Efeitos Do Itens de Preferencias em Ingles

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2017, 9999, n/a–n/a NUMBER 9999 ()

THE EFFECTS OF PREFERENCE ASSESSMENT TYPE ON PROBLEM


BEHAVIOR
SARA BETH TUNG
KENNEDY KRIEGER INSTITUTE AND UNIVERSITY OF MARYLAND, BALTIMORE COUNTY

JEANNE M. DONALDSON
LOUISIANA STATE UNIVERSITY

AND

SUNGWOO KAHNG
UNIVERSITY OF MISSOURI

Individuals with intellectual and developmental disabilities who engage in problem behavior
maintained by access to tangibles may exhibit more problem behavior during certain preference
assessments. We compared three common preference assessments to determine which resulted
in fewer problem behaviors. The paired stimulus and multiple-stimulus without replacement
assessments produced higher rates of problem behavior than the free operant (FO) assessment,
suggesting that the FO assessment may be the most appropriate assessment for individuals who
engage in problem behavior maintained by access to tangibles.
Key words: preference assessments, problem behavior, tangible function

Preference assessments are frequently admi- allowing participants to select and engage with
nistered to individuals with intellectual and or consume the selected stimulus for a period of
developmental disabilities to empirically identify time, removing the stimulus, and then repre-
stimuli that may serve as reinforcers. The most senting the next array (Hagopian, Long, &
common models of preference assessments Rush, 2004). An alternative format involves
involve presenting potential reinforcers in pairs providing free access to multiple potential rein-
(paired stimulus or PS) or larger arrays (multi- forcers simultaneously and measuring partici-
ple-stimulus without replacement; MSWO), pant engagement (a free operant [FO] format).
Despite their utility in identifying reinfor-
Sara Beth Tung, Department of Behavioral Psychology, cers, the repeated removal of stimuli during a
Kennedy Krieger Institute; Jeanne M. Donaldson, Depart- PS or MSWO assessment may exacerbate prob-
ment of Psychology, Louisiana State University; SungWoo
Kahng, Department of Health Psychology, University of lem behavior maintained by access to tangible
Missouri. items (Roane, Vollmer, Ringdahl, & Marcus,
This research is based on a project submitted in partial 1998). However, this is less of a concern when
fulfillment of requirements for the masters in arts degree
by the first author. We thank Casey Allen and Kristi Jeff- using the FO format, in that items are typically
ery for their assistance with data collection. We thank not removed during the assessment. Kang
John Borrero and Natalie Rolider for comments on previ- et al. (2010) found that two participants, whose
ous versions of this manuscript.
Sara Beth Tung is now at the Marcus Autism Center.
problem behavior was maintained by access to
Correspondence concerning this article should be tangible items, engaged in problem behavior
addressed to SungWoo Kahng, Ph.D., Department of during the PS and MSWO assessments but not
Health Psychology, University of Missouri, 205 Portland during the FO assessment. Kang et al. (2011)
St., Columbia, MO 65211. E-mail: kahngs@health.
missouri.edu similarly found higher levels of problem behav-
doi: 10.1002/jaba.414 ior during the PS and MSWO assessments
© 2017 Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior
1
2 SARA BETH TUNG et al.

relative to the FO assessment for individuals boy diagnosed with autism and ID. We con-
with problem behavior maintained by access to ducted sessions in a padded room or in a bed-
tangible items. room on the living unit. Functional analyses
Although these studies shed light on how var- indicated that all participants’ problem behav-
iations in preference assessment formats may ior was maintained by access to tangible
affect problem behavior, there were several lim- items.
itations. Although FO assessments do not
involve removing items within the assessment, Measurement and Interobserver Agreement
the therapist typically removes all items at the
Observers collected data on the problem
conclusion of the assessment, which may also
behavior (including self-injurious behavior,
occasion problem behavior. Neither Kang
aggression, and disruption) frequency, item
et al. (2010) nor Kang et al. (2011) collected
selection, and item engagement duration. We
data on problem behavior following the FO
individualized measures of problem behavior
assessment, which should be considered in
across participants and reported these measures
selecting assessment formats. Also, Kang
as a rate. Item engagement included the partici-
et al. (2010, 2011) did not hold constant the
pant manipulating an item for 3 s or more for
access duration to selected items. Longer access
nonedible items or placing an edible past the
to a potential reinforcer during the FO assess-
plane of his or her lips.
ment may function as an abolishing operation;
A second observer simultaneously but inde-
thus, reducing the probability of problem behav-
pendently scored the occurrence of the depend-
ior when the item is removed (i.e., differences in
ent variables, and we compared observers’
problem behavior may result from reinforcer
records to calculate interobserver agreement
satiation rather than frequent removal).
(IOA). A second observer collected data on prob-
The purpose of this study was to compare
lem behavior for 34% of sessions. When com-
PS, MSWO, and FO preference-assessment
pared to the primary observer using the
formats to determine problem behavior rates
proportional agreement method, IOA between
both during the assessment and following the
observers averaged 99% (range, 98%-100%),
removal of all items at the end of each assess-
92% (range, 80%-100%), 99% (range, 95%-
ment. Further, we controlled for the item-
100%), and 98% (range, 96%-100%) for Noah,
access duration across the three assessments to
Brady, Sofie, and Mason, respectively. A second
clarify the role of stimulus removal in evoking
observer collected data on item engagement dur-
problem behavior.
ing 44% of sessions and when compared to the
primary observer using a trial-by-trial agreement
METHOD method, IOA averaged 99% (range, 97%-
100%), 99% (range, 98%-100%), 99% (range,
Participants and Setting
98%-100%), and 99% (range, 97%-100%) for
Four children admitted to an inpatient unit
Noah, Brady, Sofie, and Mason, respectively.
for the assessment and treatment of problem
behavior participated. Noah was an 8-year-old
boy diagnosed with autism, intellectual disabil- Procedure
ity (ID), bipolar disorder, and attention deficit We exposed participants to FO, PS, and
hyperactivity disorder. Brady was a 9-year-old MSWO assessment formats, alternated in a
boy diagnosed with autism and ID. Sofie was counterbalanced order in a multielement
an 8-year-old girl diagnosed with autism and design. Each assessment included six items,
mild ID. Finally, Mason was a 10-year-old which we kept constant across all preference
PROBLEM BEHAVIOR DURING PREFERENCE ASSESSMENTS 3

assessments for each individual. These items PS preference assessment. We conducted the
were formally nominated by caregivers using PS assessment using the procedures outlined by
the Reinforcer Assessment for Individuals with Fisher et al. (1992). That is, we presented sti-
Severe Disabilities (Fisher, Piazza, Bowman, & muli in pairs and participant approaches to one
Amari, 1996), and we included the item from of the items resulted in access to that item for
the tangible condition of the functional analysis the designated amount of time (based on the
for each participant. We selected leisure items FO assessment). Each PS session included
for all participants except Noah whose assess- 15 trials such that every item was presented in
ments included only edible items. We consid- a pair with every other item once.
ered one complete preference assessment a MSWO preference assessment. We conducted
session, and we conducted one to three sessions the MSWO assessment using the procedures
per day. Following the completion of each pref- outlined by DeLeon and Iwata (1996). That is,
erence assessment, we continued collecting data we initially presented all six items in an array
for an additional 30 s to capture problem and following approach to an item, the thera-
behavior when all assessment materials were pist delivered the item for the duration deter-
removed. We did not deliver any social conse- mined by the FO assessment. The item was
quences for problem behavior during or follow- then removed and the remaining items in the
ing the assessment. array were re-presented. Each MSWO session
FO preference assessment. To equate the dura- included six trials such that each item was
tion of item access across assessments, we con- selected once or the session was terminated fol-
ducted the FO assessment first in each series of lowing 30 s of nonselection. This occurred
three sessions using procedures similar to those only during the first MSWO session for
described by Roane et al. (1998) except that Mason—he selected only four items.
each FO session was 2 min. During the 2-min
assessment periods, we provided free access to
the six assessed items and measured engage- RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
ment with each item. For Noah, we provided Figure 1 shows rates of problem behavior
free access to six plates with ample edible items during each preference assessment (left column
for him to consume. From the FO assessment, of panels). Noah (top row) engaged in low rates
we identified the longest duration of engage- of problem behavior during each assessment.
ment with a single item and yoked access dura- Brady (second row), Sofie (third row), and
tion during the subsequent PS and MSWO Mason (bottom row) engaged in higher rates of
assessment within each series of three sessions. problem behavior during the PS and MSWO
For example, if the longest duration of item assessments relative to the FO assessment (note
engagement during an FO assessment session the large differences in y-axis scaling across par-
was 1 min with the Barbie doll, then we pro- ticipants). These results are similar to those of
vided 1-min access to each item selected Kang et al. (2010, 2011) who showed increased
(including the Barbie doll) during the subse- problem behavior during FO assessments.
quent PS and MSWO assessments. We recalcu- We also evaluated problem behavior levels
lated these durations following each FO following the completion of each assessment
assessment, and these durations averaged 50 s and these data are displayed in the right col-
(range, 38 s to 68 s), 113.7 s (range, 110 s to umn of Figure 1. Noah engaged in low rates of
116 s), 93.7 s (range, 79 s to 109 s), and problem behaviors after each assessment (top
100.3 s (range, 85 s to 116 s) for Noah, Brady, row). Brady’s problem behavior was equally ele-
Sofie, and Mason, respectively. vated across all three assessment formats
4 SARA BETH TUNG et al.

2.5
3.0 During Preference Assessment 2.5 Following Item Removal
2.0 2.0 Noah
1.5
2.0 1.5
1.0 MSWO 1.0
0.5
1.0 0.5
0.0 0.0
-0.5
0.0 -0.5
11 4 4 7 7 10 10 1 4 7 10
FO
FO Brady
50
50
40 40
PS
Problem Behavior per Minute

30 30
20 20
10 10
0 0
-10 -10
1 4 7 1 4 7

20 Sofie
20
15
15
10 FO 10
FO
5 5
0 0
-5 1 4 7 -5 1 4 7

36 36 Mason
27 27
18 18
9 9
0 0

-9 1 4 7 -9 1 4 7

Sessions

Figure 1. Problem behavior per min during three types of preference assessments (left panel) and following the
removal of all items (right panel).

(second row). Sofie and Mason (third and bot- variable) rates were observed, however, follow-
tom row, respectively) engaged in problem ing the PS and MSWO assessments relative to
behavior following each type of preference the FO assessment.
assessment as well. These data suggest that The value of a preference assessment is deter-
although the FO assessment tends to minimize mined by its ability not only to minimize prob-
problem behavior during assessment sessions, lem behavior but also determine a preference
these assessments may still be associated with hierarchy. We present the reported preference
problem behavior at the conclusion of the ses- hierarchies determined by each assessment in
sion at rates equal to those associated with Table 1. There was a strong degree of corre-
other assessment formats. The highest (albeit spondence, particularly among the top ranked
PROBLEM BEHAVIOR DURING PREFERENCE ASSESSMENTS 5

Table 1 materials. One limitation we raised with Kang


Preference Hierarchies Across Each Assessment Type for et al. (2010) was that decreased problem behav-
Each Participant ior may have resulted from stimulus satiation
Item
rather than stimulus restriction or removal.
Participant Rank FO PS MSWO However, we observed increased problem
Noah 1 Popcorn Popcorn Popcorn behavior during PS and MSWO assessments,
2 Doritos Doritos Doritos even when equating stimulus access time across
3 Chips Crackers Crackers
4 Crackers Chips Chips
assessments, suggesting that the relevant evoca-
5 Pretzels Pretzels Pretzels tive event in the PS and MSWO assessments is
6 Potato Potato Potato the removal of items and not necessarily related
Sticks Sticks Sticks
Brady 1 iPad iPad Ball to deprivation from particular items.
2 Badge Ball iPad These collective findings suggest that the FO
3 Ball Badge Badge
4 NS Blanket Blanket assessment may be the most appropriate prefer-
5 NS Piano V-Tech ence assessment for individuals who engage in
6 NS V-Tech Piano
Sofie 1 Play Doh Play Doh Play Doh problem behavior maintained, at least in part,
2 DVD DVD DVD by access to tangible items. This assessment
3 Coloring Coloring Puzzle
4 Magazine Magazine Coloring
was associated with the least overall level of
5 NS Puzzle Magazine problem behavior and resulted in similar prefer-
6 NS Keys Keys ence hierarchies as the PS and MSWO. How-
Mason 1 DVD DVD DVD
2 Car Car Car ever, it should be noted that functional analyses
3 NS Ball Legos are not always conducted prior to preference
4 NS Book Book
5 NS Legos Ball assessments. Therefore, implementers may need
6 NS Sensory Ball Sensory Ball to select preference assessment formats based
Note. NS = not selected upon caregiver report of behavioral function
prior to conducting a functional analysis (Kang
et al., 2011). It is also possible that observing
items in each of the preference assessments problem behavior during a preference assess-
across participants. However, it is worth noting ment may be an indicator that a tangible condi-
that the FO assessment failed to identify a tion should be included in a functional
complete hierarchy for Brady, Sofie, and Mason analysis.
as they never selected some items. Similar An alternative process for determining rein-
results from an FO assessment were reported forcers for individuals who engage in problem
when activities were evaluated for adults with behavior maintained, at least in part, by access
disabilities (Hanley, Iwata, Lindberg, & Con- to tangible items may be to use pictorial-based
ners, 2003). The PS and MSWO assessments (Heinicke et al., 2016) or video-based (Clark,
always resulted in complete hierarchies, partly Donaldson, & Kahng, 2015) preference assess-
because they required participants to encounter ments. These may evoke fewer problem beha-
each stimulus. viors because highly preferred items are not
The results of the current study are similar removed from the participant during these
to those reported by Kang et al. (2010) in that assessments. The utility of pictorial-based or
we observed lower rates of problem behavior video-based preference assessments for indivi-
during the FO assessments when compared to duals who engage in problem behavior main-
the PS and MSWO assessments, presumably tained by access to tangible items requires
because the FO assessment did not involve fre- evaluation and thus remains an important area
quent removal or restriction of tangible of future research.
6 SARA BETH TUNG et al.

Identifying the minimum amount of time systematic choice assessment. American Journal on
necessary to empirically determine reinforcers Mental Retardation, 101, 15-25.
Fisher, W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G.,
would be highly valuable in future research as Hagopian, L. P., Owens, J. C., & Slevin, I. (1992). A
well. FO assessments are typically conducted in comparison of two approaches for identifying reinfor-
5-min or longer durations (Roane et al., 1998; cers for persons with severe and profound disabilities.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 491-498.
Rapp, Rojas, Colby-Dirksen, Swanson, & Mar- https://doi.org/10.1901%2Fjaba.1992.25-491
vin, 2010); however, this study used 2-min Hagopian, L. P., Long, E. S., & Rush, K. S. (2004). Pref-
assessment sessions, which produced similar erence assessment procedures for individuals with
results as the PS and MSWO assessments. developmental disabilities. Behavior Modification, 28,
668-677. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1177/
Thus, it is possible that FO assessments can be 0145445503259836
potentially more efficient than those reported Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., Lindberg, J., & Conners, J.
previously. Future research should also deter- (2003). Response-restriction analysis: I. Assessment
mine if the FO assessment would produce the of activity preferences. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 36, 47-58. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.
same results (i.e., similar preference hierarchies, 2003.36-47
low levels of problem behavior during assess- Heinicke, M. R., Carr, J. E., Pence, S. T., Zias, D. R.,
ments, and problem behavior immediately fol- Valentino, A. L., & Falligant, J. M. (2016). Assessing
the efficacy of pictorial preference assessments for
lowing the assessment) for problem behavior children with developmental disabilities. Journal of
maintained by reinforcers other than tangible Applied Behavior Analysis, 49, 848-868. https://doi.
items (e.g., attention and escape from org/10.1002/jaba.342
demands) and whether this assessment can be Kang, S., Lang, R. B., O’Reilly, M. F., Davis, T. N.,
Machalicek, W., Rispoli, M. J., & Chan J. M.
used to identify alternative reinforcers to facili- (2010). Problem behavior during preference assess-
tate tolerance for delayed reinforcement when ments: An empirical analysis and practical recommen-
reinforcers maintaining problem behavior are dations. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43, 137-
141. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2010.43-137
unavailable (Austin & Tiger, 2015).
Kang, S., O’Reilly, M. F., Fragale, C. L., Aguilar, J. M.,
Rispoli, M., & Lang, R. (2011). Evaluation of the
rate of problem behavior maintained by different
REFERENCES reinforcers across preference assessments. Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis, 44, 835-846. https://doi.
Austin, J. E., & Tiger, J. H. (2015). Providing alternative
org/10.1901%2Fjaba.2011.44-835
reinforcers to facilitate tolerance to delayed reinforce-
ment following functional communication training. Rapp, J. T., Rojas, N. C., Colby-Dirksen, A. M.,
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 48, 663-668. Swanson, G. J., & Marvin, K. L. (2010). Predicting
doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.215 preference for items during periods of extended access
Clark, D. R., Donaldson, J. M., & Kahng, S. (2015). Are based on early response allocation. Journal of Applied
video-based preference assessments without access to Behavior Analysis, 43, 473-486. http://doi.org/https://
selected stimuli effective? Journal of Applied Behavior doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2010.43-473
Analysis, 48, 895-900. doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/ Roane, H. S., Vollmer, T. R., Ringdahl, J. E., &
jaba.246 Marcus, B. A. (1998). Evaluation of a brief stimulus
DeLeon, I. G., & Iwata, B. A. (1996). Evaluation of a preference assessment. Journal of Applied Behavior
multiple-stimulus presentation format for assessing Analysis, 31, 605-620. https://doi.org/10.1901%
reinforcer preferences. Journal of Applied Behavior 2Fjaba.1998.31-605
Analysis, 29, 519-533. https://doi.org/10.1901%
2Fjaba.1996.29-519. Received March 28, 2016
Fisher, W. W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., & Final acceptance November 1, 2016
Amari, A. (1996). Integrating caregiver report with a Action Editor, Jeffrey Tiger

You might also like