Neurodevelopment and Psychiatric Down Syndrome

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

Review article 95

Neurodevelopmental and psychiatric issues in Down’s


syndrome: assessment and intervention
Stefano Vicaria, Maria Pontilloa and Marco Armandoa,b,c

Down’s syndrome (DS) is the most frequent genetic cause They also frequently show psychiatric disorders such
of intellectual disability and patients with DS show as externalizing disorders as well as depression, anxiety
significant psychopathology (18–23%). Moreover, and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Nevertheless,
individuals with DS often show a cognitive decline as for other genetic syndrome with intellectual disability,
associated with ageing characterized by a deterioration in there is a significant lack of research specifically
memory, language and cognitive functioning. According focused on treatments of psychiatric and behavioural
to these relevant findings, an overview is presented problems in DS. This is true both for psychosocial
of state-of-the-art knowledge of the neurocognitive, and for pharmacological interventions. Psychiatr Genet
neurobiological and psychopathological profile, 23:95–107 c 2013 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott
assessment and treatment of patients with DS. Williams & Wilkins.
The linguistic characteristics of DS develop differently Psychiatric Genetics 2013, 23:95–107
along distinct developmental trajectories. Thus, for
example, morphosyntax deficit, especially in production, Keywords: assessment, Down’s syndrome, intellectual disability,
neurodevelopment, psychiatric disorders, treatment
is more evident in adolescence than in early childhood and
a
lexicon is usually better preserved in all ages (at least in Department of Neuroscience, Child and Adolescence Psychiatry Unit,
Children Hospital Bambino Gesù, bDepartment of Psychiatry,
comprehension). So far, rehabilitation is the only effective PhD School ‘Early Intervention in Psychosis’, ‘Sapienza’ University, Rome,
approach for improving cognitive and linguistic abilities. Italy and cDepartment of Environmental Science, School of Psychology,
University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham, UK
However, ongoing preliminary reports on other approaches
such as transmagnetic stimulation or drugs suggest Correspondence to Marco Armando, MD, Department of Neuroscience,
Child and Adolescence Psychiatry Unit, Children Hospital Bambino Gesù,
alternative or integrative treatment for the future. Piazza Sant’Onofrio 4, I-00165 Roma, Italy
Individuals with DS show typical organization of brain Tel: + 390 06 68592475; fax: + 390 06 68592450;
e-mail: marco.armando@opbg.net
structures related to some cognitive abilities, such as
reduced volume in frontal and prefrontal areas, which is Received 2 March 2012 Revised 8 October 2012 Accepted 27 October 2012
related to poor executive and linguistic abilities.

Introduction children, IQ is not constant across the lifespan, but


Down’s syndrome (DS), or trisomy 21, is caused by the decreases progressively with age (Pennington et al., 2003).
presence of an extra chromosome 21. Standard trisomy 21, Moreover, in adults with DS, IQ can be influenced by the
characterized by the presence of a free-standing extra increased risk of early-onset dementia of the Alzheimer
chromosome 21, occurs in about 95% of cases. Approxi- type, which is often reported in this syndrome (Bush and
mately 1% of individuals with DS have a mosaicism with Beail, 2004; Lott and Dierssen, 2010).
both normal and trisomic cells. In the remaining 5% of
cases, the trisomy is related to a chromosome translocation. Compared with other groups of children and adolescents
with ID, children with DS are at a lower risk for
The clinical phenotype of individuals with DS includes significant psychopathology (Dykens and Kasari,
unique facial characteristics: a flat occiput, round cheeks, 1997; Stores et al., 1998; Dykens, 2000). In fact, 30–40%
epicanthal folds and upslanting palpebral fissures, low of children with ID show significant psychopathology
nasal bridge and a thick and a protruding tongue, which (Reiss, 1990; Einfeld and Tonge, 1996; Coe et al., 1999)
allows the diagnosis of the syndrome in newborns. compared with 18–23% of children with DS (Gath and
Gumley, 1986; Myers and Pueschel, 1991; Dykens, 2000).
The incidence of DS is about one child in 1000 live births
Nevertheless, this prevalence is higher than that in the
(Cocchi et al., 2010); indeed, it is the most frequent
general population of children and adolescents, which
genetic cause of intellectual disability (ID).
ranges between 8 and 18% (Patel et al., 2007). Therefore,
Although a few individuals with DS have been reported to psychopathology in this special population should be
have an intelligence quotient (IQ) in the normal range considered a major topic in psychiatry.
(Epstein, 1989), IQ is usually in the moderately to severely
retarded range (IQ = 25–55) and mental age is rarely above In summary, DS, compared with other genetic syndromes
8 years (Gibson, 1978). It should also be noted that in with ID and the general population, shows specific
individuals with DS, differently from typically developing neurodevelopmental, neurocognitive and psychopathological
0955-8829
c 2013 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins DOI: 10.1097/YPG.0b013e32835fe426

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
96 Psychiatric Genetics 2013, Vol 23 No 3

patterns that should be well understood and taken into communicative-linguistic development has been reported
account in clinical practice. extensively (Franco and Wishart, 1995; Singer Harris et al.,
1997; Caselli et al., 1998; Iverson et al., 2003), thus
Therefore, the purpose of this review is to provide an
suggesting preserved communicative capacities in DS
overview of the recent literature aiming: (a) to describe
children in front of their generalized verbal linguistic
the similarities and differences in the cognitive and
impairment.
functional developmental trajectory and age-related
comorbidities over the lifespan of individuals with DS All these findings seem to suggest that in children with
compared with those with other genetically determined DS, linguistic characteristics may develop differently
neurodevelopmental syndromes and the general popula- along distinct developmental trajectories. In other words,
tion; (b) to consider mechanisms for, and the implications the linguistic skills of adolescents with DS cannot be
of, such differences and to identify gaps in knowledge; predicted directly by the pattern shown at younger ages.
and (c) to evaluate the role of, and evidence for,
educative, rehabilitative, behavioural and pharmacological Visual–spatial abilities are less impaired than language in
interventions. DS (Vicari, 2006; Frenkel et al., 2009). However, a
discrepancy within the visuospatial domain has been
For clarity, the paper is divided into three main reported. Indeed, individuals with DS performed
paragraphs. The first will attempt to address the significantly worse than mental age TD matched in tasks
previously stated aims of the neurocognitive and neuro- of visual perceptual and visual imagery, but the perfor-
developmental profiles of DS, whereas the second will mance of the two groups did not differ significantly
be focused on psychiatric issues. The main gaps in when the tasks involved the processing of spatial data
knowledge and the future directions in terms of research (Vicari et al., 2006). In other words, DS individuals
and clinical practice will be highlighted in the third showed reduced visual but relatively preserved spatial
paragraph. abilities.
Individuals with DS also show different levels of
Neurocognitive profiles impairment in learning and memory. Specifically, impair-
Cognitive deficits asymmetry and ageing ment in verbal short-term and working memory has been
DS is characterized by an uneven cognitive profile that documented extensively (Wang and Bellugi, 1994; Jarrold
consists of clear weaknesses in language, verbal short- and Baddeley, 1997; Jarrold et al., 1999; Vicari and
term memory and explicit long-term memory but Carlesimo, 2002; Brock and Jarrold, 2004; Lanfranchi
relatively preserved in visuospatial abilities, associative et al., 2004; Vicari et al., 2004, 2006).
learning and implicit long-term memory (Vicari et al., Explicit and implicit long-term memory has been
2004; Lott and Dierssen, 2010). investigated in DS (for a review, see Vicari and Carlesimo,
Impaired language development of children with DS is 2002). Explicit memory involves the intentional recall or
evident when their performances are compared with recognition of experiences or information. Implicit
those of typically developing (TD) peers of the same memory is manifested as facilitation (i.e. improvement
mental age but also with those of individuals with ID of in performance) on perceptual, cognitive and motor tasks,
different aetiologies, such as Williams syndrome (Wang without any conscious reference to previous experiences.
and Bellugi, 1994; Klein and Mervis, 1999; Mervis and Although explicit memory deficits have been documen-
Robinson, 2000; Vicari et al., 2000). Studies investigating ted extensively in both the verbal and the visual–spatial
the different components of linguistic abilities in DS domain, relative preservation of implicit memory has
have pointed out marked difficulty in the area of verbal been reported in individuals with DS (Carlesimo et al.,
production and comprehension (Rondal et al., 1988; Miller, 1997; Vicari et al., 2000, 2001, 2007).
1992; Rondal, 1993; Chapman, 1995; Vicari et al.,
Individuals with DS often show a cognitive decline
2000; Naess et al., 2012). However, early linguistic
associated with ageing characterized by a deterioration in
development in DS infants presents some surprises and
memory, language and cognitive functioning that resem-
a much less even pattern with production and lexical
bles Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Already in 1876, referring
comprehension abilities that are comparable to those of
to individuals with DS, Fraser and Michell noted that: ‘in
TD children of the same mental age (Cromer, 1987;
not a few instances, however, death was attributed
Fowler, 1990). Thus, with increasing age, the split
nothing more than general decay-sort of precipitated
between lexical and morphosyntactic abilities decreases
senility’. There are well established and recognized
and a generalized picture of linguistic difficulty emerges
neuropathological and neurochemical links between DS
(Fowler, 1990; Miller, 1992; Fabbretti et al., 1997; Vicari
and AD, with both associated with chromosome 21
et al., 2004).
(Wisniewski et al., 1985). Several genes are implicated
Finally, a more extensive use of communicative gestures in AD-like neurodegenerative mechanisms in individuals
in DS infants than in TD infants at the same stage of with DS. These include Cu/Zn superoxide dismutase 1,

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Neurodevelopmental and psychiatric issues in DS Vicari et al. 97

Ets-2 transcription factors, Down’s syndrome critical Assessment


region 1 stress-inducible factor and the amyloid precursor Most of the neuropsychological assessments available for
protein (APP) gene (Tanzi et al., 1987; Lott et al., 2006). the general populations have not been properly validated
Endoproteolytic cleavage of APP yields the pathogenic in children with ID. Special challenges are associated
amyloid-b (Ab) peptides that progressively accumulate in with testing individuals who have intellectual and
the brain as the diffuse and neuritic plaques of AD as developmental disorders, including considerable floor
confirmed recently in a PET study by Landt et al. (2011). effects and the confounding effects of impaired language
Overexpression of APP in the obligate region for DS is and attention. Very recently, Edgin (2010) specifically
associated with abundant Ab plaques and tangles developed the Arizona Cognitive Test Battery (ACTB) to
consistent with Braak stage V–VI. Intraneuronal Abeta assess the cognitive phenotype in DS. The ACTB
in DS appears to trigger a pathological cascade leading to includes tasks with specific correlates with brain function
oxidative stress and a neurodegeneration typical of AD. including tests of general cognitive ability and prefrontal,
There are suggestions that an increase in subcellular hippocampal and cerebellar function. The neuropsycho-
processing of APP and factors related to membrane APP logical assessments were chosen to assess a range of skills
cleavage favours the secretion of Abeta with age in DS. nonverbally so as to not confound the assessment with
An imbalance between superoxide dismutase 1 and language demands and to be applicable to a wide age
glutathione perioxidase activity in DS has been linked range and across contexts. The tasks were drawn from the
to free radical generation. Ets-2 and Down’s syndrome Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Bat-
critical region 1 overexpression in DS has been linked to tery (CANTAB) eclipse battery or based on established
cell degeneration. Age-related accumulation of somatic paradigms (e.g. NEPSY, c–g arena and the dots task)
DNA mutations in both DS and AD contribute towards already used in earlier studies of individuals with DS
oxidative stress that exacerbates the imbalance in gene (Pennington et al., 2003; Visu-Petra et al., 2007). These
expression. This leads to enhanced Abeta deposition and studies have shown a consistent impairment on several
further neuronal vulnerability (Lott et al., 2006; Lockrow CANTAB tests, in particular the CANTAB Paired
et al., 2012). Associates Learning task. Similar spatial associative
memory measures have been found to show age-related
Although anatomical degeneration characteristic of AD is decline in DS (Alexander et al., 1997). Many of these tests
reported in most individuals with DS older than 35 years are error-based, helping to limit floor effects, and are
(Wisniewski et al., 1985), only a low percentage of them applicable to children across a wide range of ages, with
present clinical evidence of dementia (Oliver et al., 1998). alternate forms to decrease practice effects. Another
Indeed, AD is diagnosed in only 22–25% of individuals positive aspect of the CANTAB tests and the ACTB is
with DS 40 years of age or older, compared with about that there is evidence for their effective use across
2–3% of individuals with other ID (Janicki and Dalton, languages and cultures (Luciana and Nelson, 2002).
2000). However, the percentages of individuals with DS
with a diagnosis of AD increased with age and it ranges In terms of ageing, cognitive decline in DS starts from a
between 9 and 22% at 40–49 years (Prasher, 1995; lower baseline than in TD. Thus, it is crucial to establish
Sekijima et al., 1998; Janicki and Dalton, 2000) and the premorbid level of functioning to assess whether, and
between 36–66% for those aged 50–59 years (Prasher, at what rate, dementia is progressing. Assessment of
1995; Sekijima et al., 1998). Recently, Carr (2012), in a 40- history from the caregivers can provide information on
year follow-up study, found that a percentage varying psychiatric, personal, past medical and family histories, as
between 13 and 27% of the cohort followed showed a well as an evaluation of mental state. To this end, specific
cognitive decline, thus confirming previous reports cognitive mental state examination tools for adults with
(Oliver et al., 1998; Janicki and Dalton, 2000; Prasher DS are required (CAMDEX-DS; Ball et al., 2004).
et al., 2005).
Reports by the American Association on Mental Retar-
Moreover, with increasing age, individuals with DS become dation-International Association for the Scientific Study
more susceptible to some age-related physical, neurological of Intellectual Disability (AAMR-IASSID), Aylward et al.,
and psychiatric conditions than individuals who do not have 1997 and by Burt and Aylward (2000) suggested tests for
DS probably because of many age-related changes; reduced the diagnosis of dementia applied to individuals with ID.
DNA repair, increased biological ageing and mortality occur A more recent contribution is then reported in UK
at an earlier age in individuals with DS than in those who guidance [National Institute for Health and Clinical
do not have the disorder (Lott and Dierssen, 2010). Excellence (NICE), 2006]. We report below some
examples of test batteries that may be administered.
However, sensory impairments as well as hypothyroidism
may mimic dementia and, therefore, the diagnosis of The Dementia Scale for Down’s Syndrome (DSDS;
ageing in DS requires evidence of a functional impair- Huxley et al., 2000) can assess short-term and long-term
ment and change over time. This issue will be discussed memory, orientation, speech, language, praxis, fine motor
in the next paragraph. skills, practical skills, mood, activity/interest, behavioural

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
98 Psychiatric Genetics 2013, Vol 23 No 3

disturbances and seizure onset, and is designed to DS. However, this study had methodological biases and the
measure dementia in its early, middle and late stages. finding was not consistent with other studies (Bennett
et al., 1983; Bidder et al., 1989). More recently, Ellis et al.
The Dementia Questionnaire for Persons with Mental
(2008) clearly documented that daily supplementation
Retardation (DMR) (revised edition: the Dementia
with antioxidants, folinic acid or both did not alter
Questionnaire for People with Learning Disabilities;
psychomotor or language development in children with
Evenhuis et al., 2006) includes questions to assess the
DS. Thus, there is no high-quality in-vivo evidence that
sum of cognitive scores (which includes short-term and
supplementation with folate, antioxidants or both amelio-
long-term memory, spatial and temporal orientation)
rates neurodevelopment in infants with DS.
and sum of social scores (which includes speech, practical
skills, mood, activity/interest and behavioural distur- On the basis of these findings, rehabilitation is the only
bance) to aid diagnosis and prognosis. realistic approach to improve cognitive and linguistic
abilities in children with DS. However, there are many
The Adaptive Behaviour Dementia Questionnaire
different rehabilitative approaches and few studies have
(ABDQ) is a short questionnaire used as a screening tool
investigated their efficacy.
to detect changes in adaptive behaviour (Prasher et al.,
2004). Among those, early intervention programmes also invol-
ving family members have shown their validity in improv-
Individuals with little or no speech may be tested by the
ing linguistic, communicative and social skills (Mahoney
Test for Severe Impairment (modified) (Albert and
et al., 2006).
Cohen, 1992) and the Spatial Recognition Span (Moss
et al., 1986). Therefore, at present, only anecdotal results are available.
For example, Conners et al. (2008) documented that some
It is important to rule out treatable causes of dementia
of the 16 children with DS they followed using a home-
such as depression, thyroid problems, etc., in addition to
based rehearsal training may have experienced a partial
motor slowness, sensory deficits and general physical ill
improvement in their short-term memory capacities. In
health, as these can all present with symptoms similar to
addition, in a recent review, Lemons and Fuchs (2010)
those of dementia (Aylward et al., 1997). Although various
suggested that treatment based on phonological aware-
tests are available, at this time, there is no definitive
ness seemed to improve the reading of words not taught
mental status examination or neuropsychological instru-
directly, comprehension and fluency in children with DS.
ment that can aid the diagnosis of dementia in individuals
In any case, long-term, intensive interventions, possibly
with DS. There is a need to focus on issues around ease of
delivered in a one-on-one format, are necessary to achieve
use and interpretation by those administering such tests
educational gains.
(NICE, 2006). For example, neuroimaging results for
individuals with DS may appear to yield results that are More longitudinal controlled studies on the effect of early
‘false positives’ for AD from an early age if the standards intervention programmes and rehabilitative strategies are
for the general population are used. required to better clarify their real efficacy.
In terms of ageing, although DS has a high incidence of
Treatments AD, relatively little research has been carried out on its
The last decade has witnessed considerable progress in treatment. The use of medication for AD in individuals
the understanding of the neurobiological bases of the with DS is therefore more controversial than that in the
cognitive impairment in DS and recent findings high- general population (Stanton and Coetzee, 2004).
lighting promising avenues for pharmacological interven- The NICE has amended and reissued guidance following
tion in DS (Fernandez et al., 2007; Roper et al., 2006). the outcome of a judicial review, and only donepezil, a
Thus, it sounds very interesting to wait for systematic reversible inhibitor of acetylcholinesterase, galantamine,
data from this perspective in the next future. a reversible noncompetitive inhibitor of acetylcholines-
terases, and rivastigmine, an acetylcholinesterase and
However, trials of possible treatment in DS have been
butyryl-cholinesterase temporary inhibitor, were recom-
based, so far, on the hypothesis that developmental delays
mended for the treatment of AD. However, there is little
in children with DS may be related to neuronal damage
research evidence that assesses whether any of the
caused by biochemical factors such as increased oxidative
available treatments are effective in this population
stress, abnormal folate metabolism or both. Indeed,
(Prasher et al., 2004; Mohan et al., 2009a, 2009b, 2009c).
cultured neuronal cells from foetuses with DS undergo
apoptotic death more rapidly than those from unaffected
foetuses, and this is reversed by the addition of Neurobiological characteristics
antioxidants (Brooksbank and Balazs, 1984; Busciglio The cognitive and behavioural profile of individuals with
and Yankner, 1995). Consistent with this hypothesis, DS is a consequence of anomalous brain development
Lejeune (1990) suggested that folate treatment might caused by trisomy 21. Autopsy reports indicate lower
improve the psychomotor development of children with brain weight with particularly small cerebellum, frontal

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Neurodevelopmental and psychiatric issues in DS Vicari et al. 99

and temporal lobes (Wisniewski, 1990) in this population. more likely to show externalizing behaviours such as
By contrast, subcortical areas, such as lenticular nuclei stubbornness, oppositionality, inattention, speech prob-
and the posterior parietal and occipital cortical grey lems, concentration difficulties, attention seeking and
matter (GM), have relatively normal brain volumes. impulsivity (Pueschel et al., 1991; Dykens et al., 2002), as
well as depression, anxiety and obsessive-compulsive
Consistently, volumetric MRI studies of individuals with
disorder (OCD). Not surprisingly, from 6 to 8% of
DS and healthy controls confirmed abnormal volumes of
children with DS are diagnosed with attention deficit
many cerebral areas. In particular, smaller brain volume
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and from 10 to 15% of
was documented in the temporal areas and in the
children or young individuals are diagnosed with conduct
cerebellum in a DS group (White et al., 2003).
or oppositional disorders, which manifest in noncompli-
The cerebellar morphometry might play a major role in
ant, disobedient or aggressive behaviours (Myers and
the motor dysfunction and hypotonia of individuals with
Pueschel, 1991; Coe et al., 1999).
DS (Clark and Wilson, 2003; Vicari, 2006). Many studies
have suggested that the cerebellum plays a role in several Usually, a significant shift occurs in the psychiatric
cognitive functions, such as language, abstract reasoning, symptomatology of children and adolescents with
attention, working memory and executive functions, DS. Stores et al. (1998) found that adolescents with DS
which are usually impaired in DS populations (Wishart, showed significantly lower hyperactivity than children
2007). More specifically, Menghini et al. (2011) docu- with DS. Dykens et al. (2002) documented a significant
mented abnormal GM density in the posterior part of the reduction in externalizing behaviours in adolescents
cerebellum, a region believed to be mainly associated compared with children. They also found that adoles-
with high-order cognitive functions such as attentional cents showed a significant increase in internalizing
abilities and executive functions (Strick et al., 2009). symptoms, (e.g. withdrawal and depression). Nicham
Menghini et al. (2011) also reported a GM reduction of et al. (2003) showed that externalizing behaviour prob-
the temporal lobe and the hippocampus, which may be lems (dominance, opposition, hyperactivity, impulsivity,
considered responsible for the explicit long-term memory inattention) were more common in children aged 5–10
and linguistic deficits documented in individuals with DS years, whereas internalizing behaviour problems (shyness,
(Rondal et al., 1988; Fowler, 1990; Carlesimo et al., low confidence) were more common in adolescents and
1997; Vicari et al., 2005). adults.
By contrast, individuals with DS have increased GM In terms of resilience and sensitivity to mental disorders
density in the right parahippocampus, the basal ganglia (Collip et al., 2008), lower ability and lower independent
and the insula bilaterally (Kesslak et al., 1994, Raz et al., functioning level were predictive of severe problem
1995, Aylward et al., 1997; Menghini et al., 2011). Relative behaviours in adult life, as were childhood psychiatric
sparing of the putamen and caudate nucleus and, more disorder, parental mental health, quality of parental
generally, of the basal ganglia could be related to marriage and social class during childhood (McCarthy,
proficient performance on implicit and procedural learn- 2008).
ing tasks (Vicari et al., 2001, 2007). Instead, the GM
increase in the parahippocampal gyrus has been related to
Assessment
relatively good performance on some recognition memory
Even today, many mental health professionals do not
tasks, including delayed nonmatch to sample and visual
recognize or appreciate the co-occurrence of psychiatric
paired comparison tasks (Kesslak et al., 1994).
problems and ID and this is also true for DS.
In summary, MRI studies carried out with different
There are three reasons for this:
tasks show that a number of regions subserve the
performance of individuals with DS. Indeed, the organi-
(1) The presence of diagnostic overshadowing (Reiss
zation of their brain structures is typical for some
et al., 1982), which is typical of dual diagnoses, creates
cognitive abilities, such as visual and spatial memory,
a veil that leads to attributing the presence of
but abnormal for others, such as linguistic and verbal
psychiatric disorders to ID. Consequently, any
memory abilities.
psychiatric disorder is perceived as a ‘reaction’ to,
not a comorbidity of, ID. In fact, the former are
Psychiatric issues downgraded to behavioural disturbances and treated
Epidemiology and general considerations as such (Dykens, 2007; Mazzone et al., 2012).
Children and adolescents with DS have more psychiatric (2) If psychiatric disorders are associated with ID, they
and behavioural disorders than typically developing significantly alter the clinical expression of the
children (Gath and Gumley, 1986; Pueschel et al., disorder, which ranges from aspecific behavioural
1991; Des Noyers Hurley, 1996; Stores et al., 1998; Coe manifestations in cases of serious cognitive impair-
et al., 1999; Nicham et al., 2003). Compared with TD ment to psychiatric symptoms that can be correlated
controls, 4-year-old to 18-year-old children with DS are with specific disorders in cases of slight ID.

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
100 Psychiatric Genetics 2013, Vol 23 No 3

This means that the phenomenology of the disorder disorders in persons with intellectual disability attempts
changes, not the disorder itself. In fact, even in cases to provide a useful adaptation of the DSM-IV-TR for
of more serious psychiatric disturbances, such as individuals with ID. In a recent study (Fletcher et al.,
schizophrenia, it has been observed that the nucle- 2009), professionals indicated that the manual was easy to
arity of the disturbance is not differentiated in the use, aided accurate diagnoses and reduced the use of the
two subgroups (Klosterkotter et al., 1996). not otherwise specified category.
This proteiform symptomatology and aspecificity can
Another psychiatric diagnostic tool developed especially
lead to a tendency in clinical practice to formulate
for dual diagnoses is the Diagnostic Assessment for
diagnoses and treatments by focusing on the
the Severely Handicapped, 2nd modified version (DASH
symptomatological aspect of behaviour and adopting
II) (Matson, 1995); its content validity was established
a therapeutic approach that is exclusively dimen-
by deriving disorder subscales and individual items
sional rather than pathogenic and categorical.
from the DSM-III-R and previous studies of this
Nevertheless, most psychiatrists, psychologists and
population. This instrument was found to have good
social workers do not receive specific training for
inter-rater and test–retest reliability both in populations
making ‘dual diagnoses’ or for recognizing specific
with ID (Matson and Smiroldo, 1997) and specifically
psychiatric symptom presentation in patients with
in DS.
ID. Indeed, the need for specialization in this area
has been emphasized in recent years (Einfeld, To date, only few studies have focused psychiatric
1992; Einfeld et al., 2007). disorders in DS using tools especially developed
(3) Most studies on psychiatric disorders in the general for ID. Clark and Wilson (2003) assessed the behaviour
population typically exclude individuals with low IQ. of 60 children with DS using a specific instrument
Furthermore, specific studies on ID and psychiatric developed for dual diagnosis: the Reiss psychopathology
disorders are lacking. As a result, even though in- rating scale. Agreement between parent and teacher
dividuals with ID are at an increased risk for raters was over 75% and the structure of the Reiss
psychiatric problems, we know much less about the instrument was useful for distinguishing behaviours
causes, courses and treatments of these problems in potentially related to medical problems from actual
this vulnerable population. Therefore, in recent psychopathology.
years, considerable efforts have been made to develop Recently, interesting preliminary findings were obtained
specific psychiatric diagnostic criteria for patients using the minor physical anomaly (MPA) that is believed
with dual diagnoses (Einfeld, 1992; Szymanski and to reflect abnormal development of the CNS. Exploring
King, 1999). the incidence of MPA and its behavioural correlates in DS
and comparing these findings with the other causes of ID
One of the first attempts in this direction was the and normal population, Bhattacharyya et al. (2010) found
development of the Diagnostic Criteria for Psychiatric that DS has significantly more MPA and that a pattern of
Disorders for Use with Adults with Learning Disabilities/ correlation between MPA and behavioural abnormalities
Mental Retardation (DC-LD) (Royal College of Psychia- exists in DS. Nevertheless, even though most of the
trists, 2001), which represent the first psychiatric studies were carried out using assessment instruments
classificatory system devised specifically for use with developed for TD, to measure their validity, no compara-
adults with intellectual disabilities. tive studies in a DS population between these instruments
and others specifically developed for ID were carried
The use of DC-LD is aimed at adults with moderate to out. Moreover, only a few studies have focused on com-
profound intellectual disabilities, although it may be useful parison between DS and other common genetic syndromes
in some adults with mild or low average intelligence, of psychopathology and even fewer on their adaptive
depending on the individual’s presentation and has a very profiles. A recent study (Di Nuovo and Buono, 2011)
high diagnostic accuracy. Indeed, in a field trial (Cooper comparing similarities and differences in the adaptive
et al., 2003) conducted to investigate the diagnostic profiles of the most frequent genetic syndromes using the
accuracy of the instrument, in 96.3% of the 709 recruited Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale showed a specific
patients, the DC-LD diagnosis was fully concordant with adaptive profile of DS with respect to psychiatric disorders
that of the clinical opinion. comorbidity.
Subsequently, the National Association for the Dually
Diagnosed (NADD), in association with the American Treatments
Psychiatric Association (APA), developed a manual As for other genetic syndromes with ID, there is a
(Fletcher et al., 2007) designed as an adaptation of the significant lack of research specifically focused on
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed. – treatments of psychiatric and behavioural problems in
Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR). The Diagnostic Manual-Intellec- DS. This is true both for psychosocial and for pharma-
tual Disability (DM-ID): a textbook of diagnosis of mental cological interventions.

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Neurodevelopmental and psychiatric issues in DS Vicari et al. 101

Psychosocial interventions for challenging behaviour behaviours (Holden and Gitlesen, 2004). Holden and
To date, the behavioural intervention most frequently Gitlesen (2004) reported that prescriptions frequently
used in ID is the applied behaviour analysis (for more violate current guidelines, especially those given by
details, see Carr et al., 2002). This methodology was found general practitioners. For example, many prescriptions
to be effective across a wide range of populations, with were given without a diagnosis, alternatives to medica-
various disabilities. Recently, the technology of positive tions had rarely been explored and evaluation of effects
behaviour support (Carr et al., 1999, 2002) has emerged as and side effects was generally lacking. Instead, the
an application of the principles of behaviour analysis to application of specific guidelines significantly improved
not only address skill repertoires but also redesign the reliability of prescriptions. Engel et al. (2010)
individuals’ living environments with the goal of achiev- reported that medication with neuroleptics and anti-
ing enhanced quality of life and decrease problem depressive compounds has increased. Atypical and typical
behaviour (Carr et al., 1999). neuroleptics do not differ significantly in terms of side
effects, but the number of side effects in selective
Nevertheless, only a few studies have investigated the
serotonin reuptake inhibitors compared with tricyclic
effect of psychosocial and behavioural interventions in
antidepressive compounds has been reduced significantly.
this specific population. Feeley and Jones (2008), in an
extensive review of the literature, identified a small Psychotropic drugs can be divided into four general
number of studies utilizing behavioural assessment categories: psychostimulants, antipsychotics, antidepres-
strategies, as well as several intervention studies that sants and mood stabilizers. Each category has specific
addressed severe challenging behaviour in individuals target symptoms and disorders. Table 1 provides a
with DS. Even though considerable empirical research summary of possible treatment indications for specific
(Reichle and Wacker, 1993; Carr et al., 2002; Feeley and disorders.
Jones, 2006) has shown the effective use of behaviourally
based procedures to intervene on challenging behaviour
Specific disorders
in individuals with ID, close examination of the
Mood disorders
behavioural intervention literature shows relatively few
The incidence of depressive disorders is lower in children
applications with children with DS and even fewer
and adolescents than in adults with DS (Dykens et al.,
applications targeting the specific and characteristic
2002; Nicham et al., 2003; Maatta et al., 2006). The
challenges presented by these children. In the case
prevalence rates of depression in adults with DS range
studies analysed by Feeley and Jones (2008), these
from 5.2 to 11.4% (Collacott et al., 1992; Mantry et al.,
interventions show an effective decrease in challenging
2008) compared with only 4% in adults with other ID
behaviours in children with DS.
(Collacott et al., 1992). A recent longitudinal cohort study
of adolescents and adults (16 years) with DS (Mantry
Pharmacological interventions et al., 2008) who received a detailed psychiatric assess-
Most studies of psychotropic prescriptions in ID have not ment showed that the incidence of depressive disorders
specifically focused on DS and tended to be open trials, was 5.2% of the total sample. Lower rates of depression
case reports or controlled studies with small samples. are found in children with DS who are more prone to have
Reviews on this topic suggest that individuals with ID externalizing disorders (Capone et al., 2006).
respond similar to the TD population to various psycho-
Assessment: The ‘somatic’ spectrum of depressive disorders
tropic medications (Handen and Gilchrist, 2006; Ulzen
is the most common DSM-IV criterion observed in
and Powers, 2008). However, rates of response tend to be
individuals with DS and depression. The spectrum
poorer and the occurrence of side effects tends to be more
includes decreased interest, psychomotor slowing, fa-
frequent. The use of psychotropic medications in children
tigue, appetite/weight change, sleep disturbance, poor
and adolescents with ID requires even greater monitoring
concentration and reduced speech. Feelings of worthless-
and the use of lower doses and slower dosage increases
ness or guilt and agitation may also be present (Cooper
than in the general population.
and Collacott, 1994; Myers and Pueschel, 1995). Self-care
Consequently, considerable efforts have been made to routines may deteriorate, thus necessitating assistance or
develop specific guidelines. For example, Aman et al. frequent prompting. Psychosocial stressors often precede
(2004) published consensus guidelines for the treatment the onset of mood disorders in adolescents and young
of psychiatric illness and behavioural problems in adults with DS (Dodd et al., 2005).
individuals with ID, which emphasizes the presence of
Primary sleep disorders are common in adolescents with
a diagnosis, consideration of alternatives and evaluation.
DS (Resta et al., 2003) and should not be confounded
Individuals with ID tend to be overmedicated. Studies in with secondary sleep disturbances, which are typically
the last 20 years indicate that almost 50% of individuals present in depressive disorders. Primary sleep disorder
with ID who are under care take psychotropic medication can have a significant impact on mood, attention,
for the treatment of psychiatric disorders and/or problem cognition and motivation level (Andreou et al., 2002),

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
102 Psychiatric Genetics 2013, Vol 23 No 3

Table 1 Treatment indications for specific disorders


Psychiatric disorders Target symptoms First-choice drug Clinical considerations

Depression Depressed mood SSRI SSRIs are chosen over TCAs. For depressive symptoms,
fluoxetine remains the only approved treatment for
children. As sertraline is approved for treatment of children
with anxiety, it is also a reasonable option. Citalopram/
escitalopram remain other good choices because of their
higher specificity and fewer side effects
Dysphoric mood SSRI + mood stabilizer
Anhedonia, apathy, insomnia, Dual reuptake inhibitor, SSRI
social withdrawal
Obsessive-compulsive Obsession, compulsion SSRI, TCA, atypical
disorder neuroleptics
Perseveration, tics Atypical/typical neuroleptics
Anxiety Anxiety, somatic symptoms, SSRI/BDZ
restlessness
Autism spectrum Social withdrawal, Atypical neuroleptics Risperidone is the treatment of choice in children and
disorder disorganization, stereotyping, adolescents with ID. For individuals who fail to respond or
physical aggression who experience side effects, quetiapine, aripiprazole and
ziprasidone may cause less weight gain. Because of
aripiprazole’s lack of effect on weight and QT length, it may
become the treatment of choice in the near future
Psychosis Delusions, hallucinations, Atypical neuroleptics
catatonia, agitation
Negative symptoms Lower dose of atypical
neuroleptics
Manic symptoms Mood stabilizer, lithium, atypical
neuroleptics
ADHD Distractibility, hyperactivity Metilphenidate, atomoxetine Bipolar disorders in the ID population are more likely to
impulsivity guanfacine respond to valproic acid and other antiepileptic drugs,
because individuals with ID are more often atypical, mixed
or rapid cycling. However, to achieve a better outcome,
combined treatments are often needed, such as two mood
stabilizers and a low dose of an antipsychotic drug.
Stimulants remain the first-choice treatment for ADHD in
ID. Atomoxetine is considered a second-choice treatment.
a-Agonists are probably a third option
Oppositional-defiant Physical aggression, Atypical neuroleptic/mood
disorder destructiveness stabilizer

ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; BDZ, benzodiazepine; ID, intellectual disability; QT, quetiapine; TCA, tricyclic antidepressant; SSRI, selective serotonin
reuptake inhibitors.

but a full response to antidepressant medication may not individuals with ID who are unable to think abstractly
be achieved until the sleep abnormality is identified and or to count.
treated successfully (Means et al., 2003).
Not all repetitive behaviours in individuals with ID can
be defined as obsessive-compulsive phenomena (Gedye,
Obsessive-compulsive disorder 1996). Indeed, repetitive behaviours with physiologically
The prevalence of OCD in DS ranges from 0.8% (Myers rewarding properties, namely, masturbation, stealing,
and Pueschel, 1991) to 4.5% (Prasher, 1995); indeed, it is hyperventilation, overeating, polydipsia, smoking, hum-
lower than in other psychiatric disorders and not higher ming and pacing, should not be considered in making
than in the general population. Obsessive thoughts may diagnoses.
be difficult to ascertain in individuals with cognitive
impairment. As for depression and other internalizing The Compulsive Behavior Checklist for clients with ID
disorders in DS, OCD increases in adolescents and (Gedye, 1992, 1996) is a guide for collecting information
adults. to help ascertain whether OCD criteria have been
fulfilled. The Obsessive Speech Checklist (Gedye,
Assessment: Compulsions that require abstract thinking are 1998) is a guide for collecting information on obsessive
common in OCD in the general population but can be speech patterns in individuals with ID. The OCD
rare or absent in OCD in individuals with ID. Clinicians Severity Scale (Vitiello et al., 1989) rates the severity of
familiar with OCD in individuals of average intelligence OCD symptoms in individuals with ID.
may believe that OCD is absent if an individual with ID
does not engage in ‘classic’ compulsions. Compulsions When obsessive-compulsive symptoms appear abruptly
that require counting skills or abstract concepts, such as in prepubertal children, they may indicate the presence
contamination or germs or safety, do not occur in of paediatric autoimmune neuropsychiatric disorders

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Neurodevelopmental and psychiatric issues in DS Vicari et al. 103

associated with streptococcal infections or PANDAS impulsivity. Therefore, it is very important to carefully
(Snider and Swedo, 2004). explore the nature of this kind of symptom in patients
with DS (Dykens, 2007).
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and externalizing
behaviours Psychotic disorders
ADHD is diagnosed when inattention, impulsivity and There is a lower prevalence of schizophrenia and bipolar
hyperactivity are present and result in significant disorder in individuals with DS than in adults with ID
academic or social impairment. In young children with related or not to genetic syndromes (Collacott et al.,
DS, hyperactivity and impulsivity are more frequent than 1992; Armando et al., 2012), and adolescents and young
inattention (Green et al., 1989). adults without ID (Morgan et al., 2008).
Compared with TD controls, 4-year-old to 18-year-old Although positive symptoms, such as delusions and
individuals with DS are more likely to show externalizing hallucinations, may be present, negative symptoms, such
behaviours related to ADHD, OCD and conduct disorder as cognitive and behavioural disorganization, social with-
(Pueschel et al., 1991; Coe et al., 1999; Bhattacharyya et al., drawal, apathy, psychomotor slowing, reduced speech and
2009). Indeed, 6–8% of children with DS are diagnosed affective blunting, are more frequent.
with ADHD and 10–15% of children or adolescents are
In one of the largest epidemiological studies
diagnosed with conduct or oppositional disorders (Gath
(n = 245 749) on the co-occurrence of ID with schizo-
and Gumley, 1986; Myers and Pueschel, 1991; Coe et al.,
phrenia and other psychiatric illnesses, Morgan et al.
1999). Moreover, in a recent study (Maatta et al., 2006) of
(2008) found that schizophrenia, but not bipolar disorder,
129 individuals with DS, 33% had manifested ADHD
was considerably over-represented in individuals with a
symptoms during childhood and adolescence.
dual diagnosis: depending on birth cohort, 3.7–5.2% of
Assessment: Hyperactivity and impulsivity are the most those with ID had co-occurring schizophrenia. Never-
dangerous symptoms in children with DS + ADHD; in theless, DS was much less prevalent among individuals
fact, these children are at an especially high risk for with a dual diagnosis, even though it was the most
accidental injury, wandering, running away or getting lost. predominant cause of ID. To our knowledge, no studies
During assessment, ID can overshadow inattention, have been published thus far on psychotic like experi-
which should be treated as an important issue because ences (Armando et al., 2010) and at-risk mental states
of its specific effect on academic impairment. Hyper- (Yung et al., 2003) in DS populations.
thyroidism, hearing loss, sleep disturbance or sleep
In conclusion, there is some evidence that psychosis is
apnoea (Levanon et al., 1999; Fallone et al., 2002;
rare in individuals with DS (Sovner et al., 1985; Collacott
Goday-Arno et al., 2009), and medication side effects
et al., 1992; Prasher, 1995; Morgan et al., 2008). Recent
(stimulants, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors,
data (Mantry et al., 2008) suggest that the effect of
antihistamines, adrenergic agonists and caffeine) should
unknown resilience factors associated with DS may play a
be considered as possible aetiological factors when
protective role in the development of psychosis and
evaluating children with DS + ADHD.
mental illness in general. These resilience factors seem to
reduce the effect of environmental resilience factors such
Anxiety disorders as family circumstances, poor lifestyles and lack of social
To our knowledge, no specific studies have been carried support (McCarthy and Boyd, 2001).
out on the incidence and prevalence of anxiety symptoms
in DS. As so few studies have investigated this relationship
directly, the question remains open as to whether the
Evidence-based knowledge of the assessment and diag- observed difference in prevalence is real or because of
nosis of anxiety disorders in individuals with ID, diagnostic underascertainment.
especially in DS, is weak. This may be because the
current diagnostic classification systems have been
Autism spectrum disorders
validated in populations with typical intellectual func-
The autistic spectrum disorder (ASD) is characterized by
tioning. Because of the lack of insights, the subjective
qualitative impairment in reciprocal social and commu-
elements of the diagnostic criteria may not be reported
nication skills, as well as restricted interests and
and may not be applicable in this group (Einfeld, 1992).
repetitive play routines or movements; onset occurs
However, objective features of anxiety present in the
before 36 months.
general population, such as fear, trembling, flushing and
irritability, are readily observable in individuals with DS Early studies relied on clinical patient records of existing
(Fletcher et al., 2007). Often, objective features of anxiety diagnoses. They were carried out with inconsistent
are present in children with DS in comorbidity with diagnostic criteria for ASD and found very low rates of
ADHD symptoms (Green et al., 1989; Coe et al., 1999), autism, that is, 1–2% (Gath and Gumley, 1986; Myers and
and they can easily be confounded with hyperactivity and Pueschel, 1991; Collacott et al., 1992).

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
104 Psychiatric Genetics 2013, Vol 23 No 3

More recent studies (Kent et al., 1999; Pary and Hurley, Acknowledgements
2002), which were carried out using more reliable Conflicts of interest
diagnostic criteria, suggested that 7–10% of children There are no conflicts of interest.
and adolescents with DS may also have ASD.
The children with DS (compared with those with ASD in
general) were generally much older when they received References
Albert M, Cohen C (1992). The test for severe impairment: an instrument for the
their ASD diagnosis (Rasmussen et al., 2001); in fact, assessment of patients with severe cognitive dysfunction. J Am Geriatr Soc
diagnosis ages across studies ranged from 6 to 16 years of 40:449–453.
age. This was probably because of diagnostic overshadow- Alexander GE, Saunders AM, Szczepanik J, Strassburger TL, Pietrini P, Dani A,
et al. (1997). Relation of age and apolipoprotein E to cognitive function in
ing rather than later ASD onset. Down syndrome adults. NeuroReport 8:1835–1840.
Aman MG, Crismon ML, Frances A, King BH, Rojahn J (2004). Treatment of
Recent clinical studies (Capone et al., 2005; Carter et al., psychiatric and behavioral problems in individuals with mental retardation: an
2007) in which children with DS and ASD were update of expert consensus guidelines. Englewood, CA: Postgraduate
compared with children with DS without ASD showed Institute of Medicine and Jobson Education.
Andreou G, Galanopoulou C, Gourgoulianis K (2002). Cognitive status in Down
that the first group had lower IQs and more severe, odd syndrome individuals with sleep disordered breathing deficits. Brain Cogn
stereotypic behaviours, anxiety and social withdrawal. 50:145–149.
Other studies (Howlin et al., 1995) reported that patients Armando M, Nelson B, Yung AR, Ross M, Birchwood M, Girardi P, et al. (2010).
Psychotic-like experiences, self-disturbance and depressive symptoms in a
with DS and ASD also manifested significant impulsivity, community sample of adolescents and young adults. Schizophr Res
more pronounced cognitive disorganization, disruptive 119:258–265.
behaviour and self-injury. Some studies (Ghaziuddin, Armando M, Papaleo F, Vicari S (2012). Chromosome 22q11 microdeletion
syndrome, COMT functional polymorphism and cognitive/psychiatric corre-
2000; Rasmussen et al., 2001; Starr et al., 2005) reported lates. CNS Neurolo Disord Drug Targets 11:273–281.
that risk factors associated with ASD in children with DS Aylward EH, Li Q, Habbak QR, Warren A, Pulsifer MB, Barta PE, et al. (1997).
included postcardiac surgery complications, hypothyroid- Basal ganglia volume in adults with Down syndrome. Psychiatry Res 74:
73–82.
ism, lower IQs and first-degree relatives with ASD. Ball SL, Holland AJ, Huppert FA, Treppner P, Watson P, Hon J (2004). The
Infantile spasms and myoclonic seizures were associated modified CAMDEX informant interview is a valid and reliable tool for use in
the diagnosis of dementia in adults with Down’s syndrome. J Intellect Disabil
with an increased risk of developing ASD in children with
Res 48:611–620.
DS (Goldberg-Stern et al., 2001; Eisermann et al., 2003). Bennett FC, McClelland S, Kriegsmann EA, Andrus LB, Sells CJ (1983). Vitamin
and mineral supplementation in Down’s syndrome. Pediatrics 72:707–713.
Bhattacharyya R, Sanyal D, Roy K, Saha S (2009). A study of cluster behavioral
abnormalities in Down syndrome. Indian J Med Sci 63:58–65.
Conclusion Bhattacharyya R, Sanyal D, Roy K, Bhattacharyya S (2010). Correlation between
physical anomaly and behavioral abnormalities in Down syndrome. J Pediatr
DS is the most frequent genetic cause of ID. Never- Neuroscience 5:105–110.
theless, even though many neuropsychological studies Bidder RT, Gray P, Newcombe RG, Evans BK, Hughes M (1989). The effects of
have been carried out in children and adolescents with multivitamins and minerals on children with Down syndrome. Dev Med Child
Neurol 31:532–537.
DS, a significant gap that emerged in our review is that Brock J, Jarrold C (2004). Language influences on verbal short-term memory in
very few have examined the effectiveness of rehabilita- Down syndrome: item and order recognition. J Speech Lang Hear Res
47:1334–1346.
tion treatments.
Brooksbank BW, Balazs R (1984). Superoxide dismutase, glutathione peroxidase
and lipoperoxidation in Down’s syndrome fetal brain. Brain Res 318:37–44.
At the same time, despite the high prevalence of Burt DB, Aylward EH (2000). Test battery for the diagnosis of dementia in
psychiatric comorbidity in DS, very few studies have individuals with intellectual disability. J Intellect Disabil Res 44:175–180.
investigated the specific characteristics of psychiatric Busciglio J, Yankner BA (1995). Apoptosis and increased generation of reactive
oxygen species in Down’s syndrome neurons in vitro. Nature 378:776–779.
disorders in DS and even fewer studies have investigated Bush A, Beail N (2004). Risk factors for dementia in people with Down syndrome:
the effectiveness of different treatments for these issues in assessment and diagnosis. Am J Ment Retard 109:83–97.
disturbances in children and adolescents. Capone G, Goyal P, Ares W, Lannigan E (2006). Neurobehavioral disorders in
children, adolescents, and young adults with Down syndrome. Am J Med
In conclusion, many research and treatment gaps remain Genet C Semin Med Genet 142:158–172.
Capone GT, Grados MA, Kaufman WE, Bernad-Ripoll S, Jewell A (2005). Down
in the mental health arena, and these disparities are also syndrome and co-morbid autism spectrum disorder: characterization using
present in the larger field of ID. Future work on the aberrant behavior checklist. Am J Med Genet 143:373–380.
psychopathology in DS is required to help close this Carlesimo GA, Marotta L, Vicari S (1997). Long-term memory in mental
retardation: evidence for a specific impairment in subjects with Down’s
gap. In fact, research should aim to clarify specific syndrome. Neuropsychologia 35:71–79.
diagnostic criteria to enable identification of both risk and Carr EG, Horner RH, Turnbull AP, Marquis J, Magito-McLaughlin D, McAtee ML,
protective factors across the biospsychosocial spectrum et al. (1999). Positive behavior support for people with developmental
disabilities: a research synthesis. In: American Association on Mental
and to document combinations of treatments that Retardation, editors. Monograph of the American Association on Mental
ameliorate distressful symptoms and enhance positive Retardation. Washington, DC: American Association on Mental Retardation.
states. pp. 3–18.
Carr EG, Dunlap G, Horner RH, Koegel RL, Turnbull AP, Sailor W, et al. (2002).
Neurodevelopmental disorders with identified genetic Positive behavior support: evolution of an applied science. J Positive Behav
Intervent 4:4–16.
aetiologies present a unique opportunity to study gene– Carr J (2012). Six weeks to 45 years: a longitudinal study of a population with
brain–behaviour connections in child psychiatry. Down syndrome. J Appl Res Intellect Disabil 25:414–422.

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Neurodevelopmental and psychiatric issues in DS Vicari et al. 105

Carter JC, Capone GT, Gray RM, Cox CS, Kaufmann WE (2007). Autistic spectrum Epstein CJ (1989). Down syndrome. In: Scriver CR, Beaudet AL, Sly WS, Valle P,
disorders in Down syndrome: further delineation and distinction from other editors. The metabolic basis of inherited disease. New York: McGraw-Hill.
behavioral abnormalities. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 144:87–94. pp. 291–396.
Caselli MC, Vicari S, Longobardi E, Lami L, Pizzoli C, Stella G (1998). Gestures Evenhuis HM, Kengen MF, Eurlings AL (2006). Dementia Questionnaire for
and words in early development of children with Down syndrome. J Speech People with Intellectual Disabilities (DLD). Sydney: Pearson Psychocorp.
Lang Hear Res 41:1125–1135. Fabbretti D, Pizzuto E, Vicari S, Volterra V (1997). A story description task in
Chapman RS (1995). Language development in children and adolescents with children with Down syndrome: lexical and morphosyntactic abilities.
Down syndrome. In: Fletcher P, MacWinney B, editors. The handbook of child J Intellect Disabil Res 41:165–179.
language. Oxford: Blackwell. pp. 641–663. Fallone G, Owens JA, Deane J (2002). Sleepiness in children and adolescents:
Clark D, Wilson GN (2003). Behavioral assessment of children with Down clinical implications. Sleep Med Rev 6:287–306.
syndrome using the Reiss psychopathology scale. Am J Med Genet A Feeley K, Jones E (2006). Addressing challenging behaviour in children with
118:210–216. Down syndrome: the use of applied behaviour analysis for assessment and
Cocchi G, Gualdi S, Bower C, Halliday J, Jonsson B, Myrelid A, et al. (2010). intervention. Downs Syndr Res Pract 11:64–77.
International trends of Down syndrome 1993–2004: births in relation to Feeley K, Jones E (2008). Strategies to address challenging behaviour in young
maternal age and terminations of pregnancies. Birth Defects Res A Clin Mol children with Down syndrome. Downs Syndr Res Pract 12:153–163.
Teratol 88:474–479. Fernandez F, Morishita W, Zuniga E, Nguyen J, Blank M, Malenka RC, Garner CC
Coe D, Matson J, Russell D, Slifer K, Capone G, Baglio C, Stallings S (1999). (2007). Pharmacotherapy for cognitive impairment in a mouse model of Down
Behavior problems of children with Down syndrome and life events. J Autism syndrome. Nat Neurosci 10:411–413.
Dev Disord 29:149–156. Fletcher R, Loschen E, Stavrakaki C, First M (2007). Diagnostic Manual-
Collacott RA, Cooper SA, McGrother C (1992). Differential rates of psychiatric Intellectual Disability (DM-ID): a textbook of diagnosis of mental disorders in
disorders in adults with Down’s syndrome compared with other mentally persons with intellectual disability. Kingston, NY: NADD Press.
handicapped adults. Br J Psychiatry 161:671–674. Fletcher R, Havercamp SM, Ruedrich SL, Benson BA, Barnhill LJ, Cooper SA,
Collip D, Myin-Germeys I, Van Os J (2008). Does the concept of ‘sensitization’ et al. (2009). Clinical usefulness of the diagnostic manual-intellectual
provide a plausible mechanism for the putative link between the environment disability for mental disorders in persons with intellectual disability: results
and schizophrenia? Schizophr Bull 34:220–225. from a brief field survey. J Clin Psychiatry 70:967–974.
Conners FA, Rosenquist CJ, Arnett L, Moore MS, Hume LE (2008). Improving Fowler AE (1990). Language abilities in children with Down syndrome: evidence
memory span in children with Down syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res for a specific delay. In: Cicchetti D, Beeghley M, editors. Children with Down
52:244–255. syndrome: a developmental perspective. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge
Cooper SA, Collacott RA (1994). Clinical features and diagnostic criteria of University Press. pp. 302–328.
depression in Down’s syndrome. Br J Psychiatry 165:399–403. Franco F, Wishart F (1995). The use of pointing and other gestures by young
Cooper SA, Melville CA, Einfeld SL (2003). Psychiatric diagnosis, intellectual children with children with Down syndrome. Am J Ment Retard 100:
disabilities and Diagnostic Criteria for Psychiatric Disorders for Use with 160–182.
Adults with Learning Disabilities/Mental Retardation (DC-LD). J Intellect Frenkel S, Bourdin B (2009). Verbal, visual, and spatio-sequential short-term
Disabil Res 47:3–15. memory: assessment of the storage capacities of children and teenagers with
Cromer R (1987). The cognition hypothesis revisited. In: Kessel F, editor. The Down’s syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res 53:152–160.
development of language and language researches. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Gath A, Gumley D (1986). Behaviour problems in retarded children with special
pp. 48–65. reference to Down’s syndrome. Br J Psychiatry 149:156–161.
Des Noyers Hurley A (1996). Psychiatric disorders in children and adolescents Gedye A (1992). Compulsive behavior checklist for clients with mental
with mental retardation and developmental disabilities. Curr Opn Pediatr retardation. Habilitat Ment Health 11:73–77.
8:361–365. Gedye A (1996). Issues involved in recognizing obsessive-compulsive disorder in
Di Nuovo S, Buono S (2011). Behavioral phenotypes of genetic syndromes with developmentally disabled clients. Semin Clin Neuropsychiatry 1:142–147.
intellectual disability: comparison of adaptive profiles. Psychiatry Res Gedye A (1998). Behavioural diagnostic guide for developmental disabilities.
89:440–445. Vancouver: Diagnostic Books.
Dodd P, Dowling S, Hollins S (2005). A review of the emotional, psychiatric and Ghaziuddin M (2000). Autism in Down’s syndrome: a family history study.
behavioural responses to bereavement in people with intellectual disabilities. J Intellect Disabil Res 44:562–566.
J Intellect Disabil Res 49:537–543. Gibson D (1978). Down syndrome: the psychology of mongolism. London:
Dykens EM (2000). Psychopathology in children with intellectual disability. Cambridge University Press.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry 41:407–417. Goday-Arno A, Cerda-Esteva M, Flores-Le-Roux JA, Corretger JM, Chillaron-
Dykens EM (2007). Psychiatric and behavioral disorders in persons with Down Jordan JJ, Cano-Péerez JF (2009). Hyperthyroidism in a population with
syndrome. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 13:272–278. Down syndrome. Clin Endocrinol (Oxf) 71:110–114.
Dykens EM, Kasari C (1997). Maladaptive behavior in children with Prader–Willi Goldberg-Stern H, Strawsburg RH, Patterson B, Hickey F, Bare M, Gadoth N,
syndrome, Down syndrome, and non-specific mental retardation. Am J Ment Degrauw TJ (2001). Seizure frequency and characteristics in children with
Retard 102:228–237. Down syndrome. Brain Dev 23:275–278.
Dykens EM, Shah B, Sagun J, Beck T, King BH (2002). Maladaptive behavior in Green JM, Dennis J, Bennets LA (1989). Attention disorder in a group of young
children and adolescents with Down’s syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res Down’s syndrome children. J Ment Defic Res 33:105–122.
46:484–492. Handen B, Gilchrist R (2006). Psychopharmacology in children and adolescents
Edgin JO, Mason GM, Allman MJ, Capone GT, DeLeon I, Maslen C, et al. (2010). with mental retardation. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 47:871–882.
Development and validation of the Arizona Cognitive Test Battery for Down Holden B, Gitlesen JP (2004). Psychotropic medication in adults with mental
syndrome. J Neurodev Disord 2:149–164. retardation: prevalence, and prescription practices. Res Dev Disabil 25:
Einfeld S, Tonge B, Chapman L, Taffe J, Horstead S, Pumphrey R, et al. (2007). 509–521.
Inter-rater reliability of the diagnoses of psychosis and depression in individuals Howlin P, Wing L, Gould J (1995). The recognition of autism in children with
with intellectual disabilities. J Appl Res Intellect Disabil 20:384–390. Down syndrome. Implications for intervention and some speculations about
Einfeld SL (1992). Clinical assessment of psychiatric symptoms in mentally pathology. Dev Med Child Neurol 37:398–414.
retarded individuals. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 26:149–63. Huxley A, Prashner VP, Hague MS (2000). The Dementia Scale for Down’s
Einfeld SL, Tonge BJ (1996). Population prevalence of psychopathology in Syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res 44:697–698.
children and adolescents with intellectual disabilities. II. Epidemiological Iverson J, Longobardi E, Caselli MC (2003). The relationship between gestures
findings. J Intellect Disabil Res 40:99–109. and words in children with Down syndrome and typically-developing children
Ellis JM, Tan HK, Gilbert RE, Muller DP, Henley W, Moy R (2008). in the early stages of communicative development. Int J Lang Commun
Supplementation with antioxidants and folinic acid for children with Down’s Disord 38:179–197.
syndrome: randomized controlled trial. BMJ 336:594–597. Janicki MP, Dalton AJ (2000). Prevalence of dementia and impact on intellectual
Engel C, Szrama E, Habler F (2010). Psychopharmacological therapy in mental disability services. Ment Retard 38:276–288.
retardation. Comparison of the years 1991 and 2005. Psychiatr Prax Jarrold C, Baddeley AD (1997). Short-term memory for verbal and visuo-spatial
37:391–396. information in Down’s syndrome. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 1:101–122.
Eisermann MM, DeLaRaillere A, Dellatolas G, Tozzi E, Nabbout R, Dulac O, et al. Jarrold C, Baddeley AD, Hewes AK (1999). Genetically dissociated components
(2003). Infantile spasms in Down syndrome. Effects of delayed anticonvulsive of working memory: evidence from Down and Williams syndrome. Neuro-
treatment. Epilepsy Res 55:21–27. psychologia 37:637–651.

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
106 Psychiatric Genetics 2013, Vol 23 No 3

Kent L, Evans J, Paul M, Sharp M (1999). Comorbidity of autistic spectrum Morgan VA, Leonard H, Bourke J, Jablensky A (2008). Intellectual disability co-
disorders in children with Down syndrome. Dev Med Child Neurol occurring with schizophrenia and other psychiatric illness: population-
41:153–158. based study. Br J Psychiatry 193:364–372.
Kesslak JP, Nagata SF, Lott I, Nalcioglu O (1994). Magnetic resonance imaging Moss MB, Albert MS, Butters N, Payne M (1986). Differential patterns of memory
analysis of age-related changes in the brains of individuals with Down loss among patients with Alzheimer’s disease, Huntington’s disease, and
syndrome. Neurology 44:1039–1045. alcoholic Korsakoff’s syndrome. Arch Neurol 43:239–246.
Klein BP, Mervis CB (1999). Cognitive strength and weaknesses of 9- and 10- Myers BA, Pueschel SM (1991). Psychiatric disorders in persons with Down
year-olds with Williams syndrome or Down syndrome. Dev Neuropsychol syndrome. J Nerv Ment Dis 179:609–613.
16:177–196. Myers BA, Pueschel SM (1995). Major depression in a small group of adults with
Klosterkotter J, Ebel H, Schultze-Lutter F, Steinmeyer EM (1996). Diagnostic Down syndrome. Res Dev Disabil 16:285–299.
validity of basic symptoms. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 246:147–154. Næss KA, Melby-Lervåg M, Hulme C, Lyster SA (2012). Reading skills in children
Landt J, D’Abrera JC, Holland AJ, Aigbirhio FI, Fryer TD, Canales R, et al. (2011). with Down syndrome: a meta-analytic review. Res Dev Disabil 33:737–747.
Using positron emission tomography and Carbon 11-labeled Pittsburgh National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence (NICE) (2006). National
compound B to image brain fibrillar b-amyloid in adults with Down syndrome: Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence published clinical guidelines for
safety, acceptability, and feasibility. Arch Neurol 68:890–896. dementia. London, UK: NICE.
Lanfranchi S, Cornoldi C, Vianello R (2004). Verbal and visuospatial working Nicham R, Weitzdorfer R, Hauser E, Freidl M, Schubert M, Wurst E, et al. (2003).
memory deficits in children with Down syndrome. Am J Ment Retardation Spectrum of cognitive, behavioural and emotional problems in children
109:456–466. and young adults with Down syndrome. J Neural Transm Suppl 67:
Lejeune J (1990). Pathogenesis of mental deficiency in trisomy 21. Am J Med 173–191.
Genet 7:20–30. Oliver C, Crayton L, Holland A, Hall S, Bradbury J (1998). A four year prospective
Lemons CJ, Fuchs D (2010). Phonological awareness of children with Down study of age-related cognitive change in adults with Down’s syndrome.
syndrome: its role in learning to read and the effectiveness of related Psychol Med 28:1365–1377.
interventions. Res Dev Disability 31:316–330. Pary RJ, Hurley AD (2002). Down syndrome and autistic disorder. Mental Health
Levanon A, Tarasiuk A, Tal A (1999). Sleep characteristics in children with Down Asp Dev Disabil 5:64–65.
syndrome. J Pediatr 134:755–760. Patel V, Flisher AJ, Hetrick S, McGorry P (2007). Mental health of young people: a
Lockrow JP, Fortress AM, Granholm AC (2012). Age-related neurodegeneration global public-health challenge. Lancet 369:1302–1313.
and memory loss in Down syndrome. Curr Gerontol Geriatr Res 2012 Pennington BF, Moon J, Edgin J, Stedron J, Nadel L (2003). The neuropsychology
doi:10.1155/2012/463909. of Down syndrome: evidence for hippocampal dysfunction. Child Dev 74:
Lott IT, Dierssen M (2010). Cognitive deficits and associated neurological 75–93.
complications in individuals with Down’s syndrome. Lancet Neurol 9: Prasher VP (1995). Prevalence of psychiatric disorders in adults with Down
623–633. syndrome. Eur J Psychiatry 9:77–82.
Lott IT, Head E, Doran E, Busciglio J (2006). Beta-amyloid, oxidative stress and Prasher VP, Farooq A, Holder R (2004). The Adaptive Behaviour Dementia
Down syndrome. Curr Alzheimer Res 3:521–528. Questionnaire (ABDQ): screening questionnaire for dementia in Alzheimer’s
Luciana M, Nelson CA (2002). Assessment of neuropsychological function in disease in adults with Down syndrome. Res Dev Disabil 25:385–397.
children through the Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Prasher VP, Fung N, Adams C (2005). Rivastigmine in the treatment of dementia
Battery (CANTAB): normative performance in 4 to 12 year-olds. Dev in Alzheimer’s disease in adults with Down syndrome. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry
Neuropsychol 22:595–624. 20:496–497.
Maatta T, Tervo-Maatta T, Taanila A, Kaski M, Ilvanalnen M (2006). Mental health, Pueschel SM, Bernier JC, Pezullo JC (1991). Behavioral observations of children
behaviour and intellectual ability in people with Down syndrome. Downs with Down syndrome. J Ment Defic Res 35:502–511.
Syndr Res Pract 11:37–43. Rasmussen P, Borjesson O, Wentz E, Gillberg C (2001). Autistic disorders in
Mahoney G, Perales F, Wiggers B, Herman B (2006). Responsive teaching: early Down syndrome: background factors and clinical correlates. Dev Med Child
intervention for children with Down syndrome and other disabilities. Down Neurol 43:750–754.
Syndr Res Pract 11:18–28. Raz N, Torres IJ, Briggs SD, Spencer WD, Thornton AE, Loken WJ (1995).
Mantry D, Cooper SA, Smiley E, Morrison J, Allan L, Jackson A (2008). The Selective neuroanatomic abnormalities in Down syndrome and their cognitive
prevalence and incidence of mental ill-health in adults with Down syndrome. correlates: evidence from MRI morphometry. Neurology 45:356–366.
J Intellect Disabil Res 52:141–155. Reichle J, Wacker D (1993). Communicative alternatives to challenging behavior:
Matson JL (1995). The diagnostic assessment for the severely handicapped integrating functional assessment and intervention strategies. Baltimore: Paul
revised (DASH-II). Baton Rouge, LA: Disability Consultants, LLC. H. Brookes.
Matson JL, Smiroldo BB (1997). Validity of the mania subscale of the diagnostic Reiss S (1990). Prevalence of dual diagnosis in community based day programs
assessment for the severely handicapped-II (DASH-II). Res Dev Disabil in the Chicago metropolitan area. Am J Ment Def 94:578–588.
18:221–225. Reiss S, Levitan GW, Szyszko J (1982). Emotional disturbance and mental
Mazzone L, Ruta L, Reale L (2012). Psychiatric comorbidities in asperger retardation: diagnostic overshadowing. Am J Ment Def 86:567–574.
syndrome and high functioning autism: diagnostic challenges. Ann Gen Resta O, Barbaro M, Giliberti T, Caratozzolo G, Cagnazzo M, Scarpelli F, et al.
Psychiatry 25:11–16. (2003). Sleep related breathing disorders in adults with Down syndrome.
McCarthy J (2008). Behaviour problems and adults with Down syndrome: Downs Syndr Res Pract 8:115–119.
childhood risk factors. J Intellect Disabil Res 52:877–882. Rondal JA, Ghiotto M, Brédart S, Bachelet JF (1988). Mean length of utterance of
McCarthy J, Boyd J (2001). Psychopathology and young people with Down’s children with Down syndrome. Am J Ment Retard 3:64–66.
syndrome: childhood predictors and adult outcome of disorder. J Intellect Rondal JA (1993). Exceptional cases of language development in mental
Disabil Res 45:99–105. retardation: the relative autonomy of language as a cognitive system. In:
Means MK, Lichstein KL, Edinger JD, Taylor DJ, Durrence HH, Husain AM, et al. Tager-Flusberg, editor. Constraints on language acquisition: studies of
(2003). Changes in depressive symptoms after continuous positive airway atypical children. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. pp. 155–174.
pressure treatment for sleep apnea. Sleep Breath 7:31–42. Roper RJ, Reeves RH (2006). Understanding the basis for Down syndrome
Menghini D, Costanzo F, Vicari S (2011). Relationship between brain and phenotypes. PLoS Genet 2:e50.
cognitive processes in Down syndrome. Behav Genet 41:381–393. Royal College of Psychiatrists (2001). DC-LD (Diagnostic Criteria for Psychiatric
Mervis CB, Robinson BR (2000). Expressive vocabulary ability of toddlers with Disorders for Use with Adults with Learning Disabilities/Mental Retardation).
Williams syndrome or Down syndrome: a comparison. Dev Neuropsychol London: Gaskell Press.
17:111–126. Sekijima Y, Ykeda SI, Tokuda T, Satoh SI, Hidaka H, Hidaka E, et al. (1998).
Miller JF (1992). Development of speech and language in children with Down Prevalence of dementia of Alzheimer type and apolipoprotein E phenotypes in
syndrome. In: Lott IT, Mc Loy EE, editors. Down syndrome. New York: Wiley- aged patients with Down’s syndrome. Eur Neurol 39:234–237.
Liss. pp. 39–50. Singer Harris NG, Bellugi U, Bates E, Jones W, Rossen M (1997). Contrasting
Mohan M, Bennet C, Carpenter PK (2009a). Galantamine for dementia in people profiles of language in children with Williams and Down syndromes. Dev
with Down syndrome. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 21:CD007656. Neuropsychol 13:345–370.
Mohan M, Bennet C, Carpenter PK (2009b). Memantine for dementia in people Sovner R, Hurley A, Labrie R (1985). Is mania incompatible with Down’s
with Down syndrome. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 21:CD007657. syndrome? Br J Psychiatry 146:319–320.
Mohan M, Carpenter PK, Bennet C (2009c). Donepezil for dementia in people Snider L, Swedo S (2004). PANDAS: current status and research directions. Mol
with Down syndrome. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 21:CD007658. Psychiatry 9:900–907.

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Neurodevelopmental and psychiatric issues in DS Vicari et al. 107

Stanton LR, Coetzee RH (2004). Down’s syndrome and dementia. Adv Psychiatr Vicari S, Bellucci S, Carlesimo GA (2005). Visual and spatial long-term memory:
Treat 10:50–58. differential pattern of impairments in Williams and Down syndromes. Dev
Starr EM, Berument SK, Tomlinds M, Papanikolaou K, Rutter M (2005). Brief report: Med Child Neurol 47:305–311.
autism in individuals with Down syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord 35:665–673. Vicari S, Bellucci S, Carlesimo GA (2006). Evidence from two genetic syndromes
Stores R, Stores G, Fellows B, Buckley S (1998). Daytime behavior problems for the independence of spatial and visual working memory. Dev Med Child
and maternal distress in children with Down syndrome, their siblings, and Neurol 48:126–131.
nonintellectually disabled and other intellectually disabled peers. J Intellect Vicari S, Verucci L, Carlesimo GA (2007). Implicit memory is independent from IQ
Disabil Res 42:228–237. and age but not from etiology: evidence from Down and Williams syndrome.
Strick PL, Dum RP, Fiez JA (2009). Cerebellum and non motor function. Annu J Intellect Disabil Res 51:932–941.
Rev Neurosci 32:413–434. Wang PP, Bellugi U (1994). Evidence from two genetic syndromes for
Szymanski L, King BH (1999). Summary of the practice parameters for the dissociation between verbal and visual-spatial short-term memory. J Clin
assessment and treatment of children, adolescents, and adults with mental Exp Neuropsychol 16:317–322.
retardation and comorbid mental disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Wishart JG (2007). Socio-cognitive understanding: a strength or weakness in
Psychiatry 38:1606–1610. Down syndrome? J Intellect Disabil Res 51:996–1005.
Tanzi RE, Gusella J, Watkins P, Bruns G, George-Hyslop PS, Van Keuren ML, Wisniewski KE, Wisniewski HM, Wen GY (1985). Occurrence of neuropatho-
et al. (1987). Amyloid beta protein gene: cDNA, mRNA distribution, and logical changes and dementia of Alzheimer’s disease in Down’s syndrome.
genetic linkage near the Alzheimer locus. Science 235:880–884. Ann Neurol 17:278–282.
Ulzen TP, Powers E (2008). A Review of empirical evidence of somatic treatment Wisniewski KE (1990). Down syndrome children often have brain with maturation
options for the MI/DD population. Psychiatr Q 79:265–273. delay, retardation of growth, and cortical dygenesis. Am J Med Genet Suppl
Vicari S (2006). Motor development and neuropsychological patterns in persons 7:274–281.
with Down syndrome. Behav Genet 36:355–364. Yung AR, Phillips LJ, Yuen HP, Francey SM, McFarlane CA, Hallgren M (2003).
Vicari S, Caselli MC, Tonucci F (2000). Early language development in Italian Psychosis prediction: 12-month follow up of a high-risk (prodromal) group.
children with Down syndrome: asynchrony of lexical and morphosyntactic Schizophr Res 60:21–32.
abilities. Neuropsychologia 38:634–644. Visu-Petra L, Benga O, Tincaş I, Miclea M (2007). Visual-spatial processing in
Vicari S, Bellucci S, Carlesimo GA (2001). Procedural learning deficit in children children and adolescents with Down’s syndrome: a computerized assess-
with Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia 39:665–677. ment of memory skills. J Intellect Disabil Res 51:942–952.
Vicari S, Carlesimo GA (2002). Children with intellectual disabilities. In: Vitiello B, Spreat S, Behar D (1989). Obsessive-compulsive disorder in mentally
Baddeley A, Wilson B, Kopelman M, editors. Handbook of memory disorders. retarded patients. J Nerv Ment Dis 177:232–236.
West Sussex, UK: John Wiley & Sons Ltd. pp. 501–518. White NS, Alkire MT, Haier RJ (2003). A voxel-based morphometric
Vicari S, Marotta L, Carlesimo GA (2004). Verbal short-term memory in Down’s study of nondemented adults with Down Syndrome. NeuroImage 20:
syndrome: an articulatory loop deficit? J Intellect Disabil Res 48:80–92. 393–403.

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

You might also like