Big ply for enjoyment (Feinberg, Willer, & Schultz, 2014;
Two” framework of agency and communion (Abele Beersma & Van Kleef, 2011, 2012; Feinberg et al.,
& Wojciszke, 2014, 2018; Bakan, 1966; Fiske, Cuddy, 2012; Fonseca & Peters, 2018; Wu et al., 2015). In
& Glick, 2007; Judd, James-Hawkins, Yzerbyt, & terms of the target, positive gossip may also have
Kashima, 2005; Paulhus & John, 1998) to a distinction positive consequences, but gossip often takes the form
of different types of gossip and analyze how more of negative talk about others (Dunbar, 2004; Wert &
agency-related versus more communion-related nega- Salovey, 2004) and this can be harmful, both when
tive gossip affects target and receiver. We present two the target is aware or unaware of the gossip. As an
studies that test targets’ reactions and one study that example, imagine that the target heard that others had
tests receivers’ reactions toward different types gossiped that he/she had disclosed private information
of gossip. that should have been kept secret. Even if the target

CONTACT Nicole Hauke nicole.hauke@fau.de urnberg, N€agelsbachstr. 49b, D-91052


Social Psychology Group, University of Erlangen-N€
Erlangen, Germany.
Supplemental material for this article can be accessed at www.tandfonline.com/hbas.
ß 2019 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
116 N. HAUKE AND A. E. ABELE

knows that the gossip was wrong, it can nevertheless other-profitable (Peeters, 2008) and signal to the
have negative effects, because he/she might experience observer if the actor has benevolent intentions toward
a threat to his/her reputation and might also feel him/her or not.
angry or sad. The agency-communion framework has seen broad
usage. Nonetheless, there are differences in the specific
ways in which this framework is conceptualized in the
Content of gossip: agency, communion, and
various research traditions what can be seen in the
their facets
labeling of the constructs: agency is also called
Gossip involves attributing behaviors and/or traits to “competence”, “masculinity”, or “instrumentality”;
the gossip target. A review of anthropological and communion is also called “warmth”, “femininity”, or
sociological studies showed that the most common “morality” (Abele, Cuddy, Judd, & Yzerbyt, 2008;
topics of gossip are “personal qualities and idiosyncra- Fiske, Cuddy, Glick, & Xu, 2002; Judd et al., 2005;
sies, behavioral surprises and inconsistencies, charac- Spence, Helmreich, & Stapp, 1974; Wojciszke, 2005).
ter flaws, discrepancies between actual behavior and Importantly, not all of these labels show conceptual
moral claims, bad manners, socially unaccepted modes equivalence. The specific conceptualization of
of behavior, shortcomings, improprieties, omissions, “competence” (i.e. “competent”, “smart”) is somewhat
presumptions, blamable mistakes, misfortunes, and different from the specific conceptualization of
failures” (Bergmann, 1993, p. 15). Although there are “masculinity” (i.e. “decisive”, “assertive”), even though
no limits on the specific content of gossip, we suggest both competence and masculinity form a common
that most gossip can be classified into one of two cluster of agency. Similarly, the specific conceptualiza-
major content dimensions: gossip referring to compe- tion of “warmth” (i.e. “friendly”, “empathetic”) is
tence/performance/assertiveness (or lack of these char- somewhat different from the specific conceptualization
acteristics) and gossip referring to friendliness/ of “morality” (i.e. “fair”, “honest”), even though they
warmth/trustworthiness (or lack of these characteris- form a common cluster of communion (see Abele &
tics; Peters & Kashima, 2015). These two classes of Wojciszke, 2007, Study 1). The differences in the
content are the fundamental content dimensions – labeling and operationalization of constructs are not
also called the “Big Two” – that have been revealed in spurious, but they instead reflect the multi-faceted
many fields of psychology, such as social cognition, nature of the fundamental dimensions which should
personality, motivation, and interpersonal behavior be taken into account for the sake of conceptual clar-
(for a review see Abele & Wojciszke, 2018). These ity and also because the different facets showed differ-
dimensions are referred to as agency and communion ent associations in empirical studies.
(Bakan, 1966). Agency focusses on the individual per- Research has proven that the fundamental dimen-
son and the pursuit of personal goals. Themes of sions or Big Two can be further sub-divided into two
agency are self-realization, striving for power and sta- facets each (Abele et al., 2016). Agency can be reliably
tus, and acting in one’s own interest and are summar- distinguished into the facets of agency-competence
ized under the label of “getting ahead” (Hogan, 1982). (competent, efficient) and agency-assertiveness (deter-
Communion focusses on community and social inte- mined, assertive). This distinction of agency can be
gration. Themes of communion are the formation and understood from the consideration that successful
maintenance of social relationships, striving for har- goal pursuit or “getting ahead” requires both ability
mony, and acting in the interest of others and are and motivation. Agency-competence (ability, capabil-
summarized under the label of “getting along” ity) reflects the former, more ability-related compo-
(Hogan, 1982). These fundamental dimensions reflect nent and agency-assertiveness (ambition, confidence)
the two recurring challenges of human life: pursuing reflects the latter, more motivation-related component.
individual goals and belonging to social groups Research has shown that the agency-assertiveness facet
(Ybarra et al., 2008). Agency and communion are dif- is more related to self-esteem as well as to status and
ferentially related to the two perspectives of social power than the agency-competence facet (Abele et al.,
cognition. According to the Dual Perspective Model 2016; Carrier, Louvet, Chauvin, & Rohmer, 2014).
(DPM, Abele & Wojciszke, 2014), agency is more Communion can be reliably distinguished into the
important in the actor perspective since agentic traits facets of communion-warmth (warm, caring) and com-
are self-profitable (Peeters, 2008) and help to pursue munion-morality (fair, trustworthy; Abele et al., 2016).
own goals and communion is more important in the The establishment and maintenance of social relation-
observer perspective since communal traits are ships or “getting along” needs behaviors that are on
BASIC AND APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 117

one hand warm and friendly and on the other fair Dual Perspective Model, the actor does not only want
and reliable. While warmth pertains to being benevo- to pursue his/her goals (“getting ahead”; like stated in
lent to people in ways that facilitate affectionate, the DPM) but also wants to be liked and respected by
cooperative relations with them, morality refers to others (“getting along”). Getting ahead needs particu-
being benevolent to people in ways that facilitate cor- lar reliance on agentic qualities like ability or assert-
rect and principled relations with them by the adher- iveness. Getting along needs particular reliance on
ence to ethics and important social values (Brambilla communal qualities like friendliness and trustworthi-
& Leach, 2014; Kervyn, Fiske, & Yzerbyt, 2015). Both ness. It was further reasoned that different parts of
warmth and morality related traits are intentional, the self-concept are related to these objectives and
and hence motivational, components (Fiske et al., they were called “self-as-identity” (related to pursuing
2002, 2007). Empirical work has investigated the own goals) and “self-as-reputation” (related to social
impact of “sociability” (in present terms: communion- connections). It was predicted that questioning a tar-
warmth), and of communion-morality on judgments get’s characteristics by telling negative gossip about
of other people and groups. This research revealed him/her should in dependence on the content of the
that judgments of others were more influenced by gossip differentially threaten identity or reputation.
communion-morality than by communion-warmth Negative gossip about agentic qualities was predicted
(for reviews see Brambilla & Leach, 2014; to increase identity threat. More specially, as research
Ellemers, 2017). has shown that the agency-assertiveness component is
We suggest that this distinction of four facets of more related to self-esteem and status than the
agency and communion can be applied to categorizing agency-competence component (Abele et al., 2016;
gossip content; and that distinguishing these four Carrier et al., 2014) and self-esteem and status consti-
types of gossip content is a means for analyzing the tute essential parts of self-as-identity, identity threat
effects gossip can have on both gossip targets and gos- was predicted to be highest if negative gossip was
sip receivers. about a target’s agency-assertiveness. In contrast,
negative gossip about communal qualities was pre-
dicted to increase reputation threat. More specially, as
Impact of agency-versus communion-related
already described, other work concerning the commu-
gossip on self-concept
nal facets has revealed that the communion-morality
A recent series of studies (Hauke & Abele, 2019) ana- component is more influential than the communion-
lyzed the impact of gossip with different agency- ver- warmth component in judgments of people and
sus communion-related content on the self-concept of groups (Abele & Hauke, 2019; Brambilla & Leach,
the target. The self-concept can be defined as a 2014) and therefore strongly related to reputation.
person’s belief about himself or herself including the Consequently, reputation threat was predicted to be
person’s attributes (Baumeister, 1999). The purpose of highest if the negative gossip was about a target’s
these studies was to reconcile seemingly inconsistent communion-morality.
results concerning the self in the agency-communion- This reasoning was tested in a series of studies
framework. On the one hand, people consider com- (Hauke & Abele, 2019). Two studies were based on an
munion to be more desirable than agency (Paulhus & autobiographical recollection methodology where par-
Trapnell, 2008), judge their communal attributes to be ticipants had to remember self-threatening situations
higher than their agentic attributes (for a review see or negative gossip situations in which they themselves
Abele & Wojciszke, 2014), and are more troubled if had been the target. The other two of these studies
their communion is discredited than if their agency is were based on a scenario approach (see Ybarra et al.,
discredited (Ybarra, Park, Stanik, & Lee, 2012; 2012), in which participants had to imagine that an
Pagliaro, Ellemers, Barreto, & Di Cesare, 2016). On acquaintance was telling false-negative gossip varying
the other hand, self-esteem as the evaluative compo- in content (either agency-assertiveness, agency-
nent of the self-concept is more related to agency competence, communion-warmth, or communion-
than to communion (Wojciszke, Baryla, morality) about the target, but the audience believed
Parzuchowski, Szymkow, & Abele, 2011). To bring in the gossip to be true.1 Supporting our predictions we
line these contradictory results regarding the relative found that (a) identity-threatening situations were, in
importance of the Big Two in self-concept, an fact, more agency-related and reputation-threatening
extended Dual Perspective Model was proposed situations were more communion-related and (b) that
(Hauke & Abele, 2019). According to this extended identity threat was highest in case of negative
118 N. HAUKE AND A. E. ABELE

agency-assertiveness gossip, whereas reputation threat tested at once and if therefore the previous findings of
was highest in case of negative communion- Study 3 can be replicated with different operationali-
morality gossip. zation of agency-assertiveness, agency-competence,
This previous research (Hauke & Abele, 2019), as communion-warmth, and communion-morality. This
well as the present research on the one hand, advan- conceptual replication was the first objective of the
ces theory in the domain of gossip research by apply- present work. Our hypotheses were the same as in the
ing the content of the agency-communion-framework previous work:
to gossip situations. Depending on the concrete Hypothesis 1: Identity threat is highest if negative
agentic or communal content of gossip, gossip elicits gossip is about lack of agency-assertiveness (H1).
different reactions. On the other hand, the previous
and present research also helps to advance theory in Hypothesis 2: Reputation threat is highest if negative
the agency-communion-framework itself by resolving gossip is about lack of communion-morality (H2).
inconsistencies regarding the importance of agency Second, we extended our research to the perspec-
and communion for the self with the help of the tive of gossip receivers. Because previous research has
extended Dual Perspective Model. shown that impression formation about other people
The present research not only builds on these pre- is strongly influenced by information on these per-
vious findings but extends them in several ways. From sons’ communion-morality (Abele & Hauke, 2019),
a methodological point of view, it extends them by we generally expected that gossip receivers form the
conducting a conceptual replication. More importantly, most negative impression about gossip targets if the
from a theoretical point of view, the present research negative gossip content is about communion-morality
extends our previous reasoning on gossip targets by (specific hypotheses see introduction to Study 3).
introducing also the gossip’s receiver perspective. These results would support the validity of H2 since
Finally, negative gossip should not only elicit cognitive the reputation threat of gossip targets is expected to
consequences for the gossip target and the gossip be highest if gossip receivers have the most negative
receiver but also emotional consequences and these are impression about the target.
also considered in the present research. We will Third, we extended our measures. We assessed the
explain our research agenda in detail in the following. negative affect of the target (hypotheses see introduc-
tion to Study 2) and negative affect toward the target
Overview of present research (hypotheses see introduction to Study 3) induced by
the respective gossip.2
The present series of studies had several aims. First, Throughout the present paper, we do not report
we wanted to conceptually replicate our previous find- statistical tests due to concerns with null hypothesis
ings regarding the impact of negative gossip on a per- significance testing (see Trafimow & Rice, 2009).
son’s self-concept (Hauke & Abele, 2019). We
consider a conceptual replication to be necessary
because in the previous series of studies, we had only Study 1
one study (Study 3) in which all four types of gossip Study 1 was conducted to conceptually replicate the
content were experimentally manipulated by means of findings that negative agency-assertiveness-related gos-
gossip scenarios and conceptual replication is an sip elicits more identity threat (H1), whereas negative
important means for validating research (Shrout & communion-morality-related gossip elicits more repu-
Rodgers, 2018) and it is always good to have multiple tation threat (H2; Hauke & Abele, 2019). Before run-
scenarios to enhance stimulus sampling and increase ning Study 1, we ran a pilot study. It pretested the
generalizability of the results (Wells & Windschitl, gossip stories used. We wanted to ensure that
1999). Thus, in the present research, we took the participants reliably inferred agency-assertiveness,
agentic and communal scenarios from Study 2 of agency-competence, communion-warmth, and com-
Hauke and Abele (2019), i.e., scenarios regarding munion-morality from the different gossip scenarios.
negative agency-competence and negative commu-
nion-morality content and added newly developed
Method
agency-assertiveness and communion-warmth scen-
arios. We did this in order to analyze if the previous Participants and procedure
findings of Study 2 (Hauke & Abele, 2019) can be The sample comprised 197 German university stu-
found if all four facets of agency and communion are dents (99 women, 98 men) with a mean age of 23.13
BASIC AND APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 119

(SD ¼ 3.62). The data were collected using a paper- Table 1. Pilot study: descriptive statistics for the traits called
and pencil survey. As an incentive, participants were into question separated by condition.
given candies after completing the questionnaire. Assertiveness Intelligence Morality Friendliness
(AA) (AC) (CM) (CW)
Participants first received one of the negative gossip
Condition M SD M SD M SD M SD
scenarios and then answered the dependent variables.
AA 6.04 1.07 3.64 1.78 3.28 1.52 2.57 1.46
AC 2.75 1.60 5.87 1.57 3.92 1.82 3.70 1.82
Negative gossip conditions CM 2.26 1.57 2.85 1.68 6.55 0.89 5.53 1.48
CW 2.04 1.21 2.66 1.65 5.38 1.36 6.17 0.89
Participants were asked to imagine being in a situ-
Notes. AA: agency-assertiveness; AC: agency-competence; CM: commu-
ation in which someone else was transmitting negative nion-morality; CW: communion-warmth.
gossip about them to an audience who believed the
gossip to be true, the target, however, knew that is
Table 2. Study 1: descriptive statistics for the dependent vari-
was false. In the agency-assertiveness condition ables separated by condition.
(n ¼ 50), the target was asked to imagine having had a AA AC CM CW
tough argument with an acquaintance3 during which M SD M SD M SD M SD
they agreed to a mutual compromise. Later the Identity threat 3.07 1.33 2.68 1.27 2.19 1.32 3.15 1.50
acquaintance gossiped to others, telling them that the Reputation threat 4.27 1.35 4.44 1.60 5.05 1.51 4.18 1.21
target had been easily won over. In the agency- Notes. AA: agency-assertiveness; AC: agency-competence; CM: commu-
nion-morality; CW: communion-warmth.
competence condition (n ¼ 48), the acquaintance
gossiped to others, telling them that the target was
than in the other three conditions, all ds > 2.41. And,
lacking the relevant skills to solve a difficult task. In
finally, intelligence was rated to have been more rele-
the communion-warmth condition (n ¼ 50), the target
vant in the agency-competence condition than in the
was asked to imagine that he/she had listened to an
other three conditions, all ds > 1.33. Moreover, within
acquaintance who described a personal problem. Later
the four conditions, the manipulated trait was always
the acquaintance gossiped to others, telling them that rated highest compared to the other three traits (e.g.
the participant had not been supportive and had not in the communion-warmth condition friendliness was
taken his/her problem seriously. In the communion- rated to have been more relevant than morality,
morality condition (n ¼ 49), the acquaintance gossiped assertiveness, and intelligence), all ds > 0.67.
to others, telling them that the target had failed to
pay money back and is denying to have borrowed it Dependent variables: identity threat and reputa-
at all. Complete wording of the stories can be found tion threat
in the supplemental online material. Participants reported their reactions by answering six
items (Hauke & Abele, 2019).5 Three items measured
Pilot study. The aim of the Pilot Study4 was to test identity threat (e.g. “I would think negatively about
the validity of the negative gossip stories in terms of myself if such things are told about me.”; Cronbach’s
agency-assertiveness, agency-competence, commu- a ¼ 0.81). Three items measured reputation threat
nion-morality, and communion-warmth. Using a (e.g. “I would be afraid that people would form a
within-participants design, 53 German participants negative impression of me.”; Cronbach’s a ¼ 0.79).
read the four stories in a randomized order. After The order of these items was randomized, and
reading each story participants rated how much the responses were given on a 7-point Likert scale ranging
traits “friendliness”, “morality”, “assertiveness”, and from 1 ¼ very unlikely to 7 ¼ very likely.
“intelligence” were called into question by the respect-
ive gossip on a scale from 1 ¼ not at all to 7 ¼ very
strongly. Results (means and standard deviations see Results and discussion
Table 1) demonstrated that the stories were appropri- Preliminary analyses
ately targeting the relevant facets. Friendliness, or lack Descriptive statistics for the dependent variables are
of, was considered to have been more relevant in the shown in Table 2. Preliminary analyses showed that
communion-warmth condition than in the other three there were only negligible differences between the
conditions, all ds > 0.48. Morality was considered to four experimental conditions concerning the distribu-
have been more relevant in the communion-morality tion of gender, Cramer’s V ¼ 0.01, and age, g2 ¼ 0.02.
condition than in the other three conditions, all The two dependent variables of identity threat and
ds > 0.99. Assertiveness was considered to have been reputation threat were also moderately corre-
more relevant in the agency-assertiveness condition lated, r ¼ 0.33.
120 N. HAUKE AND A. E. ABELE

7 identity threat contrasts revealed that gossip targets had a consider-


reputation threat ably higher reputation threat if the negative gossip was
6 about communion-morality compared to the other
three conditions, d ¼ 0.53. Single comparisons sup-
5
Ratings (1-7)

ported that reputation threat after negative commu-


4
nion-morality gossip was considerably higher than after
negative communion-warmth gossip, d ¼ 0.64, and
3 somewhat higher than after negative agency-
competence, d ¼ 0.40, and negative agency-assertiveness,
2 d ¼ 0.55, gossip. The other three conditions did not differ
substantially from each other, all ds < 0.19.
1 In summary, Study 1 conceptually replicated previ-
AA AC CM CW
negative gossip
ous findings (Hauke & Abele, 2019) that negative gos-
sip about communion-morality was especially
Figure 1. Study 1: identity threat and reputation threat sepa-
rated by condition. AA: agency-assertiveness; AC: agency-com-
reputation threatening (H2). However, identity threat
petence; CM; communion-morality; CW: communion-warmth. was high after both negative gossip on agency-
assertiveness (as expected in H1), but unexpectedly also
Hypotheses testing after negative gossip on communion-warmth. Thus, H1
We conducted a 4 (negative gossip condition: agency- could not be conceptually replicated in this study.
assertiveness vs. agency-competence vs. communion-
morality vs. communion-warmth) x 2 (type of Study 2
reaction: identity threat vs. reputation threat) ANOVA
with repeated measures on the second factor6. There Study 2 applied the same gossip stories as Study 1.
was a huge repeated measure effect, g2 ¼ 0.58 (identity We again tested if negative agency-assertiveness-
threat: M ¼ 2.77, SD ¼ 1.40; reputation threat: related gossip elicits more identity threat (H1),
M ¼ 4.49, SD ¼ 1.46, d ¼ 1.20). Most importantly, the whereas negative communion-morality-related gossip
interaction effect of gossip condition by type of reac- elicits more reputation threat (H2). Moreover, we
tion was also large, g2 ¼ 0.20 (Figure 1). tested if communion-warmth also elicits a high
We then conducted separate ANOVAs for the two amount of identity threat, as revealed in Study 1. In
types of reactions. The ANOVA with identity threat as addition, Study 2 included a new dependent variable,
the dependent variable showed a medium-sized effect i.e. negative affect.
of condition, g2 ¼ 0.07. Supporting H1, planned con- Study 1 has shown that false-negative gossip about
trasts revealed that gossip targets had a somewhat the own person above all leads to reputation threat
higher identity threat if the negative gossip was about (see higher means overall for reputation threat than
agency-assertiveness compared to the other three con- for identity threat). Therefore, also negative affect is
ditions, d ¼ 0.29. Single comparisons supported that expected to be highest when people feel most threat-
negative agency-assertiveness gossip induced somewhat ened by the negative gossip. Consequently, our
hypothesis regarding the association between gossip
higher identity threat than negative agency-
content and negative affect is the following:
competence gossip, d ¼ 0.30, and considerably higher
identity threat than negative communion-morality gos- Hypothesis 3: Negative affect is highest if negative
sip, d ¼ 0.67. But unexpectedly, identity threat was gossip is about a lack of communion-morality (H3).
comparably high after negative communion-warmth
gossip, d ¼ 0.05. Negative communion-warmth gossip
also led to a considerably higher identity threat than Method
negative communion-morality gossip, d ¼ 0.68, and Participants and procedure
somewhat higher identity threat than negative agency- The sample comprised 175 German university stu-
competence gossip, d ¼ 0.33. Identity threat after nega- dents (79 women, 96 men) with a mean age of 22.22
tive communion-morality gossip was somewhat lower (SD ¼ 3.69). The data were collected in a paper-and-
than after negative agency-competence gossip, d ¼ 0.38. pencil format. As an incentive, participants were given
The ANOVA with reputation threat as the depend- candies after completing the questionnaire. First par-
ent variable also resulted in a medium-sized effect of ticipants received the experimental manipulation and
condition, g2 ¼ 0.06. In line with H2, planned after that answered the dependent measures.
BASIC AND APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 121

Negative gossip conditions on the second factor7. There was a huge main effect
Instructions and negative gossip conditions were the of type of reaction, g2 ¼ 0.65, such that reputation
same as in Study 1 (agency-assertiveness: n ¼ 45; threat (M ¼ 4.25, SD ¼ 1.43) was higher than the iden-
agency-competence: n ¼ 40; communion-warmth: tity threat (M ¼ 2.35, SD ¼ 1.10), d ¼ 1.48. The inter-
n ¼ 50; communion-morality: n ¼ 40). Complete action of type of reaction  condition was of medium
wording of the stories can be found in the supplemen- effect size, g2 ¼ 0.05 (Figure 2).
tal online material. We then again conducted separate ANOVAs for the
two types of reactions. In the ANOVA with identity
Dependent variables: Identity threat, reputation threat as the dependent variable, the main effect of
threat, and negative affect condition was of medium size, g2 ¼ 0.05. Supporting
Negative affect was assessed as the first dependent H1, planned contrasts revealed that gossip targets had
variable. It was measured by means of 12 items a somewhat higher identity threat if the negative gossip
(“afraid”, “angry”, “ashamed”, “depressed”, was about agency-assertiveness compared to the other
“distressed”, “guilty”, “hostile”, “hurt”, “irritable”, three conditions, d ¼ 0.25. Single comparisons sup-
“nervous”, “sad”, “upset”) which are based on appro- ported that negative agency-assertiveness gossip
priate negative items of the PANAS (Watson, Clark, induced considerably higher identity threat than nega-
& Tellegen, 1988) complemented by some items suit-
tive agency-competence gossip, d ¼ 0.46, and also
able for the gossip scenarios of the present studies.
somewhat higher identity threat than negative commu-
Participants answered the items on a 7-point Likert
nion-morality gossip, d ¼ 0.32. But again, identity
scale ranging from 1 ¼ strongly disagree to 7 ¼ strongly
threat was equally high after negative communion-
agree (Cronbach’s a ¼ 0.81). Identity threat and repu-
warmth gossip, d ¼ 0.03. Negative communion-warmth
tation threat were measured afterward with the same
gossip also led to a considerably higher identity threat
items as in Study 1. Cronbach’s a was 0.72 for identity
than negative agency-competence gossip, d ¼ 0.50, and
threat and 0.83 for reputation threat.
to somewhat higher identity threat than communion-
morality gossip, d ¼ 0.36. Identity threat after negative
Results and discussion communion-morality gossip was slightly higher than
Preliminary analyses after negative agency-competence gossip, d ¼ 0.20.
Descriptive statistics for the dependent variables are In the ANOVA with reputation threat as the
shown in Table 3. Preliminary analyses showed that dependent variable, the main effect of condition
there were only negligible differences between the was of small-to-medium effect-size, g2 ¼ 0.03.
four experimental conditions concerning the distribu- Nevertheless, the means were in accordance with H2:
tion of gender, Cramer’s V ¼ 0.12, and age, g2<0.01. Planned contrasts showed that gossip targets had a
Reputation threat and identity threat were moderately somewhat higher reputation threat if the negative gos-
correlated, r ¼ 0.37. Negative affect showed a weak sip was about communion-morality compared to the
correlation with identity threat, r ¼ 0.22, and a stron-
ger correlation with reputation threat, r ¼ 0.45. 7 identity threat
reputation threat
6
Hypotheses testing negative affect
Identity threat and reputation threat. We conducted
5
Ratings (1-7)

a 4 (negative gossip condition: agency-assertiveness vs.


agency-competence vs. communion-morality vs. com- 4
munion-warmth)  2 (type of reaction: identity threat
vs. reputation threat) ANOVA with repeated measures 3

Table 3. Study 2: descriptive statistics for the dependent vari- 2


ables separated by condition.
AA AC CM CW 1
AA AC CM CW
M SD M SD M SD M SD
negative gossip
Identity threat 2.53 1.19 2.01 1.07 2.20 0.87 2.57 1.14
Reputation threat 4.39 1.46 4.00 1.41 4.57 1.45 4.07 1.37 Figure 2. Study 2: identity threat, reputation threat, and nega-
Negative affect 3.47 0.95 3.45 0.93 3.93 0.79 3.65 0.86 tive affect separated by condition. AA: agency-assertiveness;
Notes. AA: agency-assertiveness; AC: agency-competence; CM: commu- AC: agency-competence; CM: communion-morality; CW: com-
nion-morality; CW: communion-warmth. munion-warmth.
122 N. HAUKE AND A. E. ABELE

other three conditions, d ¼ 0.29. Single comparisons gossip refers to communion-morality than if it refers
supported that reputation threat was somewhat higher to communion-warmth (H4a).
after negative communion-morality gossip than after
Hypothesis 4b: Negative gossip about both communal
negative communion-warmth, d ¼ 0.35, and agency-
facets leads to a more negative impression than
competence, d ¼ 0.40, gossip, but only very slightly negative gossip about both agentic facets (H4b).
higher than after negative agency-assertiveness gossip,
d ¼ 0.12. Negative agency-assertiveness gossip also We based these predictions once on findings showing
induced a somewhat higher reputation threat than that morality information has a higher impact on impres-
negative communion-warmth, d ¼ 0.22, and agency- sion formation than sociability (here communion-
competence, d ¼ 0.27, gossip. Reputation threat after warmth) information (Abele & Hauke, 2019; Ellemers,
negative communion-warmth and agency-competence 2017). On the other hand, we based the prediction that
gossip was comparably high, d ¼ 0.05. negative communal gossip has a stronger influence on
receivers than negative agentic gossip on findings that
Negative affect. We conducted an ANOVA with communion is generally more important than agency
negative gossip condition (agency-assertiveness vs. in the receiver perspective (Abele & Wojciszke, 2014).
agency-competence vs. communion-morality vs. com- We also assessed gossip receivers’ negative affect
munion-warmth) for the dependent variable of nega- toward the gossip target and predicted that it shows
tive affect.8 There was a small-to-medium sized main the same pattern as negative impression. That means
effect of condition, g2 ¼ 0.04 (Figure 2). In support of negative affect toward the gossip target is also
H3, planned contrasts revealed that gossip targets expected to be highest after negative communion-
reported considerably higher negative affect if the morality gossip and in general higher after negative
negative gossip was about communion-morality com- gossip about communion than negative gossip about
pared to the other three conditions, d ¼ 0.46. Single agency, given the primacy of communion, especially
comparisons supported that negative affect after nega- of communion-morality, in the receiver perspective.
tive communion-morality gossip was somewhat higher This leads to the following hypotheses:
than after negative communion-warmth gossip, Hypothesis 5a: Gossip receivers express higher
d ¼ 0.34, and considerably higher than after negative negative affect towards the gossip target if the
agency-competence, d ¼ 0.56, and agency-assertive- negative gossip refers to communion-morality than if
ness, d ¼ 0.52, gossip. Negative communion-warmth it refers to communion-warmth (H5a).
gossip led to slightly higher negative affect than
negative agency-competence, d ¼ 0.23, and agency- Hypothesis 5b: Negative gossip about both communal
facets leads to higher negative affect towards the
assertiveness, d ¼ 0.19, gossip. Negative affect after gossip target than negative gossip about both agentic
negative agency-competence and agency-assertiveness facets (H5b).
gossip were comparably high, d ¼ 0.03.
In summary, Study 2 replicated the findings of Study As the aim of Study 3 was to test our hypotheses
1 that negative gossip about agency-assertiveness and not only in a student sample but in a more heteroge-
communion-warmth led to comparably high amounts neous sample which is more representative for the
of identity threat. Thus, H1 was not supported. It was general population, we tried to get a large sample in
also replicated that reputation threat was highest after Study 3 consisting also of a substantial number
negative gossip on communion-morality (H2). As a new of employees.
finding, Study 2 supported H3 that negative affect was
higher after negative gossip about communion-morality Method
than after negative gossip about agency-assertiveness,
agency-competence, and communion-warmth. Participants and procedure
The sample consisted of 969 participants (754 women,
215 men) who were M ¼ 27.48 (SD ¼ 8.92) years old.
Study 3 It was a mixed sample with different educational
backgrounds and with different occupations. The data
Our final study was conducted from the gossip were collected online. The link to the online question-
receiver perspective and tested the follow- naire was posted on popular internet platforms and
ing hypotheses: distributed via an e-mail snowball system. Participants
Hypothesis 4a: Gossip receivers have a more negative first read the gossip story and then answered the
impression about the gossip target if the negative impression measure and then the negative affect
BASIC AND APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 123

measure. Participants received no incentive for com- Table 4. Study 3: descriptive statistics for the dependent vari-
pleting the questionnaire. ables separated by condition.
AA AC CM CW

Negative gossip conditions M SD M SD M SD M SD


Participants were asked to imagine a situation in Negative impression 2.84 0.91 2.34 0.99 4.30 1.08 4.01 1.08
Women (n ¼ 754) 2.81 0.88 2.31 0.93 4.42 1.07 4.00 1.11
which an acquaintance tells negative gossip about a Men (n ¼ 215) 2.94 1.00 2.45 1.19 3.91 1.03 4.04 0.96
mutual friend called Christian (for male participants) Negative affect 2.22 1.08 1.82 1.06 3.30 1.28 3.07 1.27
Women (n ¼ 754) 2.23 1.06 1.80 0.99 3.47 1.26 3.16 1.29
or Christina (for female participants). The content of Men (n ¼ 215) 2.18 1.15 1.87 1.29 2.74 1.16 2.71 1.14
the negative gossip conditions was the same as in Notes. AA: agency-assertiveness; AC: agency-competence; CM: commu-
nion-morality; CW: communion-warmth.
Studies 1 and 2 but modified to the gossip receiver
perspective (agency-assertiveness: n ¼ 234; agency-
7 negative
competence: n ¼ 266; communion-warmth: n ¼ 226; impression
communion-morality: n ¼ 243). Complete wording of negative affect
6
the stories can be found in the supplemental
online material.
5

Dependent variables: impression and nega- 4


tive affect
Participants assessed the impression they had about 3
Christian/Christina on 7 items (male versions: “I
would think negatively of Christian.”, “This would 2
leave a negative impression of Christian.”, “I would
doubt the impression I have had of Christian so far.”, 1
“I would think that Christian has done nothing AA AC CM CW
wrong.” (recoded), “I would not care about what the Figure 3. Study 3: gossip receiver’s negative impression of
acquaintance was telling, my opinion of Christian and negative affect toward the gossip target separated by con-
would remain the same.” (recoded), “I would shelter dition. AA: agency-assertiveness; AC: agency-competence; CM:
communion-morality; CW: communion-warmth.
Christian.” (recoded), “I would give Christian a wide
berth.”).9 These items were presented in mixed order
and were answered on a 7-point rating scale ranging Hypothesis testing
from 1 ¼ very unlikely to 7 ¼ very likely. Thus, higher Impression. We conducted a single-factor (negative
ratings represent a more negative impression. The gossip condition: agency-assertiveness vs. agency-
scale showed good reliability (Cronbach’s a ¼ 0.84). competence vs. communion-morality vs. communion-
Subsequently, participants were asked how they would warmth) ANOVA. It resulted in a large main effect of
feel toward Christian/Christina. Their negative affect condition, g2 ¼ 0.40 (Figure 3). Supporting H4a,
toward the gossip target was measured by 6 items planned contrasts revealed that gossip receivers by far
(“angry”, “confused”, “depressed”, “frightened”, had the most negative impression if the negative gos-
“irritated”, “sad”). This scale also showed good reli- sip was about communion-morality compared to the
ability (Cronbach’s a ¼ 0.86).10 other three conditions, d ¼ 1.22. Single comparisons
showed that the impression after negative commu-
nion-morality gossip was somewhat more negative
Results and discussion
than after negative communion-warmth gossip,
Preliminary analyses d ¼ 0.27, and much more negative than after negative
Descriptive statistics for the dependent variables are agency-assertiveness, d ¼ 1.48, and agency-compe-
shown in Table 4. Preliminary analyses showed that tence, d ¼ 1.90, gossip. Supporting H4b, the
there were only negligible differences between the impression of the gossip target after negative commu-
four experimental conditions concerning the distribu- nion-warmth gossip was much more negative than the
tion of gender, Cramer’s V ¼ 0.02, age, g2<.01, and impression after both negative agency-competence,
education, Cramer’s V ¼ 0.04. The two dependent var- d ¼ 1.62, and negative agency-assertiveness gossip,
iables of negative impression and negative affect were d ¼ 1.18. Moreover, the impression after negative
highly correlated, r ¼ 0.66. agency-assertiveness gossip was considerably more
124 N. HAUKE AND A. E. ABELE

negative than the impression after negative agency- participants and one for male participants. The
competence gossip, d ¼ 0.52. ANOVA with female participants resulted in a huge
main effect of condition, g2 ¼ 0.43. Supporting H4a,
Negative affect. We conducted a single-factor (nega- single comparisons revealed that female gossip
tive gossip condition: agency-assertiveness vs. receivers had the most negative impression if the
agency-competence vs. communion-morality vs. com- negative gossip was about communion-morality com-
munion-warmth) ANOVA. It resulted in a large main pared to the other three conditions, all ds > 0.39.
effect of condition, g2 ¼ 0.22 (Figure 3). Supporting Supporting H4b, the impression of the gossip target
H5a, planned contrasts revealed that gossip receivers after negative communion-warmth gossip was more
by far felt the highest negative affect toward the target negative than the impression after both negative
if the negative gossip was about communion-morality agency-competence and negative agency-assertiveness
compared to the other three conditions, d ¼ 0.80. gossip, both ds > 1.19. Moreover, the impression after
Single comparisons showed that their affect after negative agency-assertiveness gossip was more nega-
negative communion-morality gossip was slightly tive than the impression after negative agency-
more negative than after negative communion-warmth competence gossip, d ¼ 0.55. The ANOVA with male
gossip, d ¼ 0.19, and much more negative than after participants also showed a huge main effect of condi-
negative agency-assertiveness, d ¼ 0.92, and agency- tion, g2 ¼ 0.29. As expected in H4a, single compari-
competence, d ¼ 1.26, gossip. Supporting H5b, the sons revealed that male gossip receivers had a more
affect toward the target after negative communion- negative impression if the negative gossip was about
warmth gossip was much more negative than the communion-morality compared to the agency-compe-
affect after both negative agency-competence, d ¼ 1.06, tence and agency-assertiveness conditions, both
and negative agency-assertiveness gossip, d ¼ 0.72. ds > 0.96. However, the negative impression after
Moreover, the affect toward the target after negative negative communion-morality and communion-
agency-assertiveness gossip was somewhat more nega- warmth gossip were comparably high for male gossip
tive than the affect after negative agency-competence receivers, d ¼ 0.13. This is in contrast to the results
gossip, d ¼ 0.38. concerning female gossip receivers and also in con-
trast to H4a. Supporting H4b, the impression of the
Explorative analyses of gender effects gossip target after negative communion-warmth gos-
In contrast to Study 1 (where there was no interaction sip was more negative than the impression after both
effect with gender) and Study 2 (where we had an negative agency-competence and negative agency-
interaction effect with gender but the sample was too assertiveness gossip, both ds > 1.13. Moreover, also for
small for separate analyses), we analyzed interaction male participants, the impression after negative
effects with gender in Study 3. The means and stand- agency-assertiveness gossip was more negative than
ard deviations for both dependent variables separated the impression after negative agency-competence gos-
by gender and condition can be found in Table 4. sip, d ¼ 0.44.

Impression. We conducted a 4 (negative gossip condi- Negative affect. We conducted a 4 (negative gossip
tion: agency-assertiveness vs. agency-competence vs. condition: agency-assertiveness vs. agency-competence
communion-morality vs. communion-warmth)  2 vs. communion-morality vs. communion-warmth) 
(gender: female vs. male) ANOVA. It resulted in a 2 (gender: female vs. male) ANOVA. It resulted in a
large main effect of condition, g2¼0.29. The main large main effect of condition, g2 ¼ 0.13. There was
effect of gender was negligible, g2<0.01. However, the also a small main effect of gender, g2 ¼ 0.01. Women
interaction of gender and condition showed a small (M ¼ 2.64, SD ¼ 1.34) generally felt somewhat more
effect, g2 ¼ 0.01. Single comparisons revealed that negative affect toward the gossip target than men
women had a considerably more negative impression (M ¼ 2.36, SD ¼ 1.24), d ¼ 0.22. The interaction of
of the gossip target after negative communion- gender and condition also showed a small effect,
morality gossip than men, d ¼ 0.48. In the other three g2¼0.01. Single comparisons revealed that women
conditions, the negative impression of the gossip tar- only felt considerably more negative affect toward the
get was comparable high between women and men, gossip target than men after negative communion-
all ds < 0.14. morality gossip, d ¼ 0.59, and also somewhat more
In order to more closely analyze the gender effects, negative affect after negative communion-warmth gos-
we conducted two ANOVAS: one for female sip, d ¼ 0.36. In the agency-competence and agency-
BASIC AND APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 125

assertiveness conditions, the negative affect toward the relatively small, whereas the differences between the
gossip target did not differ substantially between communal and agentic conditions, in general, were
women and men, both ds < 0.07. large. These findings are in line with the prediction of
We again conducted separate ANOVAs for female primacy of communion in the receiver perspective.
and male participants. The ANOVA with female par- Explorative gender analyses also showed that the
ticipants resulted in a huge main effect of condition, effects concerning the basic dimensions (H4b and
g2 ¼ 0.26. Supporting H5a, single comparisons H5b) can be found for women and men, whereas H4a
revealed that female gossip receivers felt the most and H5a could only be supported for female partici-
negative affect if the negative gossip was about com- pants. Male gossip receivers reported a less negative
munion-morality compared to the other three condi- impression of the gossip target and felt less negative
tions, all ds > 0.24. Supporting H5b, the negative affect toward the gossip target after negative commu-
affect toward the gossip target after negative commu- nion-morality gossip compared to female participants.
nion-warmth gossip was higher than after both nega- As a consequence, within the male sample, negative
tive agency-competence and negative agency- impression and negative affect did not differ between
assertiveness gossip, both ds > 0.78. Moreover, the negative communion-morality and negative commu-
negative affect after negative agency-assertiveness gos- nion-warmth gossip.
sip was higher than after negative agency-competence
gossip, d ¼ 0.42. The ANOVA with male participants
General discussion
also showed a medium-to-large main effect of condi-
tion, g2 ¼ 0.09. As expected in H5a, single compari- Our goal in the present article was to analyze the
sons revealed that male gossip receivers felt more effects of different content of negative gossip on both
negative affect if the negative gossip was about com- the target and the receiver of the gossip. We applied
munion-morality compared to the agency-competence the “Big Two” framework of agency and communion
and agency-assertiveness conditions, both ds > 0.48. (Abele & Wojciszke, 2014, 2018) with their two facets
However, the negative affect after negative communion- each (Abele et al., 2016) to distinguish between four
morality and communion-warmth gossip did not differ types of negative gossip (negative gossip about
substantially for male gossip receivers, d ¼ 0.03. This is agency-assertiveness, agency-competence, commu-
again in contrast to the results concerning female gos- nion-warmth, and communion-morality). We studied
sip receivers and also in contrast to H5a. Supporting how these different types of negative gossip differen-
H5b, the negative affect toward the gossip target after tially influence both target’s (Studies 1 and 2) and
negative communion-warmth gossip was higher than receiver’s (Study 3) reactions. The present research is
after both negative agency-competence and negative both a conceptual replication and extension of previ-
agency-assertiveness gossip, both ds > 0.45. Moreover, ous work (Hauke & Abele, 2019), testing the validity
for male participants, the negative affect after negative of previous findings and extending them by analyzing
agency-assertiveness gossip was only somewhat higher an additional depending variable (i.e. negative affect).
than after negative agency-competence gossip, d ¼ 0.25. Moreover, combing the results of the gossip target
This is again differing from the results concerning perspective with the results of the gossip receiver per-
female gossip receivers. spective underlines the validity of the findings.
In summary, regarding the whole sample, in the
receiver perspective negative gossip about a target’s
Target reactions to negative gossip
lack of communion led to a more negative impression
than lack of agency (H4b) and lack of communion- Being the target of negative gossip can pose a threat
morality to an even more negative impression than to a person’s self-concept both regarding his/her iden-
lack of communion-warmth (H4a). Negative agency- tity and his/her reputation. Extending previous find-
assertiveness gossip also led to a more negative ings (Hauke & Abele, 2019) and in accord with our
impression than negative agency-competence gossip. predictions, reputation threat (H2), as well as negative
Negative affect (H5a and H5b) showed the same pat- affect (H3), were highest after negative communion-
tern with the highest negative affect after negative morality gossip.
communion-morality gossip and lowest negative affect Identity threat was always lower than reputation
after negative agency-competence gossip. However, threat, probably due to the fact that the targets knew
considering effect sizes, the differences between com- that the negative gossip spread about them was
munion-morality and communion-warmth were wrong. Hence, it did lead to reputation threat, but
126 N. HAUKE AND A. E. ABELE

only somewhat to identity threat. In both studies, warmth behavior are generally less clear than norms
identity threat was relatively high after negative regarding agency-assertiveness, agency-competence,
agency-assertiveness gossip, but also after negative and communion-morality behavior. What is the
communion-warmth gossip. Whereas the first finding “norm” in case a friend asks you for advice on a per-
was expected and fits our reasoning that threats to sonal problem? How much time should one spend on
agency-assertiveness are particularly identity-threaten- this issue? What is expected to be adequate?
ing (H1), the last-mentioned finding was These questions do not have a clear-cut answer and
not predicted. there is much more room for divergent interpretations
We suggest two explanations of why H1 could not than in case someone shows insufficient skill
be conceptually replicated by the present research and (agency-competence), is indecisive and hesitant
why identity threat was comparably high after nega- (agency-assertiveness), or does not give money back
tive communion-warmth gossip. One relates to the (communion-morality). We will come back to this
specific operationalization: Whereas previous research issue later.
used an operationalization of negative communion- To sum up the findings for the gossip target per-
warmth gossip that could be easily verified as being spective: Previous findings regarding reactions to
objectively wrong (gossip of having not been helpful negative communion-morality gossip (Hauke & Abele,
when in fact the target had objectively helped an 2019) could be replicated and extended. Reputation
acquaintance), the present operationalization was threat is highest after negative communion-morality
more open to debate (negative gossip of not having gossip about the own person. Moreover, negative
been attentive enough to the needs of a friend, but affect is also highest. However, the previous findings
subjectively having been attentive enough). The nega- regarding identity threat could not be clearly concep-
tive gossip in combination with the subjective nature tually replicated. In line with previous research
of judging attentiveness and the divergence in judg- (Hauke & Abele, 2019), after negative agency-
ments between the target and his/her friend might assertiveness gossip, identity threat was relatively high
lead the target on rethinking his/her attentiveness. in both studies. But gossip about other facets, espe-
And possibly, the target comes to the conclusion that cially communion-warmth, also elicited identity
he/she might in fact not have been attentive enough threat. A possible interpretation of this pattern of
although the target at first thought that he/she has results could be that agency-assertiveness is always
been sensitive to the friend’s problem. Hence, the important for self-as-identity, but that other facets are
unexpectedly high identity threat after this negative important as well at least under certain conditions,
communion-warmth gossip may be due to the fact e.g. communion-warmth in friendships or agency-
that the target reevaluates his/her attentiveness. competence in the workplace. To clarify the reasons
Another point concerning the specific operationali- for this, future studies with different gossip scenarios
zation of the gossip stories could be that the content or completely different experimental procedures
of the agency-assertiveness scenario seems to be less should be conducted.
negative than the content of the other conditions, i.e.,
being easily won over in an argument is less negative
Receiver reactions to negative gossip
compared to denying borrowing money like in the
communion-morality condition. But this is not simply The present research also extended previous findings
an unlucky operationalization of the agency-assertiveness by analyzing the effect of gossip content from the per-
gossip stories, it is rather part of the phenomenon spective of gossip receivers. Gossip receivers had the
itself. From the perspective of an observer, like the most negative impression about the gossip target
reader of this research or the receiver of the gossip, (H4a) and also the most negative affect toward him/
agentic misbehavior is always rated as less severe than her (H5a) when the negative gossip was about com-
communal misbehavior (see also DPM, Abele & munion-morality. These findings are in accordance
Wojciszke, 2014). Nevertheless, identity threat was with our hypothesis that communion-morality behav-
relatively high after negative agency-assertiveness iors are most important in the perspective of a
gossip. This can be interpreted as a hint that agency- receiver (Abele & Wojciszke, 2014). In general, results
assertiveness is in fact of high centrality for the self-as- showed that the reaction of the gossip receiver was
identity as stated in H1. more negative when the negative gossip was about
The second interpretation is more general: It could communion (also including communion-warmth gos-
be that norms or guidelines concerning communion- sip) than when it was about agency (both agency-
BASIC AND APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 127

assertiveness and agency-competence; H4b & H5b). interpretation if regarded from different perspectives
These findings add evidence to the primacy of com- (here: target versus receiver of the gossip).
munion in the receiver perspective (Abele & To sum up the findings for the gossip receiver per-
Wojciszke, 2014). Whereas previous theorizing and spective: Negative impression of the target and nega-
findings stressed the particular importance of commu- tive affect toward the target was highest after negative
nion-morality in judging others (Ellemers, 2017; communal, especially communion-morality gossip.
Harnick & Ellemers, 2014) the present findings once Combining the findings of the gossip receiver perspec-
support this reasoning and on the other hand extend tive with the findings of the gossip target perspective
it by showing that communion-warmth (particularly if underlines the validity and reasonableness of the
experienced as negative communion-warmth) has also results: Gossip targets experience the highest reputa-
impact on evaluating others. tion threat after negative communion-morality gossip
However, explorative gender analyses showed that because gossip receivers actually have the most nega-
this pattern of results only holds true for female gos- tive impression of them if negative communion-
sip receivers. Male gossip receivers also reported a morality gossip was told about them. These results
more negative impression of the gossip target and felt show that people take the perspective of others in
more negative affect toward the gossip target if the order to monitor their reputation.
negative gossip was about lack of communion than if Altogether, previous research (Hauke & Abele,
it was about lack of agency (H4b & H5b). Thus, 2019) in combination with the conceptual replication
although women are expected to show more commu- of the present research advances theory in the domain
nion, while men are expected to show more agency of gossip research by applying the content of the
(Eagly, 2009), there is a primacy of communion in the agency-communion-framework to gossip situations.
perception of others for both genders (see also Abele Depending on the concrete agentic or communal con-
tent of gossip, gossip elicits different reactions.
& Wojciszke, 2014). But men did not react more
Moreover, the present research shows that this is not
negatively following negative communion-morality
only true for cognitive reactions (identity threat and
gossip compared to negative communion-warmth gos-
reputation threat) but also for emotional reactions
sip. Hence, these results are contradictory to H4a and
(negative affect). In addition, the present research
H5a and to the results of the female gossip receivers.
expanded the focus from the reactions of gossip tar-
Hence, present results raise the question if there is a
gets also to the reactions of gossip receivers.
primacy of communion-morality only for women, but
not for men. However, these interpretation needs fur-
ther replication and future research (see also section Theoretical implications
direction for future research). The present results have shown that applying the Big
Whereas negative reactions upon negative commu- Two Framework – particularly the facet approach to
nion-warmth and communion-morality gossip were the Big Two – to the context of gossip helps to under-
stronger than negative reactions to both negative stand how targets of negative gossip, as well as
agency-assertiveness and agency-competence gossip, receivers of negative gossip, will react. According to
there was a difference in negative reactions to negative the Dual Perspective Model (Abele & Wojciszke,
agency-assertiveness versus agency-competence gossip, 2014, 2018) actor and observer have different goals in
i.e., negative agency-assertiveness gossip elicited a an interaction: The recipient/observer wants to know
more negative reaction toward the target than nega- if the actor’s actions are benevolent or harmful to
tive agency-competence gossip (at least for female gos- him/her. Hence, the recipient focuses on actor’s com-
sip receivers). This unexpected difference could be munal behaviors and reacts most negatively (impres-
explained on the operational level: The negative sion, affect), when the target has committed a failure
agency-competence gossip story told that the target in the communal (especially in the communion-mor-
produced a wrong solution when trying to help ality) domain. The actor wants to pursue his/her goals
another person. This is a failure of competence. and is therefore oriented at own agentic behaviors.
However, it also entails some communion, as the tar- Hence, negative gossip about his/her agency-assertive-
get at least tried to help the other person. Such an ness negatively affects his/her self-view, i.e., identity.
element of communion is not entailed in the negative However, as stated in the extended Dual Perspective
agency-assertiveness gossip story. On a theoretical Model (Hauke & Abele, 2019), the actor not only
level, this shows that behaviors are open to wants to pursue own goals as self-as-identity, but also
128 N. HAUKE AND A. E. ABELE

wants to be accepted and respected by others as self- 2000; for a review see Foster, 2004), and albeit we
as-reputation. Hence, he/she also has to consider know that people are adept at regularly using
others’ needs. This is why he/she experiences a repu- meta-perceptions in real life and that even though
tation threat when negative gossip about morality is when only imagining a situation, people adapt their
spread. In sum, considering “perspective” (actor ver- behaviors accordingly (see also Pagliaro et al., 2016), an
sus observer; actor-self versus observer-self) helps to additional methodology that is closer to real-life situa-
understand social evaluation, also in the context of tions would have been desirable. But because of ethical
negative gossip. Moreover, the combination of previ- reasons, we could not expose participants to real nega-
ous (Hauke & Abele, 2019) and present research helps tive gossip (see also Hauke & Abele, 2019).11 However,
to advance theory in the agency-communion- a related study (Hauke & Abele, 2019, Study 4) used
framework by resolving inconsistencies regarding the an autobiographical recollection methodology which is
importance of agency and communion for the self closer to real-life situations (Baker & Guttfreund, 1993;
with the help of the extended Dual Perspective Model: Williams, Conway, & Cohen, 2008) to test similar
Why are people more troubled if their communion is hypotheses and found comparable results.
discredited than if their agency is discredited (Ybarra Another potential limitation could be that gossip
et al., 2012; Pagliaro et al., 2016) if self-evaluation is usually contains content related to more than one of
more related to agency than to communion the facets of agency and communion. However, our
(Wojciszke et al., 2011)? The answer is that the self pilot study demonstrated that the manipulations were
consists of two parts and the relative importance of successful and participants correctly recognized the
the Big Two differs depending on the part of the self relevant content.
under consideration. Communion is more important
for self-as-reputation while the agency is more
Directions for future research
important for self-as-identity. This complex nature of
the self seems to be deeply entrenched in cognition We used the same gossip scenarios in the three stud-
since different parts of the self are not only related to ies to conceptually replicate the findings of previous
different content but are also represented in different research (Hauke & Abele, 2019). Of course, it is
parts of the brain (Fingelkurts & Fingelkurts, 2017; always good to have multiple scenarios to enhance
Fingelkurts, Fingelkurts, & Kallio-Tamminen, 2016). stimulus sampling and increase the generalizability of
The present results are also important for the con- the results (Wells & Windschitl, 1999). Thus, future
ceptualization of the Big Two into two facets each. studies could again use different scenarios.
The findings clearly show that the distinction between A point that should be further elaborated on in
agency-assertiveness versus agency-competence and future research on gossip is the role of clarity of norms
between communion-warmth and communion- and guidelines for behavior. As has been argued above,
morality is useful for understanding social evaluation. clarity of norms and guidelines for agency-assertive-
Communion-morality is particularly important for ness, agency-competence, communion-warmth, and
evaluations of others and for self-evaluations regard- communion-morality behaviors (positive and negative
ing the “observer-self”, more important than commu- ones) might be different, and therefore the interpret-
nion-warmth. Similarly, agency-assertiveness is ation of specific behaviors on the side of both actors
particularly important for the “actor-self”, more and observers (or targets and receivers of negative gos-
important than agency-competence. sip) might be more or less clear-cut. In the case of tar-
gets of negative gossip, identity threat is expected to be
triggered more easily if the norm is unclear. In con-
Limitations
trast, in the case of receivers of negative gossip, nega-
The strength of the present studies is that they applied tive evaluation might appear more easily in case of
standardized experimental procedures. However, a clear norms and violation of these norms.
potential limitation may be that participants had to Another factor that could influence the target’s
imagine the respective gossip situations and did not reaction to gossip is the (in-)accuracy of the gossip
experience them directly. Even though most experi- content. In the gossip studies used in the present
mental research on gossip has used such an approach research, the content of the gossip was inaccurate.
and thus far this approach has produced fruitful Nevertheless, the gossip targets experienced identity
results (e.g. Blumberg, 1972; Kuttler, Parker, & La threat to a little extent, especially following agency-
Greca, 2002; Wilson, Wilczynski, Wells, & Weiser, assertiveness- (and communion-warmth-)related
BASIC AND APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 129

gossip. Although gossip targets clearly know that the morality gossip since this kind of gossip most strongly
negative gossip about them concerning this special destroys the reputation of the target.
situation was false, they might ask themselves why
other people are telling such things about them and -
Concluding remarks
even worse - why gossip receivers believe this infor-
mation about them and therefore start to doubt their In this article, we tested the impact of negative gossip
own qualities in general. What if the negative gossip on the gossip target as well as on the gossip receiver.
content is accurate? In this case, levels of identity We showed that applying the perspective approach to
threat are expected to generally increase. But we social evaluation (actor versus observer) and the facet
would still expect identity threat to be highest after approach to the Big Two (agency-assertiveness,
accurate negative agency-assertiveness gossip. In con- agency-competence, communion-warmth, commu-
trast, levels of reputation threat should not be mark- nion-morality) is a theoretically promising venue for
edly influenced by the (in-)accuracy of the negative studying targets’ and receivers’ reactions (both evalu-
gossip since gossip receivers normally do not know ation and affect) to negative gossip. It does not only
that gossip is false even if it is. Thus, gossip targets matter how negative the gossip is to predict the reac-
will fear a negative reputation if the gossip is false or tions to it, but you also need to know what concrete
true. And we would expect reputation threat to be negative content was told to know how targets and
highest after accurate and inaccurate negative commu- receivers will respond to negative gossip.
nion-morality gossip.
The gender effects found in Study 3 also need fur- Ethical approval
ther studies to be completely understood. In order to
avoid confounding effects due to gender, the acquaint- All studies reported in this paper have been per-
ance in the story was always of the same sex as the formed according to APA ethical standards for the
participant, i.e. female (male) gossip receivers read a treatment of human subjects. Since data collection
story about a female (male) gossip target. We found was anonymously and involved no identifying infor-
that female gossip receivers judged negative commu- mation and no medical treatment, no ethics approval
nion-morality behavior even more negative than male for the study was needed according to the guidelines
gossip receivers, i.e. they reported a more negative of our university. In the introductions of the question-
impression of the gossip target and felt more negative naires participants were informed that their participa-
affect toward the gossip target. But can this effect be tion is voluntary, that they can cancel their
interpreted as a receiver effect, a target effect, or an participation at any time, and that their data will be
interaction of both? This means do women generally treated anonymously. The informed consent of the
judge moral shortcomings more severely than do participants was implied through survey completion.
men, irrespectively of the gender of the person who
committed the shortcoming? Or are moral shortcom- Author contributions
ings conducted by women judged more severely than
NH and AA were both responsible for all parts of the
moral shortcomings conducted by men, irrespectively
conducted research and agree to be accountable for all
of the gender of the person who judges the shortcom-
aspects of the work.
ing? Or do only women judge moral shortcomings of
women more severely than do men judge moral short-
comings of men as found in the present research? To Disclosure statement
clarify this question, it is worth to conduct the study The authors declare that the research was conducted in the
with a full 4 (gossip content)  2 (gender of gossip absence of any commercial or financial relationships that
receiver)  2 (gender of gossip target) design in could be construed as potential conflict of interest.
future research.
Future research could also investigate the effect of
Notes
content of negative gossip on more distal consequen-
ces like behavior or behavioral intensions, for 1. Moreover, negative gossip scenarios have been chosen
since the agentic and communal self-concept is
instance, the desire to keep the gossiper at distance, to
normally positive and relatively high. Thus, negative
ostracize him/her, or to reestablish a positive view in information can lead to a decrease in self-concept. In
the eyes of others. We would expect that these reac- contrary, positive information could simply lead to
tions are the highest following negative communion- ceiling effects, especially concerning communion.
130 N. HAUKE AND A. E. ABELE

2. The data of the three studies can be received upon gossip in the laboratory. But since negative
request from the authors. information about the own person told by a stranger
3. In order to avoid confounding effects due to gender, to a stranger would probably not produce the same
the acquaintance in the story was always of the same reactions as negative information about the own
sex as the participant, i.e. female participants read a person told by an acquaintance within the mutual
story about a female acquaintance and male circle of friends, the participants must be acquainted.
participants read a story about a male acquaintance. Exposing participants to ostensibly “real” gossip told
This was applied to all studies reported here. by a friend is not acceptable for obvious ethical
4. The values for the communion-morality and agency- reasons. Even if participants are fully debriefed after
competence conditions have already been reported in being exposed to seemingly “real” negative gossip,
Hauke and Abele, (2019, pilot study for Study 2). perceptions based on false information can persevere
5. A confirmatory factor analysis conducted by Hauke (Ross, Lepper, & Hubbard, 1975).
and Abele, (2019) showed that the two factors of
identity threat and reputation threat can be empirically
clearly distinguished. Moreover, a two-factorial Acknowledgments
solution allows a better interpretation of the data than
a simple one-factorial solution with “negative reaction The present research was supported by a grant from the
to gossip” as a single dependent variable. University of Erlangen-N€ urnberg to Nicole Hauke. We
6. In addition, we tested for possible gender effects by thank Kim Peters, Daniela Bernhardt, and Tamara
including gender as additional factor in the ANOVA. Hagmaier-G€ ottle for helpful suggestions to an earlier ver-
There was a small interaction effect of gender and sion of this paper. Antonia Eisele, Jessica Foreman, Yvonne
type of reaction, g2 ¼ 0.04. Women (M ¼ 3.11, Kiesel, Juliane Rehm, and Kevin Reichel helped in
SD ¼ 1.45) showed considerably higher identity threat data collection.
than men (M ¼ 2.43, SD ¼ 1.26), d ¼ 0.50. Regarding
reputation threat, means of women (M ¼ 4.55,
SD ¼ 1.41) and men (M ¼ 4.42, SD ¼ 1.50) were
ORCID
comparably high, d ¼ 0.09. However, the three-way Nicole Hauke http://orcid.org/0000-0002-4923-5105
interaction of gender, condition, and type of reaction,
g2 ¼ 0.01, was negligible.
7. In addition, we tested for possible gender effects by References
including gender as additional factor in the ANOVA.
There was a medium-sized two-way interaction of Abele, A. E., & Hauke, N. (2019). Comparing the facets of
gender and type of reaction, g2 ¼ 0.08. Women the Big Two in global evaluation of self versus other peo-
(M ¼ 4.75, SD ¼ 1.29) reported considerably higher ple. European Journal of Social Psychology, doi:10.1002/
reputation threat than men (M ¼ 3.83, SD ¼ 1.40), ejsp.2639
d ¼ 0.68. Moreover, women (M ¼ 2.49, SD ¼ 1.05) also Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2007). Agency and commu-
reported somewhat higher identity threat than men nion from the perspective of self versus others. Journal of
(M ¼ 2.23, SD ¼ 1.13), d ¼ 0.25. There was also a Personality and Social Psychology, 93(5), 751–763. doi:10.
medium-sized three-way interaction of gender, 1037/0022-3514.93.5.751
condition, and type of reaction, g2 ¼ 0.05. But we Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2014). Communal and
refrain from testing our hypotheses separated by agentic content in social cognition: A dual perspective
condition and gender because group sizes then would model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 50,
become too small (15n  25) and because gender 195–255. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-800284-1.00004-7
was equally distributed across conditions. Moreover, Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2018). Agency and commu-
the interaction of type of reaction  condition nion in social psychology. London: Routledge.
remained stable when gender was included in the Abele, A. E., Cuddy, A. J. C., Judd, C. M., & Yzerbyt, V. Y.
analysis, g2 ¼ 0.07. (2008). Fundamental dimensions of social judgment.
8. In addition, we tested for possible gender effects by European Journal of Social Psychology, 38(7), 1063–1065.
including gender as additional factor in the ANOVA. doi:10.1002/ejsp.574
The main effect of gender, g2 ¼ 0.07, was of medium Abele, A. E., Hauke, N., Peters, K., Louvet, E., Szymkow, A.,
size. Women (M ¼ 3.86, SD ¼ 0.81) generally reported & Duan, Y. P. (2016). Facets of the fundamental content
higher negative affect than men (M ¼ 3.42, SD ¼ 0.92), dimensions: Agency with competence and assertiveness -
d ¼ 0.51. However, the interaction of gender and communion with warmth and morality. Frontiers in
condition was negligibly small, g2<0.01. Psychology, 7, 1810. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01810
9. When developing this new measure, we tried to adapt Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence. An essay
some of the items to the identity threat and reputation on psychology and religion. Chicago: Rand McNally.
threat measures used in Studies 1 and 2. Baker, R., & Guttfreund, D. (1993). The effects of written
10. The items are partly different from the ones used in autobiographical recollection induction procedures on
Study 2, because we wanted to shorten the scale and mood. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 49(4), 563–568. doi:
adapt it to the perceiver perspective. 10.1002/1097-4679
11. One possible methodology that is closer to real-life Baumeister, R. F. (1999). The self in social psychology. New
situations would be to expose participants to real York, NY: Psychology Press.
BASIC AND APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 131

Beersma, B., & Van Kleef, G. A. (2011). How the grapevine and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and
keeps you in line: Gossip increases contributions to the competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
group. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2(6), 82(6), 878–902. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.878
642–649. doi:10.1177/1948550611405073 Fonseca, M. A., & Peters, K. (2018). Will any gossip do?
Beersma, B., & Van Kleef, G. A. (2012). Why people gossip: Gossip does not need to be perfectly accurate to promote
An empirical analysis of social motives, antecedents, and trust. Games and Economic Behavior, 107, 253–281. doi:
consequences. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 10.1016/j.geb.2017.09.015
42(11), 2640–2670. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2012.00956.x Foster, E. K. (2004). Research on gossip: Taxonomy, meth-
Bergmann, J. R. (1993). Discreet indiscretions: The social ods, and future directions. Review of General Psychology,
organization of gossip (J. J. Bednarz, Trans.). New York: 8(2), 78–99. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.8.2.78
Aldine de Gruyter. (Original work published 1987) Goldsmith, D. J., & Baxter, L. A. (1996). Constituting rela-
Blumberg, H. H. (1972). Communication of interpersonal tionships in talk: A taxonomy of speech events in social
evaluations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, and personal relationships. Human Communication
23(2), 157–162. doi:10.1037/h0033027 Research, 23(1), 87–114. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.1996.
Brambilla, M., & Leach, C. W. (2014). On the importance tb00388.x
of being moral: The distinctive role of morality in social Harinck, F., & Ellemers, N. (2014). How values change a
judgment. Social Cognition, 32(4), 397–408. doi:10.1521/ conflict. In C. K. W. De Dreu (Ed.), Social conflict within
soco.2014.32.4.397 and between groups. (pp. 19–36). New York, NY:
Carrier, A., Louvet, E., Chauvin, B., & Rohmer, O. (2014). Psychology Press.
The primacy of agency over competence in status percep- Hauke, N., & Abele, A. E. (2019). Two faces of the self:
tion. Social Psychology, 45(5), 347–356. doi:10.1027/1864- Actor-self perspective and observer-self perspective are
9335/a000176 differentially related to agency versus communion. Self
Dunbar, R. M. (2004). Gossip in evolutionary perspective. and Identity, 1. doi:10.1080/15298868.2019.1584582
Review of General Psychology, 8(2), 100–110. doi:10.1037/ Hogan, R. (1982). A socioanalytic theory of personality. In
1089-2680.8.2.100 M. Page (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp.
Dunbar, R. M., Marriott, A., & Duncan, N. C. (1997). 336–355). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Human conversational behavior. Human Nature, 8(3), Judd, C. M., James-Hawkins, L., Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Kashima,
231–246. doi:10.1007/BF02912493 Y. (2005). Fundamental dimensions of social judgement:
Eagly, A. H. (2009). The his and hers of prosocial behavior:
Understanding the relations between judgments of com-
An examination of the social psychology of gender.
petence and warmth. Journal of Personality and Social
American Psychologist, 64(8), 644–658. doi:10.1037/0003-
Psychology, 89(6), 899–913. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.6.
066X.64.8.644
899.
Ellemers, N. (2017). Morality and the regulation of social
Kervyn, N., Fiske, S. T., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2015).
behavior: Groups as moral anchors. New York, NY, US:
Forecasting the primary dimension of social perception:
Psychology Press.
Symbolic and realistic threats together predict warmth in
Emler, N. (1994). Gossip, reputation, and social adaptation.
In R. F. Goodman & A. Ben-Ze’ev (Eds.), Good gossip the stereotype content model. Social Psychology, 46(1),
(pp. 117–138). Lawrence, KS, US: University Press of 36–45. doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000219
Kansas. Kuttler, A. F., Parker, J. G., & La Greca, A. M. (2002).
Feinberg, M., Willer, R., & Schultz, M. (2014). Gossip and Developmental and gender differences in preadolescents’
ostracism promote cooperation in groups. Psychological judgments of the veracity of gossip. Merrill-Palmer
Science, 25(3), 656–664. doi:10.1177/0956797613510184 Quarterly, 48(2), 105–132. doi:10.1353/mpq.2002.0008
Feinberg, M., Willer, R., Stellar, J., & Keltner, D. (2012). Pagliaro, S., Ellemers, N., Barreto, M., & Di Cesare, C.
The virtues of gossip: Reputational information sharing (2016). Once dishonest, always dishonest? The impact of
as prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social perceived pervasiveness of moral evaluations of the self
Psychology, 102(5), 1015–1030. doi:10.1037/a0026650 on motivation to restore a moral reputation. Frontiers in
Fingelkurts, A. A., & Fingelkurts, A. A. (2017). Three- Psychology, 7, 586. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00586
dimensional components of selfhood in treatment-naive Paulhus, D. L., & Trapnell, P. D. (2008). Self-presentation
patients with major depressive disorder: A resting-state of personality: An agency-communion framework. In
qEEG imaging study. Neuropsychologia, 99, 30–36. doi:10. O. P. John, R. W. Robins & L. A. Pervin (Eds.),
1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.02.020 Handbook of personality psychology: Theory and research
Fingelkurts, A. A., Fingelkurts, A. A., & Kallio-Tamminen, (3rd ed., pp. 492–517). New York, NY US: Guilford
T. (2016). Trait lasting alteration of the brain default Press.
mode network in experienced meditators and the experi- Paulhus, D. L., & John, O. P. (1998). Egoistic and moralistic
ential selfhood. Self and Identity, 15(4), 381–393. doi:10. biases in self-perception: The interplay of self-deceptive
1080/15298868.2015.1136351 styles with basic traits and motives. Journal of
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal Personality, 66(6), 1025–1060. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.
dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. 00041
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 77–83. doi:10.1016/j. Peeters, G. (2008). The evaluative face of a descriptive
tics.2006.11.005 model: Communion and agency in Peabody’s tetradic
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of trait organization. European Journal of Social
model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence Psychology, 38(7), 1066–1072. doi:10.1002/ejsp.524
132 N. HAUKE AND A. E. ABELE

Peters, K., & Kashima, Y. (2014). Gossiping as moral social Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(9), 1115–1125. doi:10.1177/
action: A functionalist account of gossiper perceptions. In 01461672992512005
J. P. Forgas, O. Vincze, & J. Laszlo (Eds.), Social cognition Wert, S. R., & Salovey, P. (2004). A social comparison
and communication (pp. 185–201). New York, NY, US: account of gossip. Review of General Psychology, 8(2),
Psychology Press. 122–137. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.8.2.122
Peters, K., & Kashima, Y. (2015). Bad habit or social good? Williams, H. L., Conway, M. A., & Cohen, G. (2008).
How perceptions of gossiper morality are related to gos- Autobiographical memory. In G. Cohen & M. A. Conway
sip content. European Journal of Social Psychology, 45(6), (Eds.), Memory in the real world (3rd ed., pp. 21–90).
784–798. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2123 Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Ross, L., Lepper, M. R., & Hubbard, M. (1975). Wilson, D. S., Wilczynski, C., Wells, A., & Weiser, L.
Perseverance in self-perception and social perception:
(2000). Gossip and other aspects of language as group-
Biased attributional processes in the debriefing paradigm.
level adaptations. In C. Heyes, & L. Huber (Eds.), The
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32(5),
880–892. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.32.5.880 evolution of cognition (pp. 347–365). Cambridge, MA,
Shrout, P. E., & Rodgers, J. L. (2018). Psychology, science, US: The MIT Press.
and knowledge construction: Broadening perspectives Wojciszke, B. (2005). Morality and competence in person-
from the replication crisis. Annual Review of Psychology, and self-perception. European Review of Social
69(1), 487–510. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-122216- Psychology, 16(1), 155–188. doi:10.1080/104632805
011845 00229619
Sommerfeld, R. D., Krambeck, H., Semmann, D., & Wojciszke, B., Baryla, W., Parzuchowski, M., Szymkow, A.,
Milinski, M. (2007). Gossip as an alternative for direct & Abele, A. E. (2011). Self-esteem is dominated by
observation in games of indirect reciprocity. Proceedings agentic over communal information. European Journal of
of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(44), Social Psychology, 41(5), 617–627. doi:10.1002/ejsp.791
17435–17440. doi:10.1073/pnas.0704598104 Wu, J., Balliet, D., & Van Lange, P. A.M. (2015). When
Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R. L., & Stapp, J. (1974). The per- does gossip promote generosity? Indirect reciprocity
sonal attributes questionnaire: A measure of sex role ster- under the shadow of the future. Social Psychological and
eotypes and masculinity femininity. JSAS Catalog of Personality Science, 6(8), 923–930. doi:10.1177/
Selected Documents in Psychology, 4, 43–44. doi:10.1037/ 1948550615595272
0022-3514.64.4.624 Ybarra, O., Chan, E., Park, H., Burnstein, E., Monin, B., &
Trafimow, D., & Rice, S. (2009). A test of the null hypoth-
Stanik, C. (2008). Life’s recurring challenges and the fun-
esis significance testing procedure correlation argument.
damental dimensions: An integration and its implications
The Journal of General Psychology, 136(3), 261–269. doi:
10.3200/GENP.136.3.261-270 for cultural differences and similarities. European Journal
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). of Social Psychology, 38(7), 1083–1092. doi:10.1002/ejsp.
Development and validation of brief measures of positive 559
and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Ybarra, O., Park, H., Stanik, C., & Lee, D. (2012). Self-judg-
Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070. doi: ment and reputation monitoring as a function of the fun-
10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063 damental dimensions, temporal perspective, and culture.
Wells, G. L., & Windschitl, P. D. (1999). Stimulus sampling European Journal of Social Psychology, 42(2), 200–209.
and social psychological experimentation. Personality and doi:10.1002/ejsp.854

You might also like