Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 83

Value addition Course

The Art of being


Happy

Coordinator: Prof. Soumendra Mohan Patnaik, Professor, Department of


Anthropology, University of Delhi
Co-Coordinator: Dr. Seema Gupta, Associate Professor, Department of
Commerce, University of Delhi
Team Members: Dr. Arpita Kaul, Assistant Professor, Department of
Commerce, Sri Venkateswara College, University of Delhi & Ms. Soumya
Sharma, Assistant Professor, Department of Commerce, Daulat Ram College,
University of Delhi
[ V AC 1: THE ART OF BEING HAPPY ]
Credit distribution, Eligibility and Pre-requisites of the Course

Course Credits Credit distribution of the course Eligibility Pre-requisite


title & Lecture Tutorial Practical/ criteria of the course
Code Practice
The Art of 02 1 0 1 Pass in NIL
th
Being Class 12
Happy

Learning Objectives

• To synthesize the insights developed by Human Development experts,


Psychologists, Anthropologists on one hand, and the intellectual traditions of
Vedantic Philosophy and lndology on the other towards the experience of
happiness.
• To illustrate various factors that determine the subjective experience of
happiness in a cross cultural context.

Learning outcomes

• The students shall be able to evaluate the factors contributing to the


phenomenon of happiness in the personal, familial and community life of an
individual in different cultures in the Indian context.
• They will be able to develop healthy interpersonal relationships and wellbeing
cherishing the values of Indian culture and philosophy.
• They will be able to relate to the global phenomenon of sustainable
development and become sensitive to the needs of the planet.
• They will be able to apply the experience of Aananda at a personal level.

SYLLABUS OF THE ART OF BEING HAPPY

UNIT- I Human Ecology and Happiness Lectures (3 Weeks)


• Definitions/Factors of Happiness : Environmental and Social

74
• Physical, emotional and psychological well-being for happiness
• Physiological and hormonal basis of happiness
• Coping with Stress: A life saving skill

UNIT- II lndological Theories of Happiness (4 Weeks)

• Panch Kosh Theory & Idea ofWell-Being


• Idea of Self and other
• Hierarchy and stages of happiness

UNIT- Ill Happiness: Cross-cultural Contexts (4 Weeks)

• Culture and Happiness


• Interpersonal Relationship: Comparative Perspective
• Towards Self-Actualization

UNIT- IV Local and Global Perspective of Happiness (4 Weeks)

• Measuring happiness : Key indicators


• Happiness Index
• India in Global Happiness Indices

Practical component (if any) - (15 Weeks)

The course will be based on students' identification and operationalization of the


concept of happiness and well-being. Students will explore the indicators and
actualization of these concepts in everyday life.

• Community surveys on the facilities promoting positive mental health


practices such as Yoga and Meditation Centres, Recreation clubs, and Parks for
youth and senior citizens shall be carried out by the students .
• Extending help and social service by visiting old age homes/ hospitals/slum
areas or any other disadvantaged groups.
• Students can undertake a field work/ project independently or work as an
Intern with NGOs working in the area of happiness and well-being.
• Critical appreciation of a documentary/ film based on Happiness and Well-
being can be undertaken by the students.
• Workshops/ Sessions for the actualization of innate creative potential- (
Music, Drawing, Calligraphy, Dramatics)

75
• Hands-on Happiness: Gardening, Cleaning, Washing, Cooking, etc.
• If required, students can share their experiences in the form of a Project
Report.
• Students may share their experiences in the form of Audio-video
presentations of 15-20 minutes.
• Any other Practical/Practice as decided from time to time

Essential/recommended readings

• Banavathy, Vinayachandra & Choudry, Anuradha. (2013). Understanding


Happiness: A Vedantic Perspective. Psychological Studies. 59 . 141-152.
10.1007/s12646-013-0230-x .
• Leontiev, Dmitry. (2012). Anthropology of Happiness: the state of Well-Being
and the way of Joy,ln SocialScience,sVol43. No 2 P93-104.
• Snyder .C.R. S.J. Lopez & J.T. Pedrotti. (2015). Positive Psychology (The
Scientific and Practical Explorations of Human Strengths): Sage Publication.
(Chapter 5: Subjective Well-being: The Science of Happiness and Life
Satisfaction, Page 63 to 73)
• World Development Indicators 2016. (2016).United States: World Bank
Publications.
• Zelenski, John. (2019) Positive _Psychology: The Science of Well-Being, Carleton
University, Ottawa, Canada, Sage Publications Chapter 3:Happiness;page (77
to 110)

Suggestive readings

• Baumgardner,S&Crothers,M.(2014).Positive Psychology. New Delhi: Pearson


Education, India.
• Goleman,D.(2007).Social Intelligence: The new science of human
relationships, RHUK
• Mathews, Gordon and Carolina Izquierdo (eds) . (2010). Pursuits of Happiness:
Well being in Anthropological Perspective. Berghan Books
• Seligman,M.(2002). Authentic happiness: Using the new positive psychology
to realize your potential for lasting fulfilment. NewYork:Free Press.
• Sri Aurobindo, The Synthesis of Yoga, Part Three: The Yoga of Divine Love,
Chapter 7, The Ananda Brahman, pp. 569-570

Examination scheme and mode: Subject to directions from the Examination


Branch/University of Delhi from time to time


76
VALUE ADDITION COURSE ON THE ART OF BEING HAPPY: SUGGESTED ACTIVITIES

ACTIVITY 1

LAUGH OUT LOUD


 The entire class can be taken to the garden of the college
 The entire class makes a big circle
 Either the facilitator or any one of the student can be at the centre of the
circle
 The facilitator can put an alarm of 10 minutes and direct all to begin
laughing loud
 Initiate with a few seconds
 Take breaks in between
 Better to do it in the morning classes
 As students might be hesitant the facilitator shall initiate laughing
 At the end, all can sit in the garden and discuss how do they fell
 Every participant shall get a chance to speak

ACTIVITY 2

CREATIVE WRITING
 Choose a beautiful quote by a famous thinker
 Ponder on the thought
 Start writing on the thought
 Pour your heart out and write whatever you feel about the thought
 You can start by writing whether you agree with it or disagree
 At the end a few students can discuss the thought
 This can be a reflective thinking activity too
VALUE ADDITION COURSE ON THE ART OF BEING HAPPY: SUGGESTED ACTIVITIES

ACTIVITY 3

MEDITATION
 Visit the garden of your college
 Bring your yoga mats
 Sit down on your mats in a clean place
 Carry a water bottle
 Prefer to do it in morning classes
 Sit in a comfortable position
 Close your eyes
 Keep your palms open, facing the sky
 Take a few deep breaths
 Inhale from the nose and exhale from the mouth
 After 5 breaths, concentrate on your left feet
 Take 5 more breaths, concentrate on your right feet
 Take 5 more breaths, concentrate on your stomach
 Concentrate on your neck
 If any thought passes your mind, bring your concentration back to the
breath, start breathing loudly
 Focus in between your eyebrows
 Say in your mind, “ I AM A BEAUTIFUL PURE SOUL”
 Keep your eyes closed till you want to
 Open your eyes slowly and look around and smile
VALUE ADDITION COURSE ON THE ART OF BEING HAPPY: SUGGESTED ACTIVITIES

ACTIVITY 4

REFLECTIVE THINKING
 Think about a movie or a book you have read and identify your favourite
character
 Talk/ write about what you like in the character
 What would you like to emulate
 Why do you like the character
 What do you learn from the character
 This can be followed by a group discussion in the class

ACTIVITY 5

DIGITAL DETOX
 Decide one day you will not use your mobile phone
 Keep your mobile off for the entire day or on silent in you cupboard
 You are not going to use your mobile for 24 hours
 At the end of 24 hours write a write up on what you felt and learnt

ACTIVITY 6

VISIT
 Organise a visit to a nearby farm
 Spend an entire day in the farm
 Notice the nature, vegetables, birds, animals, people and culture of the
place
VALUE ADDITION COURSE ON THE ART OF BEING HAPPY: SUGGESTED ACTIVITIES

ACTIVITY 7

VISIT
 Organise a visit to a nearby farm
 Spend an entire day in the farm
 Notice the nature, vegetables, birds, animals, people and culture of the
place

ACTIVITY 8

WHAT MAKES ME HAPPY?


 Thing about that one thing that made you very happy
 Write about what made me happy
 Why it made me happy
 Think if the situation was reversed how will you feel
 Deliberate
International Journal of Yogic, Human Movement and Sports Sciences 2019; 4(1): 1382-1384

ISSN: 2456-4419
Impact Factor: (RJIF): 5.18
Yoga 2019; 4(1): 1382-1384 Panchakosha
© 2019 Yoga
www.theyogicjournal.com
Received: 26-11-2018
Accepted: 29-12-2018
Shiv Jakhar
Shiv Jakhar Abstract
M.A. Yoga, Udaipur, Rajasthan, Yoga is a comprehensive holistic approach to health, happiness, and wellbeing. Rather than just
India addressing the physical body, yoga ascertains that there are indeed five bodies, five layers of self or
being, that must be addressed for overall healing to take place.
A Koshas, usually rendered "sheath", is a covering of the Atman, or Self according to Vedantic
philosophy. There are five Koshas, and they are often visualised as the layers of an onion in the subtle
body.
Panchakoshas, are the layers of body that seemingly cover the Atman. The Tvam padartha of the
Mahavakya Tat Tvam Asi is determined by the analysis of Panchakoshas that are not the atman.
The Taittiriya Upanishad speaks of the human existence as being akin to the layers of an onion, which
range from the large, rough layers to the fine, tender layers. As understood by Vedanta philosophy, our
‘Atma’ or soul is covered by these Koshas or sheaths; the Anna may Kosh (Food Sheath), Pranamay
Kosh (Vital Air Sheath), Manomay Kosh (Mind Sheath), Vijnanamaya Kosh (Intellect Sheath) and
Anandmay Kosha (Bliss Sheath).
According to the yoga tradition, every one of us has five bodies, each made of increasingly finer grades
of energy. And if we intend to live a fully balanced, healthy life, it tells us, all our bodies need to be kept
in good condition.

Keywords: Yoga, health, happiness, and wellbeing, Panchakosha, Koshas, Vedanta philosophy

Introduction
Yoga is an ancient Indian science and way of life which brings about relaxation and also
induces a balanced mental state. Yoga practices intended to act at physical, emotional,
intellectual and even at spiritual levels. This yoga program is derived from principles in should
promote health at all levels.
Panchakoshas, are the layers of body that seemingly cover the Atman (Self or Consciousness).
The Tvam Padartha of the Mahavakya Tat Tvam Asi is determined by the analysis of
Panchakoshas that are not the atman. Panchakoshas are discussed in the Brahmananda
Valli Chapter of Taittiriya Upanishad which is a part of the Taittiriya Samhita of the
Krishna Yajur Veda and in which particular chapter is discussed ways and means to
achieve Brahman It gives a detailed description of the dimensions of human personality or the
dimensions of the Self.
Atman, The Sruti declares that human birth, by divine grace, is meant to strive to know and
understand the atman. The knowledge and understanding of the atman invariably results
in Jiwanmukti i.e. Moksha or "Spiritual liberation". Spiritual Liberation is of the nature of bliss
in which there is complete negation of all sorrow, it does not arise by mere study of Sastras,
sacrifice to gods, performance of karmas and meditation on the divinities, these acts do not
result in the knowledge of the unity of atman. Atman is Brahman who is of the nature
of Satyam, Jnanam and Anantam, and the knower of Brahman becomes Brahman. Knowledge
is gained after renouncing attachment to all sense-objects and all actions, for one's body, that
Harbours the mind that makes for bondage and is not the atman. The Atman is the substratum
of the consciousness of "I".
Anantam, means not-self or non-self. Anatman is everything that is not Atman. All objects of
Correspondence consciousness are called Anatman, including the mind and the ego. The samsara is the vast
Shiv Jakhar turbulent ocean which is the embodiment of avidya and its effects that cannot be crossed
M.A. Yoga, Udaipur, Rajasthan, without the aid of perfect wisdom; the samsara is the anatman. By non-realisation of the true
India
~ 1382 ~
International Journal of Yogic, Human Movement and Sports Sciences

nature of the atman, the atman is mistaken as the Karana identified as belonging to the atman by reason of its being
Sarira ("causal body"), Suksma Sarira ("subtle body") and pervaded by the Pranamaya Kosha which is effect of Vayu,
Sthula Sarira ("gross body") which bodies constitute and totally unaware and dependent.
the anatman. For a person who is unaware of the atman there
is no other go except to do karmas intended for purification of Manomaya Kosha-The mind Sheath
the mind. Manomaya Kosha belongs to the Suksma Sarira. It is the
"self" having Pranamaya Kosha as its body. The organs of
Three types of Shariras knowledge and the mind form this Kosha which is the cause
According to Sarira Traya, the Doctrine of the three bodies in of the sense of the "I" and of the "mine" and of the varying
Hinduism, the human being is composed of three Sariras or conceptions. It creates difference of names etc., because
"bodies" emanating from Brahman by avidya, "ignorance" or organs of knowledge are dependent on and determined by the
"nescience". They are often equated with the five Koshas mind which is of the nature of determination and doubt. It is
(sheaths), which cover the atman. The Three Bodies Doctrine powerful because bondage and liberation depend on the mind
is an essential doctrine in Indian philosophy and religion, which producing attachment binds a person and which by
especially Yoga, Advaita Vedanta and Tantra. creating aversion for them liberates them from that self-made
 Panchakoshas are divided in three Shariras- bondage. It pervades the Pranamaya Kosha. It is the sacrificial
 Sthula Sarira, the gross body: Made up of physical fire, the five organs are the priests who pour into this fire the
matter. This body consists of Annamaya Kosha and oblations of sense-objects, which fire fuelled by
Pranamaya Kosha. various vasanas burns out the world created and expanded by
 Sukshma Sarira, the subtle body: This body consists of the mind that when fouled by rajas ("projection") and tamas
Manomaya Kosha and Vijanamaya Kosha. ("concealment") superimposes the samsara but when free of
 Karana Sarira, the causal body: This body consists of rajas and tamas can bring about the state of being established
Ananadamaya Kosha. in Brahman.

The five Koshas Vijnanamaya Kosha-The intellect sheath


Kosha is referred to as "sheath", one of five coverings of the The word Vijnana means Knowledge. This layer is made of
Atma or Self according to Vedantic philosophy. According to the intellect and knowledge. This is the part of mind that
the Kosha system in Yogic philosophy, the nature of being makes decisions, and takes ownership of action and becomes
human-encompasses physical and psychological aspects that an ego.
function-as one holistic system. The Kosha system refers to Vijnanamaya Kosha also belongs to the Suksma Sarira and
these different aspects as layers of subjective experience. pervades the Manomaya Kosha that pervades the Pranamaya
Layers range from the dense physical body to the more subtle Kosha which pervades the Annamaya Kosha. Buddhi with its
levels of emotions, mind and spirit. Atma is wrapped inside organs of knowledge and its actions having the characteristics
Panchakosha. of an agent is the Vigyana Kosha, the cause of samsara. It has
The five interconnecting Koshas are the power of reflection of the chaitanya which it accompanies
1. Annamaya Kosha as a modification of Prakrti (Avidya) and characterised by
2. Pranamaya Kosha knowledge and action and always identified with the body,
3. Manomaya Kosha organs etc. This Kosha is endowed with Jnana and to it belong
4. Vijnanamaya Kosha the waking and dream states and the experiences of joy and
5. Anandamaya Kosha sorrow. Being very luminous in close proximity of
the Paramatman deluded by which upadhi it is subject to
Annamaya Kosha - the food sheath samsara, this atman which is compacted of vigyanana and
Anna means matter, Annam literally means food; Taittiriya shining in the heart near the Pranas being immutable becomes
Upanishad calls food the medicament of all. The gross body a doer and enjoyer in the midst of the upadhis. Its "jivabhava-
which is matter-born and matter sustained and transient and existential-character" i.e. Jivahood, persists as long as there is
subject to perception is the Annamaya Kosha whose origin is delusion as it is born of mithyajnana. Though avidya is
food eaten by parents. It is visible, dependent and impure. It is beginningless it is not eternal.
not the atman because it did not exist before its origination
and ceases to exist once it is destroyed. It is subject to Ananadamaya Kosha-The bliss sheath
origination and destruction every moment. It is the anatman Ananda means bliss. This is the bliss of the Atman. This bliss
because it is not in the beginning and at the end, is non- is experienced when we fall into deep sleep.
existent also in the present. It does not know itself. The Anandamaya Kosha is the last layer and it is the closest layer
deluded mind that does not inquire considers his atman to be to the Atman. It is a modification of avidya and appears as a
this body or Kosha. Such a person cannot enjoy bliss. reflection of the atman compacted of absolute bliss. It is fully
manifested in the dreamless deep sleep. It is not
Pranamaya Kosha - The vital energy or breath sheath the atman because it is connected with upadhis ("limitations")
Pranamaya Kosha, separate from and subtler than Annamaya and a modification of Prakrti as an effect of good deeds.
Kosha, pertains to the Sukshma Sarira, it is the sheath of the
vital airs completely enclosing and filling the Annamaya Conclusion
Kosha. The Prana in combination with the five organs of Ultimate goal of human life is to move away from the
action constitutes the Pranamaya Kosha. The Annamaya Annamaya Kosha and remain in the Vigyanamaya Kosha
Kosha is an effect of the Pranamaya Kosha. The Annamaya most of the times and progressively move into the
Kosha gets life by the Prana entering into it and engages in all Anandamaya Kosha. All five Koshas of self are
kinds of action. Prana is the life of beings and the Universal interconnected and dependent on one another. If the body is
life. Whatever happens in the Annamaya Kosha is wrongly tense, the breath is shallow, the mind is irritated, and wisdom
~ 1383 ~
International Journal of Yogic, Human Movement and Sports Sciences

and joy are absent. If there’s disconnect from spirit, indicating


a weak bliss body, there’s disharmony on all layers. On the
other hand, when you’re perfectly in tune with your bliss
body, joy and peace permeate all aspects of who you are.
The practice and philosophical application of yoga into our
everyday life help bring all the Koshas-body, breath, mind,
wisdom, and spirit-into harmony, promoting overall health
and bringing you closer to self-realization and an absolute
fullness of being. Development of all the five Koshas in
necessary for man’s complete spiritual evolution and ultimate
union with the Devine.

References
1. Iyengar BKS. Light on Yoga Taittiriya Upanishad, By Sri
Ramakrishna math Brahma sutra.
2. Satpathy B. Pancha Kosha theory of personality,
International journal of Indian psychology, 2018.
3. Critical analysis of Panch Kosha theory of yoga
philosophy, world journal of pharmaceutical science.
4. Taimini IK. The science of yoga Madras: The
Theosophical Publishing House.
5. Abraham R. Vibrations and forms. Paper presented to the
third conference on science and consciousness,
Ramakrishna mission Institute of culture, Gol Park,
Kolkata, West Bengal, India, 2006.
6. Amritanshuram R, Nagendra HR, Shastry ASN,
Raghuram NV, Nagarathna R. A psycho-oncological
model of cancer according to ancient texts of Yoga,
Journal of yoga & physical therapy, 2013.
7. Kaundinya SD, Kaundinya DV. Meditation [Dhyan]
versus relaxation: A review with comprehensive
bibliography, International journal of basic and applied
physiology, 2013.
8. Kiran Kumar SK. Perspectives on well-being in the
Indian tradition, Journal of Indian psychology, 2004.
9. Klausen SH. The notion of creativity revisited: A
philosophical perspective on Creativity research,
Creativity research journal, 2010.
10. Menon S. The ontological Pragmaticity of Karma in
Bhagavadgita. Journal of Indian Psychology, 1998.
11. Menon S. What is Indian psychology: Transcendence in
and while thinking, the journal of transpersonal
psychology, 2005.
12. Kumar RP, Dev GO. Unveiling consciousness: Vedantic
theory of personality: Theory of punch Kosh-Revisited,
Paper presented at the national conference on Indian
psychology, yoga and consciousness, Sri Aurobindo
International centre of education, Pondicherry, India,
2004.
13. Raina MK. Construction of a creative and self-
transcending life: George Sudarshan’s conception and
experience of creativity, Creativity research journal,
2013.
14. Salagame KKK. Health and well-being in Indian
traditions, Journal of Indian psychology, 2006.
15. Srivathsan KR. The ‘I’ in it: A Pancha Kosha view,
Global journal of flexible systems management, 2004.
16. Velmans M. How to define consciousness-and how not to
define consciousness, Journal of consciousness studies,
2009.

~ 1384 ~
ss2-2012-obl:Ss4-09-obl.qxd 30.04.2012 11:31 Страница 1

VOLUME 43 NUMBER 2, 2012

VOLUME 43
Essential Russian Resources
Four critical English-language journals from
Russia which no library can afford to overlook!

International Affairs
The voice of Russia's Foreign Ministry. Offers unique, first-hand
analysis of major foreign policy issues facing Russia and NIS.
Available in print, microform and online. ISSN 0130-9641. 6/yr.
Social
Subscription: Individuals $66; Institutions $571.

Military Thought
Sciences
Leading theoretical journal of Russia's Ministry of Defense.
A Quarterly Journal of the

Social Sciences
Provides critical insight into current trends in Russian military
doctrine and planning. Available in print, microform and online.
ISSN 0869-5636. 6/yr. Subscription: Individuals $114; Russian Academy of Sciences
Institutions $738.

Social Sciences
Indispensable overview of contemporary Russian scholarship in
In This Issue
the social sciences. Selected works from over 25 Russian-lan-
guage journals of Russian Academy of Sciences. Available in New Configurations of Owners in RussiaÊs Big Business
print and online. ISSN 0134-5486. 4/yr. Subscription:
Individuals $61; Institutions $435. History in the Russian Internet: Representation
and Reliability
Far Eastern Affairs From Text to Work: Vladimir NabokovÊs Novel
The journal of the Institute of the Far East, a renowned Moscow The Real Life of Sebastian Knight and Its Interpretations
think-tank researching developments in China, Japan, and the
Asia-Pacific region. Available in print and online. ISSN 0206- Epistemology through the Prism of Language:
149X. 4/yr. Subscription: Individuals $54; Institutions $375. Dialogue, Understanding, Translation
10601 Wayzata Blvd
Discussions on Missile Defense: Russia·USA·NATO
East View Minneapolis, MN 55305 USA
tel: + 1(952) 252-1201
Information Services, Inc. fax: + 1(952) 252-1202 The Anthropology of Happiness: The State of Well-Being
NUMBER 2, 2012

e-mail: eastview@eastview.com and the Way of Joy

Reforms in the Mirror of Public Opinion

Information Services
ss2-2012:Ss4-2009.qxd 30.04.2012 11:26 Страница 93

Anthropology of Happiness 93

Anthropology of Happiness:
The State of Well-Being and the Way of Joy
Dmitry LEONTIEV

The problem of happiness has since the times of Antiquity attracted genuine
interest both of philosophers and lay people, although that interest has waxed and
waned over history. On the one hand, this state is in one way or another familiar
to most people; on the other hand, it has always been loaded with philosophical,
ethical and cultural-historical contents.
By the end of the 20th century the problem of happiness transcended the
framework of purely philosophical, ethical and theological discussions to
become a mass culture mythologeme.
Pascal Bruckner critically analyzed in his book Perpetual Euphoria the mass
obsession with the idea of happiness. Bruckner said, among other things, that
forced happiness was the ideology of the second half of the 20th century, a
euphoria imposed on us that “forces us to consider everything in terms of pleas-
antness/unpleasantness” and “banishes or squeamishly gets rid of those who for
some reason do not experience it.” That ideology has direct political conse-
quences: “By becoming part of a political doctrine happiness turned into a terri-
ble weapon of mass destruction. No amount of sacrifice, no purges of the human
herd seem to be excessive if their goal is the bright future.”1
For all the diversity of concrete views, the concept of happiness has always
been identified with the highest good accessible to human being, the ultimate of
the desired. Is happiness simultaneously the highest goal and motive of human
actions? A superficial view would suggest an affirmative answer formulated not
only in the popular poetic simile “human being is created for happiness like a
bird is created for flight,” but also in the preamble to such a serious document as
the US Constitution. Bruckner begins his book by quoting a thesis on happiness
that young Victor Mirabeau was advancing in a 1738 letter to a friend, happiness
as the only goal that makes life worth living. At the same time a closer philo-
D. Leontiev, D. Sc. (Psychology), professor, Department of Psychology, Lomonosov Moscow
State University, head of Research Laboratory of Positive Psychology and Quality of Life
Studies at National Research University—Higher School of Economics. The article was first
published in Russian in the journal Chelovek, No. 5, 2011.
The study was implemented in the framework of the Program of Fundamental Studies of the
Higher School of Economics in 2011.
ss2-2012:Ss4-2009.qxd 30.04.2012 11:26 Страница 94

94 SOCIAL SCIENCES

sophical reflection proceeding from recent psychological investigations puts


such a straightforward approach into question.
Psychologists recently joined the discussion of that problem contributing
many empirically established facts and regularities: the problem of happiness is
effectively pivotal for the Positive Psychology trend that emerged at the turn of
the centuries.2 Over the past several decades so many empirical studies have been
published in this field as to lend credence to the claim that humanity learned more
about happiness during this period than in the preceding 2000 years. Most studies
use for a measure of happiness “subjective well-being,” an integral emotional-
rational assessment of a person’s life by that person. We witness psychology
becoming a new type of anthropology. Positive Psychology practically does not
question the principle of the pursuit of happiness. The only reservation is adopt-
ing as the basis not hedonism—life principle emphasizing direct, immediate here-
and-now pleasure,—but rather eudaimonism, the principle that considers the
overall balance of pleasures and the price one has to pay for them in the long term.
The aim of this paper is to analyze the main dilemmas and contradictions
connected with the role of happiness in human life. Drawing on psychological
data and theories we can formulate the problem more precisely and discover at
least two distinct realities behind that word.

Trajectory of the Development of Philosophical Ideas about Happiness

The idea of happiness as the supreme good one can attain arose in Antiquity in
the context of juxtaposition of the desire for immediate gratification (hedonism)
and the desire for a more complete, rounded and sustainable well-being (eudaimo-
nia). The content of happiness, the range of elements it contains was not specified:
some linked happiness primarily with pleasure, others with moral good believing
for example that a heroic death for one’s country in the field of battle was supreme
happiness, others again combined the two.3 Not surprisingly, back in the first cen-
tury BC the Roman philosopher Varro counted 289 different views on happiness.
Characteristically, the word “eudaimonia” literally meant the fate of a person pro-
tected by gods, just like the Russian word schastye has the same root as the word
uchast (meaning “fate” or “lot”).4 Therefore, initially happiness was associated
with luck and good fortune. But luck and good fortune do not bestow their gifts at
random. It is a gift of the gods, an expression of their benevolence, with the gods
deciding on whom to bestow their gifts. Therefore, the view of happiness as luck
and good fortune does not only contradict the concept of happiness as a deserved
good, but is rather closely intertwined with it. At the same time happiness cannot
be attained through direct effort, this is beyond the power of human being.
Such a concept of happiness implied a certain objective basis, an external
assessment, a view from the outside, and prevailed for almost two thousand
years. A kind of alternative to this concept was provided by the teachings of Plot-
inus and early Christian philosophers, St. Augustine and Thomas Aquinas, who
saw happiness exclusively as the merger of the soul with God and achieving
ss2-2012:Ss4-2009.qxd 30.04.2012 11:26 Страница 95

Anthropology of Happiness 95

heavenly bliss in the better world; these authors were skeptical about the possi-
bility to achieve genuine happiness in earthly life and in mundane pursuits.
However, in the Modern Times the objectivist concept of happiness began to
gradually give way to the subjectivist view: only the person himself or herself
can say whether he/she is happy and pleased with life. Given this understanding,
possessing goods is important for happiness (because it is hard to be happy with-
out possessions). But it does not guarantee happiness. Everything depends on
feeling, reaction and the attitude to these benefits. A tramp living under a bridge
can be much happier than a coat-tailed millionaire. The objective criterion was
supplanted by the subjective one: the main thing is how happy you feel while
objective circumstances, though they do play a certain role, are not decisive. Lev
Tolstoy’s Confessions begins with the author telling that he has all the conceiv-
able good things to the utmost degree; nevertheless the meaning of life escapes
him and he feels sometimes like shooting himself.
In the Russian language and in Russian mentality the concept of happiness
was initially associated exclusively with luck and material well-being. Vladimir
Dahl’s Dictionary of the Living Great Russian Language (1853) does not contain
a definition of happiness as an inner state of the soul. It only finds its way into
dictionaries in the late 20th century although even there the former interpretation
is prevalent.5
However, in the late 19th and early 20th centuries an intensive attack was
launched on the idea of happiness as the foundation and rationale of human life.
The main target of the critique was the utilitarian philosophy (Jeremy Bentham,
John Stuart Mill) which again put the problem of happiness to the center of philo-
sophical discussions while the biggest contribution to this critique was made, in
addition to Friedrich Nietzsche, by Russian religious philosophy. It stated that
the concept of happiness, like the concepts of utility and pleasure, has no moral
character and cannot provide the basis for ethics.6 The principle of the quest of
happiness has no meaning: it can give rise to absolutely contradictory doctrines,7
it does not specify what should be the object of activity,8 it is impossible to com-
pare the happiness experienced by various individuals and finally, happiness
means a suspension of all desires and ignorance of what is good and what is
evil.9 For the same reason the idea of happiness cannot provide a justification of
human life, and the idea of arranging one’s life on earth in accordance with the
principle of happiness is false. “It is unnatural and impossible to make this by-
product the goal that lies ahead; just like it is impossible and ugly for a ship to
move with its bow behind the attached rudder.”10

Happiness and Meaning

Vasily Rozanov, Nikolay Berdyayev, Aleksey Vvedensky and other philoso-


phers juxtaposed to the principle of the quest of happiness the principle of the
quest of meaning oriented towards something valuable in the world. What is
important is that they saw the meaning of life as something transcending life itself.
ss2-2012:Ss4-2009.qxd 30.04.2012 11:26 Страница 96

96 SOCIAL SCIENCES

“The only logical justification of the belief in the meaning of life is our belief that
our life is a journey leading us to a goal of absolute value that lies outside our life
and is implemented through life.”11 “An evaluation in terms of meaning invari-
ably presupposes standing above what is being evaluated.”12 That is why human
being is capable of enduring suffering: suffering the meaning of which is known,
differs from the suffering lacking goal and meaning.13 In that tradition happiness
is seen as an intentional experience that is not closed in itself, but is generated by
the discovery and realization of meaning through action in the world.
Essentially the same logic was presented in the existential anthropology of
Viktor Frankl in which the principle of seeking for meaning was clearly opposed
to the quest of pleasure and happiness, and not only in the philosophical-ethical,
but also in the psychological context. “The theory based on the principle of plea-
sure ignores an important quality of mental activity, i.e., intentionality. And in
general people desire not pleasure as such, they simply want what they want.”14
Elaborating this thesis Frankl stressed that what we need is not happiness per se,
but rather the reasons for it; if they are there, happiness will come by itself.15 An
attempt to sidestep the need for meaning and derive a sense of happiness direct-
ly leads to alcoholic or narcotic addiction.16 Meaning provides the basis of hap-
piness and possessing it is a prerequisite of happiness and even of the very pos-
sibility of experiencing it. Lack of meaning deprives man of the ability to expe-
rience happiness and to endure suffering.17
The idea that orientation towards meaning comes before the experience of
happiness gradually makes its way into positive psychology to which the problem
of happiness, identified with a sustained subjective sense of well-being, is central.
Some authors have come to consider meaning as a significant factor of happiness
which turns out to be “a by-product of participating in worthwhile projects and
activities that do not have as their primary focus the attainment of happiness.”18
Empirical studies in positive psychology that in practice take into account
the factor of meaning, are comparatively few, but their results are indicative.
They show that what contributes to a sense of happiness most is progress in
achieving not any, but personally significant (“internal” or “motive-congruent”)
goals and that the most powerful positive experiences are connected with being
absorbed in something that goes beyond one’s self.19 However while meaning
predicts happiness, the reverse is not true: meaning does not depend on the level
of well-being, it helps not only to attain happiness, but also to do without it.20
Thus an analysis of the problem of happiness cannot be complete without
taking into account the role of meaning in achieving it. The role of meaning is
manifested, first, in that its implementation is one of the key reasons for experi-
encing happiness. Second, it determines the direction of the quest and the quali-
ty of the very state of happiness, not only its intensity, but rather maturity. Final-
ly and thirdly, meaning is a great help in coping not only with a deficit of happi-
ness, but with still more serious adversities. Both accomplishments and discov-
eries, and gaps and blind alleys of positive psychology provide vivid evidence of
the key role of the concept of meaning in the solution of the central problems of
present-day psychology.
ss2-2012:Ss4-2009.qxd 30.04.2012 11:26 Страница 97

Anthropology of Happiness 97

Happiness As a State and Happiness As an Activity:


Following Aristotle

The opponents of the idea of happiness as the supreme goal and foundation
of human life, from Nietzsche to Solovyov to Bruckner, advance three groups of
arguments.
 The first argument is that the concept of “happiness” is vague and even
tautological: there is not a single concrete object that everybody would
describe as “happiness,” everyone invests the word with one’s own con-
tent and for everyone the word has a different meaning. An interesting
analysis of the problem of happiness was provided in Mikhail Veller’s
philosophical treatise Everything About Life.21 Analyzing logically vari-
ous ideas of happiness, which are very diverse whereas the psychological
state that this word indicates is pretty well defined, Veller concludes with
good reason: happiness is the function of distance from the current posi-
tion to the passionately desired position, whatever the latter may be. It is
the distance between the desired and the actual that lends dynamism to
life and striving this dynamism disappears once happiness is achieved.
 This leads to the second argument against happiness, the statement that
happiness viewed in the traditional way is time standing still, the end of
life. The pure metaphor of supreme happiness is “now I can (or even want
to) die.” Indeed, a person in a state of happiness has no further motivation
for life, no perspective. I am already at the highest point. From there the
only way leads downward. Psychology describes the “Martin Eden syn-
drome” after the main character of Jack London’s novel of that title. Mar-
tin Eden rose from a humble background and through hard and at times
adventurous life, effort and sheer ability made it to the top. He became a
famous writer, a member of high society and achieved everything he had
dreamt of. At the peak of his powers he committed suicide because every-
thing had been achieved and there was nothing left to live for. Is there a
way out of this impasse? The answer is no if one considers happiness to
be a purely emotional phenomenon, an experience of the state of bliss.
 The third argument is that an inner state cannot be the goal and founda-
tion of life. Strictly speaking, it refers not to the idea of happiness as such,
but the reduction of happiness to the most positive emotional state possi-
ble. One can gain a better insight into this by looking at the problem of
happiness from the viewpoint of the universal structure of self-regulation
of purposive activities of living systems. In that structure happiness can
play the role either of a feedback mechanism or a goal criterion of a
desired state. In the first case happiness, like all emotions, performs the
function of giving a signal to the subject as to how well his or her life is
proceeding. But if happiness or other positive emotions within that struc-
ture play the role of the criterion of what should be and what is desirable,
then the subject ceases seeking for really improving life if positive emo-
ss2-2012:Ss4-2009.qxd 30.04.2012 11:26 Страница 98

98 SOCIAL SCIENCES

tions can be achieved by other ways, for example, through alcohol or


drugs. Indeed, if emotions (including happiness) become the goal, life
does not necessarily have to be good and full, an illusion that everything
is fine can be obtained bypassing life.
Let us turn to Aristotle, who was invariably credited with introducing the
principle of happiness in popular domain, while few people read what he has actu-
ally written. As if foreseeing this situation, the philosopher wrote: “With regard to
what happiness is people differ and the majority do not give the same account as
the wise.” Aristotle’s ideas of happiness were very far from the current ideology
of “perpetual euphoria.” He tried to construe relationships between such concepts
as pleasure, good, virtue and happiness. The boundaries between them were and
still are not easy to identify. But most importantly, Aristotle stressed that “happi-
ness is an activity of soul in accordance with perfect virtue” and that “activity
plainly comes into being and is not present at the start like a piece of property.”
This is not an accidental formula. In his Nicomachean Ethics Aristotle per-
sistently repeated that happiness means activity (energeia). Moreover, he even
distinguished between the kind of happiness that implies activity and more lowly
kinds. “Any chance person, even a slave, can enjoy the bodily pleasures no less
than the best man; but no one assigns to a slave a share in happiness unless he
assigns to him also a share in human life for happiness does not lie in such occu-
pations but, as we have said before, in virtuous activities.”22 Thus with Aristotle
we discover a connection of genuine happiness not with an emotional state, but
with activity, virtue and with being engaged in life, at the same time, he distin-
guished his concept of happiness from more primitive views.
Thus, what leads us into an impasse is not so much the idea of happiness as
such but rather the interpretation of happiness as an emotional state, a state of
supreme bliss. That notion arose as a result of an unnoticed departure from Aris-
totle and the other “wise men” towards vulgar superficial interpretations of hap-
piness promoted by the mass consumerist culture. These elicited the following
comment from Aristotle written 2000 years ago: “Neither should the opinions of
the many (for it is they who speak at random about almost everything, and espe-
cially so). For it is absurd to apply reasonably to those who need sensation rather
than reason.”23 No comment is needed.

The Duality of Happiness

Empirical studies carried out during the past decade in the field of positive
psychology, although still assuming happiness to be a subjective state, gradually
bring to awareness that this understanding is insufficient and that it is necessary
to take into account the role of activity, meaning and personality.
At the center of generally recognized regularities of the psychology of hap-
piness and subjective well-being is the “pie model” of Sonya Lyubomirski, Ken
Sheldon and David Schkade which draws on many psychological studies of the
factors that influence our state of happiness. The model compares the contribu-
ss2-2012:Ss4-2009.qxd 30.04.2012 11:26 Страница 99

Anthropology of Happiness 99

tion of three groups of factors to the extent to which people feel happy, and in
terms of common sense that relationship turns out to be somewhat unexpected.
All that pertains to external conditions and can be described by the umbrella term
“quality of life” accounts for just about 10% of the variance of the individual
sense of happiness or subjective well-being, far less than is commonly believed.
The stable personality structure of the individual which is considered to be
hereditary to a large extent (but not entirely), accounts for up to 50% of the vari-
ance. And about 40% of the variance pertains to the factors that the authors
describe in terms of “intentional activity,” which echoes Aristotle and the ideas
of some modern authors. These are factors that have to do with the way we orga-
nize our lives, relate to our fellows, with the goals we pursue, in short, with what
is in our hands and is an object of our choice.24
Some other data put in still bolder relief the fact that happiness in different
people may differ not only quantitatively but also qualitatively. For example,
many publications of the past decade devoted to the dependence of the experience
of happiness and subjective well-being on the material welfare (per capita GDP),
based on the comparison of the data of international surveys, invariably reproduce
one and the same pattern.25 The chart that expresses the dependence of happiness
on money is divided into two parts. The lower part of the chart (when GDP val-
ues are low) reveals a direct linear dependence of the level of subjective well-
being on material welfare. However, that dependence ends as soon as a certain
level of material welfare is achieved (an average annual GDP of about $15,000
per capita by now). If one passes from international comparisons to individual dif-
ferences in one and the same country only the position of the turning point will
change but the regularity will be the same. Beginning from a certain moment the
curve becomes almost flat, which means that further improvement of well-being
hardly correlates with happiness, and a person with a fortune of 100 million dol-
lars is only marginally happier than a person with a fortune of 1 million. As
Sholom Aleichem said, having money is not as good, as it is bad having none.
In fact this curve reflects two kinds of happiness. The first is the minimum
happiness directly linked to living conditions and the satisfaction of basic needs.
If the level of welfare is low it is possible to make a person happier by improv-
ing the material welfare. But there comes a moment when the basic needs that
determine the quality of life are met. In fact, the concept of “the middle class”
corresponds to the level of material welfare at which happiness ceases to depend
on it and becomes determined by other factors. That is the maximum happiness,
individual happiness connected with meaning, activity and virtue; Aristotle
termed it eudaimonia. It depends on the person, his/her own life and relation-
ships with other people. Another recently discovered empirical regularity refutes
the long-standing belief among psychologists that every person has his/her own
stable individual range within which his or her experience of contentment with
life and psychological well-being fluctuates: particularly joyful or tragic circum-
stances may only temporarily change the individual level of happiness before it
returns to the original level.26 The data of a longitudinal study conducted in Ger-
many over many years have shown that although this is really the case for the
majority of respondents, there is a group of them (24% of the total sample) that
ss2-2012:Ss4-2009.qxd 30.04.2012 11:26 Страница 100

100 SOCIAL SCIENCES

reveal sustained shifts (mostly positive) of the level of subjective well-being by


at least one standard deviation.27
Thus, the answer to the question “is there happiness?” would be: “yes, and
more than one.” There are at least two. One can be provided for a person by help-
ing him/her to meet his/her basic needs, raising the standard and quality of life.
Such happiness can be bestowed or ensured by another person or the state, it can
be bought for money and in fact it consists in money that enables one to buy the
necessities—a shelter, nutritious and healthy food, leisure and entertainment,
family hearth, sex, security and confidence of tomorrow—“like all the other peo-
ple.” It has its upper limit that may be considered to be the psychological norm
while an insufficient level is a deviation. The second kind of happiness is indi-
vidual and can be achieved by tortuous ways, it cannot be given or established
as a norm, it can only be found in meaningful activities. It does not have an upper
limit or standards for comparison and is in general hard to quantify, whereas its
qualitative-phenomenological characteristic turns out to be very significant. One
can go along with the definition of eudaimonic happiness as the degree of suc-
cess in fulfilling one’s personal existential project.28 It means that happiness
depends not only on success, but on what kind of project it is, in other words,
alongside the quantitative measure it has a qualitative dimension.
The theory of autotelic experiences, or the theory of “flow” put forward by
Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi29 is the most widely recognized of the modern theories
that treat happiness in terms of activity. The author interviewed initially repre-
sentatives of creative professions and then of various occupational, age and eth-
nic groups to discover and describe the optimal experiences characterized by
engagement in a certain self-motivating activity. Flow is the state of total immer-
sion in one’s action, absorption in that action, when you are oblivious of time, of
yourself and when instead of fatigue you experience a surge of energy. That flow
is not bestowed on us like a blessing, but is generated by our conscious efforts, it
is in our hands. In it pleasure merges with effort and meaning generating a pow-
erful sense of enjoyment that in turn generates energy. Unlike happiness, enjoy-
ment preserves energy, preserves motivation, preserves movement and offers a
perspective and meaning. Flow experience is a reward for the effort exerted in
order to be able to cope with ever more complicated tasks and thus is the driving
force of evolution towards growing complexity. The flow is directly linked to per-
sonality characteristics, to the level of its development and maturity.
The two above described kinds of happiness are in line with the distinction
between two categories of motivational processes in one of the versions of Abra-
ham Maslow’s theory, that on deficiency-related and Being-related ones. We
speak about the first type of motivation when a person lacks something specific
and needs to urgently fill the gap. The Being motivation is connected not with a
deficit of something special but rather with the free development of the poten-
tialities inherent in a person. Maslow described for example deficiency motivat-
ed and Being motivated processes of cognition, love, etc.30 The distinction
between both types of processes corresponds well to the above mentioned facts
of dual determination of individual happiness.
ss2-2012:Ss4-2009.qxd 30.04.2012 11:26 Страница 101

Anthropology of Happiness 101

Happiness and Human Nature

Thus there are a number of reasons for distinguishing two realities which we
are used to describe by one and the same word, “happiness,” in all languages.
They correlate directly with the two regimes of human existence:
(1) the necessity mode on the basis of fixed auto-pilot mechanisms that
make it predictable and manageable, and
(2) optional mode of existence on the basis of reflexive consciousness,
autonomy and orientation towards meaningful possibilities.31
We described earlier the former state as a minimum happiness, or, to use
Maslow’s term, deficiency happiness; it can also be equated to subjective well-
being. That is a stable characteristic that varies little, has its quantitative measure
and can be applied to any person by describing that person’s position on a cor-
responding scale. It reflects the extent to which the basic human needs are met,
adjusted for the general predisposition of a particular person to positive or neg-
ative emotions, accordingly, it is determined and predicted by the chances for
needs gratification and by individual traits on which account for the ability to
fully enjoy the positive aspects of life.
The second state was described above as maximum happiness, or Being hap-
piness, to use Maslow’s term. It can be equated to such concepts as “eudaimo-
nia” or “optimal experience.” It expresses the degree to which the meaning of the
life or personal existential project is being fulfilled, conditioned by the very exis-
tence of such meaning or project (not everyone has it) and by its individual char-
acter. Therefore it has greater specificity and is better described in qualitative
than quantitative terms. It does not arise by accident or through some automatic
mechanisms, but through the intentional activity and communication thanks to
conscious and authentic efforts of the subject; it can be called self-determined.
Although potentially that kind of happiness is also within everyone’s reach, not
everyone has ever encountered it in his or her life due to the widespread phe-
nomenon of “escape from freedom,” from decision-making, choices, efforts and
consciousness. At the same time, although there are good reasons for consider-
ing it to be a higher level of happiness, it can be present in life even against the
background of very unfavorable external conditions and a low level of subjec-
tive well-being, in particular when a person can attain joy and happiness despite
low living standards through inner psychological resources and meanings. It is
not so uncommon, especially in Russia, when, to use Mikhail Zhvanetsky’s apt
remark, it is easier to be happy than contented.
As material welfare grows and people’s basic needs are met they become
happier; their subjective well-being increases along with their objective welfare.
Having experienced this dynamics and having implemented the minimum pro-
gram people try to continue moving up that trajectory, but, although they are
earning still more money happiness does not increase anymore. To succeed in
further search of happiness one has to change the trajectory of that search to a
more complicated maximum program not through gratification, but through
ss2-2012:Ss4-2009.qxd 30.04.2012 11:26 Страница 102

102 SOCIAL SCIENCES

activity, through goals and relationships. In these forms of activity we find enjoy-
ment rather than satisfaction, or subjective well-being.
Thus, the dilemma as to whether happiness is generated by objective mater-
ial welfare or subjective experiences that confer meaning on what we do is
resolved at the deficiency level through the recognition of the interaction
between both sets of factors with the subjective factors playing the key role,
while at the Being level through a dialectical synthesis that leads to the under-
standing of happiness as an intentional experience connected with meanings in
the world that impel us and with the experience of progress in their discovery and
fulfillment. One can agree with Maslow’s definition of happiness as “experienc-
ing real emotions over real problems and real tasks.”32
That view corresponds also to the well-known paradox of happiness formu-
lated by John Stuart Mill: “Not seeking happiness and being able to do without
it is the most reliable way of attaining it.” Since then philosophers and psychol-
ogists have repeatedly used various words to reaffirm this paradoxical but unas-
sailable truth: the more we seek happiness the harder it is to achieve, but it comes
by itself if you pursue other worthy goals in your life. This relates to the convic-
tion, expressed by many outstanding thinkers, that happiness can be found any-
where except within oneself. “The door to happiness opens outwards,” as Søren
Kierkegaard put it.
Since the problem of happiness is directly linked to the solution of the prob-
lem of meaning it is inseparable from the problems of personality and personal
maturity. With regard to happiness the appropriate verb is “to be” rather than “to
have.” The question is to what extent a person is capable of being happy. While
repeating the old saying that everyone is the blacksmith of one’s own happiness
we tend to forget how laborious and complicated the blacksmith’s trade is. As
Abraham Maslow pointed out, it is at the level of advanced and mature individ-
ual and advanced and mature happiness that the failure of the hedonistic world-
view is particularly apparent.33 The common type of hedonism propagated by the
consumerist culture is the life strategy of the passive majority which tends to pre-
fer the soft option. Of course, the hedonistic principle is an inalienable part of
human nature because every living organism tends to conserve resources while
seeking to obtain maximum results. It would be a mistake, however, to see the
human essence in that principle. There are higher guiding principles that become
more noticeable as the person grows up, matures and develops. Human nature is
not static: transcendence of the given is its only invariant and we develop as we
move towards higher principles of organization. Happiness can easily be
achieved at the lower levels of personal maturity when the images and criteria of
higher good are simple. Security and wealth that ensure bodily comfort and well-
being of one’s family and friends and/or infinite sexual pleasure is all that the
majority of people can imagine as a picture of complete happiness. Not surpris-
ingly, children are as a rule happier than adults. At higher levels of maturity (per-
sonal and spiritual, rather than biological one) the distance between reality and
the ideal increases and happiness acquires a different quality and becomes more
complicated and difficult to achieve. At the same time it becomes more individ-
ss2-2012:Ss4-2009.qxd 30.04.2012 11:26 Страница 103

Anthropology of Happiness 103

ual and no less authentic. One has to pay a higher price for it, but the rewards are
much greater. “Everyone has to grow up to one’s happiness.”34 Even a little high-
quality “hand-made” happiness can outweigh large amounts of primitive
“machine-made” happiness.
Thus, humans are impelled not just by seeking a certain point of bliss. A
mature personality starts seeking to raise that point which can be at different lev-
els, at different horizons. Happiness is like the horizon not only because as you
walk towards it, it recedes. The taller the person the broader his or her horizon.
Happiness is directly linked to the magnitude of the personality: a little person
cannot have genuinely great happiness although everyone can have happiness of
a suitable size. A big person has big happiness although such a person is proba-
bly less concerned with achieving it... As Maslow pointed out, it is in human
nature to seek for higher and higher heavens. And different kinds of heavens, one
may add.
Finally, happiness is independent of life only in a drug addict or a severely
mentally ill person because there is no other way towards happiness than the way
of life.
“And to the young Mirabeau I would reply: ‘I love life too much to wish
nothing but happiness.’”35

NOTES
1 P. Bruckner, L’Euphorie perpétuelle. Essais sur le devoir de Bonheur, Gasset, 2000.
2 See: M. Seligman, Authentic Happiness: Using the New Positive Psychology to Realize
Your Potential for Lasting Fulfillment, New York, 2003; I. Boniwell, Positive Psychology
in a Nutshell: A Balanced Introduction to the Science of Optimal Functioning, London,
2006.
3 V. Tatarkevich, On Happiness and the Perfection of Man, Moscow, 1981, pp. 34-35 (in
Russian).
4 A. Guseynov, R. Apresyan, Ethics, Moscow, 1998, p. 296 (in Russian).
5 See: S. Vorkachev, Happiness As a Linguocultural Concept, Moscow, 2004 (in Russian).
6 See: M. Tareyev, The Purpose and Meaning of Life: Quoted from The Meaning of Life: an
Anthology, Ed. N. Gavryushin, Moscow, 1994, p. 134 (in Russian).
7 Ibid.
8 V. Rozanov, The Purpose of Human Life, quoted from Ibid. p. 41.
9 N. Berdyayev, On the Mission of Man, Moscow, 1993, p. 77, 247-248 (in Russian).
10 V. Rozanov, op. cit., p. 42.
11 A. Vvedensky, The Conditions of Admissibility of Faith in the Meaning of Life; quoted
from The Meaning of Life: an Anthology, p. 100.
12 N. Berdyayev, op. cit., p. 37.
13 N. Berdyayev, “On Suicide,” Psikhologichesky zhurnal, 1992, No. 1, p. 91.
14 V. Frankl, Der Mensch auf der Suche nach Sinn, Stuttgart, 1972.
ss2-2012:Ss4-2009.qxd 30.04.2012 11:26 Страница 104

104 SOCIAL SCIENCES

15 V. Frankl, Der Wille zum Sinn, München, 1991, p. 20.


16 Idem, The Unheard Cry for Meaning, New York, 1978, p. 69.
17 Idem, Der leidende Mensch: Anthropologische Grundlagen der Psychotherapie, Bern,
1984, S. 54.
18 R. Emmons, “Personal Goals, Life Meaning, and Virtue: Wellsprings of a Positive Life,”
C. Keyes, J. Haidt (Eds.), Flourishing: Positive Psychology and the Life Well-Lived,
Washington (DC), 2003, p. 106.
19 See: M. Csikszentmihalyi, Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience, New York,
1990.
20 See: R. Baumeister, K. Vohs, “The Pursuit of Meaningfulness in Life,” C. Snyder, S. Lopez
(Eds.), Handbook of Positive Psychology, New York, 2002.
21 M. Veller, All About Life, St. Petersburg, 1999 (in Russian).
22 See: Aristotle, Nicomachean Ethics, Chicago, 2006.
23 Aristotle, Eudemian Ethics, Oxford, 1998, p. 9.
24 S. Lyubomirski, The HOW of Happiness: A Practical Approach to Getting the Life You
Want, London, 2007.
25 See, for example, E. Diener, S. Oishi, “Are Scandinavians Happier than Asians? Issues in
Comparing Nations on Subjective Well-Being,” F. Columbus (Ed.), Asian Economic and
Political Issues, Hauppage (NY), vol. 10, 2004.
26 See: M. Seligman, op. cit.
27 See: E. Diener, R. Lucas, C. Scollon, “Beyond the Hedonic Treadmill: Revisions to the
Adaptation Theory of Well-Being,” American Psychologist, 2006, vol. 61.
28 See: D. Galati, I. Sotgiu, “Happiness and Positive Emotions,” Ricerche di Psicologia,
2004, No. 1.
29 M. Csikszentmihalyi, op. cit.
30 A. Maslow, Toward a Psychology of Being, 2nd ed., New York, 1968.
31 D. Leontiev, “New Benchmarks in Understanding Personality in Psychology: from the
Necessary to the Possible,” Voprosy psikhologii, 2011, No. 1.
32 A. Maslow, “The Psychology of Happiness,” E. Hoffman (Ed.), Future Visions: The
Unpublished Papers of Abraham Maslow, Thousand Oaks, 1964/1996, p. 23.
33 Ibid.
34 K. Wojtyla, “The Foundations of Ethics,” Voprosy filosofii, 1991, No. 1, p. 45.
35 P. Bruckner, op. cit., p. 14.

Translated by Yevgeny Filippov


Psychol Stud
DOI 10.1007/s12646-013-0230-x

REFLECTIONS

Understanding Happiness: A Vedantic Perspective


Vinayachandra K. Banavathy & Anuradha Choudry

Received: 29 May 2013 / Accepted: 25 November 2013


# National Academy of Psychology (NAOP) India 2013

Abstract The pursuit of happiness has been the one of the Introduction
most prominent goals of humanity since time immemorial.
However, understanding and experiencing happiness varies The pursuit of happiness has been the axiomatic refer-
across cultures. While the dominant understanding on happi- ence of all human endeavours across cultures and time-
ness has developed from the Euro-American cultural back- lines. In the Indic traditions, this idea could be noted in
ground there are other significant conceptualizations present statements like, ‘Let me be happy and not sad, is the
in the Eastern philosophical and cultural traditions like India intent behind all human actions’1 or in the most com-
and China. It has been noted that Eastern psychologies offer mon wish expressed, ‘let all be happy’2. In the West
rich insights into human phenomena which are equally uni- one can note that even the formation of the modern
versal in nature because of their long-standing civilizational state and market economy are guided by this underlying
experiences. Drawing from these traditions would make the principle of happiness. ‘The United States Declaration
discourse on happiness culturally informed and more compre- of Independence’ adopted in 1776 by 13 states of the
hensive. Furthermore, Indian tradition which considers bliss East coast stated, ‘We hold these truths to be self-
as the essential nature of the individual personality can con- evident, that all men are created equal, that they are
tribute immensely in understanding and realizing lasting hap- endowed by their Creator with certain unalienable
piness. This paper explores the types and nature of happiness Rights, that among these are Life, Liberty and the
as expounded in the Vedantic tradition in the context of Indian pursuit of Happiness’. The remarkable figure of Scottish
psychology. Enlightenment, Adam Smith, notes that, ‘The happiness
of mankind as well as of all other rational creatures,
seem to have been the original purpose intended by the
Keywords Enduring happiness . Bliss . Nature of happiness . Author of Nature.’ (Smith 2009, p. 132). Thus the need
Transcendental happiness . Indian Philosophy . Indian to seek ‘happiness’ and be ‘happy’ through all one’s
Psychology . Culture actions is universally acknowledged as the main moti-
vating force in the lives of people.
The heightened interest in the study of happiness in the
V. K. Banavathy (*) : A. Choudry West, in the recent times, owes its sources to the advent of
Centre for Indian Psychology, Jain University, No. 34, I Cross, J.C. positive psychology and to the works of Martin Seligman,
Road, Bangalore 560 001, India
e-mail: vinayachandra.bk@jainuniversity.ac.in
Diener, Kahneman, Csikszentmihalyi etc. With this approach,
the study of well-being and happiness has acquired central
V. K. Banavathy focus in the discipline of psychology today (Diener, Subjec-
e-mail: aurovinaya@gmail.com tive well-being: The sceince of happiness, and a proposal for a
A. Choudry national index, 2000) (Lyubomirsky et al. 2005).
e-mail: panditanu@gmail.com

V. K. Banavathy
Development Foundation, No. 218, JP Royale, Sampige Road, 1
sukham: me syāt duhkam: me mā bhūt iti lokapravr: ttih:
2
Malleswaram, Bangalore 560003, India sarve bhavantu sukhinah:
Psychol Stud

Current Understanding on Happiness hence may not promote well-being. It is interesting to note
that in this way, eudaimonic theory distinguishes well-being
At present, there are two dominant approaches to human happi- from happiness (Ryan and Deci 2001).
ness and well‐being: Hedonic and Eudaimonic perspectives. The The concept of well-being has social dimensions as well.
hedonic perspective with its roots in Hedonism – an ancient Extending the ideas of eudaimonic approach at the social level,
school of thought which argues that pleasure is the only intrinsic Keyes has defined social well-being as the positive ‘appraisal of
good, seeks to maximize one’s happiness by maximizing one’s one’s circumstance and functioning in society’ (Keyes 1998).
pleasure and avoiding displeasures. Subjective Well-being He further identifies five dimensions of social well-being viz.
(SWB) has been associated with the hedonistic approach. Well- social acceptance, social integration, social contribution, social
being in itself refers to the optimal psychological experience and coherence and social actualization. This is based on the premise
functioning of the individual. As it involves people evaluating like in PWB that society as a whole has a collective purpose
themselves subjectively, the extent to which they experience a and each member of the society has a definite role and respon-
sense of wellness is termed as Subjective Well-being (SWB). sibility in realizing its purpose. Accordingly, maximizing hap-
SWB is operationally defined in terms of experiencing a high piness at the social level is largely dependent on one’s ability to
level of positive affect, a low level of negative affect, and a high realize her role and contribute towards its fulfilment. According
degree of satisfaction with one’s life. Strictly speaking, from the to Keyes, this approach to well-being is based on ‘classical
hedonistic perspective, it would suffice to consider the presence sociological theory and current social psychological perspec-
of positive affect and absence of negative affect as the constitu- tives’ (Keyes 1998, p. 133). Nevertheless, it is clear that the
ents of SWB and leave out life satisfaction as is not particularly a measures of social well-being are closely connected to and
hedonic concept. However, life satisfaction has been widely modelled on the lines of the measures of PWB.
accepted in the operational definition of SWB. This concept of
SWB, as earlier defined, is frequently used interchangeably with
happiness. This implies that hedonic happiness occurs when one The Need for Alternative Perspectives
is primarily seeking pleasant feelings and avoiding unpleasant
ones (Ryan and Deci 2001). Defining happiness operationally in terms of SWB and Psy-
The other important approach to happiness is the chological well-being, researchers have come up with measure-
eudaimonic perspective. This approach bases itself on the ments for both of them. Research on SWB and PWB focus on
Aristotelian philosophy of life. In his famous Nichomachean the factors that contribute to SWB – personal, socio-
Ethics, Aristotle deliberates on the nature of the happy man environmental and cultural factors (Deci and Ryan 2008) and
and then attempts to define happiness as the highest good that on the six parameters mentioned respectively. However, some
one seeks and one ought to seek as the end in itself and not as a researchers have critiqued the comprehensiveness and sophis-
means to any other end. It results from a life based on virtu- tication of the current understanding of the concept of happi-
ousness and contemplation (Aristotle 1999). ness. They have also questioned the psychological depth be-
The eudaimonic perspective also draws its substance from hind the theory, efficacy and utility of the instruments and the
the works of analytical and humanistic psychologists like applications of such ill-conceived theories (Ryff 1989). Despite
Fromm, Jung, Maslow, Allport, Rogers etc. and proposes that serious research and a good number of empirical studies being
human well-being is a process of fulfilling or realizing one’s conducted in the field of happiness, Diener himself acknowl-
true self – daimon. Here, well-being is achieved when an edges that the progress has been slow and little when it comes
individual lives in accordance with her true nature i.e. when to understand the core of this fundamental human experience
she aligns her activities and fully engages her life with the called happiness (Diener et al. 1999).
values that are deeply held within. In such a condition an Furthermore, some others have pointed out that the current
individual feels she is ‘intensely alive’, ‘complete’ and understanding of happiness is not culturally informed. Ryff
existing as she ‘really is’ (Waterman 1993). Psychological remarks that the contemporary notions of well-being obscures
well-being (PWB) is the counterpart of SWB in the the awareness that culture, history, ethnicity, class, and so on
eudaimonic tradition. Ryff defining eudiamonia or psycholog- give rise to different, perhaps competing, conceptions of well-
ical well-being ‘as the striving for perfection that represents being (Ryff 1989). It is been largely felt that the existing
the realization of one’s true potentials’ (Ryff 1995) identifies conceptualization and the measurements draw heavily from
six characters of it viz. self-acceptance, purpose in life, per- the Euro-American cultural perspective while ignoring the
sonal growth, autonomy, environmental mastery and positive Eastern perspectives (Lu 2001). While there are many com-
relationships (Ryff 1989). This conceptualization maintains mon underpinnings, the West and the East epitomizing the
that not all desires – not all outcomes that a person might have individual and social oriented societies respectively, have
– would yield well-being when achieved. Fulfilment of desire altogether different conceptualizations of happiness (Lu and
may be pleasure-producing but not necessarily good and Gilmour 2004) (Uchida et al. 2004). Hence, culture being a
Psychol Stud

significant factor in the construction of happiness, determin- with the topic of happiness amongst others that are of interest
ing its causes, conditions, motivations, correlates and out- to us in the present context.
comes, any meaningful discourse on happiness has to be In the first section of this paper, we will examine the major
culturally informed. In this backdrop, this paper presents the Upanis: ads and allied texts for their content of happiness. All
Vedantic perspective of happiness from the Indic tradition in the Upanis: ads, to a considerable extent, share a similar un-
its original context and maps out its contours. derstanding of the concept of happiness though the details
In addition, there are studies that question the direction and differ from one another. The texts that have been the focus of
utility of the researches that are happening in the field of discussion here are, in strict terms, from Vedantic school of
happiness. They observe that on a practical side the findings thought, which is one of the traditions practised even today.
of these studies have helped people little in deepening their Moreover, since most of the other schools of Indic thought
knowledge about what makes them truly happy (Nelson et al. share similar views on happiness, the term Indic is used
2013). ‘To the extent that people’s predictions about what will synonymously with the term Vendantic in this paper.
make them happy are flawed’, Wilson and Gilbert remark that, Before getting into the concept of happiness, it would be
‘people fail at maximizing their happiness’ (Wilson and Gilbert helpful and essential to take a cursory glance of the two terms
2005). Consequently, the decision and the choices that the that have been used to denote or represent the concept of
people make would be negatively impacted. On the other hand happiness.
some people have noted that there are psychological traditions
in the East that have recognised the individual’s ability to grow 1. Ānanda: This term is made of the prefix ‘Ā’ and the root
and evolve and thereby provide her a ‘sense of the unlimited ‘nand’ meaning rejoicing. Happiness, joy, enjoyment, sen-
ecstasy that could be found within a self which is freed of sual pleasure, pure happiness are the meanings of the term
personal strivings’ (Murphy and Murphy 1968). While men- as provided by Monier Williams. It is the most commonly
tioning about the Indian sages whom he personally met who used word in Vedantic literature. Generally, it covers the
had achieved higher and newer (not familiar in the West) states entire range of happiness at various levels but mainly refers
of health and well-being, Boss notes (Boss 1965, pp. 187–188): to the ultimate happiness or bliss. It is also one of the terms
used to describe the essential nature of the Self along with
And yet there were exalted figures of the sages and holy Sat (Existence) and Cit (Consciousness).
men themselves, each one of them a living example of 2. Sukha: This is the other most common term for happiness
the possibility of the human growth and maturity and of in Vedantic literature and folk culture. It means pleasant,
the attainment of an impeccable inner peace, a joyous comfort, easiness, prosperity, pleasure, happiness etc. ac-
freedom from guilt, and a purified, selfless goodness cording to the Monier Williams dictionary.
and calmness…No matter how carefully I observe the
waking lives of the holy men, no matter how ready they In common language the terms sukha and Ānanda are used
were to tell me about their dreams, I could not detect in synonymously. However, it must be noted that sukha belongs
the best of them a trace of a selfish action or any kind of to a set of concepts where dualities exist i.e. dukha (pain,
repressed or consciously concealed shadow life. sorrow, suffering etc.) stands in contrast to sukha, whereas
Ānanda is beyond sukha as it transcends both sukha and
Moreover, new research suggests that authentic happiness
dukha. As such it has no term that stands in anti-thesis to
equated with the Indic concept of ‘sukha ’ is marked with the
the experience of Ānanda.
presence of ‘plenitude, bliss, peace of mind, serenity, inner
The other terms used to describe aspects of happiness
peace or fulfilment’ (Dambrun and Ricard 2011). In this
would be explained in the subsequent passages as necessary.
context it becomes desirable to revisit some of the conceptu-
alizations of happiness and well-being in the Vedantic view
point from the Indic tradition.
Summary of Discussion on Happiness in Various
Upanis: ads

Happiness in the Vedantic Tradition It is interesting to note that the happiness and well-being of an
individual have been dealt with so extensively in the Upanis: ads.
Any discussion concerning Indian intellectual heritage must Ānanda, the term for bliss or happiness itself, appears in various
necessarily begin with the Vedic reference for a simple reason places and there are extensive discussions on the nature and
that it is the first available document of human culture and sources of happiness. This section will merely describe the
thought. When one glances through the Vedic and especially different contexts in which the concept of happiness has been
the Upanishadic (also known as Vedantic) texts, one would covered without attempting to be either interpretative or prescrip-
note the presence of significant number of passages that deal tive. A discussion on the same will follow later.
Psychol Stud

Taittirīya Upanis: ad up his worldly pursuit in quest of immortality or amr: tatvam. As


he had decided to leave, he wanted to distribute his property
To start with, in the Taittirīya Upanis: ad, the second chapter is equitably between his two wives. When he announces his deci-
called Brahmānandavallī. It is a chapter on Ānanda of the sion to them, Maitreyi questions him if he intended to distribute
Brahman. Accordingly, it starts and ends with a detailed discus- anything acquiring which she could be immortal. At the end of a
sion on the nature of happiness and realizing it as the core of detailed dialogue that ensues between them, Yajnyavalka utters
one’s being. According to it, the innermost core of the being is the oft discussed phrase of the Upanis: ad -‘ātmanastu kāmāya
: :
the ‘Ānanda’ – pure existential/transcendental bliss, along with sarvam priyam bhavati’, which means, ‘for the sake of the self
‘Sat’ (Existence) and ‘Cit’ (Consciousness). It is the core of everything else becomes dear or desirable.’
human personality around which the individual exists and func-
tions. However, there are many layers or sheaths around this core
which impede the experience of the original condition. The Māṇḍūkya Upanis: ad
sheaths that cover are five in number viz. Annamaya kośa -
physical, Prāṇamaya kośa – the vital, Manomaya kośa – the In the Māṇḍūkya Upanis: ad , the self of the individual is
mental, Vijñānamaya kośa – the intuitive, and Ānandamaya described using two words ‘ānandamaya’(filled with bliss)
kośa – the blissful (Salagame 2003). This idea is strengthened and ‘ānandabhuk ’ (experiencer of bliss). However, in the
in the third and next chapter Bhr: guvallī. Continuing further, a previous line in the concerned section, the self is qualified as
very interesting imagery of a bird is presented in another passage. the being in the dreamless state with no desires unfulfilled.
It introduces the threefold concept of priya, moda and pramoda
and says that the ‘priya’ is its head, ‘moda’ and ‘pramoda’ are
its left and right wings respectively, ‘ānanda’ is the soul and Chāndogya Upanis: ad
‘brahman’ is its base. Shankaracharya, while commenting on
this section, shares an interesting insight pertaining to happiness. In the seventh chapter of the Chāndogya Upanis: ad, in the
He notes that the happiness experienced, when an object is seen conversation between Narada, a great scholar himself and
or perceived with a sense of wanting it is ‘priya’, when the object sage Sanat Kumara, Sanat Kumara says that happiness lies
is possessed, it is ‘moda’ and when enjoyed/utilized, it is in fullness or vastness and not in a sense of limitedness.
‘pramoda’.
In another significant section, at the end of the same chap-
ter, there is a detailed discussion on the grades of happiness Kat: h hopanis: ad
that can be experienced by a person. Human happiness or
‘mānus: a ānanda ’ is considered to be the baseline and The Kat: hopanis: ad, which is in the form of a dialogue between
‘brahmānanda’ is perceived to be at the top of the scale with Yama and Nachiketa, also provides us with rich insights in
nine levels in between. Each level marks a hundredfold in- understanding the concept of happiness. At a certain point
crease of happiness than the preceding one. What is very where Yama tries to lure Nachiketa with various forms of
interesting to note apart from the gradation is the mentioning riches and sensual pleasures, the young boy, Nachiketa, is
of the determinants of human happiness, i.e., age, learning, persistent in seeking the knowledge of immortality as he
value system, psychological, physical and financial resource- knows for certain that ‘wealth cannot bring him happiness’3.
fulness. This converges largely with the determinants identified
by Argyle viz. age, health conditions, family and friends, mar-
ital status, employment, income and wealth (Argyle, The Bhagavad Gītā
Psychology of Happiness, 1987) (Argyle, Causes and
Correlates of Happiness, 1999). The Bhagavad Gītā is another important source that needs to
be examined in this context. Scholars and common people alike
have sought answers to many issues concerning life – happi-
Br: hadāraṇyaka Upanis: ad ness being a prominent one among them – from Bhagavad
Gītā. The Gītā defines it as ‘something which the individual
In the Br: hadāraṇyaka Upanis: ad, which is the most volumi- rejoices having attained with effort and which also marks the
nous of all the Upanis: ads, there is a story of Prajapati, which absence of unhappiness’4. It then goes on to classify three kinds
says that he became happy after creating a companion for of sukha – sātvika (noble), rājasika (dynamic) and tāmasika
himself emphasizing the need for relationships. (lethargic). According to the classification, sātvika sukha
Another psychologically significant parable of Yajnavalkya
appears in this Upanis: ad. Yajnayavalka, who is married to 3
Kat: hopanis: ad 1.27
4
Maitreyi and Katyayini, decides at some stage in his life to give Bhagavad Gītā 18.36
Psychol Stud

consists of such happiness which appears to be effortful (while happiness, this is experienced in a situation where the individual
pursuing it) but eventually tastes like nectar, i.e. very pleasant. is called to respond to a challenge where she has adequate
Such happiness arises as a result of intelligent and creative resources to do so. This presupposes that every individual has
efforts with a right knowledge of oneself5. Rājasika sukha is certain potentialities within which need to be expressed and
the resultant of the contact of the sense organs and the objects actualized. In other words, this is the kind of happiness a person
which appears to be pleasurable initially but unpleasant at the experiences in self-actualization. Here, the external situation
end, i.e. after the experience as it leaves a person with a sense of calls upon the person to rise above her ordinary comfort zone
wanting6. The third category, tāmasika sukha, is the happiness and excel in the required task and in response, her internal
that is delusionary in nature from beginning to the end that may system rises to the occasion successfully. While the best comes
arise from sleep, laziness, ignorance, illusion etc.7 out from within the individual, the situation or objects outside
Another important text in the Vedantic tradition which deals act as motivators or stimulants. Consequently, there is a sense of
with the idea of happiness at considerable length is the fulfilment, accomplishment, and enhanced self-worthiness that
Pañcadaśī . Later texts on dramaturgy, and aesthetics like accompanies such an experience which could be self-directed as
Nāt: yaśāstra, Dhvanyāloka etc. also reflect similar ideas. With- well as directed towards others. Such an experience is enduring
out going into a detailed discussion on the various kinds of in nature as it leaves the individual with a sense of contentment
happiness that have been mentioned as it is beyond the scope for a relatively longer period and also results in the augmentation
and purpose of the present paper, it would suffice to present of self-confidence and of psychological resources of the indi-
some key ideas provided by them. According to them, happi- vidual. This is also known as sātvika sukha.
ness can be broadly classified into three categories (levels as The first and the second conceptualization as noted could be
well). Such a classification and the nuances presented in this equated with the notions of hedonic and eudaimonic happiness
framework, in short conveys the quintessence of the Indic respectively. The causes, determinants and correlates of these
insight on happiness. They are (in the order of gross to subtle): two kinds of happiness match to a large extent with their western
counterparts. However, there is one more kind of happiness that
1. Vis: ayānanda – Sensual/Material happiness the Vedantic tradition identifies – the existential/transcendental
2. Kāvyānanda – Aesthetic happiness happiness. This conceptualization, is not completely unknown in
3. Brahmānanda – Existential/transcendental happiness the mystic traditions of both the East and the West, but nowhere
else has it been made the essential basis of the philosophy of life.
Vis: ayānanda – ‘Vis: aya’ refers to objects. Vis: ayānanda 8, The same will be explained in the following sections.
which can be equated to hedonistic happiness, is experienced Brahmānanda or transcendental happiness – ‘Brahma’, ac-
by an individual when she comes in contact with the objects of cording to Indian thought, is the supreme singular being who is
her desire. Usually, it is the pleasure associated with either the essential reality of every individual as well as the substratum
yearning, possessing, consuming, etc. of material objects. It is of the whole universe. ‘Brahmānanda’, is regarded as the highest
the grossest form of happiness. The Gītā notes that such plea- form of happiness or bliss that a person can experience. It is
surable experience based on the sense-object contact is momen- transcendental in the sense that it transcends the limitations of
tary or short lived, and may even leave the person with a sense of compartmentalized individual existence. It is also called ‘exis-
wanting and lack and consequently cause pain and suffering once tential’ because it is the core aspect of human existence in as
the external stimulus fades9. Moreover, the intensity of happiness much that it cannot be separated from human identity (Swami
experienced is said to reduce with prolonged exposure to the 2005). It is subjective in nature and not dependent on any
source. Rājasika and tāmasika sukha described in the Gītā come external object, situation or person. The ancient texts assure us
under this category. It must be noted that the Indic tradition does that every individual is capable of experiencing such a state of
not negate the role and necessity of material happiness. However, happiness through a conscious and systematic process of Self-
it very clearly brings out the limitations of such happiness and discovery as it is the very nature of one’s being and it is intrinsic
persuades the individual to go beyond immediate gratification to all of us. This kind of happiness results from Self-realization
and seek a deeper and more lasting happiness. which is qualitatively different from self-actualization. The tradi-
Kāvyānanda – ‘Kāvya’ literally means the work/product of tion claims that for those who have attained this blissful state
a ‘kavi’ – a visionary, a seer, a poet, an artist, an adventurer etc. once, it becomes the baseline for all their future actions (Table 1).
In this context, it refers to a class of activities where talent, skill,
courage, will, grace, etc. are employed. Similar to eudaimonic
Nature of Happiness
5
Ibid 18.37
6
Ibid 18.38
7 We have so far mentioned briefly sources from Vedantic texts
Ibid 18.39
8
Pañcadaśī 15.1 and traditions which have deliberations on the causes, nature,
9
Bhagavad Gītā 5.22 correlates and consequences of happiness experiences. This
Psychol Stud

Table 1 Summary of the discussion on happiness presented in Indian tradition

Sl No. Sheaths (from Level Happiness Nature Basis Indian Euro-American


gross to subtle) of happiness concept conceptualization

1 Annamaya Physical Rati (longing), Exciting Object based Visayānanda Hedonic


Priya (desire) Fleeting (External) ˙ Happiness
Moda (Joy) Exhausting
Pramoda (Excitement) Momentary
2 Prāṇamaya Vital Harśa (Delight) " " " "
Ullāsa (Merriment)
3 Manomaya Mental Santosa (Contentment) Satisfying Lower ego based Kāvyānanda Eudaimonic
Śānti ˙(Peace) Pacifying Happiness
Trpti (Satisfaction) Transient
4 Vijñānamaya Intuitive ˙
Ānanda (Relatively Refreshing Higher ego based " "
lasting happiness) Calming
Creative
Harmonious
Relatively enduring
Transcendental
happiness in parts
5 Ānandamaya Blissful Bhuma-ānanda (unlimited Transcendental Self based Brahmānanda Nil
happiness or bliss) Existential
Liberating
Enduring

section will present a synthetic view of the nature of happiness all activities and engagements. In addition, it is not only the
that has been discussed at various places with the focus on highest value against which all other possessions, accom-
transcendental happiness as it forms the core contribution of plishments and experiences are evaluated but also the su-
Indian thought. Though each source approaches the topic in its preme goal which brings meaning to all endeavours of life.
own unique way, they all share a common ground of understand- 2. Lasting happiness is realizable
ing which may or may not be mentioned explicitly. For instance, The experience of happiness may be difficult to de-
the texts on aesthetics mention the category of Brahmānanda but scribe, however, the sources assure us that it can be realized
do not venture to explain it as it comes under the scope of effectively by everyone13 and in a lasting manner depend-
spiritual texts. In another example, the equation of dreamless ing on the kind of happiness one strives to experience. This
sleep with absolute bliss cited in the Brihadāranyaka Upanisad must be noted in contrast to early Western thinking that
˙ ˙ ˙
is dealt with in greater detail in Māṇḍūkya Upanisad. However, happiness could be attained only by the ‘virtuous – the
˙
the point is that all these sources provide us with necessary happy few’ (McMahon 2009). After having stated in un-
insights into the concept of happiness from which a comprehen- equivocal terms that happiness is attainable by all, elaborate
sive theory on happiness can be constructed. Based on the road maps are provided to attain the same. The following
material available and presented thus far, the following charac- aspects are beneficial in understanding the roadmap.
teristics of happiness with the concept of the transcendental one
at its core are presented here. a. Wisdom/knowledge framework
The philosophical and spiritual traditions of India
1. Happiness is the primary motivator and guiding value of talk of several seekers who have undertaken rigorous
human life practices to experience deep and lasting happiness.
The Upanisads declare that ‘all beings emerge from Furthermore, the very fact that the Indic tradition
˙
happiness, live by happiness and to happiness they re- considers that lasting happiness is attainable implies
turn’10, ‘an individual endeavours with the prospect of that there have been people who have walked the path
finding happiness, and does not do so when there is no and described their experiences in great detail along
happiness’11. Even folk literature reiterates the same idea with the methodology they followed in order to arrive
when it says ‘all beings act with the motive of attaining at it. There is therefore, an extensive body of docu-
happiness and getting rid of unhappiness’12. Accordingly, mented knowledge which serves as a useful manual to
happiness is the greatest good that an individual considers in discover the different stages of happiness in one’s life
by providing a framework as well as practical guide-
10
Taittirīya Upanisad 3.6 lines on the roadmap to happiness.
11
Chāndogya Upanis˙ ad 7.22
12 ˙ kaṁ me mā bhūt iti lokapravrttih
sukhaṁ me syāt duh 13
sarve bhavantu sukhinah
˙ ˙ ˙ ˙
Psychol Stud

At this stage, it would be appropriate to look into


the Indian philosophical framework in order to briefly
understand the position of happiness and its pursuit in
life. In simple terms, it is considered that every indi-
vidual has to choose between two paths in life –
preyomārga (path of worldly life) and śreyomārga
(path of spiritual life). Though both paths are quite
different and demand different disciplines in adhering
to them, both of them are equally valid and accepted.
The goal of śreyomārga is apavarga (liberation or
self-realisation) and of preyomārga is bhoga (worldly Fig. 1 Two paths of life
enjoyment or gratification). Hence, the path of world-
ly life sanctions an active pursuit of kāma (desires) (path of knowledge) suggest ways of working on one-
while the path of spiritual life demands an active self from a cognitive point of view; Karma Yoga (path
pursuit of trpti (contentment). Nonetheless, both of action) follows the conative path; Rāja Yoga (path of
˙
paths aim at happiness of different kinds. Further psychic control) adopts the psychic way and Bhakti
discussion on this topic declares that contentment Yoga (path of devotion) offers techniques to work on
leads to ānanda (bliss) and the pursuit of desires leads the emotional being of the individual. In addition to
to sukha (pleasure) (Fig. 1). these, the much encouraged virtues like dāna (alms
It must be noted that, as per the texts, the pursuit of giving), dhanyatā (gratitude), prapatti (surrender),
desires and enjoyment in preyomārga also needs to be vairāgya (non-attachment), etc. are very beneficial in
done in a regulated manner on the basis of Dharma 14 preparing oneself to realize happiness. Self-cultivation is
(righteousness) (Salagame 2003). There are numerous an important aspect necessary for the transformation of
discussions on this front which are beyond the scope an individual as the Indian thought emphasizes that all
of the present paper. However, it is worth noting at knowledge has to necessarily culminate in experience15.
this point that Indian tradition which is often labelled d. Favourable conditions for experiencing happiness
as otherworldly or pessimistic has an extensive liter- Though happiness is considered to the psycholog-
ature on happiness clearly establishing it as a corner- ical state of the individual it has been acknowledged,
stone of life’s philosophy and thereby disproving any there are certain conditions which are favourable in
such notion which suggests otherwise. experiencing happiness. One of the popular verses16
b. Individual as the agent of happiness of Sanskrit literature identifies them as follows:
All the discussions about happiness lead to the
conclusion that, eventually, the individual is solely i. Physical and psychological health
responsible for her own happiness. All others – per- ii. Good, worthy and positive engagements in life
sons, situation and objects are only instrumental iii. Absence of sorrow
causes for that experience. To add to this, the choice
of being happy ultimately rests with the individual. 3. Happiness is subjective
She has to finally decide whether she wants to be Happiness is a subjective phenomenon. It has to be
happy or unhappy in any given situation. She can experienced within. Often there is a mistaken understand-
either allow external circumstances to overpower ing that things outside the being cause happiness. The
her or use her discretion and exercise control over Indian thought tradition very clearly states that happiness
their effects on her. Indian tradition suggests that has to be sought from inside. The external factors may or
individuals can systematically cultivate the capacity may not be conducive to evoke happiness within the
of exercising such a mastery and freedom. individual. It has to be experienced from inside only. In
c. Self-cultivation as the means one of the verses in Bhajagovindam , Shankara says, ‘One
A very important contribution from Indian thought to may revel in yoga or pleasures; in the company of people
the existing discourse on happiness is the detailed and or bereft of company. Only the one whose mind steadily
practical methodology developed therein for self- delights in Brahman revels; He cannot but be happy’17.
cultivation leading towards self-mastery. The entire The idea here is that objects, situations, people who are
world of yogic knowledge provides various exercises 15
Brahmasūtra śānkarabhāsyam 1.1
for self-cultivation. Branches of Yoga, like Jñāna Yoga 16
sarve bhavantu sukhinah˙, sarve santu nirāmayāh , sarve bhadrāṇi
˙
paśyantu, mā kaścit dukhabhāg bhavet ˙
14
Mahābhārata 18.5.49 17
Bhajagovindam 19 ˙
Psychol Stud

normally considered to be sources of happiness are at mentioned in the passage above, do not include feel-
most stimulants or catalysts and not its essential causes. ing of excitement or those associated with high
A wise person is happy irrespective of the external cir- arousals that give a momentary ‘kick’ as in the case
cumstances as she has discovered the source of happiness of hedonic pleasures. The positive feelings that have
within herself. been described that accompany the sense of content-
4. Happiness is a psychological state ment make the individual composed and anchored in
Happiness is a profound psychological state of satis- herself, free from any hankering for anything from
faction and contentment and is irrespective of the external outside, as the Gītā points out, ‘for one who is fully
circumstances. It is also accompanied by positive feelings content in oneself, there is nothing to strive for’21.
of joy, comfort, hope etc. This kind of happiness is regarded as being long
lasting and beyond all conflicts.
a. Satisfaction and Contentment c. Internal harmony
The satisfaction of one’s needs, from material to Happiness is also intimately linked to a state of
spiritual, is one of the means for happiness. Fulfilment harmony: harmony at various levels, as a conflict-
of needs and gratification of desires are the most ridden mind will never experience happiness. The
acknowledged causes for happiness. However, East- process of yoga employed in self-cultivation is itself
ern thought, especially the Indian, stresses on the need defined in terms of balance or equanimity22. Proceed-
for developing a deep sense of contentment as a ing further, it is to be noted that harmony has to be
practical means to realize happiness. This highlights achieved at various levels of the being from
the intimate relationship between the sense of con- annamyakośa to ānandamayakośa, at individual and
tentment and the feeling of happiness. In fact, social levels and from natural to the spiritual. The idea
Patanjali’s Yoga Sūtra contends that by practising of achieving internal harmony as a correlate to happi-
‘santośa (contentment) one can attain the highest ness is an important aspect in many Asian cultures
state of happiness’18. One of the verses of folk and traditions as noted by Lou Lu and Gilmour (Lu
wisdom states that ‘contentment is the greatest wealth and Gilmour 2004).
that a man can possess’19. In the Mahābhārata, the 5. Happiness is the essential nature of the Self
virtue of contentment is brought out in unambiguous One of the singularly significant contributions of the
terms as it states that ‘contentment is the highest Indian thought to happiness studies, as noted earlier, is the
heaven, contentment is the highest happiness. There recognition of happiness as the essential nature of the Self.
is nothing (in the world) that is higher than content- The central epithet used to describe the nature of the self is
ment’20. In a similar manner, one can go on enlisting ‘Sat -Cit -Ānanda ’ – ‘Existence-Consciousness-Bliss’.
several sources that brings forth the need for practis- Accordingly, every individual represents blissful and con-
ing contentment in order to realize happiness. scious existence. The innermost essence of one’s being is
b. Positive emotions bliss or absolute happiness – ānandamaya – full of bliss,
Though happiness, in transcendental terms, is go- as depicted in the pañcakos´a model of the Taittirīya
ing beyond emotions, the texts add that the sense of Upanisad . This being the nature of the Self, Self-
˙
contentment makes one capable of experiencing discovery or Self-realization necessarily culminates in
many other positive emotions frequently. While de- the realization/experience of happiness. Such happiness
lineating certain characteristics of happy people as is regarded as pure as it is absolutely independent from the
documented in Indian tradition, Srivastava and presence or absence of every conceivable external object
Mishra note that ‘they (happy people) are bestowed of happiness. This is the highest kind of happiness one can
with the company of the wise, are loved by the close experience referred to earlier as Brahmānanda (Taittirīya
ones, remain free from debts, and have certainty of Upanisad) and hence it has been termed as transcendental
˙
livelihood. They are magnanimous, not plagued by happiness.
bad or negative thoughts, are optimistic, and full of The Vedantic tradition highlights this particular notion
enthusiasm…..Bliss is said to give rise to knowledge of happiness as worthy of pursuit and encourages people
(to self) and love (for others). People enjoy every to channelize their energies towards recognizing their real
aspect of life if they are happy inside.’ (Srivastava nature which is almost/usually ‘forgotten’. Shankara in
and Misra 2011). The striking feature of the emotions the series of verses called ‘Ātmasatkam’ brings out this
˙ ˙
idea in unequivocal terms when he declares that ‘I am
18
Pātañjala Yoga Sūtram 2.32
19 21
santosa eva purusasya paraṁ nidhānam Bhagavad Gītā 3.17
20 ˙
Mahābhārata ˙
12.21.2 22
Bhagavad Gītā 2.48
Psychol Stud

That Pure Consciousness and Bliss. I am Śiva ; I am others, others interests are to be taken into consideration
Śiva’.23 as they become important determinants for one’s overall
Furthermore, understanding Brahman as the experience of happiness unlike in individualistic cultures/
source of all happiness as described by Vedanta, offers societies of the West. Kautilya, in is work on polity,
the scope to explain the most ordinary sorts of pleasure advocates that the ‘king’s happiness lies in the happiness
and happiness experienced in life. ‘All (worldly) joys of the people’28. There are many verses in the spiritual,
experienced by all beings are just a minute fraction of this scholastic and folk traditions that vividly drive home this
(Brahmānanda)’24 and are ‘doorways to experience the point that individual interests are subordinate to the inter-
ultimate bliss’.25 This position is all-inclusive in the sense ests of the family which in turn are subordinate to those of
that every other pleasure or happiness, gross or higher/ the society and so on. This is very much in line with
subtle finds its due place in relation to this highest happi- similar notions present in Eastern cultures as Luo Lu
ness. However, this position stands quite at the other et al. points out that ‘self-restrain and consideration for
extreme to Freud’s contention that the origins of all forms the other’s welfare are vital for achieving a harmonious
of happiness including the higher ones are nothing but the Chinese relationship’ that would make way for enduring
expressions of sexual pleasure. Such a theory would fail happiness, based on a comparative study of the Eastern
to explain the basis of even simple joys that would arise and Western conceptions of happiness (Lu and Gilmour
out of watching a smiling baby or a rising sun. Alterna- 2004).
tively, this meta-theory of happiness can not only accom- 7. Happiness as freedom and completeness
modate all pleasures, joys described in terms of both the Freedom is defined as the ‘possibility to choose’ from
hedonic and eudaimonic views but also reconcile them the given set of choices (Veenhoven 2000). At a personal
harmoniously with the conceptualization of the third one, level, an individual is said to be free if she can exercise her
i.e. transcendental happiness. freedom to select, to act without being inhibited by her
6. Happiness is active interconnectedness with others internal conditionings, opinions, prejudices, conflicts,
Another significant aspect of happiness brought out by doubts etc. provided the environment offers her the ‘op-
the Upanisads is that it is not just self-directed. It extends portunity to choose’. In the natural course, the individual’s
˙
to others as well, as it is based on the perception of an saṁskāras 29 and the vāsanās 30 act as determinants of
underlying interconnectedness with others and everything one’s character/behaviour. It is similar to the sub-
else in this creation. Others are seen as an extension of conscious and the unconscious in Freudian terms or ‘con-
oneself. The experience of happiness related to self- ditioning’ according to the behaviourists. These restrict
realisation does not perceive individualized/ the individual’s ability to exercise freedom in selecting her
compartmentalized spaces but see themselves as part of response to the given situation. For instance, of the given
a common larger space which is temporarily demarcated ten responses an individual can chose, if she is compelled
for functional purposes. Speaking from a philosophical by her internal/mental frame to make a particular choice, it
perspective, as one deeply loves oneself and others, being amounts to limiting the choice of the individual. This
the extension of the same self, there is a natural tendency limitation imposed on the self of the individual curtails
‘to love and to be loved’. In the Rāmāyana Advayam, it is the freedom of the individual and narrows down the
˙
noted ‘that coming into a state of self-sameness with all cognitive, functional and emotional ability of the individ-
beings is the source of happiness of the wise people’.26 ual. In short the being becomes constricted.
Relating to others becomes both a consequence and a On the other hand, the Indian thought emphasizes that
source of happiness. As a result of this view, a high a human being can consciously outgrow her limitations
premium is placed on ‘satsaṅga’ – company of the wise and overcome the shadow effect of her vāsanās and
people in the process of discovering happiness. Shankara saṁskāras and thereby exercise full control over the
claims that ‘…in all the three worlds, only the company of situation and also exercise human freedom to a large
the wise will help one to cross the hardship of life’27 and extent as she is essentially free from such limiting factors.
move towards happiness. These limiting factors, according to it, are incidental and
Furthermore, a significant feature to be noted of the not essential features of the individual. It demands that
Indian concept of happiness is that, while relating to one has to discover that freedom within and exercise it in
the given circumstance. Nevertheless, it stands firmly as a
23
Ātmasatkam 1
24 ˙ ˙ yaka Upanisad 4.3.32
Brhadāran 28
Arthaśāstra 1.19.34
25 ˙
Pañcadaśī˙ 15.1 ˙ 29
The impressions left behind by experiences and actions that are said to
26
http://www.krishnamurthys.com/kvforp/Appa/Ramayana- shape future actions and behaviours
30
advayampage4.html#_ftnref2. verse no. 203 Strong desires especially originating from drives or other inborn
27
Bhajagovindam 13 tendencies
Psychol Stud

human possibility. Some great thinkers, even in the west, spiritual. One’s ability to achieve such an expansion is free-
have arrived at such an insight through their own experi- dom and that brings great joy and happiness.
ences. Allport, in his preface to Victor Frankl’s ‘Man’s Also, freedom is one of the greatest joys one can experi-
Search for Meaning’ writes: ence. It would certainly include individual freedom protected
and advanced by individualistic societies, collective freedom
In the concentration camp every circumstance conspires to earn and live, and financial freedom, promised by capital-
to make the prisoner lose his hold. All the familiar goals istic societies, as equally valid and essential. However, spiri-
in life are snatched away. What alone remains is “the last tual freedom nurtured by non-materialistic societies, for one to
of human freedoms”—the ability to “choose one's atti- grow towards completeness and perfection internally assumes
tude in a given set of circumstances.” This ultimate significance in this perspective. Hence, in the Indian tradition,
freedom, recognized by the ancient Stoics as well as moksa – liberation or freedom – has been presented as the
˙
by modern existentialists, takes on vivid significance in highest purusārtha – objective of life.
˙
Frankl’s story. The prisoners were only average men, 8. Happiness is not absence of unhappiness
but some, at least, by choosing to be “worthy of their The relationship between happiness and unhappiness
suffering” proved man’s capacity to rise above his out- has been a matter of debate in most cultures. Indian
ward fate. thought, at a deeper philosophical level, posits the cyclic
Vicktor Frankl remarks in his book, nature of the experience of happiness and unhappiness32.
We who lived in concentration camps can remember the Rather than seeing them as dialectically opposite entities,
men who walked through the huts comforting others, it presents them as one complimentary to the other in
giving away their last piece of bread. They may have helping the individual grow. The dualities of life, happi-
been few in number, but they offer sufficient proof that ness and sorrow, pain and pleasure, gain and loss, fame
everything can be taken from a man but one thing: the last and notoriety, etc. including life and death are seen as the
of the human freedoms – to choose one’s attitude in any necessary and complimentary experiences that make an
given set of circumstances, to choose one’s own way. individual’s life complete. As shown earlier, the purpose
of the life experience is to enable a person to grow and to
Happiness, on the other hand is deemed as a sense of
evolve into a complete being. The experience of sorrow
expansion of the being. There is a constant urge for and
and joy is seen as contributing towards that end. Echoing
momentum towards growth and expansion. Expansion here
this line of thought, a Sri Aurobindo remarks, ‘Pain is the
refers to the process of overcoming the sense of limitedness
hand of Nature sculpturing men to greatness’ (Aurobindo
due to one’s conditioning. Chāndogya Upanisad declares
˙ 1997, p. 444). At a lower plane, both happiness and
‘that which is ever-expanding is happiness and there is no
unhappiness appear to be dialectically opposing one an-
happiness in littleness or narrowness’31, i.e. ‘happiness is
other, but at a higher plane, both are reconciled and
plenum, happiness is completeness, happiness is totality’.
complement each other with a singularity of purpose –
Accordingly, the experience of growing or expanding towards
to give a sense of completeness of experience. Hence, in
completeness is happiness. Incidentally, in addition to the
the larger perspective of transcendental happiness, they
experience of expansion itself, all aids, instruments that lead
(pleasure and pain) are to be understood as mutually
one to experience the expansion are also seen as sources of
qualifying and complimentary experiences.
happiness. Hence an ice cream in a hot afternoon, a shower at
the end of the day, a touch of the beloved, possession of a new
smart phone, an increment in the salary, a recognition in the
community everything becomes the causes of happiness. Im-
mediately, an equation between the objects, persons, situa- Conclusions and Implications for the Contemporary
tions, etc. is established equating them with happiness. In Discourse on Happiness
reality, it can be seen that these are just catalysts, that tend to
make an individual feel complete though momentarily, which Having a fragmented picture of the human being as a socio-
makes one happy. The yearning is for the experience of biological entity and attempting to map all his experiences
expansion and completeness as the very nature of the self is including the most significant of them – happiness, in this
completeness and perfection according to the Vedantic backdrop, have yielded little results in the mainstream ap-
thought. The objects, sensations, feelings, ideas, thoughts proach to psychology based on Euro-American culture. Basic
etc. aid in achieving it in the initial stage of the journey. Hence, questions concerning human happiness as to what truly makes
it encompasses every form of expansion from material to an individual happy remain while the issue of attaining hap-
piness gets even more complicated. One may quote numerous
31 32
Chāndogya Upanisad 7.23 Adhyātmarāmāyaṇam 2.6.1
˙
Psychol Stud

studies that have been conducted in the areas of Subjective Economics and Sociology is a frontrunner in determining
and Psychological well-being. However, neither can one the policies on governance and welfare of states. While argu-
claim that the world is getting happier day by day, nor can ing for integrating the insights from Buddhist and Eastern
one deny the fact that the world seems to be slipping into a Psychologies with eudaimonic concepts, Tomer notes that
darker shade with pain, suffering, agony, conflicts, and vio- the economists, in determining human happiness, ‘have large-
lence at all levels from individual to international. The in- ly emphasized the hedonic, utilitarian, material and tangible
creasing crime and violence rate around the world is no less aspects of human life’ (Tomer 2010) so far. Hence it becomes
empirical an evidence than the data collected in a laboratory or important to identify what insights are supplied from psychol-
in an artificial set-up. Furthermore, one who sticks to the third- ogy to other academic and social disciplines. In all likelihood,
person methodology and statistics even when their effective- a partial or skewed understanding might yield limited results.
ness in enhancing human happiness and well-being is ques- On the other hand, with the larger picture in view, with the
tionable is no more rational than who seeks alternative tools. acknowledgment of transcendental happiness, one could esti-
Rats might be happy with more cheese that one might have mate the possible outcomes and its impact on various fields
provided them (they also would eventually seek freedom) but including economics and development. A simple shift of
a human being cannot be happy with a little more of every- focus from hedonic to eudaimonic happiness would also
thing that she needs and wants – food, sex, wealth, security, call for a shift in private and public policies concerning
belongingness, recognition. ‘More the merrier’ philosophy the welfare of the state. Gross National Happiness (GNH)
works to a certain extent but fails to deliver the most essential (Priesner 2006) of Bhutan, largely echoing the eudaimonic
– the key to lasting happiness. This compels one to question notions of happiness, is an outstanding example of one such
the basic premise on which the dominant theories of human major shift in measuring the progress of a society in devia-
happiness have been developed and also the methods adopted tion to the GDP based growth assessment followed by most
therein to study them. Not that the painstaking research in the nations which is again dependent on the hedonic conception.
field deserve any less merit, but it could be probable that some And in all likelihood, a shift towards transcendental happi-
critical aspect of happiness might have been overlooked in the ness would only brighten the prospects of better governance
process. The point, then, is how do we supplement that and welfare systems with more holistic and integrated
essential character and where to do we find it. approaches.
At this stage, considering the insights on happiness Moreover, introducing insights from not only Indian but
provided by the Indian perspective delineated above, we cultures outside the Euro-American world would enable the
can reasonably argue that the Vedantic approach can discourse on happiness to be more culturally informed. De-
offer such an essential unifying element as Ananda spite the presence of universal underpinnings of happiness, the
Coomaraswamy rightly notes, ‘The heart and essence of study would be incomplete unless culture dependent factors
Indian experience is to be found in a constant intuition of become an integral part of it, as many empirical studies have
the unity of all life, and the instinctive and ineradicable provided considerable evidence to claim that ‘happiness and
conviction that the recognition of this unity is the highest well-being are significantly grounded in socio-cultural modes
good and the uttermost freedom’, (Coomaraswamy 2013). of being a person and interacting with others’ (Uchida et al.
However, it needs to be examined how best we could inte- 2004).
grate them with the existing body of knowledge to build a Finally, there are many issues that are to be effectively
meta-theory of happiness that could without undermining the resolved after having admitted transcendental happiness into
grossest form of happiness, provides central space for the the theoretical discourse, such as: What are its parameters?
subtlest and most enduring kind of happiness and allow one How to ensure its tangibility while retaining its essential
to evaluate various kinds of happiness from the standpoint of intangible character? What kinds of instruments, both quali-
transcendental happiness while providing her with efficient tative and quantitative, are capable of measuring those param-
tools to explore and experience it. eters and how to construct them? What are the systemic ways
Furthermore, happiness being the fundamental experience of achieving lasting happiness? And would it be possible to
and primary motivator of every human action, it influences all demonstrate with compelling empirical evidence that such an
other fields of individual and social life. It might appear that approach accomplishes what it claims to do - providing key to
one is stretching the argument too far, however, if we observe lasting happiness universally? Besides, the challenges in
carefully, all our social and economic institutions and their building such a meta-theory of happiness are many as it
policies, both at national and international levels, are largely demands collaborative efforts on behalf of psychologists,
influenced by the perceived notion of what makes a human phenomenologists, philosophers, sociologists, economists
being happy. Happiness Economics, an emerging field of and other social science researchers. Nevertheless, it is worth
knowledge that attempts to integrate the understanding on the effort if it could make an individual sing in ecstasy ‘I am
happiness from psychology with other fields such as bliss’, ‘I am bliss’ irrespective of the circumstances.
Psychol Stud

References Nelson, S. K., Kurtz, J. L., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2013). What psycholog-
ical science knows about achieving happiness. In S. J. Lynn, W.
O’Donohue, & S. Lilienfeld (Eds.), Better, stronger, wiser:
Argyle, M. (1987). The psychology of happiness. London: Routledge. Psychological science and well-being. New York: Sage. in press.
Argyle, M. (1999). Causes and correlates of happiness. In D. Kahneman, Priesner, S. (2006, September 6). Gross National Happiness – Bhutan’s
E. Diener, & N. Schwartz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of Vision of Development and its Challenges. Retrieved from Bhutan
hedonic psychology (pp. 353–373). New York: Russel Sage Monarchy Website: http://www.bhutan2008.bt/ndlb/typescripts/10/
Foundation. GNH_Ch3_Priesner.pdf.
Aristotle. (1999). Nicomachean ethics. Kitchener: Batoche Books. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A
Aurobindo, S. (1997). Savitri . Pondicherry: Sri Aurobindo review of research on hedonic and eudiamonic well-being. Annual
Ashram Trust. Review of Psychology, 141–166.
Boss, M. (1965). A psychiatrist discovers India. London: Oswald Wolff. Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the
Coomaraswamy, A. K. (2013). The dance of Shiva. New Delhi: Rupa meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and
Publication. Social Psychology, 57(6), 1069–1081.
Dambrun, M., & Ricard, M. (2011). Self-centeredness and selflessness: a Ryff, C. D. (1995). Psychological well-being in adult life. Current
theory of self-based psychological functioning and its consequences Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 99–104.
for happness. Review of General Psychology, 15(2), 138–157. Salagame, K. K. (2003). An Indian conception of well being. In J. Henry
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2008). Hedonia, eudiamonia, and well-being: (Ed.), European positive psychology proceedings. Leicester: British
an introduction. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9(1), 1–11. Psychological Society.
Diener, E. (2000). Subjective well-being: the sceince of happiness, and a Smith, A. (2009). The theory of moral sentiments. Munich: Grin.
proposal for a national index . American Psychologist, 34–43. Srivastava, A. K., & Misra, G. (2011). Happiness and well-being: An
Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective Indian perspective. In G. Misra (Ed.), Handbook of psychology in
well-being: three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, India (pp. 299–310). New Delhi: Oxford University Press.
125(2), 276–302. Swami, T. (2005). Viewpoint: Joy and sorrow. Journal of Hindu-
Keyes, C. L. (1998). Social well-being. Social Psychology Quarterly, Christian Studies, 18, 49–50.
61(2), 121–140. Tomer, J. F. (2010, November 12). Enduring happiness: Integrating the
Lu, L. (2001). Understanding happiness: a look into the Chinese folk hedonic and eudiamonic approaches. Retrieved from Social
psychology. Journal of Happiness Studies(2), 407–432. Science Research Network: http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.
Lu, L., & Gilmour, R. (2004). Culture and conceptions of happiness: cfm?abstract_id=1708212.
individual oriented and social oriented SWB. Journal of Happiness Uchida, Y., Norasakkunkit, V., & Kitayama, S. (2004). Cultural construc-
Studies, 269–291. tions of happiness: theory and empirical evidence. Journal of
Lyubomirsky, S., Sheldon, K. M., & Schkade, D. (2005). Pursuing Happiness Studies(5), 223–239.
happiness: the architecture of sustainable change. Review of Veenhoven, R. (2000). Freedom and Happiness: A comparativestudy in 46
General Psychology, 111–131. nations in the early 1990’s. In E. Diener & E. M. Suh (Eds.), Culture
McMahon, D. M. (2009). The history of happiness and contemporary and subjective wellbeing (pp. 257–288). Cambridge: MIT Press.
happiness studies. In A. K. Dutt & B. Radcliff (Eds.), Happiness, Waterman, A. S. (1993). Two conceptions of happiness: contrasts of
economics and politics (pp. 25–32). Cheltenham: Edward Elgar personal expressiveness (eudiamonia) and hedonic enjoyment.
Publishing Limited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 678–691.
Murphy, G., & Murphy, L. B. (Eds.). (1968). Asian psychology. New Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2005). Affective forecasting: knowing what
York: Basic Books. to want. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(3), 131–134.
5

Subjective Well-Being

The Science of Happiness and Life Satisfaction

Ed Diener, Richard E. Lucas, & Shigehiro Oishi

Definition of Subjective Well-Being Utilitarians such as Jeremy Bentham, however,


argued that the presence of pleasure and the ab-
Since ancient times humans have wondered sence of pain are the defining characteristics of
about what makes a good life. Scientists who a good life (1789/1948). Thus, the Utilitarians
study subjective well-being assume that an es- were the intellectual forerunners of subjective
sential ingredient of the good life is that the well-being researchers, focusing on the emo-
person herself likes her life. Subjective well- tional, mental, and physical pleasures and pains
being is defined as a person’s cognitive and af- that individuals experience. Although there are
fective evaluations of his or her life. These eval- other desirable personal characteristics beyond
uations include emotional reactions to events as whether a person is happy, the individual with
well as cognitive judgments of satisfaction and abundant joy has one key ingredient of a good
fulfillment. Thus, subjective well-being is a life.
broad concept that includes experiencing pleas- Early in the 20th century, empirical studies
ant emotions, low levels of negative moods, and of subjective well-being began to take shape. As
high life satisfaction. The positive experiences early as 1925, Flugel studied moods by having
embodied in high subjective well-being are a people record their emotional events and then
core concept of positive psychology because summing emotional reactions across moments.
they make life rewarding. Flugel’s work was the forerunner of modern ex-
perience sampling approaches to measuring
subjective well-being on-line as people go about
History their everyday lives. After World War II, sur-
vey researchers began polling people about their
Throughout history, philosophers and religious happiness and life satisfaction using simple
leaders have suggested that diverse characteris- global survey questionnaires. The pollsters
tics, such as love, wisdom, and nonattachment, studied large numbers of people who were often
are the cardinal elements of a fulfilled existence. selected to produce representative samples of

63
64 PART III. EMOTION-FOCUSED APPROACHES

nations. George Gallup, Gerald Gurin and his think and feel about their lives. People are not
colleagues, and Hadley Cantril pioneered the content to have experts evaluate their lives;
use of large-scale surveys as an assessment they believe that their opinions matter. In ad-
technique. They asked people questions such as dition, the study of subjective well-being flour-
“How happy are you?” with simple response ished because of the growing trend toward in-
options varying from “very happy” to “not dividualism around the globe. Individualists are
very happy.” Recently, Diener (2000a) proposed concerned with their own feelings and beliefs,
that a national index be created in which sub- and thus the study of subjective well-being cor-
jective well-being would be tracked over time. responds well with the Western zeitgeist. Fi-
Although early subjective well-being studies nally, the field increased in popularity because
were characterized by very short scales, many researchers succeeded in developing scientific
important discoveries were made. In 1969, for methods for studying subjective well-being. For
example, Norman Bradburn showed that pleas- these reasons the scientific study of subjective
ant and unpleasant affect are somewhat inde- well-being is now poised to grow into a major
pendent and have different correlates—they are scholarly and applied discipline.
not simply opposites of one another. Thus, the
two affects must be studied separately to gain a
complete picture of individuals’ well-being. This Measurement
finding had important implications for the field
of subjective well-being: It showed that clinical Early survey instruments usually posed a single
psychology’s attempts to eliminate negative question about people’s happiness or life satis-
states would not necessarily foster positive faction. Psychometric evaluations of these sim-
states. The elimination of pain may not result ple scales showed that they possess a degree of
in a corresponding increase in pleasure; ridding validity. For example, Andrews and Withey
the world of sadness and anxiety will not nec- (1976) found that global questions about peo-
essarily make it a happy place. ple’s overall evaluation of their lives yielded
Wilson reviewed the meager amount of re- scores that converged well with one another. As
search on “avowed happiness” in 1967, and the field matured, more multi-item scales ap-
Diener (1984) provided a review of the much peared, with greater reliability and validity than
larger database on subjective well-being that the single-item instruments. Lucas, Diener, and
had accumulated by the mid-1980s. By that Suh (1996) demonstrated that multi-item life
time, the field was becoming a science. Since satisfaction, pleasant affect, and unpleasant af-
Diener’s review was published, a number of fect scales formed factors that were separable
books have appeared on the topic of subjective from each other, as well as from other con-
well-being (e.g., Argyle, 1987; Myers, 1982; structs such as self-esteem. A number of hap-
Strack, Argyle, & Schwarz, 1991), and in 1999, piness, affect, and life satisfaction measures are
Diener, Suh, Lucas, and Smith authored a new now available (see Andrews & Robinson, 1992,
review of the literature in Psychological Bulle- for a review), and we present the five-item Sat-
tin. A handbook volume of chapters related to isfaction With Life Scale (Diener, Emmons, Lar-
hedonic psychology (Kahneman, Diener, & sen, & Griffin, 1985; Pavot & Diener, 1993) in
Schwarz, 1999) and a book dedicated to cross- the appendix.
cultural differences in subjective well-being A major concern of researchers in the field is
(Diener & Suh, 2000) also provide more thor- whether self-report instruments are valid. After
ough reviews of this area. all, people might report that they are happy yet
The scientific discipline of subjective well- not truly experience high subjective well-being.
being grew rapidly. One reason for this is that Sandvik, Diener, and Seidlitz (1993) found that
people in the Western nations have achieved a the self-report measures converge with other
level of material abundance and health that al- types of assessment, including expert ratings
lows them to go beyond mere survival in seek- based on interviews with respondents, experi-
ing the good life. People around the globe are ence sampling measures in which feelings are
entering a “postmaterialistic” world, in which reported at random moments in everyday life,
they are concerned with issues of quality of life participants’ memory for positive versus nega-
beyond economic prosperity. Subjective well- tive events in their lives, the reports of family
being also is popular because it is particularly and friends, and smiling. Despite the positive
democratic—it grants respect to what people psychometric qualities of global subjective well-
CHAPTER 5. SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING 65

being measures, however, we recommend a constructed judgment may not faithfully cor-
multimethod battery to assess subjective well- respond to the average mood or level of satis-
being when this is possible. Additional assess- faction experienced across many different mo-
ment devices based on memory, informant re- ments. Thomas and Diener (1990) found only a
ports, and experience sampling are likely to modest match between people’s reports of mo-
supplement the information obtained from mentary moods and their recall of those moods.
global measures and guard against response ar- Thus, estimates of happiness and reports of af-
tifacts, and in some cases the alternative mea- fect over time are likely to be influenced by a
sures may yield different answers about who is person’s current mood, his or her beliefs about
happiest (e.g., Oishi, 2000). happiness, and the ease of retrieving positive
The use of multiple methods also allows re- and negative information.
searchers to understand how people construct A fascinating picture of subjective well-being
subjective well-being judgments. Schwarz, is emerging in which we can differentiate be-
Strack, and their colleagues, for example, tween a person’s momentary feelings and
showed that situational variables can exert a thoughts about well-being, and larger, more
substantial impact on life satisfaction and mood global constructions. At the momentary level,
reports (Schwarz & Strack, 1999). Schwarz and we can examine people’s reports of moods,
Strack’s findings illustrate that life satisfaction pleasures, pains, and satisfactions recorded on-
judgments are not immutable, stored values line through the experience sampling method.
that are reported when requested. Instead, re- For example, in our laboratory we use palm-
spondents seem to use currently salient infor- sized computers to signal people randomly.
mation to construct life satisfaction judgments. When signaled, respondents complete a survey
Building on this finding, Diener and his col- of their feelings at that moment. Kahneman
leagues (e.g., Diener & Diener, 1995; Suh, (1999) suggested that these types of data offer
Diener, Oishi, & Triandis, 1998) showed that the most accurate estimate of subjective well-
certain information is chronically salient to being because they are less distorted by artifacts
some individuals but not to others (Suh & and biases.
Diener, 1999). Thus, any single piece of infor- Global reports of subjective well-being also
mation may or may not be used by an individ- are valuable, however, because they offer an in-
ual to construct her or his life satisfaction judg- sight into the fascinating psychological proc-
ments. For instance, people in individualistic esses by which people construct global judg-
nations may base their life satisfaction judg- ments about their lives. In global reports of
ments on the extent to which they feel high subjective well-being, we discover how a person
self-esteem, whereas people in collectivistic cul- summarizes her or his life as a whole, and this
tures may base their judgments on the opinions synopsis may only be moderately correlated
of other people (Diener & Diener, 1995). Thus, with on-line reports. For example, we find that
a person may use both situationally induced and people in cultures where subjective well-being
chronically salient information to construct life is valued are more likely to weight their most
satisfaction judgments. positive domains in calculating a global life sat-
People also may use different metastrategies isfaction judgment; people in cultures in which
in seeking the information upon which to base happiness is not an important value are more
their life satisfaction judgments. For example, likely to weight their most negative domains in
some people may search for information about calculating a life satisfaction judgment (Diener,
the positive aspects of their lives, whereas oth- 2000b). If people believe that life satisfaction is
ers might seek information about problematic desirable, they may be more likely to search for
areas (Diener et al., in press). Likewise, people positive information when reporting global life
differ in the degree to which they weigh their satisfaction judgments. Thus, the relation be-
moods and emotions when calculating life sat- tween satisfaction with specific domains such as
isfaction judgments (Suh & Diener, 1999). work and satisfaction with life as a whole is
Thus, life satisfaction reflects different infor- likely to be dependent on people’s beliefs about
mation for different people and can change de- what types of information should be considered
pending on what is salient at the moment. when judging life in its entirety. In a sense,
When participants report on any aspect of then, these are two varieties of happiness and
global subjective well-being, they must con- satisfaction—evaluations of specific aspects of
struct a judgment about their well-being. This life and on-line at-the-moment feelings of well-
66 PART III. EMOTION-FOCUSED APPROACHES

being versus larger, global judgments about Cantor, 1996) emphasized the importance of ac-
one’s happiness and satisfaction. tive participation in life tasks. For instance, Har-
low and Cantor (1996) found that social partic-
ipation was a strong predictor of life satisfaction
Theoretical Approach for retired elders. Sheldon, Ryan, and Reis
(1996) found that people were happiest on days
Many theories of happiness have been proposed when they engaged in activities for intrinsic
since Aristotle’s brilliant insights. These theo- reasons (because of the fun and enjoyment).
ries can be categorized into three groups: (1) Goal researchers (e.g., Emmons, 1986; Little,
need and goal satisfaction theories, (2) process 1989) agree that having important goals and
or activity theories, and (3) genetic and person- pursuing them are reliable indicators of well-
ality predisposition theories. The first constel- being, and therefore goal theories can combine
lation of theories centers around the idea that the elements of tension reduction and pleasur-
the reduction of tensions (e.g., the elimination able activity in explaining subjective well-being.
of pain and the satisfaction of biological and People who have important goals tend to be
psychological needs) leads to happiness. Freud’s more energetic, experience more positive emo-
(1933/1976) pleasure principle and Maslow’s tions, and feel that life is meaningful (e.g.,
(1970) hierarchical needs model represent this McGregor & Little, 1998).
approach. In support of this view, Omodei and Both needs theorists and activity theorists ar-
Wearing (1990) found that the degree to which gue that subjective well-being will change with
individuals’ needs were met was positively as- the conditions in people’s lives. When individ-
sociated with the degree of their life satisfaction. uals are approaching their goals or are engaged
Goal theorists argue that individuals attain in interesting activities, they should experience
subjective well-being when they move toward positive well-being. However, other theorists
an ideal state or accomplish a valued aim (the argue that there is an element of stability in
standard). Other researchers have extended this people’s levels of well-being that cannot be ex-
idea to incorporate the degree of discrepancy plained by the stability in the conditions of peo-
from other potential comparison standards. For ple’s lives. These theorists argue that subjective
example, Michalos (1985) postulated that hap- well-being is strongly influenced by stable per-
piness is inversely related to the degree of dis- sonality dispositions.
crepancy from multiple standards, including Subjective well-being judgments reflect cog-
what one wants, what one has had in the past, nitive and emotional reactions to life circum-
and what relevant others have. Likewise, Hig- stances. Because circumstances can be short-
gins (1987) posited that discrepancies from lived and changeable or relatively stable,
one’s “ideal self” and one’s “ought self” lead to researchers study both momentary and long-
the experiences of negative emotions. Need and term subjective well-being. Not surprisingly,
goal satisfaction theorists argue that the reduc- momentary levels of affect fluctuate quite a bit.
tion of tension and satisfaction of biological and For example, Diener and Larsen (1984) found
psychological needs and goals will cause happi- that when people’s emotions were sampled at
ness. random times throughout the day, a single re-
One implication of tension-reduction theories port of momentary pleasant affect on average
is that happiness occurs after needs are met and correlated only about .10 with pleasant affect in
goals are fulfilled. In other words, happiness is other random moments. People react to chang-
a desired end state toward which all activity is ing circumstances, and these reactions are re-
directed. These theories can be compared with flected in momentary reports of subjective well-
models of happiness in which engagement in an being.
activity itself provides happiness. Most notably, Although it is difficult to predict how happy
Csikszentmihalyi (1975) suggested that people an individual will be at any given moment,
are happiest when they are engaged in interest- when affect is averaged across many occasions,
ing activities that match their level of skill. He stable patterns of individual differences emerge.
called the state of mind that results from this For example, Diener and Larsen (1984) reported
matching of challenges and skill “flow,” and ar- that mean levels of pleasant affect experienced
gued that people who often experience flow tend in work situations correlated .74 with average
to be very happy. Similarly, Cantor and her col- levels of pleasant affect experienced in recrea-
leagues (Cantor & Blanton, 1996; Harlow & tion situations. Similarly, average life satisfac-
CHAPTER 5. SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING 67

tion in social situations correlated .92 with av- tive affect are indistinguishable after controlling
erage life satisfaction when alone. Based on for measurement error. While other personality
these results, it appears that although emotions traits from the Five Factor Model (e.g., agree-
fluctuate, individuals do have characteristic ableness, conscientiousness, and openness to ex-
emotional responses to a variety of situations perience) do correlate with subjective well-
and life circumstances. These characteristic being, these relations are smaller and less
emotional responses are also moderately to consistent (see, e.g., Watson & Clark, 1992).
strongly stable across long periods of time. Thus, it can be said that an extraverted non-
Magnus and Diener (1991) found a correlation neurotic has a head start in achieving happiness,
of .58 between life satisfaction measures as- but that other traits, as well as life circum-
sessed over a 4-year interval. Costa and McCrae stances, matter as well.
(1988) reported substantial stability coefficients Differences in subjective well-being also re-
for affective components of subjective well- sult from stable individual differences in how
being over a period of 6 years. people think about the world. Differences in the
These results have led some theorists to sug- accessibility of pleasant versus unpleasant in-
gest that although life events can influence sub- formation, as well as the accuracy and efficiency
jective well-being, people eventually adapt to with which people process pleasant versus un-
these changes and return to biologically deter- pleasant information influence subjective well-
mined “set points” or “adaptation levels” (e.g., being. Certain people attend to and recall the
Headey & Wearing, 1992). For instance, Diener, pleasant aspects of life more than others. Sim-
Sandvik, Seidlitz, and Diener (1993) found that ilarly, certain cognitive dispositions such as
stability in subjective well-being was compara- hope (Snyder et al., 1991), dispositional opti-
ble among people whose income went up, down, mism (e.g., Scheier & Carver, 1993), and ex-
or stayed the same over 10 years. Similarly, pectancy for control (Grob, Stetsenko, Sabatier,
Costa, McCrae, and Zonderman (1987) reported Botcheva, & Macek, 1999) appear to influence
that people who lived in stable circumstances subjective well-being. It is not just who we are
were no more stable than people who experi- that matters to happiness, but how we think
enced major life changes (e.g., divorce, widow- about our lives.
hood, or job loss).
One reason for the stability and consistency
of subjective well-being is that there is a sub- Current Findings
stantial genetic component to it; to some degree
people are born prone to be happy or unhappy.
Demographic Correlates of Subjective
Tellegen et al. (1988), for example, examined
Well-Being
monozygotic twins who were reared apart and
compared them with dizygotic twins who were The strong association between temperament
reared apart, as well as with monozygotic and and subjective well-being does not mean that
dizygotic twins who were raised together. After events and circumstances are irrelevant to peo-
comparing the similarities of the various types ple’s subjective well-being. In the first major re-
of twins, Tellegen et al. estimated that 40% of view of happiness, Wilson (1967) showed that
the variability in positive emotionality and 55% both personality and demographic factors cor-
of the variability in negative emotionality could relate with subjective well-being. He stated that
be predicted by genetic variation. These esti- the happy person is a “young, healthy, well-
mates allow for environmental influences, but educated, well-paid, extroverted, optimistic,
genes do appear to influence characteristic emo- worry-free, religious, married person with high
tional responses to life circumstances. self-esteem, job morale, modest aspirations, of
When one examines personality influences in either sex and of a wide range of intelligence”
more detail, the traits that are most consistently (p. 294). However. Campbell, Converse, and
linked to subjective well-being are extraversion Rodgers (1976) studied the well-being of Amer-
and neuroticism (Diener & Lucas, 1999). Lucas icans and found that demographic variables such
and Fujita (2000) used meta-analytic and con- as age, income, and education did not account
firmatory factor analytic techniques to show for much variance in reports of well-being,
that extraversion is consistently correlated echoing earlier findings by Bradburn (1969) and
moderately to strongly with pleasant affect; and others. In the past 30 years, researchers syste-
Fujita (1991) found that neuroticism and nega- matically cataloged the various demographic
68 PART III. EMOTION-FOCUSED APPROACHES

correlates of subjective well-being (Diener et al., reported health correlates positively with sub-
1999), with a number of replicable findings jective well-being (e.g., Okun, Stock, & Haring,
emerging: (a) demographic factors such as age, 1984), the correlation is weak when objective
sex, and income are related to subjective well- health ratings are examined (Watten, Vassend,
being; (b) these effects are usually small; and (c) Myhrer, & Syversen, 1997). Subjective well-
most people are moderately happy, and thus, being influences the subjective perception of
demographic factors tend to distinguish between health, and this inflates the correlation between
people who are moderately happy and those subjective well-being and subjective health. It
who are very happy (Diener & Diener, 1996). appears that the way people perceive the world
Income, for example, is consistently related to is much more important to happiness than ob-
subjective well-being in both within-nation jective circumstances.
(e.g., Diener et al., 1993; Haring, Stock, & Other demographic characteristics such as
Okun, 1984) and between-nation analyses (e.g., marital status and religious activity are also
Diener et al., 1993); but at both the individual positively correlated with subjective well-being;
and the national level, income change over time but the effects of marriage can differ for men
has little net effect on subjective well-being and women, and the effects of religious activity
(Diener et al., 1993; Diener & Suh, 1998). Goals may depend on the specific type of religiosity
and expectations must be taken into account to being assessed. Thus, the answer to whether
understand the relation between income and particular demographic factors increase subjec-
subjective well-being; the benefits of a rising in- tive well-being is likely dependent on people’s
come are offset if one’s material desires increase values and goals, personality, and culture.
even faster than wealth.
Age and sex are related to subjective well-
Culture and Subjective Well-Being
being, but these effects are small, too, and de-
pend on the component of subjective well-being In recent years, cultural differences in subjective
being measured. For example, in an inter- well-being (see Diener & Suh, 2000) have been
national sample of 40 nations, Diener and Suh explored, with a realization that there are pro-
(1998) found that although pleasant affect de- found differences in what makes people happy.
clined across age cohorts, life satisfaction and Self-esteem, for example, is less strongly asso-
unpleasant affect showed little change. In two ciated with life satisfaction (Diener & Diener,
separate international samples consisting of ap- 1995), and extraversion is less strongly associ-
proximately 40 nations each, Lucas and Gohm ated with pleasant affect (Lucas, Diener, Grob,
(2000) found that sex differences in subjective Suh, & Shao, 2000) in collectivist cultures than
well-being were small (only about one fifth of in individualist cultures. Similarly, Suh (1999)
a standard deviation difference), with women found that there are cultural differences in the
reporting greater unpleasant and pleasant affect importance of personality congruence. Person-
(though only significant differences in unpleas- ality congruence reflects the extent to which a
ant affect were replicated across both interna- person’s behaviors are consistent across situa-
tional samples). Based on these results, one tions and with the person’s inner feelings. Al-
could not simply say that men are happier than though the importance of personality con-
women or that the young are happier than the gruence is often emphasized in Western
old. The conclusion depends on the component psychology, it is not universally important. Suh
of subjective well-being that is measured. found that collectivists are less congruent than
Diener et al. (1999) argued that if theory in this individualists, and that congruence is less
area is to progress, researchers must study the strongly related to subjective well-being among
separable components of subjective well-being— collectivists. Suh et al. (1998) also found that
“happiness” is not a single thing. among collectivists, the extent to which one’s
Similarly, researchers must be careful about life accords with the wishes of significant others
the conceptualization and measurement of in- is more important than the emotions that the
dependent variables. For example, Wilson person feels in predicting his or her life satis-
(1967) concluded that physical health is corre- faction.
lated with subjective well-being. However, re- By examining between-nation differences in
cent findings qualify this conclusion: The rela- wealth and subjective well-being, researchers
tion depends on whether self-report or objective have arrived at a more complete understanding
ratings of health are assessed. Although self- of the relation between income and happiness.
CHAPTER 5. SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING 69

Some argue that wealth leads to higher subjec- outlook, and working on a healthy personality.
tive well-being only within the poorest nations. Fordyce found that the program produced in-
According to this idea, wealth influences sub- creases in happiness compared with a placebo
jective well-being when basic needs are in dan- control, as well as compared with participants in
ger of not being met. However, Diener, Diener, conditions receiving only partial information. In
and Diener (1995) found that even when levels a follow-up 9 to 28 months after the study, For-
of basic needs were controlled, income had a dyce found that there were lasting effects of his
significant and moderate effect on national sub- intervention.
jective well-being. Thus, people in the wealthi- Seligman, Reivich, Jaycox, and Gillham
est nations tend to be the happiest. This might (1995) performed an experimental study with
be because they possess more material goods, children in which the treatment groups were
but it also could be because the wealthiest exposed to optimism training. Through cogni-
nations experience higher levels of human tive training and social-problem solving, ele-
rights, greater longevity, and more equality. mentary school children who were at risk for
Because demographic variables have different depression were taught to see the bright side of
consequences in different cultures, these corre- events. After the intervention, the treatment
lates can vary in importance. For example, mar- groups were significantly less depressed than
riage is an important demographic correlate of the control group, and this effect grew over the
subjective well-being (Diener, Gohm, Suh, & period of the study’s 2-year follow-up.
Oishi, 2000). However, it is unclear whether the Clearly, more efforts to enhance subjective
benefits of marriage result from the love and well-being are needed, along with rigorous
companionship that accompany long-term rela- methods to evaluate these interventions. For ex-
tionships or from the social approval that mar- ample, more diverse dependent variables and
ried couples receive. Diener, Gohm, Suh, and measuring instruments would be salutary, as
Oishi (2000) found that unmarried individuals well as explorations of which interventions are
who lived together were happier than married or most beneficial, and why. The positive benefits
single individuals in individualist cultures (sug- of the few existing experiments, however, sug-
gesting that in these cultures companionship is gest that programs designed to enhance subjec-
more important than social approval), but un- tive well-being can be quite effective.
married partners who lived together were less
happy than married or single individuals in col-
lectivist cultures (suggesting that in these cul- Future Research
tures social approval is an important benefit of
marriage). Thus, cultural norms can change the In terms of measurement and research methods,
correlates of subjective well-being. many researchers have relied solely on global
retrospective self-reports. A series of construct
validation studies by Diener and colleagues
Interventions (e.g., Lucas et al., 1996; Sandvik et al., 1993)
illustrated that global self-reports have a degree
Interventions to increase subjective well-being of validity. However, it is still unclear to what
are important not only because it feels good to extent individual and cultural differences found
be happy but also because happy people tend to in global reports are accurate reflections of dif-
volunteer more, have more positive work be- ferences in on-line experiences or are manifes-
havior, and exhibit other desirable characteris- tations of processes related to global ways peo-
tics. Because of the roots of the field of subjec- ple see themselves. What is needed is a battery
tive well-being in survey research, few direct of subjective well-being measures based on on-
intervention efforts have been implemented. line experiences, informant reports, biological
However, Fordyce (1977, 1983) published sev- measures, and cognitive measures that assess
eral studies in which he evaluated a program the accessibility of positive events in memory.
designed to boost people’s happiness. The pro- In addition to better measures, we need many
gram is based on the idea that people’s subjec- more longitudinal studies in order to assess
tive well-being can be increased if they learn to variables in a temporal order.
imitate the traits of happy people, characteristics In terms of substantive areas, more attention
such as being organized, keeping busy, spending should be paid to developmental processes in-
more time socializing, developing a positive volving subjective well-being. In particular,
70 PART III. EMOTION-FOCUSED APPROACHES

given recent advances in infant/child tempera- In most ways my life is close to my


ment research (e.g., Goldsmith, 1996; Rothbart ideal
& Ahadi, 1995), the link between positive af- The conditions of my life are excellent
fectivity in infancy and childhood and subjective
I am satisfied with my life
well-being in adulthood should be explored, not
only in terms of stability but also with respect So far I have gotten the important
to the mechanisms that operate in maintaining things I want in life
or changing one’s susceptibility to positive If I could live my life over, I would
stimuli throughout life. Similarly, a longitudi- change almost nothing
nal approach should be taken in an investigation
of society and culture. Specifically, the way in
which changes in macro systems (e.g., political, Scoring and Interpretation of the Scale
economic, and cultural) have an impact on peo- Add up your answers to the five items and use
ple’s well-being should be examined more care- the following normative information to help in
fully to create the happy societies Bentham and “interpretation:”
others envisioned.
In 1949, Henry Murray and Clyde Kluck- 5–9 Extremely dissatisfied with your life
hohn claimed that “Aristotle’s assertion that the
10–14 Very dissatisfied with your life
only rational goal of goals is happiness has
never been successfully refuted as far as we 15–19 Slightly dissatisfied with your life
know, but, as yet no scientist has ventured to 20 About neutral
break ground for a psychology of happiness” 21–25 Somewhat satisfied with your life
(p. 13). As demonstrated in this chapter, scien-
tists have now begun the scientific study of hap- 26–30 Very satisfied with your life
piness. Although the happy person is more 31–35 Extremely satisfied with your life
likely to be from a wealthy nation and have
enough resources to pursue his or her particular Most Americans score in the 21–25 range. A
goals, characteristics such as a positive outlook, score above 25 indicates that you are more sat-
meaningful goals, close social relationships, and isfied than most people. The Satisfaction with
a temperament characterized by low worry are Life Scale (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin,
very important to high subjective well-being. 1985).
We look to the day when effective interventions
based on scientific findings will provide a readily
available way to increase happiness. References

Andrews, F. M., & Robinson, J. P. (1992). Mea-


APPENDIX sures of subjective well-being. In J. P. Robinson,
P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Mea-
Satisfaction with Life Scale sures of personality and social psychological at-
titudes (pp. 61–114). San Diego, CA: Academic
Below are five statements that you may agree Press.
or disagree with. Using the 1–7 scale below in- Andrews, F. M., & Withey, S. B. (1976). Social in-
dicate your agreement with each item by plac- dicators of well-being. New York: Plenum.
ing the appropriate number on the line preced- *Argyle, M. (1987). The psychology of happiness.
ing that item. Please be open and honest in your London: Methuen.
responding. Bentham, J. (1789/1948). Introduction to the prin-
ciples and morals of legislation. London: Uni-
7 Strongly agree versity of London Athlone Press.
6 Agree Bradburn, N. M. (1969). The structure of psycho-
logical well-being. Chicago: Aldine.
5 Slightly agree Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., & Rodgers, W. L.
4 Neither agree nor disagree (1976). The quality of American life. New York:
3 Slightly disagree Russell Sage Foundation.
Cantor, N., & Blanton, H. (1996). Effortful pursuit
2 Disagree of personal goals in daily life. In J. A. Bargh &
1 Strongly disagree P. M. Gollwitzer (Eds.), The psychology of ac-
CHAPTER 5. SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING 71

tion: Linking cognition and motivation to be- Diener, E., Sandvik, E., Seidlitz, L., & Diener, M.
havior (pp. 338–359). New York: Guilford. (1993). The relationship between income and
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1988). Personality subjective well-being: Relative or absolute? So-
in adulthood: A six-year longitudinal study of cial Indicators Research, 28, 195–223.
self-reports and spouse ratings on the NEO Per- Diener, E., & Suh, E. M. (1998). Subjective well-
sonality Inventory. Journal of Personality and being and age: An international analysis. In
Social Psychology, 54, 853–863. K. W. Schaie & M. P. Lawton (Eds.), Annual re-
Costa, P. T., Jr., McCrae, R. R., & Zonderman, A. view of gerontology and geriatrics (Vol. 17,
(1987). Environmental and dispositional influ- pp. 304–324). New York: Springer.
ences on well-being: Longitudinal follow-up of *Diener, E., & Suh, E. M. (Eds.). (2000). Subjective
an American national sample. British Journal of well-being across cultures. Cambridge, MA:
Psychology, 78, 299–306. MIT Press.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and *Diener, E., Suh, E., Lucas, R., & Smith, H.
anxiety. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades
*Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psycho- of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 276–
logical Bulletin, 93, 542–575. 302.
Diener, E. (2000a). Subjective well-being: The sci- Emmons, R. A. (1986). Personal strivings: An ap-
ence of happiness and a proposal for a national proach to personality and subjective well-being.
index. American Psychologist, 55, 34–43. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Diener, E. (2000b, April). Subjective well-being 51, 1058–1068.
across cultures. Paper presented at the annual Flugel, J. C. (1925). A quantitative study of feeling
meeting of Social and Personality Psychologists and emotion in everyday life. British Journal of
Around the Midwest. Psychology, 9, 318–355.
Diener, E., & Diener, C. (1996). Most people are Fordyce, M. W. (1977). Development of a program
happy. Psychological Science, 7, 181–185. to increase personal happiness. Journal of Coun-
Diener, E., & Diener, M. (1995). Cross-cultural seling Psychology, 24, 511–520.
correlates of life satisfaction and self-esteem. Fordyce, M. W. (1983). A program to increase hap-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, piness: Further studies. Journal of Counseling
68, 653–663. Psychology, 30, 483–498.
Diener, E., Diener, M., & Diener, C. (1995). Fac- Freud, S. (1976). New introductory lectures on
tors predicting the subjective well-being of psychoanalysis. In J. Strachey (Ed. & Trans.),
nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- The complete psychological works (Vol. 16).
chology, 69, 851–864. New York: Norton. (Original work published
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffen, 1933).
S. (1985). The Satisfaction With Life Scale. Fujita, F. (1991). An investigation of the relation
Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71– between extraversion, neuroticism, positive af-
75. fect, and negative affect. Unpublished master’s
Diener, E., Gohm, C. L., Suh, E., & Oishi, S. thesis, University of Illinois, Urbana-Cham-
(2000). Similarity of the relations between mar- paign.
ital status and subjective well-being across cul- Goldsmith, H. H. (1996). Studying temperament
tures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 31, via construction of the toddler behavior assess-
419–436. ment questionnaire. Child Development, 67,
Diener, E., & Larsen, R. J. (1984). Temporal sta- 218–235.
bility and cross-situational consistency of affec- Grob, A., Stetsenko, A., Sabatier, C., Botcheva, L.,
tive, behavioral, and cognitive responses. Jour- & Macek, P. (1999). A cross-national model of
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, subjective well-being in adolescence. In F. D. Al-
580–592. saker & A. Flammer (Eds.). (1999). The adoles-
Diener, E., & Lucas, R. E. (1999). Personality and cent experience: European and American ado-
subjective well-being. In D. Kahneman, E. lescents in the 1990s. Research monographs in
Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The adolescence (pp. 115–130). Mahwah, NJ: Erl-
foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 213– baum.
229). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Haring, M. J., Stock, W. A., & Okun, M. A. (1984).
Diener, E., Lucas, R. E., Oishi, S., & Suh, E. M. (in A research synthesis of gender and social class
press). Looking up and looking down: Weight- as correlates of subjective well-being. Human
ing good and bad information in life satisfaction Relations, 37, 645–657.
judgments. Personality and Social Psychology Harlow, R. E., & Cantor, N. (1996). Still partici-
Bulletin. pating after all these years: A study of life task
72 PART III. EMOTION-FOCUSED APPROACHES

participation in later life. Journal of Personality & C. Kluckhohn (Eds.), Personality in nature,
and Social Psychology, 71, 1235–1249. society, and culture (pp. 3–32). New York:
Headey, B., & Wearing. A. (1992). Understanding Knopf.
happiness: A theory of subjective well-being. *Myers, D. G. (1992). The pursuit of happiness:
Melbourne: Longman Cheshire. Who is happy and why. New York: William
Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory Morrow.
relation self and affect. Psychological Review, Oishi, S. (2000). Culture and memory for emo-
94, 319–340. tional experiences: On-line vs. retrospective
Kahneman, D. (1999). Objective happiness. In D. judgments of subjective well-being. Unpub-
Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), lished doctoral dissertation, University of Illi-
Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psy- nois, Urbana-Champaign.
chology (pp. 3–25). New York: Russell Sage Okun, M. A., Stock, W. A., & Haring, M. J. (1984).
Foundation. Health and subjective well-being: A meta-
*Kahneman, D., Diener, E., & Schwarz, N. (Eds.). analysis. International Journal of Aging and
(1999). Well-being: The foundations of hedonic Human Development, 19, 111–132.
psychology. New York: Russell Sage Founda- Omodei, M. M., & Wearing, A. J. (1990). Need
tion. satisfaction and involvement in personal proj-
Little, B. R. (1989). Personal projects analysis: ects: Toward an integrative model of subjective
Trivial pursuits, magnificent obsessions, and the well-being. Journal of Personality and Social
search for coherence. In D. Buss & N. Cantor Psychology, 59, 762–769.
(Eds.), Personality psychology: Recent trends Pavot, W., & Diener, E. (1993). Review of the sat-
and emerging directions (pp. 15–31). New York: isfaction with life scale. Psychological Assess-
Springer-Verlag. ment, 5, 164–172.
Lucas, R. E., Diener, E., Grob, A., Suh, E. M., & Rothbart, M. K., & Ahadi, S. A. (1995). Tempera-
Shao, L. (2000). Cross-cultural evidence for the ment and the development of personality. Jour-
fundamental features of extraversion. Journal of nal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 55–66.
Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 452–468. Sandvik, E., Diener, E., & Seidlitz, L. (1993). Sub-
Lucas, R. E., Diener, E., & Suh, E. (1996). Discri- jective well-being: The convergence and stability
minant validity of well-being measures. Journal of self-report and non-self-report measures.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 616– Journal of Personality, 61, 317–342.
628. Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1993). On the
Lucas, R. E., & Fujita, F. (2000). Factors influencing power of positive thinking: The benefits of being
the relation between extraversion and pleasant optimistic. Current Directions in Psychological
affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Science, 2, 26–30.
chology, 79, 1039–1056. Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. (1999). Reports of sub-
Lucas, R. E., & Gohm, C. (2000). Age and sex dif- jective well-being: Judgmental processes and
ferences in subjective well-being across cultures. their methodological implications. In D. Kahne-
In E. Diener & E. M. Suh (Eds.), Subjective well- man, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-
being across cultures. Cambridge, MA: MIT being: The foundations of hedonic psychology
Press. (pp. 61–84). New York: Russell Sage Foun-
Magnus, K., & Diener, E. (1991, April). A longi- dation.
tudinal analysis of personality, life events, and Seligman, M. E. P., Reivich, K., Jaycox, L., & Gill-
subjective well-being. Paper presented at the ham, J. (1995). The optimistic child. New York:
63rd Annual Meeting of the Midwestern Psy- Harper Perennial.
chological Association, Chicago. Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R., & Reis, H. T. (1996).
Maslow, A. H. (1970). Motivation and personality. What makes for a good day? Competence and
New York: Harper and Row. autonomy in the day and in the person. Person-
McGregor, I., & Little, B. R. (1998). Personal proj- ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 1270–
ects, happiness, and meaning: On doing well and 1279.
being yourself. Journal of Personality and So- Snyder, C. R., Harris, C., Anderson, J. R., Hol-
cial Psychology, 74, 494–512. leran, S. A., Irving, L. M., Sigmon, S. T., Yosh-
Michalos, A. C. (1985). Multiple discrepancies the- inobu, L., Gibb, J., Langelle, C., & Harney, P.
ory (MDT). Social Indicators Research, 16, 347– (1991). The will and the ways: Development and
413. validation of an individual differences measure
Murray, H. A., & Kluckhohn, C. (1949). Outline of hope. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
of a conception of personality. In H. A. Murray chology, 60, 570–585.
CHAPTER 5. SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING 73

*Strack, F., Argyle, M., & Schwarz, N. (Eds.). Thomas, D., & Diener, E. (1990). Memory accu-
(1991). Subjective well-being: An interdiscipli- racy in the recall of emotion. Journal of Person-
nary perspective. Oxford: Pergamon. ality and Social Psychology, 59, 291–297.
Suh, E. M. (1999). Culture, identity, consistency, *Veenhoven, R. (1993). Bibliography of happi-
and subjective well-being. Unpublished doctoral ness: 2472 contemporary studies on subjective
dissertation, University of Illinois, Urbana– appreciation of life. Rotterdam, Netherlands:
Champaign. RISBO.
Suh, E. M., & Diener, E. (1999). The use of emo- *Veenhoven, R. World Database of Happiness
tion information across cultures, individuals Web site, Erasmus University, Rotterdam,
and persons: The case of life satisfaction. Man- Netherlands: http://www.eur.nl/fsw/research/
uscript submitted for publication, University of happiness.
Illinois, Urbana–Champaign. Watten, R. G., Vassend, D., Myhrer, T., & Syver-
Suh, E. M., Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Triandis, H. sen, J. L. (1997). Personality factors and somatic
(1998). The shifting basis of life satisfaction symptoms. European Journal of Personality, 11,
judgments across cultures: Emotions versus 57–68.
norms. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1992). On traits and
chology, 74, 482–493. temperament: General and specific factors of
Tellegen, A., Lykken, D. T., Bouchard. T. J., Wil- emotional experience and their relation to the
cox, K. J., Segal, N. L., and Rich, S. (1988). Per- five-factor model. Journal of Personality, 60,
sonality similarity in twins reared apart and to- 441–476.
gether. Journal of Personality and Social Wilson, W. (1967). Correlates of avowed happi-
Psychology, 54, 1031–1039. ness. Psychological Bulletin, 67, 294–306.
Chapter VII

The Ananda Brahman

T
HE WAY of devotion in the integral synthetic Yoga will
take the form of a seeking after the Divine through love
and delight and a seizing with joy on all the ways of his
being. It will find its acme in a perfect union of love and a perfect
enjoyment of all the ways of the soul’s intimacy with God. It may
start from knowledge or it may start from works, but it will then
turn knowledge into a joy of luminous union with the being of
the Beloved and turn works into a joy of the active union of our
being with the will and the power of being of the Beloved. Or it
may start directly from love and delight; it will then take both
these other things into itself and will develop them as part of the
complete joy of oneness.
The beginning of the heart’s attraction to the Divine may be
impersonal, the touch of an impersonal joy in something univer-
sal or transcendent that has revealed itself directly or indirectly
to our emotional or our aesthetic being or to our capacity of spir-
itual felicity. That which we thus grow aware of is the Ananda
Brahman, the bliss existence. There is an adoration of an imper-
sonal Delight and Beauty, of a pure and an infinite perfection to
which we can give no name or form, a moved attraction of the
soul to some ideal and infinite Presence, Power, existence in the
world or beyond it, which in some way becomes psychologically
or spiritually sensible to us and then more and more intimate
and real. That is the call, the touch of the bliss existence upon
us. Then to have always the joy and nearness of its presence, to
know what it is, so as to satisfy the intellect and the intuitional
mind of its constant reality, to put our passive and, so far as we
can manage it, our active, our inner immortal and even our outer
mortal being into perfect harmony with it, grow into a necessity
of our living. And to open ourselves to it is what we feel to be
the one true happiness, to live into it the sole real perfection.
594 The Yoga of Divine Love

A transcendent Bliss, unimaginable and inexpressible by the


mind and speech, is the nature of the Ineffable. That broods
immanent and secret in the whole universe and in everything in
the universe. Its presence is described as a secret ether of the bliss
of being, of which the Scripture says that, if this were not, none
could for a moment breathe or live. And this spiritual bliss is
here also in our hearts. It is hidden in from the toil of the surface
mind which catches only at weak and flawed translations of
it into various mental, vital and physical forms of the joy of
existence. But if the mind has once grown sufficiently subtle and
pure in its receptions and not limited by the grosser nature of
our outward responses to existence, we can take a reflection of
it which will wear perhaps wholly or predominantly the hue of
whatever is strongest in our nature. It may present itself first as a
yearning for some universal Beauty which we feel in Nature and
man and in all that is around us; or we may have the intuition
of some transcendent Beauty of which all apparent beauty here
is only a symbol. That is how it may come to those in whom
the aesthetic being is developed and insistent and the instincts
which, when they find form of expression, make the poet and
artist, are predominant. Or it may be the sense of a divine spirit
of love or else a helpful and compassionate infinite Presence in
the universe or behind or beyond it which responds to us when
we turn the need of our spirit towards it. So it may first show
itself when the emotional being is intensely developed. It may
come near to us in other ways, but always as a Power or Presence
of delight, beauty, love or peace which touches the mind, but is
beyond the forms these things take ordinarily in the mind.
For all joy, beauty, love, peace, delight are outflowings from
the Ananda Brahman, — all delight of the spirit, the intellect,
the imagination, aesthetic sense, ethical aspiration and satisfac-
tion, action, life, the body. And through all ways of our being
the Divine can touch us and make use of them to awaken and
liberate the spirit. But to reach the Ananda Brahman in itself the
mental reception of it must be subtilised, spiritualised, univer-
salised, discharged of everything that is turbid and limiting. For
when we draw quite near or enter into it, it is by an awakened
The Ananda Brahman 595

spiritual sense of a transcendent and a universal Delight which


exists within and yet behind and beyond the contradictions
of the world and to which we can unite ourselves through a
growing universal and spiritual or a transcendental ecstasy.
Ordinarily, the mind is satisfied with reflecting this Infinity
we perceive or with feeling the sense of it within and with-
out us, as an experience which, however frequent, yet remains
exceptional. It seems in itself so satisfying and wonderful when
it comes and our ordinary mind and the active life which we
have to lead may seem to us so incompatible with it, that we
may think it excessive to expect anything more. But the very
spirit of Yoga is this, to make the exceptional normal, and to
turn that which is above us and greater than our normal selves
into our own constant consciousness. Therefore we should not
hesitate to open ourselves more steadily to whatever experience
of the Infinite we have, to purify and intensify it, to make it our
object of constant thought and contemplation, till it becomes
the originating power that acts in us, the Godhead we adore
and embrace, our whole being is put into tune with it and it is
made the very self of our being.
Our experience of it has to be purified of any mental alloy
in it, otherwise it departs, we cannot hold it. And part of this
purification is that it shall cease to be dependent on any cause
or exciting condition of mind; it must become its own cause
and self-existent, source of all other delight, which will exist
only by it, and not attached to any cosmic or other image or
symbol through which we first came into contact with it. Our
experience of it has to be constantly intensified and made more
concentrated; otherwise we shall only reflect it in the mirror
of the imperfect mind and not reach that point of uplifting and
transfiguration by which we are carried beyond the mind into the
ineffable bliss. Object of our constant thought and contempla-
tion, it will turn all that is into itself, reveal itself as the universal
Ananda Brahman and make all existence its outpouring. If we
wait upon it for the inspiration of all our inner and our outer
acts, it will become the joy of the Divine pouring itself through
us in light and love and power on life and all that lives. Sought
596 The Yoga of Divine Love

by the adoration and love of the soul, it reveals itself as the


Godhead, we see in it the face of God and know the bliss of
our Lover. Tuning our whole being to it, we grow into a happy
perfection of likeness to it, a human rendering of the divine
nature. And when it becomes in every way the self of our self,
we are fulfilled in being and we bear the plenitude.
Brahman always reveals himself to us in three ways, within
ourselves, above our plane, around us in the universe. Within
us, there are two centres of the Purusha, the inner Soul through
which he touches us to our awakening; there is the Purusha in
the lotus of the heart which opens upward all our powers and the
Purusha in the thousand-petalled lotus whence descend through
the thought and will, opening the third eye in us, the lightnings
of vision and the fire of the divine energy. The bliss existence
may come to us through either one of these centres. When the
lotus of the heart breaks open, we feel a divine joy, love and
peace expanding in us like a flower of light which irradiates the
whole being. They can then unite themselves with their secret
source, the Divine in our hearts, and adore him as in a temple;
they can flow upwards to take possession of the thought and
the will and break out upward towards the Transcendent; they
stream out in thought and feeling and act towards all that is
around us. But so long as our normal being offers any obstacle
or is not wholly moulded into a response to this divine influence
or an instrument of this divine possession, the experience will
be intermittent and we may fall back constantly into our old
mortal heart; but by repetition, abhyāsa, or by the force of
our desire and adoration of the Divine, it will be progressively
remoulded until this abnormal experience becomes our natural
consciousness.
When the other upper lotus opens, the whole mind becomes
full of a divine light, joy and power, behind which is the Divine,
the Lord of our being on his throne with our soul beside him
or drawn inward into his rays; all the thought and will become
then a luminosity, power and ecstasy; in communication with the
Transcendent, this can pour down towards our mortal members
and flow by them outwards on the world. In this dawn too there
The Ananda Brahman 597

are, as the Vedic mystics knew, our alternations of its day and
night, our exiles from the light; but as we grow in the power
to hold this new existence, we become able to look long on the
sun from which this irradiation proceeds and in our inner being
we can grow one body with it. Sometimes the rapidity of this
change depends on the strength of our longing for the Divine
thus revealed, and on the intensity of our force of seeking; but at
others it proceeds rather by a passive surrender to the rhythms
of his all-wise working which acts always by its own at first
inscrutable method. But the latter becomes the foundation when
our love and trust are complete and our whole being lies in the
clasp of a Power that is perfect love and wisdom.
The Divine reveals himself in the world around us when we
look upon that with a spiritual desire of delight that seeks him
in all things. There is often a sudden opening by which the veil
of forms is itself turned into a revelation. A universal spiritual
Presence, a universal peace, a universal infinite Delight has man-
ifested, immanent, embracing, all-penetrating. This Presence by
our love of it, our delight in it, our constant thought of it returns
and grows upon us; it becomes the thing that we see and all else
is only its habitation, form and symbol. Even all that is most
outward, the body, the form, the sound, whatever our senses
seize, are seen as this Presence; they cease to be physical and are
changed into a substance of spirit. This transformation means
a transformation of our own inner consciousness; we are taken
by the surrounding Presence into itself and we become part of
it. Our own mind, life, body become to us only its habitation
and temple, a form of its working and an instrument of its
self-expression. All is only soul and body of this delight.
This is the Divine seen around us and on our own physical
plane. But he may reveal himself above. We see or feel him as
a high-uplifted Presence, a great infinite of Ananda above us,
— or in it, our Father in heaven, — and do not feel or see him
in ourselves or around us. So long as we keep this vision, the
mortality in us is quelled by that Immortality; it feels the light,
power and joy and responds to it according to its capacity;
or it feels the descent of the spirit and it is then for a time
598 The Yoga of Divine Love

transformed or else uplifted into some lustre of reflection of the


light and power; it becomes a vessel of the Ananda. But at other
times it lapses into the old mortality and exists or works dully or
pettily in the ruck of its earthly habits. The complete redemption
comes by the descent of the divine Power into the human mind
and body and the remoulding of their inner life into the divine
image, — what the Vedic seers called the birth of the Son by
the sacrifice. It is in fact by a continual sacrifice or offering, a
sacrifice of adoration and aspiration, of works, of thought and
knowledge, of the mounting flame of the Godward will that we
build ourselves into the being of this Infinite.
When we possess firmly this consciousness of the Ananda
Brahman in all of these three manifestations, above, within,
around, we have the full oneness of it and embrace all existences
in its delight, peace, joy and love; then all the worlds become
the body of this self. But we have not the richest knowledge of
this Ananda if it is only an impersonal presence, largeness or
immanence that we feel, if our adoration has not been intimate
enough for this Being to reveal to us out of its wide-extended joy
the face and body and make us feel the hands of the Friend and
Lover. Its impersonality is the blissful greatness of the Brahman,
but from that can look out upon us the sweetness and intimate
control of the divine Personality. For Ananda is the presence of
the Self and Master of our being and the stream of its outflowing
can be the pure joy of his Lila.
Journal of Social Change
2017, Volume 9, Issue 1, Pages 4–31
©Walden University, LLC, Minneapolis, MN
DOI: 10.5590/JOSC.2017.09.1.02

Happiness Index Methodology


Laura Musikanski
Happiness Alliance

Scott Cloutier
Arizona State University

Erica Bejarano
Arizona State University

Davi Briggs
Arizona State University

Julia Colbert
Arizona State University

Gracie Strasser
Arizona State University

Steven Russell
Arizona State University

The Happiness Index is a comprehensive survey instrument that assesses happiness, well-
being, and aspects of sustainability and resilience. The Happiness Alliance developed the
Happiness Index to provide a survey instrument to community organizers, researchers, and
others seeking to use a subjective well-being index and data. It is the only instrument of its
kind freely available worldwide and translated into over ten languages. This instrument can
be used to measure satisfaction with life and the conditions of life. It can also be used to
define income inequality, trust in government, sense of community and other aspects of well-
being within specific demographics of a population. This manuscript documents the
development the Happiness Index between 2011 and 2015, and includes suggestions for
implementation.

Keywords: survey, questionnaire, subjective well-being indicators, sustainability indicators,


happiness, well-being, quality of life, beyond GDP, community indicators

Please address queries to: Laura Musikanski, Happiness Alliance. Email: laura@happycounts.org
Musikanski et al., 2017

Introduction
The Happiness Alliance is a nonprofit organization envisioning a world where all beings can thrive,
founded on a mission to improve societal well-being by increasing public understanding and
appreciation of factors that lead to life satisfaction, resilience, and sustainability. The mission of the
organization is based on evidence that when people and society implement happiness measures then
the value of individual happiness, the well-being of others, and concern for the sustainability of the
planet increase (Cloutier, Jambeck, & Scott, 2014; Pfeiffer & Cloutier, 2016; Zidanšek, 2007). The
increase is reflected via changed mindsets, decisions, and behaviors that improve personal and
community happiness, ecological sustainability, and public policy (Frey & Stutzer, 2011). The
Happiness Alliance’s mission is implemented through: (1) the provision of social engagement tools
and resources; (2) the sharing of general knowledge of economic, social, environmental, and
governance indicators; and (3) the support for grassroots activism. Although the Happiness Alliance
initially used the 2010 Greater Victoria Well-Being Survey as its evaluative tool, in 2011, the
organization developed its own Happiness Index (Victoria Foundation, 2010; Happiness Alliance,
2014b), modeled after the Kingdom of Bhutan’s Gross National Happiness Index (GNHI)—an
alternative measure of progress to the frequently used gross domestic product (GDP).

The Concept of Gross National Happiness


The concept of gross national happiness (GNH) was developed of by the Kingdom of Bhutan in
response to pressures to use Gross National Product as the primary goal and metric for the
government (Ura et. al, 2012). In Bhutan, GNH is measured using the GNHI. The Bhutanese Gross
National Happiness Commission (n.d.), composed of the Prime Minister, Secretaries of all ministries
as well as other high level officials, has the mandate to “ensure all development policies and plans
are formulated and implemented in line with the principles of GNH” (p. 5). In 2015, the government
issued its third GNH report, Compass Towards a Just and Harmonious Society (The Center for
Bhutan Studies & GNH Research, 2015), finding that aspects of well-being that had improved
included mental well-being, physical health, youth literacy, participation in community events
countered by increases in working hours, satisfaction with government performance, and sense of
belonging. The sense in Bhutan is that the country’s development has been “comparatively
successful. Partly…due to the concept of GNH…” (p. 42).

The Concept of the Happiness Index


The Happiness Alliance took inspiration from Bhutan and followed suit. Between 2011 and 2016, the
Happiness Alliance’s survey instrument was called the Happiness Index and the GNHI
interchangeably. It was issued with a Creative Commons Attribution noncommercial
(https://creativecommons.org/licenses/) license; meaning users could use it for any noncommercial
purposes as long as they credit the Happiness Alliance.

Domains of Happiness
The Happiness Index measures life satisfaction, the feeling of happiness, and other happiness
domains: psychological well-being, health, time balance, community, social support, education, arts
and culture, environment, governance, material well-being, and work (Happiness Alliance, 2014c).
The qualities measured in the domains are as follows:

Journal of Social Change 5


Musikanski et al., 2017

● Psychological Well-Being: optimism, senses of purpose and of accomplishment;


● Health: energy level and ability to perform everyday activities;
● Time Balance: enjoyment, feeling rushed, and sense of leisure;
● Community: sense of belonging, volunteerism, and sense of safety;
● Social Support: satisfaction with friends and family, feeling loved, and feeling lonely;
● Education, Arts, and Culture: access to cultural and educational events and
diversity;
● Environment: access to nature, pollution, and conservation;
● Governance: trust in government, sense of corruption, and competency;
● Material Well-Being: financial security and meeting basic needs; and
● Work: compensation, autonomy, and productivity. (Happiness Alliance, 2014c)

Intended Uses of the Happiness Index


The Happiness Index is a tool for the use of researchers, community organizers and policy makers
seeking to understand and enhance individual happiness, community well-being, social justice,
economic equality, and environmental sustainability. The index was formed with the intent to
promote social change by making the survey instrument and data freely available to community
organizers, educators, researchers, students, organizations, government, and more. The index is
unique in that it is the only widely comprehensive index available for free online that survey takers
to access their own scores in comparison the entire data set, while also allowing users to customize
the tool for a target population, add their own questions to the survey instrument, and readily access
data for their own sample as well as comparison data to the entire data set.

Basis for Practical Utility


The aims of this resource are to (a) explain how the Happiness Index was created and refined in
iterative rounds, and (b) describe how the Happiness Index can contribute to a new paradigm of
sustainability, social justice, and happiness. A robust statistical analysis of the Happiness Index over
time is in consideration for a future paper. However, with over 200 applications of the index to
groups to date, and over half a decade of development, it is reasonable to deem that the index has
passed the rigor of practical utility. The Happiness Alliance has worked with many of these
practitioners and academics to apply the index since 2011, and has integrated feedback to improve
the tool through iterations. Users of the survey instrument, including scholars, policy makers and
community organizers, generally agree that the index passes face validity and is effective in
measuring happiness in communities and for groups, as intended.

Literature Review
Historically, governments have used GDP as a primary indicator of national well-being and growth
(Ovaska & Takashima, 2006). This is an incomplete strategy due to the disconnect between GDP and
personal income, and the gap between personal income and happiness. Not only can a country’s GDP
grow while per capita income declines for the vast majority of a population (Layard, 2005) but
personal income is only one factor of personal happiness and well-being (Diener, Tay & Oishi, 2013).
Other happiness factors include, but are not limited to: personal relationships, economic freedom,
political freedom, health, education, and income distribution (Ovaska & Takashima, 2006); although
these factors vary between individuals and across cultures. Several countries have started to
measure happiness in addition to or in lieu of GDP (Musikanski & Polley, 2016).

Journal of Social Change 6


Musikanski et al., 2017

Happiness and the Economy


Generally, GDP cannot distinguish when economic activities have a positive or negative impact on
well-being (Ovaska & Takashima, 2006). For example, GDP does not take into account the hidden
costs of economic development, such as inflation and unemployment, and an overemphasis of GDP
diminishes the value of important well-being factors such as natural capital, knowledge, health, and
social capital (Frank, 1997; Ovaska & Takashima, 2006). Measuring happiness, therefore, should not
only consider observable objective well-being measures (e.g., health and socioeconomic status), but
also subjective well-being measures, such as domain satisfaction and quality of life. Easterlin (1974,
1995, 2001) argues that in the long-term monetary gains have relatively small effects on quality of
life.

Finally, as people may be the best judges of their own happiness, subjective well-being is measured
by asking individuals to assess their happiness by answering a single question or a multiple-item
survey (Frey & Luechinger, 2007; Diener & Pavot, 1993). The Happiness Alliance suggests that a
more holistic way to measure progress for governments and communities is by using a nonmonetary,
multidimensional approach that measures satisfaction and advancements across various life
domains, including economy, governance, environment, community, social support, culture, learning,
health, time-balance, and work. The Happiness Alliance considers the Happiness Index a holistic
measure, similar to alternative measures of progress employed by the Happy Planet Index and
GNHI (Marks, Abdallah, Simms, & Thompson, 2006; Ura, Alkire, Zangmo, & Wangdi, 2012).

The Happiness Index Development


While the use of a happiness metric is, in itself, a means to influence social change, the validity and
reliability of the survey and collected data is equally important. After working in communities, with
researchers, policy makers and others, the Happiness Alliance decided to publish this article as an
explanation of the development processes behind the Happiness Index. The goal is to inform users of
the index on the validity and reliability of the instrument and data collected to date. It is worth
reiterating here that the Happiness Alliance intends to undertake more robust statistical analyses in
future studies. But, the Happiness Alliance contends that the index, as it stands currently, has
established face validity through a cocreative iterative development process, spanning several years
of research by multiple academic and professional institutions.

The development process of the Happiness Index has been guided by the Happiness Alliance’s goals
of supporting positive social change, the quest for social justice, enhanced individual and societal
happiness and a sustainable future as well as governmental adoption of wider measures of well-
being to guide public policy. Modifications to the Happiness Index have been undertaken through an
organic and responsive process working with communities and community organizers since 2010.
The survey instrument is evolving, with core questions that do not change, much like the European
Social Survey (2006) and other instruments. The index is intended to be easily administered, quickly
completed by survey takers, while validly reflecting the main constructs that influence happiness. It
is also intended to produce data that community organizers and researchers can use to contribute to
a transformation of the economic and social paradigm. Examples of how researchers and community
organizers have used the survey instrument to gather data, analyzed and reported the data, and
used it to educate the public and inform policy can be seen on the Organization for Economic
Cooperation and Development’s Wikiprogress page dedicated to the Happiness Alliance
(http://wikiprogress.org/data/organization/happiness-alliance).

Journal of Social Change 7


Musikanski et al., 2017

Development Overview
The Happiness Alliance hired the Personality and Well-Being Lab at San Francisco State University
to develop the Happiness Index in 2011, which resulted in Round 1 of the instrument. The
subsequent rounds were undertaken by staff and advisors to the Happiness Alliance. The following
description of the development phases of the survey instrument concisely explains the process of the
survey and index development to date.

The development procedures are described so survey instrument users understand the history of
data collected by the Happiness Alliance and can use the information when analyzing data. The
information may serve researchers seeking to understand the historical context of their own data
collection and to compare their data to other collected by the Happiness Index and by other survey
instruments. The description of the development procedures also ensures the reliability and validity
of the survey instrument.

Data Sources
The majority of the data collected by the Happiness Index are from convenience sampling that began
in January of 2011. A small portion of the data (less than 5%) was collected through random
samples. Most survey takers learned of the Happiness Index through word of mouth, media, or from
community organizers who took trainings from the Happiness Alliance on how to use the survey
instrument in their community, company, city, or state. The majority of survey takers come from the
United States (over 70%), with the balance from other countries. Over 85% of surveys are taken in
English, with the rest in certified translations of the survey instrument in Spanish, Chinese,
Japanese, Korean, French, Brazilian Portuguese, Italian, and other languages. Between 2011 and
2016, over 65,000 responses had been gathered at a rate of about 1,000–1,500 responses per month.

Survey Development Rounds


The survey instrument was developed in four distinct rounds (see Figure 1).

Rounds of Development
Round 1 of the Happiness Index assessed the relevance of the Bhutanese kingdom GNHI domains to
happiness, well-being, and quality of life. The GNHI domains include psychological well-being,
health, community vitality, living standard, governance, environment diversity, culture, education,
and time use (Ura et al., 2012). Round 2 focused on decreasing the time required to complete the
survey. Rounds 3 and 4 both focused on standardizing subjective well-being indicators and reducing
the survey length and time of completion.

Round 1: Survey Development


Round 1 of the survey was completed in five distinct phases (see Figure 2).

Journal of Social Change 8


Musikanski et al., 2017

Figure 1. Happiness Index Development in Four Rounds Flowchart

Journal of Social Change 9


Musikanski et al., 2017

Figure 2. Happiness Index Round 1 Flowchart

Round 1, Phase 1. In Round 1, Phase 1, a questionnaire was arranged corresponding to the domains
of the GNHI developed by Bhutan, with several domains added. It contained the following domains
in order: (a) Satisfaction With Life; (b) Positive and Negative Experiences; (c) Domain Satisfaction;
(d) Psychological Well-Being; (e) Health; (f) Time Balance; (g) Community Vitality; (h) Social
Support; (i) Access to Education, Arts, and Culture; (j) Your Neighborhood; (k) Environmental
Quality; (l) Governance; (m) Material Well-Being; and (n) Work, followed by a section on
demographics (Happiness Alliance, 2011).

Round 1, Phase 1 resulted in an hour-long online survey. The survey was distributed to
approximately 10,000 people via existing email lists gathered by Sustainable Seattle, a national
nonprofit internationally renowned for creating the first regional sustainability indicators, and Take
Back Your Time, a national organization that raises awareness on the issue of overworking (Holden,
2006; Take Back Your Time, n.d.). A total of 515 individuals completed the survey. At the end of the
survey, participants were asked to provide any additional comments regarding survey improvement.

Journal of Social Change 10


Musikanski et al., 2017

Round 1, Phase 2. Round 1, Phase 2 consisted of a factor analysis, corrected item-total correlation
analysis, reliability analyses, convergent correlation analysis, and analysis of participant feedback to
determine the questions that best fit each domain. (The Personality and Well-Being Lab at San
Francisco State University did not provide the results of the analysis to the Happiness Alliance, and
hence this data is not included in this resource. They did, however, provide assurance of validity
determined by their own analysis.) Results of Round 1, Phase 2 yielded 15 domains. After this phase,
the time it took to take the survey was reduced from approximately 1 hr to approximately 30 min.

Round 1, Phase 3. Next, Round 1, Phase 3 was initiated. During this phase, the refined survey was
shared through Amazon.com’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk) website. MTurk participants were paid a
nominal fee for their participation. The objective of Round 1, Phase 3 was to reduce the number of
items for the domains from approximately 14 to eight or less with the criteria that the items would
maximize the internal consistency and predictive validity of the Happiness Index survey while also
providing a breadth of coverage. The only sections that were not reduced during this phase were
Positive and Negative Experience, Domain Satisfaction, and Your Neighborhood. To better test for
convergent validity, the survey included a question asking each participant to indicate their level of
satisfaction with the operational definitions of the 10 Happiness Index domains. As a result of Round
1, Phase 3, the number of items for each domain was reduced to an average of eight and the survey
time decreased from approximately 30 min to approximately 15 min.

Round 1, Phase 4. Round 1, Phase 4 was subsequently implemented to test the new survey version.
The survey was reposted on Mturk and the original survey takers were excluded from participation.
Phase 4 included one additional modification: The response scales became 5-point Likert scales for
all domains except the first two, which were scaled between 0 and 10. The domains were then
assessed by the Personality and Well-Being Lab at San Francisco State University to ensure that (a)
they formed a single factor using factor analysis, (b) they were internally consistent with their alpha
coefficients being greater than .70, and (c) the sum of the scale significantly (p < .05) correlated with
the satisfaction rating at the beginning of the survey it was intended to predict.

Round 1, Phase 5. Finally, in Round 1, Phase 5, a nationally representative sample of adults was
recruited through SurveyMonkey.com. During this phase 578 participants completed the survey.
Subsequently, a factor analysis of variance, internal consistency of data, and correlation with the
satisfaction rating confirmed the validity of the survey.

Round 2: Domain and Question Reduction


As with Round 1, Phase 3, the purpose of Round 2 was to reduce the time it took to take the survey.
The value that guided the shortening of the survey instrument was usefulness to community
organizers and professors administering the survey instrument for courses. Early conversations with
professional pollsters from Gallup (J. de Graff, personal communication, November 2, 2010) and
other polling companies (A. Davis, personal communication, November 11, 2011) confirmed that a
survey longer than 12 min to administer rendered the instrument overwhelming to most survey
takers. An aspect of usefulness considered was the applicability of the survey for random polling, in
which professional pollsters strongly advised the Happiness Alliance to ensure the survey
instrument took less than 12 min to complete or about 60 questions, not including demographic
questions.

Round 2: Domain and Question Reduction


During Round 2, questions and domains were eliminated: four questions from the Access to
Education, Arts, and Culture section after a correlation analysis, and the two domains of Your
Neighborhood and Domain Satisfaction. The domains of Your Neighborhood and Domain

Journal of Social Change 11


Musikanski et al., 2017

Satisfaction had initially been included to verify that data gathered by convenience sampling
correlated to well-being. The Happiness Alliance conducted a statistical analysis of this correlation
during this phase and deemed that it was no longer necessary to gather the data from these
domains. Furthermore, survey participant feedback suggested that the data collected within these
domains does not necessarily bolster the survey’s intended purpose.

The second domain, Positive and Negative Experiences, was reduced from 12 to four questions. In
the end, Round 2 of the survey contained the same domains as Round 1, in the same order, with the
exclusion of Domain Satisfaction and Your Neighborhood. Round 2 resulted in the following domains
in order: (a) Satisfaction With Life; (b) Positive and Negative Experiences; (c) Psychological Well-
Being; (d) Health; (e) Time Balance; (f) Community Vitality; (g) Social Support; (h) Access to
Education, Arts, and Culture; (i) Environmental Quality; (j) Governance; (k) Material Well-Being; (l)
Work; and (m) Demographics.

Round 3: Survey Standardization


The values that guided Round 3 were to foster standardization among subjective well-being
indicators and to further shorten survey time. The primary benefit of standardization among
indicators is that it allows comparability across geographic, demographics and time. While the
harmonization of subjective well-being indicators is important, whereby each population develops its
own indicators tailored to its own unique circumstances, the standardization of subjective well-being
indicators, in which each population shares a common set of indicators, is central to progress
towards the adoption of wider indicators of well-being by national and local governments seeking to
protect and secure people’s right to life, liberty and pursuit of happiness.

In Round 3, the second domain, Positive and Negative Experience, was replaced by questions from
the U.K. Office for National Statistics. This replacement was made in an effort to contribute to
progress in the happiness movement and to help standardize subjective well-being indicators.
Questions about fiscal and monetary policy, social policy, political orientation, household net wealth,
late payments to creditors, and methods for meeting unplanned expenses were eliminated from the
demographic section. The domains within the survey did not change.

Round 4: Survey Honing


The purpose of Round 4 was to eliminate unnecessary questions and shorten the survey. Questions
were deemed unnecessary when they yielded the same statistical analysis as another question,
thereby not providing new information. Correlation analyses were conducted by staff and board
directors of the Happiness Alliance using data gathered through a convenience sampling conducted
between November 2011, and July 2014. Over 95% of the sampling was conducted online and
participants learned about the survey through media, word of mouth, and from grassroots activists
who had taken the trainings conducted by the Happiness Alliance and used the Happiness Index in
their work. More than 85% of the survey takers were from the United States, with the remainder
from around the world. The survey had been translated into Spanish, Chinese Traditional, Chinese
Simple, Vietnamese, Filipino (Tagalog), Romanian, Oromo, and Somali. All of the data were collated
in one database.

In total, 14 questions were eliminated resulting in a survey with 50 domain questions and 15
demographic questions, a total of 65 questions (see Appendices A and B). Additionally, in Round 4,
sections were renamed and section numbers revised so as to no longer contain references to
eliminated sections. Round 4 resulted in the following domains in the following order: (a) Cantril
Ladder; (b) Satisfaction With Life; (c) Psychological Well-Being; (d) Health; (e) Time Balance; (f)

Journal of Social Change 12


Musikanski et al., 2017

Lifelong Learning, Arts, and Culture; (g) Community; (h) Social Support; (i) Environment; (j)
Governance; (k) Standard of Living/Economy; (l) Work; and (m) Demographics.

Cantril Ladder
The Happiness Index draws on well-known reliable and valid scales like the Cantril Ladder to assess
domains of happiness. The Cantril Ladder, also known as Cantril’s Self-Anchoring Ladder of Life
Satisfaction, measures life satisfaction by asking participants to imagine their ideal life and hopes
for the future as well as the worst future scenario of their lives and fears associated with that future.
They are then are presented with a series of pictures of a ladder and mark where they see
themselves in the past, present, and future (Cantril, 1965; Levin & Currie, 2014). The Cantril’s
Ladder method is frequently applied in the literature on well-being and is often used to test the
validity of other measures (Cramm, Starting, de Vreede, Steverink, & Nieboer, 2012; Oliver, Huxley,
Priebe & Kaiser, 1997). It has also been integrated widely into questionnaires and composite indexes
across disciplines (Jaarsma et al., 1999; Kahneman & Deaton, 2010; Levin & Currie, 2014;
Svindseth, Nøttestad, & Dahl, 2013; Szramka-Pawlak et al., 2014).

Providing a Comprehensive Survey Instrument


Through Rounds 2–4, the survey was shortened in an attempt to improve survey completion rates,
while also maintaining survey validity and reliability. Round 4 resulted in a survey instrument that
is shorter in length than the other rounds and offers a more precise measure for each domain of well-
being. Round 4 of the survey instrument provides continuity in the comprehensive measuring of
well-being as encompassed by all the domains.

Because the Happiness Index is primarily used for convenience sampling, and data is collected
without compensation, it is paramount that the survey be as attractive and easy to use for volunteer
participants as possible. Likewise, due to the fact that policy makers, community organizers,
academics, consultants, therapists, and others use data gathered through the Happiness Index, it is
vital that the questions measure well-being as accurately as possible. The refinement process
undertaken in rounds allowed the Happiness Alliance to balance brevity and precision within a
comprehensive survey instrument: the Happiness Index.

Suggestions for Implementing the Happiness Index


The Happiness Index was created as a tool for organizations, researchers, and social actors. Through
the refinement process and the use of the survey for groups by community organizers, researchers
and others since 2011, the Happiness Alliance has identified a number of functions the survey and
its data can serve: group assessment, individual assessment, identification of vulnerability in
populations, fundraising, policy and program guidance, resource allocation, awareness raising,
education and outreach, life-skill development, academic research, community engagement, and
program or project evaluation, among other functions.

Assessing and Using the Happiness Index


The Happiness Index can be readily accessed online and users can create a group for administering
the survey online (http://www.happycounts.org/how-happy-are-you-take-the-gross-national-
happiness-index-survey.html). When using the Happiness Index for a group, it is most facilely
distributed via online, such as web pages, social media cites, newsletters, memos, briefings, or other
media. In some cases, the survey is distributed in a paper form as survey takers do not have access
to the Internet. The Happiness Alliance provides a downloadable file for printing for these cases

Journal of Social Change 13


Musikanski et al., 2017

(http://www.happycounts.org/for-researchers.html). Guides, videos, presentations, and examples


based on prior use of the survey instrument by community organizers, researchers, media, nonprofits
and others are provided freely online by the Happiness Alliance (http://www.happycounts.org/happy-
community-toolkit.html).

Measuring for Positive Social Change


The Happiness Index can be used to measure: the impact of social injustice, climate injustice, income
inequality, disengagement from the democratic process, loneliness, isolation, ill health, and other
aspects of human suffering within a population or within specific demographics of a population. The
data gathered by the index provides measurement of often-excluded concepts such as social justice,
where objective data does not always provide a complete picture. Moreover, analyses of varying
demographic aspects of a population often reveal unexpected information about who is suffering and
who is thriving within a population. The survey can be used, and has been used by communities to
measure gaps in well-being according to race, income level, zip code, education, age, gender, and
household characteristics. Finally, summarized data and recommendations can be, and has been,
shared with policy makers, and used to inform public conversations and policy makers’ decisions
about budgeting.

Measuring for Education


In an educational setting, the Happiness Index can contribute to a variety of learning activities. For
example, lecturers can invite their audiences to take the survey before or after an event. If the
questionnaire is used before an event, the scores can be incorporated into a lecture or informal talk
and used for an interactive session with the audience. Explaining the relationship of the domains of
happiness to individual happiness can provide a personalized way to understand the concepts of
sustainability, well-being, quality of life, and happiness. The Happiness Index can also contribute to
class assignments and research projects and act as a qualitative methods learning tool. The
Happiness Alliance provides full data sets to students and researchers who agree to the protection of
personal data policy (however, emails and profile information are never shared). The survey has
undergone institutional review board reviews at several universities, and was included within course
curricula. It has been administered for a random sample on a campus by a student class learning the
concepts of survey development, random sampling, and data analysis. It has been used in classes as
extra-credit assignments where an institutional review board review process was not performed.
Finally, the data collected since 2011 has been used for analysis in master’s theses and doctoral
dissertations.

Conclusion
The Happiness Alliance’s Happiness Index has been used by over 200 groups since 2011. It was
developed out of a growing interest to (a) understand and evaluate personal and community well-
being as it relates to happiness, sustainability, and quality of life; (b) address the need for, and lack
of, existing freely available valid survey-based well-being measurement tools for community
organizers, researchers, and others; (c) shape personal, community, organizational, and societal
strategies and policies in a way that enhances the connected concepts of happiness, well-being,
quality of life, and sustainability; and, ultimately, (d) the adoption of wider measures of well-being
by governments protecting and securing people’s right to life, liberty, and pursuit of happiness. The
survey was developed over a series of four rounds, each of which sought to streamline and clarify the
measurement instrument. Based on experience gathered by the use of the Happiness Index for
groups since 2011, the Happiness Index has and can serve as a valuable tool for advancing
happiness, well-being, quality of life, sustainability, and social change. Researchers, teachers,

Journal of Social Change 14


Musikanski et al., 2017

students, policy makers, community organizers, managers and business leaders, therapists and
consultants, speakers and authors, and others are invited to use the Happiness Index in their work.
The Happiness Index is one piece of the puzzle that completes a picture conveying the importance of
happiness, and the happiness movement as a new environmental, social, and economic paradigm to
enhance quality of life and sustainability for all beings.

References
Cantril, H. (1965). The pattern of human concern. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Cloutier, S., Jambeck, J., & Scott, N. (2014). The sustainable neighborhoods for happiness index
(SNHI): A metric for assessing a community's sustainability and potential influence on
happiness. Ecological Indicators, 40, 147–152. doi:10.1016/j.ecolind.2014.01.012
Cramm, J., Starting, M., de Vreede, P., Steverink, N., & Nieboer, A. (2012). Validation of the self-
management ability scale (SMAS) and development and validation of a shorter scale (SMAS-
S) among older patients shortly after hospitalization. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes,
10, 9. doi:10.1186/1477-7525-10-9
Diener, E., & Pavot, W. (1993). The affective and cognitive context of self-reported measures of
subjective well-being. Social Indicators Research, 28, 1–20. doi:10.1007/BF01086714
Diener, E., Tay, L., & Oishi, S. (2013). Rising income and the subjective well-being of nations.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 267–276. doi:10.1037/a0030487
Easterlin, R. (1974). Does economic growth improve the human lot? In P. David & R. Reder (Eds.),
Nations and households in economic growth: Essays in honor of moses (pp. 89–125). New
York, NY: Academic Press. Retrieved from
http://graphics8.nytimes.com/images/2008/04/16/business/Easterlin1974.pdf
Easterlin, R. (1995). Will raising the incomes of all increase the happiness of all? Journal of
Economic Behavior and Organization, 27, 35–48. doi:10.1016/0167-2681(95)00003-B
Easterlin, R. (2001). Income and happiness: Towards a unified theory. The Economic Journal, 111,
465–484. doi:10.1111/1468-0297.00646
European Social Survey. (2006). Final source questionnaire (Round 3, 2006/7) amendment 03.
London, United Kingdom: Centre for Comparative Social Surveys, City University London.
Retrieved from
http://www.europeansocialsurvey.org/docs/round3/fieldwork/source/ESS3_source_main_quest
ionnaire.pdf
European Social Survey. (2011). ESS Round 5 source questionnaire. London, United Kingdom:
Centre for Comparative Social Surveys, City University London. Retrieved from
https://www.europeansocialsurvey.org/docs/round5/fieldwork/source/ESS5_source_main_ques
tionnaire.pdf
Frank, R. (1997). The frame of reference as a public good. Economic Journal, 107, 1832–1847.
Retrieved from http://www.robert-h-
frank.com/PDFs/The%20Frame%20of%20Reference%20as%20a%20Public%20Good.pdf
Frey, B., & Luechinger, S. (2007). Concepts of happiness and their measurement. Hessen, Germany:
Metropolis Verlag.

Journal of Social Change 15


Musikanski et al., 2017

Frey, B., & Stutzer, A. (2011). The use of happiness research for public policy. Social Choice and
Welfare, 38, 659–674. doi:10.1007/s00355-011-0629-z
Gallup. (2012, October). Global states of mind new metrics for world leaders. Retrieved from
http://allafrica.com/download/resource/main/main/idatcs/00050692:e395af671dee55626aaabb
7817184b80.pdf
Gallup World Poll. (2008). World poll questions. Retrieved from
http://media.gallup.com/dataviz/www/WP_Questions_WHITE.pdf
Gross National Happiness Commission. (n.d.). Mandate. Retrieved from
http://www.gnhc.gov.bt/mandate
Happiness Alliance. (2011). Gross National Happiness Index Round 1 – 2011. Retrieved from
http://www.slideshare.net/TheHappinessInitiative/happiness-initiativegnh-index2011
Happiness Alliance. (2014a). Gross National Happiness Index Round 4 – 2014. Retrieved from
http://www.slideshare.net/TheHappinessInitiative/gross-national-happiness-index-2015
Happiness Alliance. (2014b). Project history of the Happiness Alliance home of the Happiness Index
and Gross National Happiness Index. Retrieved from
http://www.slideshare.net/TheHappinessInitiative/happiness-alliance-home-of-the-
happiness-initiatives-project-history
Happiness Alliance. (2014c). The domains of happiness for the Gross National Happiness Index.
Retrieved from http://www.slideshare.net/TheHappinessInitiative/the-domains-of-happiness-
for-the-gross-national-happiness-index
Heartland Institute of Financial Education. (2006). Personal well-being scale and score
interpretation. Retrieved from http://www.hife-
classes.org/images/HIFE_Personal_Financial_Well-Being_Survey.pdf
Helliwell, J., & Wang, S. (2011). Trust and well-being. International Journal of Well-being, 1, 42–78.
Holden, M. (2006). Revisiting the local impact of community indicators projects: Sustainable Seattle
as prophet in its own land. Applied Research in Quality of Life, 1, 253–277.
doi:10.1007/s11482-007-9020-8
Hone, L., Jarden, A., Schofield, G., & & Duncan, S. (2014). Measuring flourishing: The impact of
operational definitions on the prevalence of high levels of well-being. International Journal of
Well-being, 4, 62–90. Retrieved from
http://www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org/index.php/ijow/article/view/286
Huppert, F., Marks, N., Clark, A. E., Siegrist, J., Stutzer, A., Vitterso, J. & Wahrendorf, M. (2009).
Measuring well-being across Europe: description of the ESS well-being module and
preliminary findings. Social Indicators Research, 91, 301–315. doi: 10.1007/s11205-008-
9346-0
Huppert, F., & So, T. (2011). Flourishing across Europe: Application of a new conceptual framework
for defining well-being. Social Indicators Research, 110, 837–861. doi: 10.1007/s11205-011-
9966-7
International Well-Being Group. (2006). Personal Well-Being Index (4th ed.). Melbourne, Australia:
Deakin University Centre on Quality of Life. Retrieved May 26, 2016, from
http://www.deakin.edu.au/research/acqol/instruments/wellbeing-index/pwi-adult-english.pdf

Journal of Social Change 16


Musikanski et al., 2017

International Well-Being Group. (2013). Personal Well-Being Index (5th ed.). Melbourne, Australia:
Deakin University Centre on Quality of Life. Retrieved May 26, 2016, from
http://www.acqol.com.au/iwbg/wellbeing-index/pwi-a-english.pdf
Jaarsma, T., Halfens, R., Abu‐Saad, H., Dracup, K., Stappers, J., & Ree, J. (1999). Quality of life in
older patients with systolic and diastolic heart failure. European Journal of Heart Failure, 1,
151–160. doi:10.1016/S1388-9842(99)00007-0
Kahneman, D., & Deaton, A. (2010). High income improves evaluation of life but not emotional well-
being. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences, 107, 16489–16493.
doi:10.1073/pnas.1011492107
Kasser, T., & Sheldon, K. (2009). Time affluence as a path toward personal happiness and ethical
business practice: Empirical evidence from four studies. Journal of Business Ethics, 84, 243–
255. doi:10.1007/s10551-008-9696-1
Kessler, R., Mickelson, K., & Williams, D. (1999). The prevalence, distribution, and mental health
correlates of perceived discrimination in the United States. Journal of Health and Social
Behavior, 40, 208–230. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28399649
Layard, R. (2005). Happiness: Lessons from a new science. New York, NY: Penguin.
Levin, K., & Currie, C. (2014). Reliability and validity of an adapted version of the Cantril ladder for
use with adolescent samples. Social Indicators Research, 119, 1047–1063.
doi:10.1007/s11205-013-0507-4
Marans, R. (2001). Detroit Area Study, 2001: Quality of life in the metro-Detroit area (ICPSR29441-
v1). Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-University Consortium for Political and Social Research.
doi:10.3886/ICPSR29441.v1
Marks, N., Abdallah, S., Simms, A., & Thompson, S. (2006). The (un) happy planet
index 2.0. Retrieved May 10, 2016, from http://www.neweconomics.org/page/-
/files/The_Happy_Planet_Index.pdf
Morton, A., & Edwards, L. (2012). Community well-being indicators, survey template for local
government. Sydney, Australia: University of Technology Australian Centre of Excellence for
Local Government. Retrieved May 26, 2016, from
http://www.acelg.org.au/file/1610/download?docId=206
Musikanski, L., & Polley, C. (2016). Life, liberty and pursuit of happiness: Measuring what matters.
Journal of Social Change, 7, 48–72. doi:10.5590/JOSC.2016.08.1.05
Oliver, J., Huxley, P., Priebe, S., & Kaiser, W. (1997). Measuring the quality of life of severely
mentally ill people using the Lancashire quality of life profile. Social Psychiatry and
Psychiatric Epidemiology, 32, 76–83. doi:10.1007/BF00788924
National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health (NIOSH). (n.d.). General Social Survey 2010:
SECTION D: Quality of Worklife Module: NIOSH. Retrieved from
http://www.cdc.gov/niosh/topics/stress/pdfs/qwl2010.pdf
Organisation for Economic Co-Operation and Development (OECD). (2013, March 20). OECD
guidelines on measuring subjective well-being. Paris, France: OECD Publishing.
doi:10.1787/9789264191655-en
Ovaska, T., & Takashima, R. (2006). Economic policy and the level of self-perceived well-being: An
international comparison. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 35, 308–325.
doi:10.1016/j.socec.2005.11.054

Journal of Social Change 17


Musikanski et al., 2017

Pfeiffer, D., & Cloutier, S. (2016). Planning for happy neighborhoods. Journal of the American
Planning Association, 83, 267–279. doi:10.1080/01944363.2016.1166347
Russell, D. (1996). The UCLA Loneliness Scale (Version 3): Reliability, validity, and factor
structure. Journal of Personality Assessment, 66, 20–40. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa6601_2
Smith, T., Marsden, P., Hout, M., & Kim, J. (2013). General social surveys, 1972–2012 [cumulative
file]. Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-University Consortium for Political and Social Research.
Retrieved from http://people.wku.edu/douglas.smith/GSS%201972_2012%20Codebook.pdf
Svindseth, M., Nøttestad, J., & Dahl, A. (2013). Perceived humiliation during admission to a
psychiatric emergency service and its relation to socio-demography and psychopathology.
BMC Psychiatry, 13, 217–217. doi:10.1186/1471-244X-13-217
Szramka-Pawlak, B., Dańczak-Pazdrowska, A., Rzepa, T., Szewczyk, A., Sadowska-Przytocka, A., &
Żaba, R. (2014). Quality of life and optimism in patients with morphea. Applied Research in
Quality of Life, 9, 863–870. doi:10.1007/s11482-013-9273-3
Take Back Your Time. (n.d.). Our mission. Retrieved from https://www.takebackyourtime.org/about-
us/mission/
The Center for Bhutan Studies & GNH Research. (2015). A compass towards a just and harmonious
society. Retrieved from http://www.socioeco.org/bdf_fiche-document-5358_pt.html
Tran, K. (2006, October 4). Measuring discrimination in social surveys: Experiences from Canada
[PowerPoint slides]. Retrieved from
http://www.ceg.ul.pt/metropolis2006/WorkshopPresentations/Culturgest/KellyTran_metropol
is2006.pdf
U.K. Office for National Statistics. (2013, April 11). Measuring national well-being: Older people and
loneliness, 2013. Retrieved from http://www.ons.gov.uk/ons/dcp171766_304939.pdf
U.K. Office for National Statistics. (2015). Measuring national well-being: Personal well-being in the
U.K., 2014 to 2015. Retrieved from http://www.ons.gov.uk/ons/rel/wellbeing/measuring-
national-well-being/personal-well-being-in-the-uk--2014-15/stb-personal-well-being-in-the-
uk--2014-15.html#tab-3--Measuring-personal-well-being-in-the-UK
Ura, K., Alkire, S., Zangmo, T., & Wangdi, K. (2012, May). An extensive analysis of GNH index.
Centre for Bhutan Studies. Retrieved May 29, 1016, from
http://www.grossnationalhappiness.com/wp-
content/uploads/2012/10/An%20Extensive%20Analysis%20of%20GNH%20Index.pdf
Victoria Foundation. (2010). 2010 Greater Victoria Well-Being Survey. Retrieved from
http://www.slideshare.net/TheHappinessInitiative/2010-greater-victoria-wellbeing-survey
World Health Organization. (2002). WHOQOL-HIV instrument. Geneva, Switzerland: Author.
Retrieved from
http://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/10665/77774/1/WHO_MSD_MER_Rev.2012.01_eng.pdf?ua=
1
World Values Survey. (n.d.a) World Values Survey Wave 4 1999–2004 (official aggregate v.20240429).
Retrieved May 29, 2016, from
http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/WVSDocumentationWV4.jsp

Journal of Social Change 18


Musikanski et al., 2017

World Values Survey. (n.d.b). World Values Survey Wave 5 2005–2008 (official aggregate.
v.20140429). Retrieved May 29, 2016, from
http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/WVSDocumentationWV5.jsp
World Values Survey. (n.d.c). World Values Survey Wave 6 (official aggregate v.20150418). Retrieved
May 29, 2016, from http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/WVSDocumentationWV6.jsp
Zidanšek, A. (2007). Sustainable development and happiness in nations. Energy, 32, 891–897.
doi:10.1016/j.energy.2006.09.016

(Appendices follow)

Journal of Social Change 19


Musikanski et al., 2017

Appendix A
Questions in Round 4 of the Happiness Index and Sources
Below are the sources for each question in Round 4 of the Happiness Index. It is organized by
domains. Each domain lists the full text of the questions with answer choices followed by the
sources.

Domain 1: Cantril Ladder


The first domain has one question: the Cantril Ladder.

Question
There is one question in Domain 1:

Please imagine a ladder with steps numbered from zero at the bottom to ten at the top.
Suppose we say that the top of the ladder represents the best possible life for you and the
bottom of the ladder represents the worst possible. If the top step is 10 and the bottom step is
0, on which step of the ladder do you feel you personally stand at the present time?
(Organisation for Economic Co-Operation and Development [OECD], 2013, p. 249)

Answers
The answers are on an 11-point scale rated from 0 (worst possible life for you) to 10 (best possible life
for you). This question is Cantril’s self-anchoring ladder (OECD, 2013).

Domain 2: Satisfaction With Life


The second domain has four questions. They are the same as those used by the Government of the
United Kingdom for measuring well-being.

Questions
There are four questions in Domain 2: “Overall, how satisfied are you with your life nowadays?”
“Overall, to what extent do you feel the things you do in your life are worthwhile?” “Overall, how
happy did you feel yesterday?” “Overall, how anxious did you feel yesterday?”

Answers
The answer choices are on an 11-point scale rated from 0 (not at all) to 10 (completely). The source of
the questions is the U.K. Office for National Statistics Personal Well-Being (2015).

Domain 3: Psychological Well-Being


There are five questions in Domain 3. They measure mental well-being, also termed psychological
health or flourishing.

Questions
The five questions in Domain 3 ask to what extent participants agree with the following statements:
“I lead a purposeful and meaningful life,” “I am engaged and interested in my daily activities”
(OECD, 2013), “I am optimistic about my future,” “Most days I feel a sense of accomplishment from
what I do,” and “In general, I feel positive about myself” (Huppert & So, 2011, p. 843).

Journal of Social Change 20


Musikanski et al., 2017

Answers
The answer choices in this domain and in all subsequent domains except the demographic section
are on a 5-point scale rated 0 (strongly disagree), 1 (disagree), 2 (neither agree nor disagree), 3 (agree),
and 4 (strongly agree). The source for the first two questions is Diener and Biswas’ Psychological
Well-Being Scale (OECD, 2013). The source for the last three questions is the European Social
Survey 2003 (Huppert & So, 2011).

Domain 4: Health
There are four questions in Domain 4 that measure physical health.

Questions
The first question is “In general, I would say my health is (poor, fair, good, very good, excellent).”
This question is adapted from the World Health Organization (2002), in which the question is “How
satisfied are you with your health?” (p. 14). The second is “Please indicate how much of the time
during the past week you had a lot of energy.” This question is adapted from the World Health
Organization question, “Do you have enough energy for everyday life?” (p. 11).

The third and fourth questions ask participants to rate their level of satisfaction. “How satisfied
were you with your ability to perform your daily living activities?” “How satisfied were you with the
quality of your exercise?” The source of the third question is the World Health Organization (2002),
in which the question is “How satisfied are you with your ability to perform your daily living
activities?” (p. 15). The fourth question is an adaptation of a question used by the United Kingdom
Department of Health (2006), in which the question is “During the last week, how many hours did
you spend on each of the following activities? Physical exercise such as swimming, jogging, aerobics,
football, tennis, gym, workout, etc.” (p. 2).

Answers
The answer choices for the second question are 0 (very rarely or never), 1 (rarely), 2 (sometimes), 3
(often), and 4 (very often or always; World Health Organization, 2002, p. 11). The answer choices for
the third and fourth questions are 0 (very dissatisfied), 1 (dissatisfied), 2 (neither satisfied nor
dissatisfied), 3 (satisfied), and 4 (very satisfied; p. 15).

Domain 5: Time Balance


There are three questions in the Time Balance domain. They address time balance from three
perspectives. The time balance domain does not include a question about work–life balance. A
question about work–life balance is included in the work domain.

Questions
The first question is “In a typical week, how much of your time are you able to spend doing the kinds
of things that you enjoy?” The second two questions ask participants to think specifically about how
things were for them over the past week. “My life has been too rushed.” “I have plenty of spare time.”

Answers
The answers choices for the first question are 0 (none of my time), 1 (not much of my time), 2 (some of
my time), 3 (most of my time), and 4 (all of my time). The answer choices for the second and third
questions are 0 (strongly agree), 1 (disagree), 2 (neither agree nor disagree), 3 (agree), and 4 (strongly
agree). The source of both questions is Kasser and Sheldon (2009, p. 247).

Journal of Social Change 21


Musikanski et al., 2017

Domain 6: Lifelong Learning, Arts, and Culture


There are four questions in the Lifelong Learning, Arts, and Culture domain. They are about access
to lifelong learning, arts, and culture as well as about diversity and inclusion.

Questions
The first three questions ask how satisfied participants are in their neighborhood or community with
“Your access to sports and recreational activities?” “Your access to artistic and cultural activities?”
and “Your access to activities to develop skills through informal education?” (Victoria Foundation,
2010, p. 15). Local governments in Australia used similar questions in 2012 (Morton & Edwards,
2012). The questions were as follows: “How adequate are the opportunities in your local community
for you to effectively engage in (a) sports and recreation, (b) art and cultural activities?” (p. 13) and
“How would you rate the adequacy of the following services in your local community in terms of your
needs and well-being: Education?” (p. 19).

The fourth question in this domain is “How often do you feel uncomfortable or out of place in your
neighborhood because of your ethnicity, culture, race, skin color, language, accent, gender, sexual
orientation, or religion?” (Tran, 2006, p. 6). Tran’s question is “How often do you feel uncomfortable
or out of place in Canada now because of your ethnicity, culture, race, skin colour, language, accent
or religion?” (p. 6). This question is similar to a question in the 2010 Greater Victoria Well-Being
Survey, except that the time frame is the last 5 years and the question begins with the sentence
“Discrimination may happen when people are treated unfairly because they are being seen as
different from others.” (p. 15). This question is also similar to two questions used by Kessler,
Mickelson, and William (1999): “How many times in your life have you been discriminated against in
each of the following ways because of such things as your race, ethnicity, gender, age, religion,
physical appearance, sexual orientation, or other characteristics?” (Section VI) and “(number of times
in your life...) You were prevented from remaining in a neighborhood because neighbors made life so
uncomfortable?” (Section VI).

Answers
The answer choice to the first three questions are 0 (very dissatisfied), 1 (dissatisfied), 2 (neither
satisfied nor dissatisfied), 3 (satisfied), and 4 (very satisfied; Victoria Foundation, 2010, p. 15). The
answer choices for the fourth questions are 0 (never), 1 (rarely), 2 (some of the time), 3 (most of the
time), and 4 (all of the time; Tran, 2006, p. 6).

Domain 7: Community
There are seven questions in the Community domain, including questions about a sense of belonging,
volunteerism, donation activity, and trust, as well as a question about a lost wallet.

Questions
The first question in the community domain is “How would you describe your feeling of belonging to
your local community?” (Victoria Foundation, 2010, p. 6). The answer choices are “very weak,
somewhat weak, neither weak nor strong, somewhat strong, very strong” (p. 6). The source of the
questions is the 2010 Greater Victoria Well-Being Survey (Victoria Foundation, 2010), in which the
question is “How would you describe your sense of belonging to your local community? Would you say
it is?” (p. 6). This question is similar to a question in the Detroit Area Study 2001: Quality of Life in
the Metro-Detroit Area (Marans, 2001), in which the question is “Here are some statements about
neighbors and neighborhoods. For each statement, please tell me if you strongly agree, agree, neither
agree nor disagree, disagree, or strongly disagree? You feel like you belong to a community” (pp. 25–

Journal of Social Change 22


Musikanski et al., 2017

26). It is also similar to a question in the World Values Survey Wave 5 (World Values Survey, n.d.b),
in which the question is “People have different views about themselves and how they relate to the
world. Using this card, would you tell me how strongly you agree or disagree with each of the
following statements about how you see yourself? I see myself as part of my local community” (p. 19).

The second questions is “Please tell us how many of the following people you trust: Your neighbors”
(Victoria Foundation, 2010, p. 6). The source of the second question is the 2010 Greater Victoria
Well-Being Survey (Victoria Foundation, 2010), in which the question is “How much do you
trust…Most strangers that you encounter” (p. 6). The World Values Survey Wave 6 (n.d.c) has a
similar question. It is “Could you tell me for each whether you trust people from this group
completely, somewhat, not very much or not at all? Your neighborhood” (p. 8).

The third questions is “Please tell us how many of the following people you trust: Businesses in your
community” (Victoria Foundation, 2010, p. 6). The source of the third question is the 2010 Greater
Victoria Well-Being Survey (Victoria Foundation, 2010), in which the question is “How much do you
trust…Businesses in your community” (p. 6). The World Values Survey Waves 5 (n.d.a) and 6 (n.d.c)
have a similar question. It is “…could you tell me how much confidence you have in them: is it a
great deal of confidence, quite a lot of confidence, not very much confidence or none at all? Major
companies.” (World Values Survey, n.d.c, p. 9; World Values Survey, n.d.b, p. 11). The General Social
Survey (Smith, Marsden, Hout, & Kim, 2013) also has a similar question. It is “Confidence? Major
companies?” (Smith et al., 2013, p. 163).

The fourth question is “Imagine that you lost a wallet or purse that contained $200. Please indicate
how likely you think it would be to have all of your money returned to you if it was found by someone
who lives close by” (Victoria Foundation, 2010, p. 17). The source of this question is the 2010 Greater
Victoria Well-Being Survey, in which the question is “If you lost a wallet or purse that contained two
hundred dollars, what is the likelihood of it being returned with the money in it if it was found by:
Someone who lives close by” (p. 17). This question is similar to the one used by the Gallup World Poll
2006 (Helliwell & Wang, 2011). The question is “In the city or area where you live, imagine that you
lost your wallet or something holding your identification or address and it was found by someone
else. Do you think your wallet (or your valuables) would be returned to you if it were found by
strangers” (Helliwell & Wang, 2011, pp. 74–75).

The fifth question is “How satisfied are you with your personal safety in your city or town?” The fifth
question is similar to a question in the International Well-Being Group’s (2013) Personal Well-Being
Index, in which the question is “How satisfied are you with…personal safely? How safe do you feel?”
(p. 11). The World Health Organization (2002) has similar questions including “How safe do you feel
in your daily life?” (p. 9), “Do you feel you are living in a safe and secure environment?” (p. 9), and
“How satisfied are you with your safety and security?” (p. 16). The Detroit Area Study 2001: Quality
of Life in the Metro-Detroit Area (Marans, 2001) also has similar questions including “How safe do
you feel about being out alone in your neighborhood during the day?” (p. 34), “How about at night—
how safe do you feel about being out alone in your neighborhood at night?” (p. 34), and “On a scale of
1 to 7, where 1 is completely dissatisfied and 7 is completely satisfied, how satisfied are you with
your personal safety in this neighborhood?” (p. 34). Gallup World Poll (2008) also has a similar
question, which is “Do you feel safe walking alone at night in the city or area where you life?” (p. 4).

Journal of Social Change 23


Musikanski et al., 2017

The sixth and seventh questions are “Using the scale below, please indicate how frequently you have
done these activities in the past 12 months: Volunteered your time to an organization. Donated
money to a charity” (Smith et al., 2013, pp. 634–635). The source of the questions is the General
Social Survey 2002. The questions in the General Social Survey 2002 are “During the past 12 months
how often have you done each of the following things: Done volunteer work for a charity” (Smith et
al., 2013, p. 634) and “During the past 12 months how often have you done each of the following
things: Given money to a charity” (Smith et al., 2013, p. 635). The Gallup World Poll (2008) has
similar questions, which are “Have you volunteered your time to an organization in the past month?”
(p. 2) and “Have you donated money to a charity in the past month?” (p. 2).

Answers
The answer choices for the second and third question about trust are 0 (trust none of them), 1 (trust a
few of them), 2 (trust some of them), 3 (trust most of them), and 4 (trust all of them; Victoria
Foundation, 2010, p. 6).

The answer choices for the fourth question regarding a lost wallet are 0 (not at all likely), 1
(somewhat likely), 2 (fairly likely), 3 (very likely), and 4 (extremely likely; Victoria Foundation, 2010,
p. 17).

The answer choices for the fifth question regarding personal safety are 0 (very dissatisfied), 1
(dissatisfied), 2 (neither satisfied nor dissatisfied), 3 (satisfied), and 4 (very satisfied).

The answer choices for the sixth and seventh questions regarding volunteerism and donating activity
are 0 (at least once a month), 1 (at least once every 3 months), 2 (at least once every 6 months), 3 (once
in the last year), and 4 (never; Smith et al., 2013, pp. 634–635).

Domain 8: Social Support


There are four questions in the Social Support domain. The questions measure satisfaction with
relationships, and feeling cared for, loved, and lonely.

Questions
The first question is “Please rate your level of satisfaction. How satisfied are you with your personal
relationships?” (World Health Organization, 2002, p. 15).

The second question is “To what extent do you agree with the following statement? People in my life
care about me” (Victoria Foundation, 2010, p. 8). The source of the question is the 2010 Greater
Victoria Well-Being Survey (Victoria Foundation, 2010). This question is similar to a question in the
European Social Survey (2006), in which the question is “please say to what extent you agree or
disagree with each of the following statements…There are people in my life who really care about
me” (p. 52).

The third question is “Please indicate how much of the time during the past week...You felt loved.”
This question is similar to Seligman’s PERMA flourishing scale question “To what extent do you feel
loved?” (Hone, Jarden, Schofield, & Duncan, 2014, p. 70).

Journal of Social Change 24


Musikanski et al., 2017

The fourth question is “Please indicate how much of the time during the past week…You felt lonely”
(OECD, 2013, p. 250). The source of question is the European Social Survey well-being module
developed by Huppert et al. (2009). The question is “Please tell me how much of the time during the
past week…you felt lonely” (OECD, 2013, p. 250). This question is similar to a question in the U.K.
Office for National Statistics (2013) report Measuring National Well-being: Older people and
loneliness, 2013 that used data from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing, Wave 5, 2009–2010
for the question “How often do you feel lonely?” (U.K. Office for National Statistics, 2013, p. 19). The
fourth question is also similar to the UCLA Loneliness Scale’s question “Indicate how often each of
the statements below is descriptive of you. 11. I feel completely alone” (Russell, 1996, p. 2).

Answers
The answer choices are 0 (very dissatisfied), 1 (dissatisfied), 2 (neither satisfied nor dissatisfied), 3
(satisfied), and 4 (very satisfied; World Health Organization, 2002, p. 15). The answer choices for the
second question about feeling cared about are 0 (strongly disagree), 1 (disagree), 2 (neither agree nor
disagree), 3 (agree), and 4 (strongly agree; Victoria Foundation, 2010, p. 8). The answer choice for the
third and fourth questions about feeling lonely and loved are 0 (very rarely or never), 1 (rarely), 2
(sometimes), 3 (often), and 4 (very often or always; OECD, 2013, p. 250).

Domain 9: Environment
There are four questions in the Environment domain. They measure access to nature, sense of
healthy or toxic environment, local natural preservation efforts and satisfaction with air quality.

Questions
The first question is “How healthy is your physical environment?” World Values Survey (1999–2004,
p. 17).

The second and third questions are “Please rate your level of satisfaction: How satisfied are you with
the efforts being made to preserve the natural environment in your neighborhood? How satisfied are
you with the opportunities that you have to enjoy nature” (Victoria Foundation, 2010, p.19). The
source of these questions is the 2010 Greater Victoria Well-Being Survey (Victoria Foundation,
2010). The Gallup World Poll (2008) has a similar question to the second question, which is “In your
country, are you satisfied or dissatisfied with efforts to preserve the environment?” (p. 3).

The fourth question is “How satisfied are you with the air quality in your environment?” (OECD,
2013, p. 262). The source of the fourth question is the International Well-Being Group’s (2006)
Personal Well-Being Index, in which the question is “How satisfied are you with the air quality in
your environment?” (OECD, 2013, p. 262). The fourth question is similar to a question in the Detroit
Area Study 2001: Quality of Life in the Metro-Detroit Area (Marans, 2001, p. 41), in which the
question is “…first issue is the loss of natural scenic areas. Would you say that this is a very serious
problem, a somewhat serious problem, not a serious problem, or not a problem at all in all. Air
pollution in (NAME OF COUNTY)” (p. 41). The World Values Survey (n.d.a) also has a similar
question, which is “How satisfied are you with your physical environment (e.g. pollution, climate,
noise, attractiveness)?” (p. 17). The Gallup World Poll (2008) also has another similar question,
which is “In your city or area where you live, are you satisfied or dissatisfied with the quality of air?”
(p. 3). In addition, Gallup’s (2012) Global States of Mind New Metrics for World Leaders report
included a similar question, which is “In the city or area where you life, are you satisfied or
dissatisfied with the quality of air?” (p. 5).

Journal of Social Change 25


Musikanski et al., 2017

Answers
The answer choice for the first question regarding the health of the environment are 0 (not at all), 1
(a little), 2 (somewhat), 3 (very), and 4 (extremely; World Values Survey, n.d.a, p. 17). The answer
choices for the second and third question regarding satisfaction with access to and preservation of
nature are 0 (very dissatisfied), 1 (dissatisfied), 2 (neither satisfied nor dissatisfied), 3 (satisfied), and
4 (very satisfied; Victoria Foundation, 2010, p.19). The answer choices for the fourth question
regarding air quality are 0 (very dissatisfied), 1 (dissatisfied), 2 (neither satisfied nor dissatisfied), 3
(satisfied), and 4 (very satisfied; OECD, 2013, p. 262).

Domain 10: Governance


There are four questions in the governance domain. They include questions about sense of
corruption, sense of local officials caring about what people think, and satisfaction with local and
national government.

Questions
The first question is “State your level of agreement with the following statements: Corruption is
widespread throughout the government in my city or town” (Gallup World Poll, 2008, p. 4). The
source of question is the Gallup World Poll (2008), in which the question is “Is corruption is
widespread throughout the government in my city or town?” (p. 4).

The second question is “State your level of agreement with the following statements: The public
officials in my city or town pay attention to what people think” (Marans, 2001, p. 7). The source of
the question is the Detroit Area Study 2001: Quality of Life in the Metro-Detroit Area (Marans,
2001), in which the question is “Public officials in (NAME OF CITY/TOWNSHIP) pay attention to
what people think. How much do you agree or disagree?” (p. 7).

The third and fourth questions are “Please indicate how much confidence you have in the following
organizations: National government and. Local government” (Victoria Foundation, 2010, pp. 17–18).
The source of the third and fourth questions is the 2010 Greater Victoria Well-being Survey (Victoria
Foundation, 2010), in which the questions are “For the following organizations, please indicate how
much confidence you have in them: is it a great deal of confidence, quite a lot of confidence, not very
much confidence or no confidence? Federal government. Local government” (pp. 17–18). The World
Values Survey (n.d.a) Wave 4 has a similar question, which is “…how much confidence you have
in…The government (in your nation’s capital)” (p. 13). The Gallup World Poll (2008) also has a
similar question, which is “In this country, do you have confidence in national government?” (p. 4).

Answers
The answer choices for the third and fourth question regarding confidence in local and national
government are 0 (no confidence), 1 (not very much confidence), 2 (a fair amount of confidence), 3
(quite a lot of confidence), and 4 (a great deal of confidence; Victoria Foundation, 2010, pp. 17–18).
The answer choices for the first question about corruption are 0 (strongly disagree), 1 (disagree), 2
(neither agree nor disagree), 3 (agree), and 4 (strongly agree; Gallup World Poll, 2008, p. 4). The
answer choices for the second question regarding sense that public official pay attention to what
people think are 0 (strongly disagree), 1 (disagree), 2 (neither agree nor disagree), 3 (agree), and 4
(strongly agree; Marans, 2001, p. 7).

Journal of Social Change 26


Musikanski et al., 2017

Domain 11: Standard of Living/Economy


There are four questions in the Standard of Living/Economy domain. They measure stress about
personal finance, getting by paycheck to paycheck, inability to afford food and sense of having
enough money.

Questions
The first question is “In general, how much stress do you feel about your personal finances?”
(Heartland Institute of Financial Education, 2006, p. 8). The source of the question is the Heartland
Institute of Financial Education, in which the question is “How stressed do you feel about your
personal finances in general?” (p. 8). The Heartland Institute of Financial Education has another
similar question, which is “What do you feel is the level of your financial stress today?” (p. 1). The

Gallup World Poll (2008) also has a similar question, which is “How concerned are you…based on
your current financial situation. Are you very worried, moderately worried, not too worries or not at
all worried about not being able to maintain the standard of living you enjoy?” (p. 7).

The second question is “How frequently do you find yourself just getting by financially and living
paycheck to paycheck?” (Heartland Institute of Financial Education, 2006, p. 7). The source of the
question is the Heartland Institute of Financial Education in which the question is “How frequently
do you find yourself just getting by financially and living paycheck to paycheck?” (p. 7).

The third question is “Please indicate how frequently you have had the following experiences in the
past 12 months. You ate less because there wasn’t enough food or money for food.” The question is
similar to a question in the Greater Victoria Well-being Survey (Victoria Foundation, 2010), in which
the question is “In the past 12 months, did any of the following happen to you? Decreased the size of
your meal or skipped meals because there wasn’t enough food or money for food?” (p. 3). This
question is similar to the Gallup World Poll (2008) question “Have there been times in the past 12
months when you did not have enough money to buy the food that you or your family?” (Gallup
World Poll, 2008, p. 3). Gallup’s (2012) Gallup World Path poll also has a similar question, which is
“Have there been times in the past 12 months when you did not have enough money to buy the food
that you and your family needed?” (p. 4). The Heartland Institute of Financial Education (2006) also
has a similar question, which is “How often does this happen to you? You want to go out to eat, go to
a movie or do something else and don’t go because you can’t afford to?” (p. 6).

The fourth question is “To what extent do you agree with the following statement? I have enough
money to buy things I want” (Kasser & Sheldon, 2009, p. 247). The source of the question is Kasser
and Sheldon, in which the question is “I have had enough money to buy the things that are
important to me” (p. 247).

Answers
The answer choices for the first question about stress over personal finances are “overwhelming
stress, high stress, moderate stress, low stress, no stress at all” (Heartland Institute of Financial
Education, 2006, p. 8). The answer choices for the second question regarding getting by paycheck to
paycheck are 0 (all the time), 1 (most of the time), 2 (sometimes), 3 (rarely), and 4 (never; p. 7). The
answer choices for the third question regarding having enough money for food are 0 (at least once a
month), 1 (at least once every 3 months), 2 (at least once every 6 months), 3 (once in the last year), and
4 (never). The answer choices are for the fourth question regarding having enough money are 0
(strongly disagree), 1 (disagree), 2 (neither agree nor disagree), 3 (agree), and 4 (strongly agree)
(Kasser & Sheldon, 2009, p. 247).

Journal of Social Change 27


Musikanski et al., 2017

Domain 12: Work


There are six questions in the Work domain. They measure satisfaction with work, work–life
balance, autonomy, pay, productivity, and job interest.

Questions
The first question asks participants to answer the following questions about their satisfaction with
their current working situation (if they are not working [unemployed, retired, a student,
homemaker, volunteer, or other], they are instructed to answer only the questions that apply to their
main activity or occupation). “All things considered, how satisfied are you with your current work
life? (Note: if you work or volunteer at more than one job, you should answer about the job you spend
the longest time working at).” The question is similar to a question in the General Social Survey
2010 quality of work life module, in which the question is “All in all, how satisfied would you say you
are with your job?” (p. 12). The General Social Survey 2002 (Smith et al., 2013) also has a similar
question, which is “All in all, how satisfied would you say you are with your job?” (p. 603).

The second question is “How satisfied are you with the balance between the time you spend on your
job and the time you spend on other aspects of your life?” The source of the question is in Round 5 of
the European Social Survey (2011), in which the question is “How satisfied are you with the balance
between the time you spend on your paid work and the time you spend on other aspects of your life?”
(p. 72). Round 3 of the European Social Survey (2006) also has the same question, which is “How
satisfied are you with the balance between the time you spend on your paid work and the time you
spend on other aspects of your life?” (p. 53).

The third question is “How much of the time do you find your current work life interesting?” The
source of the question is in Round 3 of the European Social Survey (2006), in which the question is
“How much of the time do you find your job…interesting?” (p. 51). Round 5 of the European Social
Survey (2011) also has a similar question, which is “the reason I put effort into my work is…because
my work tasks are interesting?” (p. 65).

The fourth question is “Please state your level of agreement with each of these statements.
Considering all my efforts and achievements in my job I feel I get paid appropriately” (European
Social Survey, 2006, p. 53). The source of this question is Round 3 of the European Social Survey
(2006).

The fifth question is “Please state your level of agreement with each of these statements. The
conditions of my job allow me to be about as productive as I could be” (The National Institute for
Occupational Safety and Health [NIOSH], n.d., p. 5). The source this question is the NIOSH quality
of work life module (n.d.).

Journal of Social Change 28


Musikanski et al., 2017

The sixth question is “Please state your level of agreement with each of these statements. I am
allowed to decide how to go about getting my job done” (NIOSH, n.d., p. 5). The source this question
is the NIOSH (n.d) quality of work life module used by the General Social Survey, a survey
instrument developed by the National Opinion Research Center (NORC) at the University of
Chicago. This question is similar another question in the NIOSH (n.d) quality of work life module,
which is “I am given a lot of freedom to decide how to do my own work” (p. 6). The General Social
Survey conducted by NORC at the University of Chicago in 2002 using the NIOSH, work life module
also contains a similar question, which is “I am given a lot of freedom to decide how to do my own
work” (Smith et al., 2013, p. 892). Round 5 of the European Social Survey (2011) also has a similar
question, which is “how much the management at your work allows/allowed you…to decide how your
own daily work is/was organized? (p. 44).

Answers
The answer choices for the first question regarding satisfaction with work are 0 (very dissatisfied), 1
(dissatisfied), 2 (neither satisfied nor dissatisfied), 3 (satisfied), and 4 (very satisfied). The answer
choices for the second question about work-life balance are 0 (very dissatisfied), 1 (dissatisfied), 2
(neither satisfied nor dissatisfied), 3 (satisfied), and 4 (very satisfied; European Social Survey, 2011,
p. 72). The answer choices for the third question regarding interest in work are 0 (very rarely or
never), 1 (rarely), 2 (sometimes), 3 (often), and 4 (very often or always; European Social Survey, 2006,
p. 51). The answer choices for the fourth question regarding pay are 0 (strongly disagree), 1
(disagree), 2 (neither agree nor disagree), 3 (agree), and 4 (strongly agree; p. 53). The answer choices
for the fifth question regarding productivity are 0(strongly disagree), 1 (disagree), 2 (neither agree nor
disagree), 3 (agree), and 4 (strongly agree; NIOSH, n.d., p. 5). The answer choices for the sixth
question regarding autonomy are 0 (strongly disagree), 1 (disagree), 2 (neither agree nor disagree), 3
(agree), and 4 (strongly agree; NIOSH, n.d., p. 5).

Journal of Social Change 29


Musikanski et al., 2017

Appendix B
Demographic Questions in Round 4 of the Happiness Index
Below is a list of demographic questions within Round 4 the Happiness Index (Happiness Alliance,
2014a). The questions are listed in the order presented in the survey, with the answer choices listed.
Users of the Happiness Index who use the index for a population are able to access their results for
each of the aspects of demographics, for example a data query may compare females, age 50–54, with
children under the age of 18 with males, age 50-54, with children under the age of 18 or any other
aspect of demographic qualities included in the index.

Demographic Questions
The demographic questions are listed with answer choices following each question.

Question: What is your current age?

Answers: <12 years old, 12–17 years old, 18–24 years old, 25–29 years old, 30–34 years old, 35–39
years old, 40–44 years old, 45–49 years old, 50–54 years old, 55–59 years old, 60–64 years old, 65–69
years old, 70–74 years old, 75–79 years old, 80–84 years old, 85–89 years old, 90 years or older

Question: Which gender do you identify as?

Answers: Female, male, other.

Question: What race or ethnicity do you identify as?

Answers: Black/African, East Asian, Hispanic, Middle Eastern, South Asian,


White/European/Caucasian, two or more, other, prefer not to say.

Question: What is your current marital status?

Answers: Married, domestic partnership, never married and/or never in a domestic partnership,
separated, divorced, widowed, other.

Question: How many people currently reside in your household, including you?

Answers: 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, 14, 15, 16, 17, 18, 19, 20 or more

Question: Do you have any children under 18?

Answers: Yes, no.

Question: What is your postal code? (we just want the more general part; e.g. 98101 for U.S. zip
Codes or SE11 for U.K. postcodes).

Question: How spiritual do you consider yourself to be?

Answers: Not at all, not very, somewhat, moderately, very.

Question: What is the highest level of education that you have completed?

Journal of Social Change 30


Musikanski et al., 2017

Answers: Less than Grade 9 (no high school), More than Grade 9 but less than Grade 12 (started
high school but did not graduate), high school graduate or equivalent, Trade, technical or vocational
training, associate’s degree (AA, AS, etc.), bachelor's degree (BA, AB, BS, etc.), Graduate university
degree (MA, MS, MBA, etc.), professional degree (MD, DDS, DVM, LLB, JD, etc.), doctoral degree
(PhD, EdD, etc.), other.

Question: What was your total household income from all sources last year?

Answers: <$10,000, $10,000–$19,999, $20,000–$29,999, $30,000–$39,999, $40,000–$49,999,


$50,000–$59,999, $60,000–$69,999, $70,000–$79,999, $80,000–$89,999, $90,000–$99,999, $100,000–
$109,999, $110,000–$119,999, $120,000–$129,999, $130,000–$139,999, $140,000–$149,999,
$150,000–$159,999, $160,000–$169,999, $170,000–$179,999, $180,000–$189,999, $190,000–
$199,999, ≥$200,000.

Question: Regarding employment, which of the following options best describe your current work
life?

Answers: Full-time employee, part-time employee, working independently/self-employed, military,


volunteer, homemaker, unemployed looking for work, unemployed not looking for work, retired,
student or in training, unable to work, other.

Question: What is the one thing you would like to do to improve your own well-being and the
wellbeing of your community and our planet?

Question: In one word, what makes you happy?

The Journal of Social Change, sponsored by Walden University, welcomes manuscripts focusing
on interdisciplinary research in social change that improves the human condition and moves
people, groups, organizations, cultures, and society toward a more positive future.

Walden University Publishing: http://www.publishing.waldenu.edu

Journal of Social Change 31


References for Reading Material
1. Shiv Jakhar. Panchakosha. Int J Yogic Hum Mov Sports Sciences 2019;4(1):1382-1384.
https://www.theyogicjournal.com/archives/2019/4/1/Y/5-1-29

2. Leontiev, Dmitry. (2012). Anthropology of Happiness: The State of Well-Being and the
Way of Joy. Social sciences. 43. 93-104.
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/281115995_Anthropology_of_Happiness
_The_State_of_Well-Being_and_the_Way_of_Joy

3. Banavathy, Vinayachandra & Choudry, Anuradha. (2013). Understanding Happiness: A


Vedantic Perspective. Psychological Studies. 59. 141-152. 10.1007/s12646-013-0230-
x.
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/271914581_Understanding_Happiness_A
_Vedantic_Perspective

4. Snyder. C.R. S.J. Lopez & J.T. Pedrotti. (2015). Positive Psychology (The Scientific and
Practical Explorations of Human Strengths): Sage Publication.
https://www.pdfdrive.com/positive-psychology-the-scientific-and-practical-
explorations-of-human-strengths-3rd-edition-e194579393.html Snyder. C.R. & S.J.
Lopez (2002) Handbook of Positive Psychology (Chapter 5: Subjective Well-being: The
Science of Happiness and Life Satisfaction, Page 63 to 73): Oxford University Press.
http://www.ldysinger.com/@books1/Snyder_Hndbk_Positive_Psych/Snyder_Lopez_
Handbook_of_Positive_Psychology.pdf

5. Sri Aurobindo, The Synthesis of Yoga, Part Three: The Yoga of Divine Love, Chapter 7,
The Ananda Brahman, pp. 569-570
https://www.sriaurobindoashram.org/sriaurobindo/downloadpdf.php?id=37

6. Laura Musikanski, “Happiness Index Methodology”, Journal of Social Change, Vol. 9,


No. 1, pp. 4-31, 2017
https://scholarworks.waldenu.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1131&context=jsc

7. World Development Indicators 2016. (2016). United States: World Bank


Publications.
https://documents.worldbank.org/en/publication/documents-
reports/documentdetail/805371467990952829/world-development-indicators-2016

8. Zelenski, John. (2019) Positive _Psychology: The Science of Well-Being,


Carleton University, Ottawa, Canada, Sage Publications Chapter 3: Happiness; page
(77 to 110)
https://www.perlego.com/book/1431859/positive-psychology-the-science-of-
wellbeing-pdf
9. Baumgardner, S & Crothers, M. (2014). Positive Psychology. New Delhi: Pearson
Education, India. https://doku.pub/documents/positive-psychology-6lkv7vmpdj04

10. Goleman, D. (2007). Social Intelligence: The new science of human relationships, RHUK
https://kupdf.net/download/social-intelligence-daniel-
goleman_5afe296ce2b6f58b4a3cf754_pdf

11. Mathews, Gordon and Carolina Izquierdo (eds). (2010). Pursuits of Happiness: Well
being in Anthropological Perspective. Berghan Books
https://vdoc.pub/documents/pursuits-of-happiness-well-being-in-anthropological-
perspective-61sagvhrmf10

12. Seligman,M.(2002). Authentic happiness: Using the new positive psychology to realize
your potential for lasting fulfilment. NewYork:Free Press.
https://www.savetovalistebalans.rs/wp-content/uploads/2021/06/Authentic-
Happiness-Using-the-New-Positive-Psychology-to-Realize-Your-Potential-for-Lasting-
Fulfillment-by-Martin-E.P.-Seligman.pdf

13. Saptaparno Ghosh. 2022, March 22. Explained | What is the World Happiness Report.
The Hindu. https://www.thehindu.com/news/international/explained-what-is-the-
world-happiness-report/article65240772.ece

14. Rojina Shrestha. 2022, August 5. Happiness Index (HI): Definition, Domains, Relation
between HDI and HI. Public Health Notes.
https://www.publichealthnotes.com/happiness-index/

You might also like