Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Int J Adolesc Med Health 2015; 27(1): 85–91

Cameron Lister*, Ray M. Merrill, David Vance, Joshua H. West, P. Cougar Hall
and Benjamin T. Crookston

Predictors of peer victimization among Peruvian


adolescents in the young lives cohort
Abstract Keywords: adolescent health; Peru; victimization.

Background: Bully-victimization is a widespread public


DOI 10.1515/ijamh-2014-0012
health issue with significant negative effects on both Received February 25, 2014; accepted June 6, 2014; pre­viously pub-
social function and psychological well-being. Exist- lished online September 9, 2014
ing research in Peru shows high prevalence of bullying.
However, researchers have yet to fully understand the
phenomenon of victimization in developing regions.
Objectives: The purpose of this study was to characterize Introduction
victimization patterns over time, along with the predic-
tors of victimization from a cohort of Peruvian adolescents Bully-victimization is a widespread public health issue
enrolled in the young lives (YL) study. with significant negative effects on both social function
Materials and methods: This study examined data from and psychological wellbeing (1, 2). Children who are
YL, a longitudinal study of poverty, health, and develop- bullied are nearly twice as likely to have psychosomatic
ment, which examined data from the older cohort of chil- problems compared with peers who are not bullied (3).
dren in Peru across three rounds (ages 8, 12, and 15 years). Further, victimization is also predictive of internalizing
The sample consisted of 714 children from 74 communities disorders (e.g., sadness, anxiety, etc.) (4). Victims are
that represent 20 districts in Peru. more likely than their peers to suffer from anxiety disor-
Results: After adjusting for urban/rural setting, there ders later in life (5) and to feel abandoned, lonely, or sui-
remained a significantly lower wealth index for children cidal (6). Evidence indicates depressive symptoms may
who were bullied at ages 8 and 12 years. Exploratory anal- last for years after bullying has ended (7).
ysis showed that although those in the lowest quartile of Previous research has also found internalizing symp-
body mass index (BMI) were significantly more likely to be toms to be predictors of future bully-victimization (4).
bullied at age 8 years, this association waned over time. Students who are victimized often have trouble interact-
A worse caregiver assessment of child’s health compared ing with other students and struggle with anxiety (8, 9).
with others was associated with a significantly greater risk Other studies have shown that bullies tend to victimize
of bully-victimization. At age 8 years, caregiver education children who lack the skills to fight back or stand up for
was significantly lower among those bullied compared themselves (10, 11), thriving on an imbalance of power
with those who were not bullied. (12, 13). In addition, more males are involved in bullying
Conclusion: This study showed several factors as the pre- and victimization than females (14).
dictors of victimization in the early years, including being Many studies have focused on measuring bullying
male and having low BMI, low socioeconomic status, and and victimization over time, with the aim of longitudinally
low parental/caregiver education. Further longitudinal establishing rates and trends. Longitudinal studies have
studies should be conducted to determine the extent to shown that victimization decreases over time as children
which these predictors vary in significance over time. transition from primary to secondary school (15). This
has been established in multiple settings, including the
US (16, 17), and European countries (18). Approximately
10% to 30% of youth in these regions report either being
*Corresponding author: Cameron Lister, Department of Health a bully, a victim, or both (2, 14, 17–19). Yet, few of these
Science, Brigham Young University, 213 Richards Building, Provo,
longitudinal studies have been conducted in developing
UT 84602, USA, E-mail: camearlister@gmail.com
Ray M. Merrill, David Vance, Joshua H. West, P. Cougar Hall and
countries and resource-poor regions, leaving a dearth of
Benjamin T. Crookston: Department of Health Science, Brigham data on trends and predictors of bullying and victimiza-
Young University, Provo, UT, USA tion in these parts of the world.

Brought to you by | University of New South Wales


Authenticated
Download Date | 8/20/15 10:14 PM
86      Lister et al.: Predictors of peer victimization

The existing research in Peru shows high prevalence SD = 3.8), and primary caregiver (M = 7.1, SD =4.3). Grade level was
of bullying, with approximately half of Peruvian teens measured as none (0), individual grades (1 to 11), some technical
college (12), completed technical college (13), some university (14),
being involved in some form of bullying (20–23). However,
and completed university (15). Caregivers were asked to evaluate the
these studies are largely cross-sectional and do not allow children’s health at ages 8 and 12 years as the same, better, or worse
for examining changes in victimization patterns over the than other children of a similar age. A wealth index ranging from 0 to
course of childhood and adolescence. The purpose of this 1 was constructed using household information on consumer dura-
study was to characterize victimization patterns over time bles, housing quality, and services. The higher the index score, the
wealthier a household was considered. A social ladder question was
along with the predictors of victimization from a cohort
also used to determine the child’s and caregiver’s perception of qual-
of Peruvian adolescents enrolled in the young lives (YL) ity of life. The child or caregiver was asked: “There are nine steps
study. on this ladder. Suppose we say that the ninth step, at the very top,
represents the best possible life for you and the bottom represents
the worst possible life for you. Where on the ladder do you feel you
personally stand at the present time”?

Materials and methods


Data analysis
Study design and background
Means and percentages were used to summarize and describe the
This study examines data from YL, a study of poverty, health, and data. Independent group means were compared for equality using
development in a total of 12,000 children living in countries that the t statistic. Regression analysis was used to evaluate the asso-
include Peru, Ethiopia, India, and Vietnam. Each country sample ciation between grade level and victimization. Trends in the wealth
is comprised of two cohorts; an older cohort beginning among chil- index were compared by the victimization variables. These were then
dren aged 8 years, and a younger cohort of children enrolled in 2002 assessed using Wilks’ lambda. Trends were also assessed using the
around 1 year after birth. To date, YL participants have been assessed Mantel-Haenszel χ2-test for trends. The McNemar test was used to
three times at approximately 4-year intervals. evaluate whether there was a significant relationship between being
For the purposes of studying bullying in Latin America, this bullied at age 8 years and at age 15 years. Two-sided tests of hypoth-
study only examined data from the older cohort of children in Peru. eses were evaluated using a p < 0.05 level of significance. Analyses
The older cohort consists of a total of 714 children from 74 communi- were performed using the Statistical Analysis System (SAS) software,
ties, representing 20 districts in Peru. Children and their caregivers version 9.3 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA, 2010).
were surveyed at enrollment (round 1) and again at ages 12 (round 2)
and 15 (round 3) years. Detailed information on YL methods for sam-
pling, recruitment, and interviewing can be found at http://www.
younglives.org.uk and www.ninosdelmilenio.org. Approval for this
Results
study was received by the institutional review board at a Brigham
Young University.
Participants

Of 714 children in this study (54% male and 46% female),


Measures
47% of the 8-year-olds were reported by the primary car-
egiver to have been bullied. At ages 12 and 15 years, 32%
Measures of victimization: First round variables included caregiver
report of child being bullied (“Is NAME picked on or bullied by other and 22% of children reported being bullied by other chil-
children”?); the second round included both the caregiver report and dren, respectively. There was no statistically significant
child report of being bullied (“Pupils in my class never tease me at difference in the percentage of males and females bullied
school” and “Has NAME ever been bullied by peers”?); and the third at ages 8 or 15 years, although at both ages more males
round included a child report of being bullied (“Other pupils in my
reported being bullied. However, a significant difference
class tease me at school”). Children who were reported to have been
was found at age 12, with significantly more males being
bullied at rounds 1 or 2 through the self-report or caregiver report,
but did not report being bullied at a subsequent round (rounds 2 or 3) bullied than females (38% vs. 25%, 13.27, p = 0.0003).
were characterized as outgrowing victimization. Children and adults Approximately 5% of children were victimized throughout
were interviewed in their preferred language. childhood and adolescence (victimized at all three ages)
while 21% were victimized at two of the ages surveyed,
Child and household characteristics: Child measures included and 42% were victimized at one of the ages surveyed.
sex, age, health, and body mass index (BMI) of child. Household
Nearly one-third (32%) never reported being victimized.
indicators included wealth index, and caregiver education, includ-
ing maternal, paternal, and other caregiver educational attainment
Information about bully-victimization at both ages 8 and
levels. Measures of the highest educational attainment level were 15  years was available for 680 children. There was a posi-
obtained from the child’s mother (M = 7.3, SD =4.2), father (M = 8.8, tive association between victimization status at ages 8 and

Brought to you by | University of New South Wales


Authenticated
Download Date | 8/20/15 10:14 PM
Lister et al.: Predictors of peer victimization      87

Table 1 Victimization status (Yes/No) at ages 8 and 15 years, according to selected variables.

  No/No   Yes/No   No/Yes   Yes/Yes   F Statistic


294 (43.2%) 237 (34.9%) 64 (9.4%) 85 (12.5%) p-valuea

  Mean   Mean   Mean   Mean  


Wealth index   0.60   0.55   0.65   0.59   0.0006
Body mass index   21.0   21.1   21.4   21.2   0.8440
Caregiver educationb   7.6   6.3   8.1   6.9   0.0020
Social ladder   5.9   6.0   6.2   6.0   0.8576
  %   %   %   %   χ2
          p-value
Perceived health as worse than   3.8   6.4   4.8   11.9   0.0087
other children their age

Note that the wealth index and BMI were available at age 15 years. Perception of their place on the social ladder (1 low and 9 high) and
health compared with other children their age was available at both ages 12 (parental report and child report). The education of the car-
egiver was recorded at age 12.
a
Based on type III sums of squares.
b
Caregivers education was measured as none (0), grades 1–11, some technical college (12), completed technical college (13), some univer-
sity (14), and completed university (15).

15 years (McNemar’s test = 99.43, p < 0.0001). The combination index and caregiver education. The wealth index was not
of victimization at ages 8 and 15 years and how these com- significantly associated with sex, but was significantly
binations relate to selected variables are shown in Table 1. lower among those bullied at ages 8 and 12, but not age 15
Those bullied at both ages 8 and 15 years were more likely to (Table 2). Bully-victimization was more common in rural
perceive their health as worse than other children their age. areas at ages 8 (55% vs. 45%, p = 0.0136) and 12 (38% vs.
29.5%, χ2 p = 0.0435), but less common in rural areas at age
15 (15% vs. 24%, χ2 p = 0.0100). After adjusting for urban/
Wealth index rural setting, there remained a significantly lower wealth
index for children who were bullied at ages 8 and 12 years.
An increasing trend in the wealth index was observed, from
ages 8 (M = 0.46, SD = 0.19) to 12  years (M = 0.52, SD = 0.23),
and on to the age of 15  years (M = 0.59, SD = 0.19) (Wilks’ Body mass index (BMI) and health status of
lambda F = 2 82.9, p < 0.0001). On the one hand, those not children
bullied at the age of 8 years, but bullied when they were
15 years old had the highest wealth index and highest car- The increasing trend in BMI from ages 8 (M = 16.8, SD = 1.8)
egiver level of education (wealth index was measured at to 12 years (M = 19.1, SD = 3.0), and then to age 15 (M = 21.1,
age 15 years, caregiver education at age age 12 years). On SD = 3.0) (Wilks’ lambda p < 0.0001), did not significantly
the other hand, those bullied at the age of 8 years, but not vary according to victimization status, with both victims
bullied when they were 15 years old had the lowest wealth and non-victims growing at comparable rates (Wilks’

Table 2 Mean wealth index according to bullying status at ages 8, 12, and 15 years.

  No   %   Mean   Standard   F statistic   Adjusted mean for   Adjusted F statistic


deviation p-value urban/rural setting p-value

Age 8              
 Bullied   338   47.3   0.42   0.17    < 0.0001   0.37    < 0.0001
 Not bullied   376   52.7   0.49   0.19     0.43  
Age 12              
 Bullied   217   31.6   0.43   0.18   0.0226   0.41   0.0024
 Not bullied   470   68.4   0.47   0.18     0.46  
Age 15              
 Bullied   149   21.9   0.48   0.17   0.1115   0.54   0.3431
 Not bullied   531   78.1   0.45   0.19     0.53  

Brought to you by | University of New South Wales


Authenticated
Download Date | 8/20/15 10:14 PM
88      Lister et al.: Predictors of peer victimization

lambda p = 0.4827). Exploratory analysis showed that Table 5 Mean perceived place on the ladder according to bullying
those in the lowest quartile of BMI were significantly more status at the same age.

likely to be bullied at age 8 years. Those in the lowest


  Mean   Standard   F statistic   Adjusted   Adjusted f
quartile of BMI continued to show increased risk of vic-
deviation p-value mean for statistic
timization at ages 12 and 15 years, albeit at a lower level. urban/rural p-value
Caregivers were asked to evaluate the children’s setting
health conditions at the ages of 8 and 12  years as the
Child perception of place on the ladder (age 12)
same, better, or worse than other children of a similar age  Bullied   5.5   2.23    < 0.0001   5.5    < 0.0001
(Table 3). When the caregiver evaluated the child’s health  Not Bullied  6.2   1.88     6.2  
compared with others, a worse assessment was associated Parent perception of place on the ladder (age 12)
with a significantly greater risk of bully-victimization.  Bullied   4.5   2.04   0.0065   4.5   0.0085
 Not Bullied  5.0   2.03     5.0  
Child perception of place on the ladder (age 15)
 Bullied   6.0   1.77   0.5087   5.5    < 0.0001
Caregiver education

At age 8 years, caregiver education was significantly lower


bullied (Table 4). At the age of 12, there continued to be a
among those bullied compared with those who were not
greater risk of victimization with lower caregiver educa-
tion. However, when a child reaches the age of 15 years,
Table 3 Bullying according to personal health. caregiver education was no longer significantly associated
with victimization.
  Bullied   Not bullied  

Perceived health compared   No.   %   No.   %   χ2


with other children p-value Social ladder
Age 8 yearsa          
 Same   184   54.4   225   59.8   0.0086 Children who were victimized when they were 12 years
 Better   106   31.4   124   33.0   old viewed themselves as being significantly lower on
 Worse   48   14.2   27   7.2  
the social ladder than their peers (Table 5). However, by
Age 12 yearsa          
 Same   135   62.5   292   62.5    < 0.0001 the time they reached 15 years, there was no longer a
 Better   52   24.1   154   33.0   significant difference between victims and non-victims.
 Worse   29   13.4   21   4.5   Adjusting for urban/rural setting did not affect the
Age 12 yearsb           results.
 Same   130   60.2   297   63.6   0.1924
 Better   69   31.9   149   31.9  
 Worse   17   7.9   21   4.5  
Age 15 yearsb
 Same

  82

  54.3

  284

  54.8

  0.7325
Discussion
 Better   62   41.1   217   41.9  
 Worse   7   4.6   17   3.3   The current study focused primarily on demographic
predictors of victimization. A number of demographic
Note: because of missing data, not all numbers for a given age
and physical factors were found to be predictive of vic-
group sum to 714.
a
Question asked to caretaker. timization, although the strength of some associations
b
Question asked to child. waned with increasing age. Overall, victimization stead-
ily decreased with age from 47% at age 8 years, to around
Table 4 Bullying according to the education level of the mother, 20% at age 15 years. Males were more likely to be bullied
father, or caregiver.
than females. This is similar to other studies, which
reported that bullying decreases with age (14, 15), and
  Mother’s education   Father’s education
that males are more prone to bully and to be victimized
Bullied   Slope (SD)   T Statistic   Slope (SD)   T Statistic (15, 24). Those from the poorest households were more
p-value p-value
likely to be bullied at younger ages, and were also more
Age 8   –1.19 (0.32)   0.0002   –0.72 (0.20)   0.0143 likely to have parents/caregivers with low educational
Age 12   –1.52 (0.35)    < 0.0001   –1.31 (0.31)    < 0.0001 attainment. Additionally, those with a lower BMI were at
Age 15   0.26 (0.39)   0.5011   0.28 (0.35)   0.4252
higher risk of victimization.

Brought to you by | University of New South Wales


Authenticated
Download Date | 8/20/15 10:14 PM
Lister et al.: Predictors of peer victimization      89

Wealth or separated parents) is also predictive of victimization


(28), in addition to a poor family environment, or frequent
In the study population, victims had a lower average marital and/or family disputes (14). Considering the sig-
wealth index than peers at ages 8 and 12 years. In addition, nificant link between educational status and SES, it could
a lower wealth index was significantly correlated with vic- be assumed that wealth and educational status are com-
timization at age 8 years. Given that childhood exposure pounding variables in regards to risk of victimization.
to poverty is often associated with higher risk of stunt-
ing (25), this higher risk of victimization among youth of
lower socioeconomic status (SES) might be explained by BMI and health status
nutritional deficiencies, which in turn, give adolescents
smaller frames and mark them as easier targets for victim- Results indicated that children with lower BMI were at
ization. In addition, disadvantaged youths might be more higher risk of victimization at age 8 years, as has been
socially ostracized than their wealthier classmates, and demonstrated in other studies (29, 30). It is possible that
therefore, are more likely to be victimized. their smaller size marks these children as easier targets
However, by age 15 years, the wealth index of victims for bullying. However, in the current study, the predictive
was higher than that of non-victims, and a lower position power of low BMI declined between the ages of 12 and 15
on the wealth index was no longer significantly corre- years, which could be attributed to the fact that smaller
lated with victimization. One possible explanation could children develop coping mechanisms, which enable them
be that victimized students who were low on the wealth to better avoid being victimized by bullies. An alternative
index experienced an increase in wealth, which in turn, explanation is that the general increases in wealth index
raised the average wealth index of victims. An alternative and BMI over the course of the study raised part of the
explanation is that, given that income inequality has been lowest quartile out of malnourishment, thus decreasing
shown to be a predictor of increased levels of bully-victim- their risk of victimization. However, previous studies in
ization (11, 24, 26), it is possible that the overall increase other countries have shown a correlation between being
in wealth over the course of the study helped alleviate obese or overweight and higher likelihood of being victim-
some of the socioeconomic contributors to victimization. ized (28, 29, 31).
Finally, it could be possible that less wealthy adolescents A low caregiver assessment of child health was also
were able to “outgrow” the factors that caused their vic- significantly associated with victimization. It is possible
timization. Further studies are needed to determine that less-healthy kids are bullied more if their poor health
whether such “outgrowing” is possible, especially given makes them easier targets for bullies. Many studies have
that an international study of 162,305 European and North researched this correlation, showing that those who
American students reported that low SES was signifi- are victims are more likely to be physically weaker than
cantly correlated with bullying victimization across three their peers (9, 32). It is also possible that victimization
age groups (ages 11, 13, and 15 years), with no indication adversely affects child health, making poor health an
of victimization outgrowth (27). outcome of bullying rather than a risk-factor. Hodges and
Perry showed that victimization may be a self-sustaining
peer rejection cycle, suggesting that factors that cause or
Caregiver education precede victimization may also be consequences of bully-
ing (32).
Children of caregivers with lower education levels were
found to be more likely to experience being bullied at
ages 8 and 12 years. However, the current study demon- Limitations
strated a decreasing predictive strength of parental or
caregiver education as a predictor of victimization at later This study has several limitations. First, the YL study is
ages. This is an interesting and important finding given focused primarily on childhood poverty, not on bullying
that an international study of children and adolescents and victimization. Hence, questions related to victimiza-
found no significant link between parental education tion varied across the three rounds of surveys. Second, it
and victimization (27). However, other studies have found is left to the parents and adolescents to designate whether
a link between students who come from families with a behavior occurred based on their own definitions of “bul-
low SES and child victimization (11, 24, 26). Research has lying” or being “victimized”. In general, that is, in media
also shown that having a non-intact family (i.e., divorced and school surveys, there is a weakness in the terminology

Brought to you by | University of New South Wales


Authenticated
Download Date | 8/20/15 10:14 PM
90      Lister et al.: Predictors of peer victimization

or in the concept of either bullying or victimization. Fur- 5. Sourander A, Elonheimo H, Niemelä S, Nuutila A-M, Helenius H,
thermore, this study is limited in not having standard- et al. Childhood predictors of male criminality: a prospective
population-based follow-up study from age 8 to late adoles-
ized diagnoses available on the youth group studied. The
cence. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2006;45:578–86.
rating scale approach offers useful information given the 6. Roberts Jr. WB, Coursol DH. Strategies for intervention with child-
rather large number of subjects in the present study, but hood and adolescent victims of bullying, teasing, and intimida-
lacks the specificity and additional depth that structured tion in school settings. Elem Sch Guid Couns 1996;30:204–12.
or semi-structured psychiatric interviews might provide. 7. Olweus D. Bullying at school: what we know and what we can
do. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers, USA, 1993.
8. Arseneault L, Walsh E, Trzesniewski K, Newcombe R, Caspi A,
et al. Bullying victimization uniquely contributes to adjustment
Conclusion problems in young children: a nationally representative cohort
study. Pediatr 2006;118:130–8.
9. Olweus D. Bullying or peer abuse at school: facts and interven-
Bully-victimization is a trend that has both short- and tion. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 1995;4:196–200.
long-term negative consequences on child and adolescent 10. Egan SK, Perry DG. Does low self-regard invite victimization?
social and psychological wellbeing. Victimization preva- Dev Psychol 1998;34:299.
lence has been shown to be high among Peruvian children 11. Arseneault L, Bowes L, Shakoor S. Bullying victimization in
youths and mental health problems: “Much ado about noth-
and adolescents, thus requiring national attention and
ing”? Psychol Med 2010;40:717–29.
evidence-based interventions to reduce the occurrences 12. Frisén A, Holmqvist K, Oscarsson D. 13-year-olds’ perception of
and impacts of bullying. This study showed several factors bullying: definitions, reasons for victimisation and experience
that serve as the predictors of victimization in the early of adults’ response. Educ Stud 2008;34:105–17.
years, including being male as well as having low BMI, 13. Maunder RE, Harrop A, Tattersall AJ. Pupil and staff perceptions
low SES, and low parental/caregiver education. Further of bullying in secondary schools: comparing behavioural defini-
tions and their perceived seriousness. Educ Res 2010;52:
longitudinal studies should be conducted to determine
263–82.
the extent to which these predictors vary in significance 14. Cook CR, Williams KR, Guerra NG, Kim TE, Sadek S. Predictors
over time. of bullying and victimization in childhood and adolescence: A
meta-analytic investigation. Sch Psychol Q 2010;25:65–83.
Acknowledgments: The data used in this study come from 15. Pellegrini AD, Long JD. A longitudinal study of bullying, dominance,
and victimization during the transition from primary school through
Young Lives, a 15-year survey investigating the changing
secondary school. Br J Dev Psychol 2002;20:259.
nature of childhood poverty in Ethiopia, India (Andhra 16. Smith PK, Cowie H, Olafsson RF, Liefooghe APD. Definitions of
Pradesh), Peru and Vietnam (www.younglives.org.uk). bullying: a comparison of terms used, and age and gender dif-
Young Lives is core-funded by UK aid from the Depart- ferences, in a fourteen-country international comparison. Child
ment for International Development (DFID) and co- Dev 2002;73:1119–33.
funded from 2010 to 2014 by the Netherlands Ministry of 17. Nansel TR, Overpeck M, Pilla RS, Ruan WJ, Simons-Morton B, et al.
Bullying behaviors among US youth: prevalence and association
Foreign Affairs. The findings and conclusions contained
with psychosocial adjustment. J Am Med Assoc 2001;285:2094.
within are those of the authors and are not necessarily 18. Analitis F, Velderman MK, Ravens-Sieberer U, Detmar S,
those of Young Lives, the University of Oxford, DFID or Erhart M, et al. Being bullied: associated factors in children and
other funders. adolescents 8 to 18 years old in 11 european countries. Pediatr
2009;123:569–77.
19. Zimmerman FJ, Glew GM, Christakis DA, Katon W. Early cognitive
stimulation, emotional support, and television watching as
References predictors of subsequent bullying among grade-school children.
Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2005;159:384–8.
1. Kaltiala-Heino R, RimpelÄ M, Rantanen P, RimpelÄ A. Bullying at 20. Amemiya I, Oliveros M, Barrientos A. Factores de riesgo de
school: an indicator of adolescents at risk for mental disorders. violencia escolar severa en colegios privados de tres zonas de
J Adolesc 2000;23:661–74. la sierra del Perú. An la Fac Med 2009;70:255–8.
2. Lien L, Green K, Welander-Vatn A, Bjertness E. Mental and somatic 21. Donohue MO, Achata AB. Incidencia y factores de riesgo de la
health complaints associated with school bullying between 10th intimidación (bullying) en un colegio particular de Lima-Perú,
and 12th grade students; results from cross sectional studies in 2007. Rev Peru Pediatr 2007;60:150–5.
Oslo, Norway. Clin Pract Epidemiol Ment Heal 2009;5:6. 22. Oliveros M, Figueroa L, Mayorga G, Cano G, Quispe Y,
3. Gini G, Pozzoli T. Association between bullying and psychoso- ­Barrientos A. Intimidación en colegios estatales de secundaria
matic problems: a meta-analysis. Pediatr 2009;123:1059–65. del Perú. Rev Peru Pediatr 2009;62:68–78.
4. Reijntjes A, Kamphuis JH, Prinzie P, Telch MJ. Peer victimization 23. Oliveros MD, Figueroa A, Mayorga GR, Cano BU, Quispe YA,
and internalizing problems in children: a meta-analysis of longi- et al. Violencia escolar (bullying) en colegios estatales de prima-
tudinal studies. Child Abuse Negl 2010;34:244–52. ria en el Perú. Rev Peru Pediatr. 2008;61:215–20.

Brought to you by | University of New South Wales


Authenticated
Download Date | 8/20/15 10:14 PM
Lister et al.: Predictors of peer victimization      91

24. Wolke D, Woods S, Stanford K, Schulz H. Bullying and 28. Kim YS, Boyce WT, Koh Y-J, Leventhal BL. Time trends, trajec-
­victimization of primary school children in England and tories, and demographic predictors of bullying: a prospective
Germany: Prevalence and school factors. Br J Psychol study in Korean adolescents. J Adolesc Heal 2009;45:360–7.
2001;92:673. 29. Puhl R, Luedicke J. Weight-based victimization among adoles-
25. Adair LS, Guilkey DK. Age-specific determinants of stunting in cents in the school setting: Emotional reactions and coping
Filipino children. J Nutr 1997;127:314–20. behavior. J Youth Adolesc 2012;41:24–40.
26. Barker ED, Boivin M, Brendgen M, Fontaine N, Arseneault L, 30. Wang J, Ianotti RJ, Luk JW. Bullying victimization among under-
et al. Predictive validity and early predictors of peer-vic- weight and overweight U.S. youth: differential associations for
timization trajectories in preschool. Arch Gen Psychiatry boys and girls. J Adolesc Heal 2010;47:99–101.
2008;65:1185–92. 31. Puhl RM, Latner JD. Stigma, obesity, and the health of the
27. Due P, Merlo J, Harel-Fisch Y, Damsgaard MT, Holstein BE, et al. nation’ s children. Psychol Bull 2007;133:557–80.
Socioeconomic inequality in exposure to bullying during ado- 32. Hodges EVE, Perry DG. Personal and interpersonal antecedents
lescence: a comparative, cross-sectional, multilevel study in 35 and consequences of victimization by peers. J Pers Soc Psychol
countries. Am J Public Health 2009;99:907–14. 1999;76:677–85.

Brought to you by | University of New South Wales


Authenticated
Download Date | 8/20/15 10:14 PM

You might also like