Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Beyond conflict: exploring the spectrum of

human–wildlife interactions and their


underlying mechanisms
SALONI BHATIA, STEPHEN MARK REDPATH
K U L B H U S H A N S I N G H S U R Y A W A N S H I and C H A R U D U T T M I S H R A

Abstract Humans have lived alongside and interacted with Supplementary material for this article is available at
wild animals throughout evolutionary history. Even though https://doi.org/./SX
wild animals can damage property, or injure humans and
domesticated animals, not all interactions between humans
and wildlife are negative. Yet, research has tended to
Introduction
focus disproportionately on negative interactions leading to
negative outcomes, labelling this human–wildlife conflict.
Studies have identified several factors, ranging from gen-
der, religion, socio-economics and literacy, which influence
H umans have a long, complex relationship with wild
animals, varying between appreciation, reverence,
retaliation, utilization and acceptance (Treves & Naughton-
people’s responses to wildlife. We used the ISI Web of Treves, ; Ingold, ; Lescureux & Linnell, ;
Knowledge database to assess quantitatively how human– Ghosal & Kjosavik, ). Studies have tried to understand
wildlife interactions are framed in the scientific literature these relationships by characterizing their nature and by
and to understand the hypotheses that have been invoked examining the challenges of living with wildlife, especially
to explain these. We found that the predominant focus of with species that are responsible for negative impacts such
research was on human–wildlife conflict (%), with little as damage to property, competition for resources, injury
coverage of coexistence (%) or neutral interactions (%). or loss of life (Bostedt & Grahn, ; Carter et al., ).
We suggest that such a framing is problematic as it can The management of negative impacts is an important con-
lead to biases in conservation planning by failing to consider servation concern as retaliatory killing of wild animals can
the nuances of people’s relationships with wildlife and the endanger their populations, and prohibiting retaliation can
opportunities that exist for conservation. We propose a typ- anger communities sharing space with them (Madden, ;
ology of human responses to wildlife impacts, ranging from Woodroffe et al., ). Negative interactions between
negative to positive, to help moderate the disproportionate people and wildlife are often framed as human–wildlife
focus on conflict. We suggest that standardizing termin- conflict. However, framing human–wildlife relationships
ology and considering interactions beyond those that are predominantly through the lens of conflict can create a
negative can lead to a more nuanced understanding of strong negative impact on peoples’ psyche and influence
human–wildlife relations and help promote greater coexis- perceptions of risk from wild animals (Gore et al., ).
tence between people and wildlife. We also list the various Peterson et al. () proposed that narratives using the
influential factors that are reported to shape human–wildlife human–wildlife conflict frame tend to represent animals as
interactions and, to generate further hypotheses and re- consciously combating people, dichotomizing humans and
search, classify them into  proximate (correlates) and nature. The way human–wildlife relationships are framed
five ultimate (mechanisms) factors. also has repercussions on how these are interpreted and
Keywords Attitude, behaviour, human–wildlife coexistence, managed. A biased framing thus provides salience to
human dimensions, human–wildlife conflict, human–wildlife certain aspects of the relationship, glossing over the nuances
interactions, wildlife damage that are crucial for wildlife conservation and management
(Peterson et al., ). Studies have also suggested that
human–wildlife conflicts can be split into two components:
() human–wildlife impacts, and () human–human or
SALONI BHATIA*† (Corresponding author), KULBHUSHANSINGH SURYAWANSHI* and conservation conflicts that represent the ideological tensions
CHARUDUTT MISHRA* Nature Conservation Foundation, 1311, “Amritha”, 12th between stakeholders that affect wildlife, for example,
Main, Vijayanagar 1st Stage, Mysore 570017, India
E-mail saloni86@gmail.com preservation of nature vs local livelihoods or human safety
STEPHEN MARK REDPATH University of Aberdeen, Aberdeen, UK
(Redpath et al., ; Young et al., ).
More recently, some researchers have suggested re-
*Also at: Snow Leopard Trust, Seattle, USA
†Also at: Manipal Academy of Higher Education, Manipal, India placing the term human–wildlife conflict, which usually
Received  October . Revision requested  November . has a negative connotation, with non-negative ones such as
Accepted  December . First published online  November . human–wildlife coexistence, or human–wildlife interactions.

Oryx, 2020, 54(5), 621–628 © 2019 Fauna & Flora International doi:10.1017/S003060531800159X

https://doi.org/10.1017/S003060531800159X Published online by Cambridge University Press


622 S. Bhatia et al.

By facilitating the recognition of the ambivalence in the of human–wildlife relationships. The articles were classified
attitudes and behaviours of people towards wildlife, these into their predominant frame using a predeveloped typology
debates have infused some diversity into the narratives, (Table ). Two coders began by analysing a portion of the
and have highlighted the unfavourable consequences of a articles (n = ) and established % inter-coder agreement.
predominantly negative framing for conservation (Peterson Disagreements, if any, were resolved by SB.
et al., ; Bruskotter & Fulton, ; Carter & Linnell, Based on the results, we characterized a range of human
; Kansky et al., ; Mishra et al., ). Nevertheless, responses towards wildlife impacts. We built our under-
few studies have explored the spectrum of human responses standing of responses based on prior models of persuasion
to wildlife impacts. Frank () suggested that understand- (e.g. Value-Attitude-Behaviour, Theory of Reasoned Action,
ing this spectrum can help practitioners assess the relative Theory of Planned Behaviour; Fishbein & Ajzen, ;
intensity and strength of negative, positive and ambivalent Ajzen, ; Homer & Kahle, ). We inferred that in
responses, enabling them to create specific conservation our context, attitude and behaviour together comprised a
interventions and strategies. An improved conceptual response, considering that previous definitions of tolerance
understanding of the spectrum can also enable us to better and intolerance comprised both these components. For
assess the factors responsible for peoples’ responses. example, Carter & Linnell () defined coexistence as a
Previous studies have enumerated several socio-economic, behavioural state where humans and wild animals have
psychological and ecological factors that influence peo- learnt to co-adapt with minimal negative impacts on each
ples’ attitudes towards, and intention to kill, wildlife other. Bruskotter & Fulton (, p. ) defined tolerance as
(St. John et al., ; Marchini & Macdonald, ; a ‘passive restraint or inaction’ on the part of humans up
Kansky et al., ). For example, socio-demographic factors to a threshold of wildlife numbers. Although their concept
such as age, gender, wealth, occupation and education are centred largely around human behaviour, they suggested the
often correlated with attitudes and behaviours towards approach could also be used to examine human intentions
wildlife (Kellert, ; Peyton et al., ; Dickman, ; and attitudes.
Lindsey et al., ). Similarly, descriptive factors such as Treves () pointed out that tolerance and intolerance
knowledge of animal behaviour, social norms and taboos are states of mind, and therefore emphasis should be placed
about wild animals, and familiarity with the risk posed by largely on intentions and attitudes. Similarly, Kansky et al.
wildlife, have also been associated with human responses (, p. ) defined tolerance as ‘the ability and willingness
(McComas, ; Marchini & Macdonald, ). However, of an individual to absorb the extra potential or actual costs
few studies have attempted to move from a correlational to of living with wildlife’. The term ‘interactions’, on the other
a mechanistic understanding of factors. hand, has been applied more neutrally to illustrate both
Here, we examine the bias in framing of human–wildlife positive and negative attitudes and behaviours towards
relationships in the scientific literature and propose a shift wildlife (Redpath et al., ). Attitudes represent mental
towards recognizing the spectrum of human responses to constructs (e.g. thought, feeling) while behaviours repre-
wildlife impacts. We also review and organize the available sent actions and, together, they have the potential to pro-
information on various factors that influence these re- vide a fuller understanding of human responses to wildlife
sponses. Our aims are to () understand the framing of impacts.
literature around human–wildlife interactions, () develop In the next step, we also identified factors influencing
a typology to assess peoples’ responses towards wildlife human attitudes and behaviours. We made efforts to com-
impacts, and () strengthen the understanding of factors plement the literature review with an unstructured review to
that influence responses. strengthen our understanding of factors. This was done by
exploring the key papers and concepts explained in the
articles that comprised the review.
Methods

We used the ISI Web of Knowledge database to identify Results


articles on human–wildlife interactions, with the keywords
‘human–wildlife conflict’, OR ‘human–wildlife coexistence’, Seventy-one per cent of the  articles made use of the
OR ‘human–wildlife relationship’, OR ‘human–wildlife human–wildlife conflict frame (Table ). Within this
interaction’, AND ‘factors’, AND ‘drivers’, AND ‘causes’, frame, % pertained to human–wildlife impacts and %
under ‘Topic’. The search yielded a total of  results for described conservation conflicts. Two per cent of the
– (Supplementary Material ). Of these, the most  articles discussed coexistence between humans and
recent  articles were shortlisted for further analysis wildlife, % employed a neutral frame, % invoked both
(i.e. September –July ) based on the rationale that conflict and coexistence with wildlife, and % could not
these would better reflect the contemporary understanding be classified.

Oryx, 2020, 54(5), 621–628 © 2019 Fauna & Flora International doi:10.1017/S003060531800159X

https://doi.org/10.1017/S003060531800159X Published online by Cambridge University Press


Beyond human–wildlife conflict 623

TABLE 1 Typology of frames used to categorize peer-reviewed


scientific publications.

Frame Meaning
Human–wildlife Explicit use of the term ‘human–wildlife
conflict conflict’, mention of human values, atti-
tudes & behaviours that indicate intoler-
ance or affect wildlife; mention of wildlife
damage or animal behaviour & ecology
that can precipitate intolerance (e.g. actual
or perceived losses caused by wildlife, im- FIG. 1 Visual representation of human response to wildlife
pact of animal presence & movement in impacts. Manifested intolerance comprises responses where
human dominated landscapes, livestock both attitude and behaviour are negative towards wildlife. Latent
depredation). Articles that explicitly intolerance indicates responses where attitudes are negative,
framed the issue as human–human or but behaviour is not. Neutral comprises responses where both
conservation conflict (i.e. disagreements attitude and behaviour are ambivalent. Appreciation comprises
about wildlife management) were also responses where attitudes are positive, but no corresponding
included in this category positive behaviour can be found. Finally, stewardship indicates
Human–wildlife Positive human values, attitudes & beha- responses where both attitude and behaviour are positive.
coexistence viours indicating tolerance for wildlife (e.g.
cultural values that encourage reverence
towards species that cause damage) resource dependence, perceptions of risk, and the nature
Neutral Values, attitudes or behaviours associated of interaction with the animal (Table , Fig. ).
with wildlife that don’t explicitly refer to
conflict or coexistence (e.g. change in
peoples’ wildlife value orientation, human
behaviour & its link to the persistence of Discussion
wildlife). Use of the terms ‘interaction’ or
‘human–animal relations’ Inordinate focus on human–wildlife conflict
Conflict & Both tolerant & intolerant human values,
coexistence attitudes & behaviours associated with Our findings reiterate that there is a disproportionate
wildlife emphasis on human–wildlife conflict as a frame and few
Unclassified Articles that did not fit into any of the studies refer to human–human conflict. This may be coun-
above frames (e.g. wildlife disease, terproductive to conservation as it creates a bias in our un-
behavioural observations)
derstanding of peoples’ relationship with wildlife (Redpath
et al., ). It also compounds the real conflict, which usu-
ally takes place between communities and the de facto re-
Five types of human responses emerged from our re- presentatives of conservation by assuming wildlife to be
view (Fig. ): () manifested intolerance, in which negative deliberately antagonistic towards people (Peterson et al., ).
attitudes translated into negative behaviours, () latent It may be more useful to examine people’s responses not
intolerance, in which negative attitudes did not translate just in terms of conflict or coexistence but along a
into negative behaviours, () neutral or ambivalent atti- spectrum, ranging from negative to positive attitudes and
tudes, which did not translate into negative or positive behaviours.
behaviours, () appreciation, in which positive attitudes
did not translate into positive behaviours, and () stew-
ardship, in which positive attitudes translated into positive Characterizing human responses to wildlife impacts
behaviours.
Upon further review, we found that % of the  arti- We identified a gradient of attitudinal and behavioural
cles elaborated on factors that influenced human attitudes intensity in articles that described human–wildlife
or behaviours towards wildlife. The review resulted in a list relationships, ranging from shades of positive to shades of
of  factors, spanning socio-cultural, economic, psy- negative. Most incidents of violent confrontation between
chological and ecological dimensions. We labelled these as people and wildlife, such as retaliatory killing, could be
proximate factors. Proximate factors could be viewed as considered examples of manifested intolerance (e.g. Simms
variables with a connection to human responses that do not et al., ; Swanepoel et al., ; Hazzah et al., ).
invoke a causal relationship (Alcock, ). The proximate However, there could be instances where behaviours
factors were grouped into five ultimate factors that repre- are negative even though attitudes are positive, such as
sented the potential underlying mechanisms or causes of in situations where an individual is forced to act against
human response: value orientations, social interactions, his/her preference or beliefs because of the prevailing social

Oryx, 2020, 54(5), 621–628 © 2019 Fauna & Flora International doi:10.1017/S003060531800159X

https://doi.org/10.1017/S003060531800159X Published online by Cambridge University Press


624 S. Bhatia et al.

TABLE 2 Typology of the ultimate factors underlying human re- What affects human responses to wildlife impact?
sponses to wildlife impacts.
Similar to Anand & Radhakrishna (), we found that a
Ultimate factor Meaning modest subset of studies on human–wildlife interactions
Value orientations A preferred organization of beliefs that described factors. The  factors that we identified were con-
mediates an individual’s relationship sidered proximate factors or correlates because they were
with the society & the environment
pattern-oriented rather than cause-oriented. For example,
(Kluckhohn, 1951)
Social interactions The extent of cooperation, faith & gender, a proximate factor, may influence human responses
confidence between the individual & to wildlife, but by itself it does not help us understand why
the community, & between the com- a particular gender should have a negative or positive re-
munity & the managing agencies in sponse towards wild animals. The differential engagement
dealing with wildlife impacts of genders with conservation organizations or their varied
Resource dependence The extent of market & non-market perceptions of risk could instead be the drivers of their
dependence on a resource that could
response (Gillingham & Lee, ; Prokop & Fančovičová,
overlap with the needs of wildlife
(based on Marshall, 2011) ). The five ultimate factors identified in the study were
Perceptions of risk Judgements regarding negative wildlife value orientation, social interactions, resource dependence,
impacts, based on cognitive & affective perceptions of risk and nature of interaction with the
evaluations (based on Slovic, 1987) animal.
Nature of interaction The context in which humans & wild- Value orientation can be understood as a preferred or-
with the animal life interact ganization of beliefs that mediates an individual’s relation-
ship with society and the environment (Kluckhohn, ).
Values enable an individual to choose a conduct that is per-
circumstances. Similarly, individuals may succumb to social sonally or socially preferable (Rohan, ; Vauclair, ).
pressure to relocate a damage-causing animal from their Value orientations are affected by ethnicity, religious and
locality despite not feeling strongly about it (Ghosal & cultural beliefs, personal and social norms about the
Kjosavik, ). Such instances could be classified as mani- animal in question, a sense of social identity (for example,
fested intolerance, considering that the ultimate outcome is whether one is a hunter or a farmer), and can be influenced
a negative action towards wildlife. by one’s environment (e.g. rural vs urban; Shen et al., ;
Latent intolerance can be identified in situations where Manfredo, ; Marchini & Macdonald, ; Inskip et al.,
people’s attitudes are negative, but no corresponding nega- ; Koziarski et al., ; Pooley, ; Amit & Jacobson,
tive behaviour can be found (Manfredo & Dayer, ). The ).
intolerance could be a result of unfavourable conditions that Studies have examined how value orientations influ-
have inhibited people from acting negatively, such as per- ence attitudes and behaviour towards nature and wildlife
ceptions of human incapability or the lack of resources to (Rokeach, ; Homer & Kahle, ; Ajzen, ; Stern &
engage in an intolerant response, or the legal/social implica- Dietz, ; Natori & Chenoweth, ; Dietsch et al., ).
tions of engaging in a negative response (e.g. Bhatia et al., Hazzah et al. () pointed out that people’s attitude is not
). A Buddhist, for example, may feel inclined towards defined purely by the economic impacts caused by wildlife
injuring a damage-causing carnivore but may not execute but is also affected by the cultural significance of the loss.
this desire because of legal costs or religious philosophy Cattle hold greater cultural value for the Maasai compared
(Bhatia et al., ). This also implies that when the un- to small livestock (sheep and goats) and therefore lion
favourable conditions are removed, people may, not neces- depredation on cattle provokes greater resentment towards
sarily, switch from latent to forms of manifested intolerance. the carnivore (Hazzah et al., ).
Neutral or ambivalent responses are those where indivi- Social interactions refer to the extent of cooperation,
duals tend not to act either way or are undecided about how faith and confidence between the individual and the
they feel towards the wild animal and its impact (Kansky community, and between the community and conservation
et al., ). Appreciation indicates that the individual or agencies, when dealing with wildlife impacts. These could
community values the existence of the wild animal and have an overarching influence on the way people perceive
chooses to accept the negative impacts even though they wildlife in their landscape, who they attribute the ownership
may not positively engage with conservation (Dorresteijn of wildlife to, and whether they consider themselves to be
et al., ). Finally, stewardship comprises situations where marginalized or empowered (Mutanga et al., ; Pooley
the individual or community protects the wild animal even et al., ).
in the face of wildlife damage, owing, perhaps, to the conser- The extent of cooperation or conflict over shared re-
vation or cultural significance of the species (Bruskotter & sources, a strong social network, and the presence of an en-
Fulton, ; Li et al., ). vironment in which economic and social burdens are shared

Oryx, 2020, 54(5), 621–628 © 2019 Fauna & Flora International doi:10.1017/S003060531800159X

https://doi.org/10.1017/S003060531800159X Published online by Cambridge University Press


Beyond human–wildlife conflict 625

FIG. 2 Factors influencing human responses towards wildlife impacts. Proximate factors (correlates) are listed inside the circles,
with the number of times they were mentioned in the articles. The ultimate factors (mechanisms) are in the boxes next to the circles.
Except for ‘media’ and ‘heuristics’ (see risk perception circle), the remainder were identified in a systematic literature review.
Some of the proximate factors influence more than one ultimate factor.

can provide opportunities for people to respond collectively Karlsson & Sjöstrom, ; Humle & Hill, ). Furthermore,
to wildlife impacts (Romañach et al., ). Similarly, the occupation and wealth are important considerations, and
nature and the extent of interaction that an individual has diversification of income sources provides a buffer against
with conservation agencies and the extent of consonance be- loss (Dickman, ; Pont et al., ). Marshall ()
tween the expectations of the stakeholders involved will de- further suggested that resource dependence has social,
termine the level of faith that the community places in the economic and environmental dimensions.
agencies (Zajac et al., ; Dorresteijn et al., ; Nyhus, It is also important to understand people’s perception of
; Amit & Jacobson, ; Mishra et al., ; Pooley risk from wildlife because this can influence their willing-
et al., ). These interactions are played out against a back- ness to coexist with it (Webber & Hill, ). Risk percep-
drop of wildlife laws and legal enforcement, political power tions are the judgements people make when examining
and media involvement (Bhatia et al., ; Rust et al., ). and evaluating personal and social threats (Slovic, ).
Resource dependence has a direct bearing on the eco- Individuals may perceive certain wild animals to be a threat
nomic and psychological costs of living with wildlife. If a to property or life, or simply be afraid to encounter them
significant proportion of time, labour and money has been (Dorresteijn et al., ; Koziarski et al., ; Nyhus,
invested in a resource that is perceived to be in competition ). On the other hand, they may feel awe and admira-
with the needs of wildlife, then an individual is likely to tion for the animal despite the costs associated with the
have a more negative response towards wildlife (Gadd, ; interaction (Goldman et al., ). The outcome is often

Oryx, 2020, 54(5), 621–628 © 2019 Fauna & Flora International doi:10.1017/S003060531800159X

https://doi.org/10.1017/S003060531800159X Published online by Cambridge University Press


626 S. Bhatia et al.

a trade-off between people’s perceptions of the negative interactions within a community and between a conserva-
impacts of risk and the perceived/expected benefits from tion agency and the community. Similarly, the nature of
it (Banerjee et al., ; Kansky et al., ). Risk per- interaction with the animal may influence resource depen-
ception has two important dimensions, the cognitive and dence as well as perceptions of risk. Resource dependence
the affective. The cognitive dimension involves people’s as- may have a bearing on risk perceptions and vice-versa.
sessment of the probability of occurrence of an event and Social interactions may influence perceptions of risk, and
the affective dimension involves the instinctive and spon- vice-versa, at the level of the individual and community.
taneous response of people when they experience it (Riley
& Decker, ; Gore et al., ). Wildlife damage can
also be evaluated in terms of its catastrophic potential, de- Conclusion
fined as a rare devastating event that can strongly influence
peoples’ responses (McComas, ; Dickman, ). Human–wildlife conflict, although a predominant narra-
Gore et al. () suggested two predominant types tive, is not the only form of interaction between people
of influences on people’s perceptions of risk from wild- and wild animals. Moving beyond conflict to alternative
life: personal/individual capacity to control, and agency conceptualizations can affect how we tackle the intellec-
capacity. The former includes factors such as personal tual and practical challenges of living with wildlife. In
volition, perceived probability of exposure to risk, fre- this regard, intention and choice can be viewed as playing
quency and intensity of exposure, predictability and abil- a key role in influencing people’s responses towards wild-
ity to control the risk, knowledge about the risk, and affect life. Based on our findings, we suggest that it may be use-
(Dorresteijn et al., ; Pont et al., ). Agency capacity ful to define tolerance as ‘the state of neutral or positive
includes external variables such as trust in the intentions attitude manifested as a neutral to positive behaviour
and capabilities of the agency/individuals responsible for towards wildlife despite their real or potential negative
mitigation (McComas, ; Gore et al., ; Earle, impacts’. Manifested and latent intolerance thus comprise
). intolerant responses arising from negative attitude or
The nature of interaction with the animal is the setting in behaviour.
which people encounter wildlife. Some specific factors that Finally, a move towards exploring the causal linkages or
define people’s interactions include the action, target, con- mechanisms influencing human responses could enable
text and time (Fishbein & Ajzen, ). For instance, what practitioners to develop more predictive and proactive mod-
was the impact (action), who caused it (target), where and els of conservation and management. Our study also aligns
when did the incident occur (context and time)? Thus, the with Bruskotter et al. () who proposed that values and
location of the species, the type of animal, the magnitude risk perceptions were two key mechanisms driving toler-
of impact, and animal behaviour are important influences ance towards carnivores. Human responses to wildlife differ
(Dorresteijn et al., ; Nyhus, ). Every interaction across individuals and communities, across species, across
need not be negative as there could be situations in which cultures and over time. Greater coverage of geographies
the animal has not caused harm (e.g. it is merely encoun- and cultural contexts would help improve our understand-
tered in the landscape) or there could also be instances in ing of this important subject.
which the animal has had a positive impact on the individ- Globally, interactions between humans and wildlife
ual (e.g. nature lovers who search for encounters with ani- are expected to increase as suitable wildlife habitats
mals in the wild). The nature of interaction can also affect shrink, climate changes and some wild populations recover
knowledge and beliefs about the species and thereby influ- (Nyhus, ). A better knowledge of capacity to tolerate
ence other ultimate factors such as perceptions of risk, and wildlife will, therefore, help in facilitating coexistence
even value orientation. between humans and wild animals with minimal reper-
Proximate factors such as age, gender, education and the cussions to each other.
hidden costs of living with damage-causing species work
through multiple pathways (Fig. ). For example, people’s Acknowledgements We thank Juliette Young, Yash Veer
Bhatnagar, Suri Venkatachalam, Munib Khanyari, and Phalguni
perception of risk, their resource dependence and their value
Ranjan for their input and support.
orientations may differ depending on their age, gender
and level of education (Koziarski et al., ; Manfredo Author contributions Study design, fieldwork and data analysis: SB;
et al., ). Similarly, the hidden costs of human–wildlife writing: all authors.
interactions will affect perceptions of risk and resource
Conflicts of interest None.
dependence of individuals (Humle & Hill, ).
The five ultimate factors may also interact with and itera- Ethical standards This research did not involve any animal or
tively influence each other. For example, value orientations human subjects, and otherwise abided by the Oryx guidelines on
may influence perceptions of risk and the type of social ethical standards.

Oryx, 2020, 54(5), 621–628 © 2019 Fauna & Flora International doi:10.1017/S003060531800159X

https://doi.org/10.1017/S003060531800159X Published online by Cambridge University Press


Beyond human–wildlife conflict 627

References G H O S A L , S. & K J O S AV I K , J.D. () Living with leopards: negotiating


morality and modernity in western India. Society & Natural
A J Z E N , I. () From intentions to actions: a theory of planned Resources, , –.
behavior. In Action-Control: from Cognition to Behavior (eds G I L L I N G H A M , S. & L E E , P.C. () The impact of wildlife-related
J. Kuhl & J. Beckmann), pp. –. Springer, Heidelberg, Germany. benefits on the conservation attitudes of local people around the
A J Z E N , I. () The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Selous Game Reserve, Tanzania. Environment Conservation, ,
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, , –. –.
A L C O C K , J. (ed.) () Animal Behavior: an Evolutionary Approach. G O L D M A N , M.J., R O Q U E , J., P I N H O , D. & P E R R Y , J. () Human
Sinauer, Sunderland, USA. dimensions of wildlife maintaining complex relations with large
A M I T , R. & J AC O B S O N , S.K. () Understanding rancher coexistence cats: Maasai and lions in Kenya and Tanzania. Human Dimensions
with jaguars and pumas: a typology for conservation practice. of Wildlife, , –.
Biodiversity and Conservation, , –. G O R E , M.L., K A H L E R , J.S. & S O M E R S , M. () Gendered risk
A N A N D , S. & R A D H A K R I S H N A , S. () Investigating trends in perceptions associated with human–wildlife conflict: implications
human–wildlife conflict: is conflict escalation real or imagined? for participatory conservation. PLOS ONE, , e.
Journal of Asia-Pacific Biodiversity, , –. G O R E , M.L., K N U T H , B.A., C U R T I S , P.D. & S H A N A H A N , J.E. ()
B A N E R J E E , K., J H A L A , Y.V., C H A U H A N , K.S. & D AV E , C.V. () Factors influencing risk perception associated with human–black
Living with lions: the economics of coexistence in the Gir forests, bear conflict. Human Dimensions of Wildlife, , –.
India. PLOS ONE, , e. G O R E , M.L., W I L S O N , R.S., S I E M E R , W.F., H U D E N KO , H.W., C L A R K E ,
B H AT I A , S., A T H R E Y A , V., G R E N Y E R , R. & M AC D O N A L D , D.W. () C.E., H A R T , P.S. et al. () Application of risk concepts to wildlife
Understanding the role of representations of human–leopard management: special issue introduction. Human Dimensions of
conflict in Mumbai through media-content analysis. Conservation Wildlife, , –.
Biology, , –. H A Z Z A H , L., B AT H , A., D O L R E N R Y , S., D I C K M A N , A. & F R A N K , L.
B H AT I A , S., R E D P AT H , S.M., S U R YAWA N S H I , K. & C H A R U D U T T , M. () From attitudes to actions: predictors of lion killing by Maasai
() The relationship between religion and attitudes toward large warriors. PLOS ONE, , e.
carnivores in northern India? Human Dimensions of Wildlife, , H A Z Z A H , L., M U L D E R , M.B. & F R A N K , L. () Lions and warriors:
–. social factors underlying declining African lion populations and the
B O S T E D T , G. & G R A H N , P. () Estimating cost functions for the effect of incentive-based management in Kenya. Biological
four large carnivores in Sweden. Ecological Economics, , –. Conservation, , –.
B R U S KOT T E R , J.T. & F U LT O N , D.C. () Will hunters steward H O M E R , P. & K A H L E , L. () A structural equation test of the
wolves? A comment on Treves and Martin. Society and Natural ‘value-attitude-behaviour hierarchy’. Journal of Personality and
Resources, , –. Social Psychology, , –.
B R U S KOT T E R , J.T., V U C E T I C H , J.A., M A N F R E D O , M.J., K A R N S , G.R., H U M L E , T. & H I L L , C. () People primate interactions: implications
W O L F , C., A R D , K. et al. () Modernization, risk, and for primate conservation. In An Introduction to Primate
conservation of the world’s largest carnivores. BioScience, , –. Conservation (eds A.W. Serge & A. J. Marshall), pp. –.
C A R T E R , N.H. & L I N N E L L , J.D. () Co-adaptation is key to Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK.
coexisting with large carnivores. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, I N G O L D , T. () From trust to domination an alternative history of
, –. human–animal relations. In The Perception of the Environment:
C A R T E R , N.H., S H R E S T H A , B.K., K A R K I , J.B., P R A D H A N , N.M.B. & Essays in Livelihood, Dwelling and Skill (ed. T. Ingold), pp. –,
L I U , J. () Coexistence between wildlife and humans at fine Routledge, London, UK.
spatial scales. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, I N S K I P , C., C A R T E R , N., R I L E Y , S., R O B E R T S , T. & M A C M I L L A N , D.
, –. () Toward human–carnivore coexistence: understanding
D I C K M A N , A.J. () Complexities of conflict: the importance of tolerance for tigers in Bangladesh. PLOS ONE, , e.
considering social factors for effectively resolving human–wildlife K A N S K Y , R., K I D D , M. & K N I G H T , A.T. () Meta-analysis of
conflict. Animal Conservation, , –. attitudes toward damage-causing mammalian wildlife. Conservation
D I C K M A N , A.J. () From cheetahs to chimpanzees: a comparative Biology, , –.
review of the drivers of human–carnivore conflict and K A N S K Y , R., K I D D , M. & K N I G H T , A.T. () A wildlife tolerance
human–primate conflict. Folia Primatologica, , –. model and case study for understanding human wildlife conflicts.
D I E T S C H , A.M., T E E L , T.L. & M A N F R E D O , M.J. () Social values Biological Conservation, , –.
and biodiversity conservation in a dynamic world. Conservation K A R L S S O N , J. & S J Ö S T R Ö M , M. () Subsidized fencing of livestock
Biology, , –. as a means of increasing tolerance for wolves. Ecology and Society,
D O R R E S T E I J N , I., M I L C U , A.I., L E V E N T O N , J., H A N S P AC H , J. & , –.
F I S C H E R , J. () Social factors mediating human–carnivore K E L L E R T , S.R. () Public perecptions of predators, particularly the
coexistence: understanding thematic strands influencing wolf and coyote. Biological Conservation, , –.
coexistence in Central Romania. Ambio, , –. K L U C K H O H N , C. () Values and value-orientations in the theory of
E A R L E , T.C. () Trust in risk management: a model-based review of action: an exploration in definition and classification. In Toward a
empirical research. Risk Analysis, , –. General Theory of Action (eds T. Parsons & E. Shils), pp. –.
F I S H B E I N , M. & A J Z E N , I. (eds) () Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Harvard University Press, Cambridge, USA.
Behavior: an Introduction to Theory and Research. Addison-Wesley, K O Z I A R S K I , A., K I S S U I , B. & K I F F N E R , C. () Patterns and
Reading, USA. correlates of perceived conflict between humans and large
F R A N K , B. () Human–wildlife conflicts and the need to include carnivores in Northern Tanzania. Biological Conservation,
tolerance and coexistence: an introductory comment. Society and , –.
Natural Resources, , –. L E S C U R E U X , N. & L I N N E L L , J.D.C. () Knowledge and perceptions
G A D D , M. () Conservation outside of parks: attitudes of local of Macedonian hunters and herders: the influence of species specific
people in Laikipia, Kenya. Environmental Conservation, , –. ecology of bears, wolves, and lynx. Human Ecology, , –.

Oryx, 2020, 54(5), 621–628 © 2019 Fauna & Flora International doi:10.1017/S003060531800159X

https://doi.org/10.1017/S003060531800159X Published online by Cambridge University Press


628 S. Bhatia et al.

L I , J., W A N G , D., Y I N , H., Z H A X I , D., J I A G O N G , Z., S C H A L L E R , G.B. P RO KO P , P. & F A N Č OV I Č O V Á , J. () Perceived body condition is
et al. () Role of Tibetan Buddhist monasteries in snow leopard associated with fear of a large carnivore predator in humans.
conservation. Conservation Biology, , –. Annales Zoologici Fennici, , –.
L I N D S E Y , P.A., H AV E M A N N , C.P., L I N E S , R., P A L A Z Y , L., P R I C E , A.E., R E D P AT H , S.M., B H AT I A , S. & Y O U N G , J. () Tilting at wildlife:
R E T I E F , T. et al. () Determinants of persistence and tolerance of reconsidering human–wildlife conflict. Oryx, , –.
carnivores on Namibian ranches: implications for conservation on R I L E Y , S.J. & D E C K E R , D.J. () Wildlife stakeholder acceptance
southern African private lands. PLOS ONE, , e. capacity for cougars in Montana. Wildlife Society Bulletin, ,
M A D D E N , F. () Creating coexistence between humans and –.
wildlife: global perspectives on local efforts to address human– R O H A N , M.J. () A rose by any name? The values construct.
wildlife conflict. Human Dimensions of Wildlife, , –. Personality and Social Psychology Review, , –.
M A N F R E D O , M.J. (ed.) () Who cares about wildlife? In Social R O K E AC H , M. (ed.) () The Nature of Human Values. Free Press,
Science Concepts for Exploring Human–Wildlife Relationships and New York, USA.
Conservation Issues, pp. –, Springer-Verlag, New York, USA. R O M A Ñ AC H , S., L I N D S E Y , P. & W O O D R O F F E , R. () Determinants
M A N F R E D O , M.J. & D AY E R , A.A. () Concepts for exploring of attitudes towards predators in central Kenya and suggestions
the social aspects of human–wildlife conflict in a global context. for increasing tolerance in livestock dominated landscapes. Oryx,
Human Dimensions of Wildlife, , –. , –.
M A N F R E D O , M.J., T E E L , T.L. & D I E T S C H , A.M. () Implications of R U S T , N.A., T Z A N O P O U LO S , J., H U M L E , T. & M AC M I L L A N , D.C.
human value shift and persistence for biodiversity conservation. () Why has human–carnivore conflict not been resolved in
Conservation Biology, , –. Namibia? Society and Natural Resources, , –.
M A R C H I N I , S. & M AC D O N A L D , D.W. () Predicting rancher’s S H E N , S.X., Z I N N , H. & W A N G , A.Y. () Assessing wildlife value
intention to kill jaguars: case studies in Amazonia and Pantanal. orientations in China: an exploration of the concepts and
Biological Conservation, , –. methodology. In Proceedings of the  Northeastern Recreation
M A R S H A L L , N.A. () Assessing resource dependency on the Research Symposium (eds R. Burns & K. Robinson), pp. –,
rangelands as a measure of climate sensitivity. Society and Natural Northern Research Station, New York, USA.
Resources, , –. S I M M S , A., S A L A H U D I N , Z.M., A L I , H., A L I , I. & W O O D , T. ()
M C C O M A S , K.A. () Defining moments in risk communication Saving threatened species in Afghanistan: snow leopards in the
research: –. Journal of Health Communication, , –. Wakhan Corridor. International Journal of Environmental Studies,
M I S H R A , C., R E D P AT H , S.R. & S U R Y AWA N S H I , K.S. () Livestock , –.
predation by snow leopards: conflicts and the search for solutions. S LOV I C , P. () Perception of risk. Science, , –.
In Snow Leopards: Biodiversity of the World: Conservation from S T J O H N , A.V.F.A., E D WA R D S -J O N E S , G.A. & J O N E S , J.P.G. ()
Genes to Landscapes (eds T. McCarthy & D. Mallon), pp. –. Conservation and human behaviour: lessons from social
Elsevier, Amsterdam, The Netherlands. psychology. Wildlife Research, , –.
M I S H R A , C., Y O U N G , J.C., F I E C H T E R , M., R U T H E R F O R D , B. & S T E R N , P.C. & D I E T Z , T. () The value basis of environmental
R E D P A T H , S.M. () Building partnerships with communities concern. Journal of Social Issues, , –.
for biodiversity conservation: lessons from Asian mountains. S WA N E P O E L , L.H., S O M E R S , M.J. & D A L E R U M , F. () Functional
Journal of Applied Ecology, , –. responses of retaliatory killing versus recreational sport hunting of
M U TA N G A , C.N., M U B O KO , N. & G A N D I WA , E. () Protected leopards in South Africa. PLOS ONE, , e.
area staff and local community viewpoints: a qualitative T R E V E S , A. () Tolerant attitudes reflect an intent to steward: a reply
assessment of conservation relationships in Zimbabwe. to Bruskotter and Fulton. Society & Natural Resources, , –.
PLOS ONE , e. T R E V E S , A. & N A U G H T O N -T R E V E S , L. () Risk and opportunity
N AT O R I , Y. & C H E N O W E T H , R. () Differences in rural landscape for humans coexisting with large carnivores. Journal of Human
perceptions and preferences between farmers and naturalists. Evolution, , –.
Journal of Environmental Psychology, , –. V A U C L A I R , C. () Measuring cultural values at the individual-level:
N Y H U S , P.J. () Human–wildlife conflict and coexistence. Annual considering morality in cross-cultural value research. Revista de
Review of Environment and Resources, , –. Administração Mackenzie, , –.
P E T E R S O N , M.N., B I R C K H E A D , J.L., L E O N G , K., P E T E R S O N , M.J. & W E B B E R , A.D. & H I L L , C.M. () Using Participatory Risk Mapping
P E T E R S O N , T.R. () Rearticulating the myth of human–wildlife (PRM) to identify and understand people’s perceptions of crop loss
conflict. Conservation Letters, , –. to animals in Uganda. PLOS ONE, , e.
P E Y T O N , R.B, B U L L , P.A. & H O L S M A N , R.H. () Measuring the W O O D R O F F E , R., T H I R G O O D , S. & R A B I N OW I T Z , A. () The future
Social Carrying Capacity for Gray Wolves in Michigan. Unpublished of coexistence resolving human–wildlife conflicts in a changing
report. Michigan State University, Michigan, USA. world. In People and Wildlife, Conflict or Coexistence? (eds
P O N T , A.C., M A R C H I N I , S., E N G E L , M.T., M AC H A D O , R., O T T , P.H., R. Woodroffe, S. Thirgood & A. Rabinowitz), pp. –,
C R E S P O , E.A. et al. () The human dimension of the conflict Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.
between fishermen and South American sea lions in southern Brazil. Y O U N G , J.C., M A R Z A N O , M., W H I T E , R.M., M C C R A C K E N , D.I.,
Hydrobiologia, , –. R E D P AT H , S.M., C A R S S , D.N. et al. () The emergence of
P O O L E Y , S. () A cultural herpetology of Nile crocodiles in Africa. biodiversity conflicts from biodiversity impacts: characteristics
Conservation & Society, , –. and management strategies. Biodiversity and Conservation, ,
P O O L E Y , S., B A R U A , M., B E I N A R T , W., D I C K M A N , A., H O L M E S , G., –.
L O R I M E R , J. et al. () An interdisciplinary review of current and Z A J AC , R.M., B R U S KO T T E R , J.T., W I L S O N , R.S. & P R A N G E , S. ()
future approaches to improving human–predator relations. Learning to live with black bears: a psychological model of
Conservation Biology, , –. acceptance. The Journal of Wildlife Management, , –.

Oryx, 2020, 54(5), 621–628 © 2019 Fauna & Flora International doi:10.1017/S003060531800159X

https://doi.org/10.1017/S003060531800159X Published online by Cambridge University Press

You might also like