Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

Development and Psychopathology (2019), 31, 1143–1156

doi:10.1017/S0954579419000658

Special Issue Article

Borderline personality disorder and emotion dysregulation


Alexander L. Chapman
Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, Vancouver, BC, Canada

Abstract
Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is a severe and complex disorder characterized by instability across many life domains, including
interpersonal relations, behavior, and emotions. A core feature and contributor to BPD, emotion dysegulation (ED), consists of deficits
in the ability to regulate emotions in a manner that allows the individual to pursue important goals or behave effectively in various contexts.
Biosocial developmental models of BPD have emphasized a transaction of environmental conditions (e.g., invalidating environments and
adverse childhood experiences) with key genetically linked vulnerabilities (e.g., impulsivity and emotional vulnerability) in the development
of ED and BPD. Emerging evidence has begun to highlight the complex, heterotypic pathways to the development of BPD, with key heritable
vulnerability factors possibly interacting with aspects of the rearing environment to produce worsening ED and an adolescent trajectory con-
sisting of self-damaging behaviors and eventual BPD. Adults with BPD have shown evidence of a variety of cognitive, physiological, and
behavioral characteristics of ED. As the precursors to the development of ED and BPD have become clearer, prevention and treatment efforts
hold great promise for reducing the long-term suffering, functional impairment, and considerable societal costs associated with BPD.
Keywords: borderline personality disorder, development, emotion, emotion dysregulation
(Received 30 November 2018; accepted 24 March 2019)

Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is a severe and complex could help facilitate the development of more efficient, targeted
disorder characterized by instability in interpersonal relations, treatments.
behavior, and emotions (American Psychiatric Association, Over the past few decades, considerable progress has been
2013). Findings have suggested that the defining and associated made in our understanding of the developmental processes con-
features of BPD are best characterized by a phenotype consisting tributing to BPD and ED, as well as in the treatment of BPD
of emotion dysregulation (ED), relational dysfunction (often and related clinical problem areas among children, adolescents,
referred to as relational disturbance or instability), and impulsiv- and adults. Consistent with a developmental psychopathology
ity (Gunderson, 2007; Links, Helsegrave, & van Reekum, 1999; perspective on the development of personality disorders
McCloskey et al., 2009). Among these features, ED, involving dif- (Cicchetti, 2014), this review addresses theory and research on
ficulty modulating emotional responding, appears particularly the possible biopsychosocial process operating at various levels
central to many of the serious behavioral difficulties associated of development to contribute to the development of ED and
with BPD, including nonsuicidal self-injury (NSSI), suicidal BPD. Contemporary developmental psychopathology models of
behavior, and self-damaging impulsive behaviors (e.g., substance BPD and ED have suggested that key underlying biologically
use, risky sexual behavior, and recklessness). Therefore, under- based, heritable vulnerabilities (e.g., trait impulsivity) manifest
standing how ED develops and crystallizes into a core feature of in a variety of ways throughout development (e.g., attention-
BPD should be considered a high priority. deficit/hyperactivity disorder, early conduct, and mental health
A clearer understanding of the developmental processes con- problems) and confer vulnerability to key environmental risk fac-
tributing to ED could illuminate possible targets for preventative tors (e.g., childhood maltreatment), which exacerbate the develop-
and efficient, targeted treatment interventions. Prevention, in ment of ED and eventual BPD (e.g., see Beauchaine, Hinshaw, &
turn, has the potential to forestall the escalation of serious behav- Bridge, 2019; Crowell, Beauchaine, & Linehan, 2009; Crowell,
ioral problems, functional impairments, and heavy utilization of Kaufman & Beauchaine, 2014).
health and mental health resources associated with BPD. Within
later adolescence and earlier adulthood, when BPD is established,
understanding the processes contributing to and maintaining ED BPD
Many of the defining features of BPD either include or are related to
ED. The symptoms of BPD, represented in both the DSM-V
Author for Correspondence: Alexander L. Chapman, Department of Psychology, (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) and the tenth edition of
Simon Fraser University, 8888 University Drive, Burnaby, BC, Canada V5A 1S6; the ICD-10 (World Health Organization, 2016), represent marked
E-mail: chapman_lab@sfu.ca.
Cite this article: Chapman AL (2019). Borderline personality disorder and emotion
instability in several life domains. According to the DSM-V, five of
dysregulation. Development and Psychopathology 31, 1143–1156. https://doi.org/ nine criteria are required for the diagnosis; these criteria largely cap-
10.1017/S0954579419000658 ture difficulties in relationships, impulsive and self-damaging
© Cambridge University Press 2019
1144 A. L. Chapman

behavior, identity and mood disturbance, and affective instability. emotion regulation and dysregulation. Definitions of emotion
Similarly, in the ICD-10, BPD symptoms fall within the category tend to emphasize that emotions (a) have several components,
“emotionally unstable personality disorder” and include distur- including cognitive, physiological, and behavioral features; (b)
bances in emotions, cognition, relationships, and behavior. ED or evolved over millennia to motivate adaptive behavior and facilitate
instability are central features of BPD within both diagnostic systems. social cohesion in a manner that maximized survival; and (c) gen-
BPD is associated with considerable emotional suffering, erally arise in motivationally relevant contexts, such as contexts
severe behavioral problems, heavy utilization of health and mental that are relevant to an individual’s goals (e.g., the goal of survival,
health resources, and functional impairment. Approximately 70% connecting with others, maintaining social standing, and so
to 80% of people with BPD have a history of NSSI and/or suicide forth; Gross, 2014; Lazarus, 1991; Mauss, Levenson, McCarter,
attempts, and approximately 10% die by suicide (Black, Blum, Wilhelm, & Gross, 2005; Scherer, Schorr, & Johnstone, 2001).
Pfohl, & Hale, 2004; Gunderson, 2001; Paris & Zweig-Frank, When a friend cancels plans, for example, an individual with
2001; Skodol et al., 2002). The high prevalence of co-occurring BPD who is particularly sensitive to rejection or abandonment
disorders further underscores the considerable suffering associ- might respond with intense fear, consisting of physiological
ated with BPD and complicates treatment (as clinicians must be changes (e.g., alterations to heart rate and hormonal, neurotrans-
reasonably proficient in many area). The vast majority of patients mitter, and sympathetic nervous system responding), appraisals
with BPD have at least one additional diagnosis (Fyer, Frances, of the situation as disastrous or threatening (e.g., “He hates
Sullivan, Hurt, & Clarkin, 1988; Zanarini et al., 1998). Studies me.” “He doesn’t want to be my friend.”), and actions functioning
have suggested that up to two-thirds of patients with BPD have to prevent perceived negative outcomes (e.g., texting or calling
substance use problems, over half have PTSD, and well over the friend repeatedly, seeking reassurance). The situation may be
half have a co-occurring anxiety disorder (up to 88% in one especially relevant to the individual’s goals if he is afraid to be
study of inpatients; Zanarini et al., 1998; Zarini, Frankenburg, alone, highly values the relationship, or historically has experi-
Hennen, Reich, & Silk, 2004). Major depressive disorder is partic- enced severe rejection or abandonment.
ularly common, affecting approximately 80% of people with BPD Emotion regulation entails alterations to these different aspects
(Lieb, Zanarini, Schmahl, Linehan, & Bohus 2004; Zanarini et al., of the multicomponent emotion system, involving changes in the
1998, 2004). Consistent with this complex clinical picture and form, frequency, experience, or expression of emotions
high prevalence of self-damaging behaviors, people with BPD (Beauchaine, 2015; Gross, 1998, 2014; Thompson, 1994).
heavily utilize emergency, inpatient, outpatient psychiatric, and Emotion regulation also can occur at various stages of the emo-
primary care services, costing an average of $12,000–$30,000 tion generative process. The process model of emotion regulation
per patient per year (in USD; Comtois, Elwood, Holdcraft, (Gross, 1998), for example, indicates which types of emotion reg-
Smith, & Simpson, 2007; Linehan & Heard, 1999). The prevalence ulation processes occur at different points in the sequence of
of BPD (1.4% to 5.9%) also is comparable to or higher than that events related to an emotional response. The individual could
of other costly disorders, such as schizophrenia and bipolar disor- engage in situation selection by deciding not to put himself in a
der (Lenzenweger, Lane, Loranger, & Kessler, 2007). position where a friend might cancel plans by avoiding the friend
Although rates of remission and recovery (approximately 60% or mitigating the situation by asking if they can reschedule for the
at 16 years after hospital discharge; Zanarini, Frankenburg, next evening. If the situation arises, he could decide to modify his
Reich, & Fitzmaurice, 2012) suggest a prognosis that is more hope- attention by distracting from the situation or related emotions and
ful than previously thought, substantial, and persistent functional focusing on other activities (e.g., binge watching a favorite show)
impairment persists among a majority of BPD patients (Biskin, or focusing on less emotionally evocative aspects of the situation
2015). A sizable proportion of BPD patients experience impair- (e.g., by paying attention when the friend says he wants to get
ments in social and occupational functioning requiring public together net week). He could also engage in cognitive reappraisal
assistance, including psychiatric disability support and Social (e.g., “He doesn’t hate me; he’s just busy. I have a great opportu-
Security disability income benefits (Bateman & Fonagy, 2003; nity to catch up on my show.”). Later in the chain of events, he
Bateman & Fonagy, 2008; Grant et al., 2008; Jørgensen et al., might use response modulation strategies (e.g., diaphragmatic
2009; Koons et al., 2006; Soloff & Chiappetta, 2017; Zanarini, breathing or progressive muscle relaxation) to dampen the phys-
Jacoby, Frankenburg, Reich, & Fitzmaurice, 2009). In one longitu- iological arousal associated with fear of abandonment. He also
dinal study of patients with BPD, 69.4% fell within the range of might modify his emotional expressive behavior by changing
poor psychosocial functioning at baseline assessment, and this pro- his facial expression or body language. Other options are to
portion only reduced to 51.2% at the 8-year mark of the study avoid reaching out repeatedly or asking for reassurance and
(Soloff & Chiappetta, 2017). In another study examining long-term engaging in behavior that is less compatible with or opposite to
follow-up data following mentalization-based treatment, 54% of fears of loneliness (e.g., “opposite action”; Linehan, 1993b, 2015).
patients remained within the range of poor psychosocial function- An extended version of this process model (Gross, 2014;
ing 5 years following treatment, despite findings supporting the Sheppes, Suri, & Gross, 2015) encapsulates the sequence of emo-
possible efficacy of mentalization-based treatment (Bateman & tion regulation into three key processes: (a) identification, involv-
Fonagy, 2008). Given the likely role of ED in many of the behav- ing the recognition and identification of an emotional response;
ioral and functional difficulties associated with BPD, understand- (b) selection, involving the selection of a strategy to regulate emo-
ing the developmental trajectory of ED will hopefully illuminate tions; and (c) implementation, involving the implementation of an
ways to curtail persistent functional impairment and suffering. emotion regulation strategy. Within each stage, emotion regula-
tion problems can be characterized as aberrations in the percep-
tion or valuation (cost-benefit analysis) of emotional events,
Emotion Regulation and Dysregulation
experiences, or potential regulation strategies. Emotion regulation
Understanding the specific characteristics and development of ED difficulties could also come in the form of problems with the
in BPD requires a broader framework in which to conceptualize selection or implementation of regulation strategies.
Development and Psychopathology 1145

Some researchers have operationalized ED in terms of the Biosocial Developmental Theories of BPD
characteristics of emotional responding (e.g., overly intense or
Among theories of the development of BPD, Linehan’s (1993a)
prolonged responses), whereas others have defined ED primarily
biosocial theory most strongly emphasizes ED. In her early treat-
in terms of deficits in the ability to use explicit emotion regulation
ment development efforts, Linehan (1993a) discovered that a sub-
strategies. Beauchaine (2015), for example, has distinguished
stantial proportion of highly suicidal patients met criteria for
emotion regulation from dysregulation, defining ED as the pres-
BPD. Many of the hallmark interpersonal, emotional, and behav-
ence of emotional responses that are too prolonged, intense,
ioral problems accompanying BPD presented during treatment,
labile, or situationally inappropriate, or maladaptive behaviors
making it difficult to proceed with standard cognitive or behavio-
related to emotional states. Within this framework, evidence for
ral interventions, which largely focused on changing thoughts and
ED would consist of measures of the intensity, duration, or
behavior and building skills. Patients with BPD (who frequently
changeability of emotional responses in particular contexts. The
had a history of pervasive invalidation or maltreatment) often
client who feels intensely distraught for the whole evening after
experienced the change-oriented focus of cognitive–behavioral
a friend has canceled plans would be considered emotionally dys-
therapy as invalidating, became emotionally dysregulated, and
regulated by virtue of maladaptively intense and prolonged emo-
withdrew; became critical or hostile toward the therapist; or
tional responding. Other indications of ED might include the
dropped out. To address these problems, Linehan (1993a) system-
client repeatedly texting the friend or repeatedly seeking reassur-
atically incorporated into this emerging treatment the balance and
ance, expressing hostility or anger, or self-injuring. Gratz and
synthesis of acceptance and change-oriented therapeutic styles
Roemer (2004), in contrast, have framed ED as difficulty with
and strategies within a dialectical theoretical framework. She
the use of key, explicit emotion regulation tasks or behaviors.
also used existing theory and research on BPD to assemble a
Within this model, ED consists of difficulties in one or more of
working model, the biosocial theory, to better understand the
the following areas: emotional awareness and clarity, the ability
development and maintenance of BPD. Along with dialectical
to accept emotional experiences and inhibit impulsive behavior,
and behavioral theory, the biosocial theory became the basis for
and the capacity to regulate emotions. Similarly, another concep-
a comprehensive cognitive–behavioral treatment approach
tualization of ED, advanced in the context of dialectical behavior
(Linehan, 1993a, 1993b, 2015) emphasizing the role of ED in
therapy (DBT), is “the inability, even when one’s best efforts are
the development and maintenance of BPD.
applied, to change in a desired way emotional cues, experiences,
Within the biosocial theory, the transaction of a biologically
actions, verbal responses, and/or nonverbal expressions under
based vulnerability to emotions (emotion vulnerability) with an
normative conditions” (Linehan, Bohus, & Lynch, 2007, p. 583).
invalidating rearing environment contributes to the development
Similar to Beauchaine’s (2015) definition, this conceptualization
of ED. Emotion vulnerability consists of a combination of a low
emphasizes the importance of goals (i.e., “in a desired way”),
threshold for emotional elicitation (emotional sensitivity), intense
and similar to Gratz and Roemer (2004), emphasis is placed on
emotional responses (emotional reactivity), and a prolonged
particular emotion regulation capacities or skills.
return to baseline emotional responding (slow return to baseline).
For the purposes of the present paper, I conceptualize ED
The invalidating environment consists of the indiscriminate rejec-
broadly as a deficit in the implicit (e.g., through physiological
tion of the child’s communication of internal experiences (emo-
homeostatic mechanisms) or explicit (e.g., through effortful emo-
tions, thoughts, and sensations), intermittent reinforcement of
tion regulation) regulation of the emotion system that hampers
emotional escalation (e.g., inconsistent provision of attention
the individual’s ability to engage in effective goal- or value-
and support that is sometimes contingent on extreme emotional
oriented behavior. Persons with BPD, therefore, may evidence ED
expression), and the oversimplification of the ease of problem
through indicators of heightened or prolonged emotional respond-
solving. The biosocial model is a transactional model, whereby
ing, if such responding impedes value- or goal-directed action or
the child’s emotional temperament influences the invalidating
contributes to suffering. Other indicators of ED would include def-
environment, and vice versa. High emotionality elicits invalida-
icits in the skills needed to select or implement effective emotion
tion, and invalidation exacerbates high emotionality and does
regulation strategies (i.e., deficits in explicit emotion regulation).
not provide the type of emotional socialization needed to develop
The distinction between an overly intense or prolonged emo-
core emotion regulation capacities. Over time, the individual
tional responding and an individual’s capacity to effectively regu-
develops ED, and various behavioral problems characteristic of
late emotions has important developmental implications and is
BPD emerge (e.g., NSSI, suicide attempts, substance use, and
likely to continue to be a subject of debate. One model is that
harmful impulsive behaviors) and are negatively reinforced and
ED in BPD is essentially a behavioral skill deficit fostered by
maintained by the reduction of intolerable emotional arousal
the invalidating overlaid on an emotionally vulnerable tempera-
and/or changes in the social environment. Within this framework,
ment (as suggested by Linehan’s original theory; Linehan,
many of the serious, harmful behaviors (e.g., NSSI, suicidal behav-
1993a). Alternatively, the situation may be more complex, with
ior, reckless impulsivity, drug and alcohol use, etc.) associated
ED consisting of both characteristics of emotional responding
with BPD occur in response to or function to reduce ED
and a deficit in the capacities needed to regulate emotions (as sug-
(Linehan, 1993a). For example, NSSI and other problem behav-
gested by Beauchaine, 2015; Crowell et al., 2009, 2014). In both
iors characteristic of BPD often result in negative or positive rein-
cases, developmental processes and interventions that encourage
forcement through the up- or downregulation of emotions
the development of emotion regulation capacities or skills
(Brown, Comtois, & Linehan, 2002; Chapman, Gratz, & Brown,
would be important. With the more complex model, however, it
2006; Kleindienst et al., 2008; Linehan, 1993a; Nock &
would also be important to examine how various psychosocial
Prinstein, 2004). These behaviors also further contribute to dis-
and biological processes shape how emotional response character-
ruptions in functioning, relationships, and so forth.
istics (e.g., high reactivity and sensitivity, or slow return to base-
At the time of the publication of Linehan’s (1993a) biosocial
line), rather than simply coping or emotion regulation behaviors,
theory, the literature on emotions and emotion regulation in
develop over time.
1146 A. L. Chapman

BPD was scant. Even the broader literature on emotions and emo- precursors to the development of adult BPD. These and other
tion regulation was in its infancy until the early 1990s (Gross, behaviors commonly associated with BPD (e.g., drug and alcohol
1998, 2014; Thompson, 1994). Much of the developmental liter- use, reckless, and self-damaging behavior) become entrenched
ature on psychopathology focused on syndromes of disinhibition due to repeated reinforcement in the form of emotion regulation
(Patterson & Newman, 1993), such as attention-deficit/hyperac- and/or changes in the social environment. Fifth, in adolescence,
tivity disorder (ADHD), and the developmental factors influenc- the presence of ED more generally and NSSI and suicidal behavior
ing the progression from child conduct problems to adult more specifically continue to contribute to risk for the develop-
antisocial personality disorder (e.g., coercive behavioral processes; ment of adult BPD.
Patterson, DeBaryshe, & Ramsey, 1989). The biosocial theory was,
therefore, based primarily on clinical observations, findings on the
enduring effects of childhood maltreatment, and existing emotion The Development of ED in BPD
and behavioral science. Since then, the literature on the develop-
As described by Crowell et al. (2014), the development of BPD
ment of psychopathology more broadly, and BPD and ED more
more broadly and ED more specifically, likely follows a complex,
specifically, has proliferated, providing an expanded picture of
heterotypic trajectory (Belsky et al., 2012). Key heritable factors
the potential factors contributing to ED in BPD.
(e.g., trait impulsivity and possibly neuroticism) appear to elevate
the risk of a range of childhood emotional and behavioral prob-
Crowell et al.’s (2009) biosocial developmental model of BPD lems and may contribute to vulnerability to the adverse environ-
mental circumstances that foster the development of ED. Key
Crowell et al. (2009, 2014) have proposed an expanded biosocial
signs of worsening ED often emerge in adolescence, where under-
developmental model of BPD, incorporating developments in
lying dispositional factors (such as impulsivity) may increase vul-
research on developmental psychopathology as well as biological
nerability to alterations in the development of the social cognitive
and psychosocial factors associated with BPD. Compared with
capacities needed to effectively regulate emotions. During this
Linehan’s (1993a) biosocial theory, which proposes that emotion
period, worsening ED and related interpersonal and behavioral
vulnerability is the key biologically based vulnerability factor,
problems, along with various peer and other environmental influ-
Crowell et al. (2009, 2014) more strongly emphasize the role of
ences fostering maladaptive coping (e.g., through peer and possi-
trait impulsivity, a heritable genetic predisposition that confers
bly parental modeling processes and influences), elevate the risk
vulnerability to various childhood behavioral disorders (e.g.,
of NSSI and suicidal behavior, which further contribute to the
oppositional defiant disorder and ADHD). Crowell et al. (2009,
development of BPD. In adulthood, where BPD tends to show
2014; see also Beauchaine, Hinshaw, et al., 2019) propose that
more temporal stability (compared with adolescence and child-
trait impulsivity moderates the effects of invalidating or adverse
hood), findings have shown evidence of a variety of biological,
rearing contexts, such that those with high trait impulsivity are
cognitive, and behavioral characteristics of ED that reflect some
particularly vulnerable to developing ED and related behavioral
of these earlier developmental processes.
problems (e.g., NSSI and suicide attempts) in these contexts.
Instead of necessarily serving as a biologically based vulnerability
factor, within Crowell et al.’s (2009, 2014) model, emotion vulner-
Key genetic and heritable vulnerability factors
ability and dysregulation develop in the context of the invalidating
rearing environment, primarily through coercive conflict- Heritable factors may play an important role in the development
escalation processes, the intermittent reinforcement of emotional of BPD and ED, predisposing individuals to be vulnerable to the
lability, abuse, neglect, and other adverse circumstances. effects of adverse environments in childhood and adolescence. In
This expanded biosocial developmental model has five key turn, environmental risk factors may influence the expression of
tenets, some of which have garnered additional support since particular genetic and biological propensities (e.g., see Amad,
the model was articulated about 10 years ago. First, as mentioned, Ramoz, Thomas, Jardri, & Gorwood, 2014, regarding a “plastic-
trait impulsivity is a key biologically based and heritable vulnera- ity” theory). As mentioned, Crowell et al. (2009, 2014) have sug-
bility factor for the development of BPD and other externalizing gested that impulsive children are particularly likely to develop
behavioral problems sometimes referred to as syndromes of disin- behavioral and emotional difficulties in adverse or invalidating
hibition (e.g., oppositional defiant disorder, ADHD, conduct dis- environments. Heritable factors also may operate in a transac-
order, eventual adult antisocial personality disorder, etc.). Second, tional manner, whereby the heritable trait and its manifestations
adverse, invalidating environmental contexts shape the develop- influence and are exacerbated by environmental factors. Highly
ment of emotional vulnerability (referred to as “emotional labil- impulsive children may be particularly difficult to parent, present-
ity”), which can be considered both biologically based and ing behavioral challenges through inattentiveness, hyperactivity,
highly susceptible to environmental influence. Third, trait impul- noncompliance, and so forth. Parents who share with their chil-
sivity continues to interact with environmental risk factors to dren a propensity toward impulsivity may be prone to use coer-
exacerbate ED and disrupt normative social and emotional devel- cive tactics to control their children’s behavior as well as to
opment. Among youth who are higher in impulsivity, various erratically and intermittently reinforce emotional escalation.
environmental risks may have a more potent effect on their devel- Invalidation, abuse, or neglect, along with parental modeling of
opment of behavioral, social, and emotional difficulties, compared maladaptive behaviors (e.g., poor impulse control and emotional
with those who are lower in trait impulsivity. Fourth, in adoles- lability) may further exacerbate the child’s existing propensities
cence, at-risk individuals begin to engage in maladaptive coping toward heightened emotionality and impulsivity. Heritable factors
and problematic behaviors that pose risk for BPD, further disrupt may also influence alterations in the development of social cogni-
the development of emotion regulation capacities, and carry their tive capacities needed to regulate emotions. Some have suggested,
own negative interpersonal and intrapersonal consequences. In for example, that impulsivity plays a key role in the development
particular, NSSI and suicide attempts are considered key of neuropsychological deficits in executive functioning that
Development and Psychopathology 1147

influence later conduct problems (Caspi, Henry, McGee, Moffit, behavioral indicators of impulsivity (e.g., delay discounting, risk
& Silva, 1995). taking, and behavioral disinhibition; Bevilacqua & Goldman,
Findings from several studies have suggested an important role 2013). Similarly, findings on the heritability of the FFM traits
of heritable factors in the propensity to develop BPD and ED. have generally indicated significant and moderate heritability esti-
First-degree relatives of individuals with BPD are several times mates for neuroticism (ranging from 15% to 50%; Power & Pluess,
more likely to have BPD than the general population (Baron, 2015; Vukasović & Bratko, 2015). Children with high trait impul-
Gruen, Asnis, & Lord, 1985; Links, Steiner, & Huxley, 1988; sivity and neuroticism may be particularly vulnerable to the types
Zanarini, Gunderson, Marino, Schwartz, & Frankenburg, 1988), of environmental influences that shape the development of ED
and twin studies have suggested moderate heritability for BPD and eventual BPD. Further, the high co-occurrence of mood
(.40 to around .70). Torgersen et al. (2000), for example, found and anxiety disorders with BPD suggests a heterotypic model in
a heritability for BPD of .69 among 221 twin pairs assessed for that neuroticism may manifest in different ways at various stages
the presence of personality disorders. Another study of 5,000 of development, culminating (with the right mix of experiences)
twins in the Netherlands, Belgium, and Australia found consistent in disorders characterized by anxiety and mood disturbance.
heritability estimates of .42 using a self-report measure of BPD Therefore, it seems likely that, when it comes to underlying her-
features (Distel et al., 2008). Another study, focused on itable vulnerabilities and their manifestations throughout devel-
Norwegian twins, found evidence that BPD features are best opment, BPD straddles the internalizing and externalizing
accounted for by a model including a moderately heritable over- dimensions.
arching latent “BPD” factor (Reichborn-Kjennerud et al., 2013),
with heritable and unique environmental factors accounting for
Adverse rearing environments
55% and 45% of this factor, respectively. Within this model,
two subfactors included an interpersonal factor, which had low Adverse childhood environments have received considerable
heritability (2.2%), and an affective instability factor, with herita- attention as key environmental factors that influence the develop-
bility of 29.3%. In addition, the BPD impulsivity criterion, con- ment of BPD and ED (Calkins & Hill, 2007; Eisenberg et al.
sisting of self-inflicted injuries and impulsive, self-damaging 1999). The literature on childhood maltreatment in relation to
behaviors, received the majority of its genetic influence (which BPD is substantial, so I will highlight a few key findings here.
was 44.6%) from what the authors referred to as “criterion- Adults with BPD tend to report high rates of childhood adversity,
specific” factors not accounted for by the heritable influences suggesting an important role for trauma or abuse, at least in some
related to the overall BPD factor. These findings support the exis- cases. Rates of reported past childhood sexual abuse among per-
tence of a highly heritable impulsivity dimension. In contrast, sons with BPD are high (Zanarini et al., 2002). Findings also have
unique environmental factors seem to account for the majority suggested that persons with BPD report a higher prevalence of
of the variability in the interpersonal and affective symptoms of childhood sexual (Paris, Zweig-Frank, & Guzder, 1994; Yen
BPD. Together, these findings provide at least indirect support et al., 2002) and physical abuse (Paris et al., 1994) than those
for the notion that impulsivity is a key heritable risk factor for with other personality disorders. A chart-review study found a
BPD, and that aspects of the rearing environment substantially prevalence of past sexual abuse of 50% among inpatient adoles-
influence the development of ED and interpersonal dysfunction. cents with BPD (Westen, Ludolph, Misle, Ruffins, & Block,
Studies also have examined potential heritable vulnerabilities 1990). Given these findings, some have speculated that BPD rep-
to BPD within the framework of normal personality theory, resents complex PTSD, suggesting that past trauma drives the ED
largely anchored in the Five-Factor Model (FFM) of personality, characteristic of BPD (Cloitre, Garvert, Weiss, Carlson, & Bryant,
consisting of openness, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeable- 2014; Ford & Courtois, 2014; Trippany, Helm, & Simpson, 2006;
ness, and neuroticism. Distel et al. (2009), for example, examined van der Kolk, Pelcovitz, Roth, & Mandel, 1996). Other researchers
FFM traits in relation to BPD in a large study of twins from the have suggested that past abuse or trauma are neither necessary
Netherlands, Belgium, and Australia. The overall heritability esti- nor sufficient for a BPD diagnosis (Zanarini et al., 1997).
mate for BPD was .45, and findings indicated that all of the Although it appears that childhood adversity is unusually com-
genetic variance in BPD was shared by FFM traits. Among asso- mon in BPD, up to 50% of adults with BPD do not report child-
ciations of FFM traits with BPD, neuroticism had the strongest hood sexual abuse, and up to 54% do not meet criteria for PTSD
association, accounting for 45% of the variability in BPD traits, (Lieb et al., 2004). It is likely, therefore, that trauma and severe
and the additive genetic correlation of BPD with neuroticism childhood adversity plays an important role for some people,
was the highest among the FFM traits (r = .95). It is interesting and that characteristics of childhood environments likely transact
to note that nearly one-third of the variance in unique environ- with biological and other factors in complex ways to influence the
mental effects on BPD was not shared with the FFM traits. The development of ED and BPD (Chapman, Hope, & Turner, in
authors concluded that heritable vulnerabilities conferred by press; Crowell et al., 2009, 2014).
high neuroticism and low conscientiousness and agreeableness Some research has examined the types of childhood maltreat-
might make people vulnerable to some of the unique environ- ment that might be particularly associated with BPD. One study,
mental factors that play a role in the development of BPD. for example, examined BPD features among maltreated and non-
These findings, in conjunction with findings on the heritability maltreated children (Hecht, Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Crick, 2014).
and association of trait impulsivity with BPD and possible precur- Findings indicated that greater chronicity of maltreatment was
sors to BPD, suggest that heritable vulnerability to BPD and ED associated with more severe BPD features. Further, the number
may consist of a combination of trait impulsivity and neuroticism. of developmental periods in which maltreatment occurred pre-
Findings have indicated that both neuroticism and impulsivity are dicted whether individuals fell within a high-risk for the BPD
at least moderately heritable. One study examining heritability for group (those with high scores on three or more categories of
different forms of impulsivity found heritability of 45%–50% for symptoms; Hecht et al., 2014). Maltreatment status was associated
impulsivity assessed by trait measures and 28%–55% for with higher scores on all categories of BPD features, suggesting
1148 A. L. Chapman

that maltreatment may not uniquely influence the development of and other brain regions (Liston, McEwen, & Casey, 2009).
particular types of symptoms, such as emotional instability or These findings indicate possible mechanisms for the lingering
ED-related features. effects of childhood adversity on the emotion regulation system
Some findings also have suggested that ED may play an impor- (Diamond, 2018). Studies examining brain structure and func-
tant mediating role in the association between childhood experi- tioning in BPD, therefore, should incorporate measures of child-
ences and the later development of BPD. In a study examining hood adversity and clinical control groups with PTSD or past
the influence of mothers’ personality disorder symptoms on child abuse.
child psychopathology, Kaufman, Puzia, Mead, Crowell, and
Beauchaine (2017) found that children’s difficulty regulating
Emerging BPD and related behavioral problems in adolescence
emotions partially mediated the association of a composite of
maternal BPD/antisocial personality disorder symptoms on inter- Adolescence appears to be a particularly important period mark-
nalizing and externalizing symptoms a year later. In another study ing increased severity of ED and the emergence of some of the key
examining the association of childhood parental emotion social- behavioral problem areas associated with BPD (e.g., NSSI and sui-
ization (reported retrospectively) with ED and self-harm among cide attempts). Increasingly complex social dynamics, academic
college students, emotional invalidation (specifically, punishment demands, conflicts between autonomy and dependence, among
and neglect) predicted self-harm, and difficulty evaluating emo- other factors, likely test and shape emotion regulation capabilities
tions (an aspect of difficulties with emotion regulation) mediated during adolescence (Ahmed, Bittencourt-Hewitt, & Sebastian,
the relationship between these parental responses and self-harm 2015; Casey et al., 2008). In an excellent review of the develop-
(Buckholdt, Parra, & Jobe-Shields, 2009). Another study exam- ment of neurocognitive foundations of emotion regulation,
ined the association of reports of childhood parental emotion Ahmed et al. have argued that the amygdala, various regions of
socialization and trauma with current BPD features among college the prefrontal cortex, and the connectivity of prefrontal and lim-
students, finding that difficulties regulating emotions mediated bic regions all mature during adolescence (with some of these
the association of emotion socialization and parental psychologi- regions continuing to develop into young adulthood) and are
cal control (but not childhood trauma) with BPD features (Hope directly relevant to the development of emotion regulation capac-
& Chapman, in press). Some of this literature on parenting, ities (Ahmed et al., 2015). This pattern of development is partic-
trauma, abuse, and other childhood experiences is limited by its ularly relevant to ED and BPD.
focus on adult reports of childhood experiences, and lack of Findings have indicated that some of the cognitive and social
examination of interactions of heritable and environmental cognitive capacities that facilitate emotion regulation show partic-
factors. ularly steep development during adolescence (Blakemore &
Addressing some of these limitations, Belsky et al. (2012) Robbins, 2012; Shaw et al., 2008). Adults with BPD show deficits
examined the potential role of environmental and heritable fac- in a variety of neurocognitive domains, some of which are partic-
tors in the development of BPD in a longitudinal cohort study, ularly important for effective emotion regulation (e.g., attention,
following twins (N = 1,116) from birth to age 12. Findings indi- cognitive flexibility, and planning; Ruocco, 2005; Unoka &
cated that maltreatment in childhood more strongly predicted Richman, 2016). The onset of various internalizing and external-
BPD features at 12 years among children with family histories izing behavioral problems in adolescence (Lancefield, Raudino,
of psychiatric disorder. Findings from this study painted a com- Downs, & Laurens, 2016) could indicate difficulties in the devel-
plex picture of the development of BPD and indicated that opment of emotion regulation capacities, perhaps stemming from
some of the unique environmental influences accounting for a earlier and ongoing transactions of a vulnerable temperament
proportion of the variance in BPD features (at least among with aspects of the rearing environment. In particular, the devel-
those with high levels of BPD features) may include high maternal opment of certain types of behavioral problems, such as NSSI, sui-
negative expressed emotion. cidal behavior, and substance use problems, might at least
Some research on the neurobiological correlates of adverse indirectly indicate that ED is beginning to crystallize. Therefore,
childhood experiences has suggested potential mechanisms it appears that (a) basic capacities and characteristics of brain
underlying a link between childhood adversity, ED, and BPD. functioning necessary for emotion regulation develop differently
As discussed later, adults with BPD appear to be prone to poor in those with BPD, and (b) such developmental differences
frontal regulation of the limbic system, whereby the connectivity begin to become more apparent in adolescence.
of prefrontal regions with the amygdala appears reduced in BPD Findings on the trajectory of BPD symptoms in childhood,
patients. Typically, increases in the activity of certain frontal areas adolescence, and adulthood have further suggested continuity in
(e.g., the ventrolateral and medial prefrontal cortex in particular) these symptoms and indicated that adolescence may be a key
are associated with reductions in the activity of the amygdala period in which to identify and try to prevent the further devel-
(Lieberman et al., 2007). Some findings, however, have suggested opment of ED. A study of the association of childhood psychopa-
that persons exposed to significant early life stress may show a thology and the later development of BPD (the Pittsburgh Girls
positive association of ventrolateral prefrontal and amygdala acti- Study; N = 1,233; Stepp, Burke, Hipwell, & Loeber, 2012) found
vation (Taylor, Eisenberger, Saxbe, Lehman, & Lieberman, 2006). that childhood ADHD and oppositional defiant disorder symp-
If the prefrontal cortex typically acts as the “brakes” on the amyg- toms predicted poor impulse control, problems with self-
dala in normally developing individuals, the opposite may occur regulation, and BPD symptoms in adolescence. These findings
among those exposed to adverse childhood events. Further, ani- support the biosocial developmental model’s (Crowell et al.,
mal research has suggested that the prefrontal cortex is particu- 2009, 2014) hypothesis that early impulse control problems are
larly dense with cortisol receptors (Sanchez, Young, Plotsky, & key indicators of a developmental trajectory toward increasing
Insel, 2000), and that stress increases dopamine activity in the ED and BPD.
prefrontal cortex (Roth, Tam, Ida, Yang, & Deutch, 1988) and BPD symptoms in adolescence also predict later behavioral
may disrupt the communication between the prefrontal cortex problems in early adulthood. The Children in the Community
Development and Psychopathology 1149

Study, for example, followed a diverse group of 800 children in insular cortex volume among adolescent participants with BPD
various areas of New York over a 20-year period, examining the and healthy controls and found no differences in volume between
trajectory of adolescents experiencing symptoms of BPD these groups. BPD participants, however, with a history of violent
(Cohen, Crawford, Johnson, & Kasen, 2005). Some of the findings behavior over the past 6 months demonstrated a smaller bilateral
suggested that childhood sexual abuse predicted BPD but not insular volume, compared with BPD participants with no such
other personality disorder symptoms in adolescence. BPD symp- history (Takahashi et al., 2009). Taken together, these findings
toms in early adolescence (mean age = 14) predicted BPD symp- suggest that some of the brain differences associated with BPD
toms as well as lower academic and occupational attainment and may begin to emerge in a narrower fashion (differences in volume
greater psychosocial impairment in early adulthood (mean age = in fewer areas compared with adulthood) in adolescence com-
33; e.g., lower global assessment of functioning and lower role pared with adulthood, that these brain differences may not be
function). Consistent with other research (Zanarini et al., 2004), unique to BPD, and that the association of violence and brain vol-
the Children in the Community Study found age-related declines ume and functioning should be further explored. Violence may be
in some BPD symptoms (Winograd, Cohen, & Chen, 2008). a more extreme indicator of high trait impulsivity and ED among
Some of these behavioral problems likely are influenced by and adolescents at risk for BPD.
influence the development of ED and BPD. Adolescents with
developing BPD symptoms, for example, may be particularly Self-injury and suicidal behavior
prone to substance use problems. Substance use, in turn, may Models of the development of ED, BPD, and related problems
have direct effects on brain development, further exacerbate psy- (e.g., NSSI and suicidal behavior) have emphasized important
chosocial stressors (e.g., impairing functioning and contributing transitions and transactions occurring in adolescence. The devel-
to academic problems and relationship discord), and worsen the opment of NSSI and suicidal behavior in early to middle adoles-
trajectory of BPD symptoms (Bornovalova et al., 2018; Zanarini cence, in particular, has received particular attention. Beauchaine,
et al., 2004). A recent study of female twins investigated genetic Hinshaw, et al. (2019) have proposed a similar model as the bio-
and environmental influences on the association of BPD features, social developmental model of BPD to explain the development of
major depressive disorder, and alcohol use disorders (AUD; NSSI and suicidal behavior among adolescents. In particular, chil-
Bornovalova et al., 2017). Following adolescents from age 14 dren with impulsive temperaments are especially vulnerable to the
through early adulthood (age 24), the findings of this study sug- effects of maltreatment, and are likely to have interpersonal diffi-
gested that genetic and unique environmental factors accounted culties and to be involved in peer contexts that support the devel-
for the covariation of BPD with major depressive disorder and opment of severely maladaptive coping behaviors in early
AUD over time, and that AUD predicted lower decline in BPD adolescence, such as NSSI (i.e., through modeling and contagion
features. These findings suggest that at least some of the behavio- effects; Derbidge & Beauchaine, 2014). NSSI emerges as an indi-
ral problems co-occurring with BPD may worsen the BPD cator of worsening ED and a way to regulate ED. In support of
symptom trajectory, possibly through their effects on brain devel- this model, findings from one study suggested that adolescents
opment and exacerbation of ED and contextual factors (e.g., inter- with histories of recent NSSI or suicide attempts (at least one epi-
personal problems and stress). Similarly, other longitudinal sode in the past 6 months) may be particularly reactive to mater-
research with adults has indicated that the presence of SUDs, nal behavior during discussions of conflictual topics (Crowell
PTSD, and mood and anxiety disorders reduces the likelihood et al., 2014). In this study, adolescents with NSSI or suicide
of remission from BPD (Zanarini et al., 2004). Another recent attempt histories demonstrated both behavioral and physiological
longitudinal study examined self-harm and suicidal behaviors as reactivity to mothers’ behavior, but depressed adolescents only
predictors of the development of BPD 5 years later among adoles- showed behavioral reactivity, suggesting a potentially unique bio-
cent psychiatric inpatients (Homan, Sim, Fargo, & Twohig, 2017). logical vulnerability among self-injuring or suicidal adolescents.
Findings suggested that only suicide threats uniquely predicted Findings from other research have suggested that children with
BPD 5 years later (Homan et al., 2017). As one of the key func- ADHD are particularly vulnerable to developing self-inflected
tions of emotions is to communicate, suicide threats may indicate injury if they have had a history of childhood maltreatment (see
a combination of high ED and deficits in communication skills, Beauchaine, Hinshaw, et al., 2019, for a review; see also
along with a learning history reinforcing emotional escalation. Derbidge & Beauchaine, 2014; Guendelman, Owens, Galan,
Some studies have investigated whether some of the differences Gard, & Hinshaw, 2015; Swanson, Owens, & Hinshaw, 2014).
in the volume of certain brain areas that have been observed Similarly, Miller and Prinstein (2019) have proposed that
among adults with BPD are also observable among adolescents abnormal development of the biological stress response system
at risk of BPD (e.g., those engaging in self-injury) or with an influences the emergence of NSSI and suicidal behavior in adoles-
early diagnosis of BPD. One study, for example, found that self- cence. Within this model, normative changes in the biological
injuring adolescent girls demonstrated reduced volume in the stress response system, such as changes in the hypothalamic–pitu-
insular cortex and right inferior frontal gyrus, compared with itary–adrenal axis and sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous
non-self-injuring controls (Beauchaine, Sauder, Derbridget, & system reactivity, result in a period of enhanced sensitivity to
Uyeji, 2019). Another investigation compared the volume of pre- stressors among adolescents. This may be particularly the case
frontal and orbitofrontal gray matter among adolescents with for interpersonal stressors, given the importance of peer relation-
BPD versus psychiatric and nonpsychiatric controls (Brunner ships in adolescence and evidence of increased sensitivity to social
et al., 2010). Findings indicated that adolescents with BPD rewards and punishments (Casey, Jones, & Hare, 2008) and pre-
showed reduced volume in the dorsolateral frontal gyrus and frontal brain response to social evaluation (Sommerville, 2013).
left orbitofrontal gyrus compared with nonpsychiatric controls, Miller and Prinstein (2019) have suggested that abnormalities in
but that there were no differences in the volume of these or the development of the stress response system (e.g., reduced con-
other regions between clinical controls and BPD participants nectivity of the prefrontal and limbic systems) may begin to
(Brunner et al., 2010). Takahashi et al. (2009) also examined appear in adolescence and influence the development of NSSI
1150 A. L. Chapman

and suicidal behavior. Some studies, accordingly, have found that times in the past several years (Carpenter & Trull, 2013; Dixon-
youth with suicide histories have demonstrated reduced amygdala Gordon, Haliczer, & Conkey, in press; Rosenthal et al., 2008).
and PFC connectivity in response to emotional stimuli, a finding
that parallels some of the research on adult BPD (discussed later
Cognitive aspects of ED
in this review; Johnston et al., 2017). As mentioned earlier, struc-
tural brain differences associated with self-injury and BPD may Cognitively, persons with BPD tend to have difficulty with emo-
begin to emerge in adolescence as well, albeit in a narrower fash- tional clarity and awareness (Leible & Snell, 2004), identifying and
ion than among adults patients with BPD. describing emotions (Wolff, Stiglmayr, Bretz, Lammers, &
Auckenthaler, 2007; Derks, Westerhof, & Bohlmeijer, 2016),
and a tendency toward less specific differentiation among negative
The Course of BPD
emotions and lower emotional granularity (i.e., the tendency to
In this section, I briefly summarize some of the research on the represent emotions in a nonspecific manner; Suvak et al., 2011).
course of BPD and BPD symptoms. As many of the studies Effective emotion regulation often requires and is facilitated by
have begun following participants with BPD at a mean age corre- clarity and awareness of an individual’s emotional state. Not
sponding to young adulthood, this research is probably best knowing what specific emotion one is feeling would make it dif-
viewed as a snapshot of how BPD progresses through young ficult to select adaptive emotion regulation strategies. Persons
adulthood. In addition, although there have been many studies with BPD also may be especially prone to appraise certain situa-
of the course of BPD, I will focus primarily on findings from tions or emotional experiences as excessively negative or intoler-
the McLean Study of Adult Development, a large study of BPD able or as having disastrous consequences. Linehan (1993a) has
inpatients, followed posthospitalization for several years suggested that some persons with BPD are phobic of emotions.
(Zanarini, Frankenburg, Hennen, & Silk, 2003). Findings from Clinically, persons with BPD often describe their emotions as
this research have suggested a fairly hopeful prognosis for many overwhelming, out of control, and intolerable.
people with BPD, with findings from the 16-year point indicating Other cognitive characteristics of ED include difficulty regulat-
that 99% of patients achieved at least 2 years of remission from the ing attention to emotionally evocative events. Linehan (1993a) has
diagnosis of BPD, 60% of patients recovered, and 5% died by sui- proposed that persons with BPD may have difficulty disengaging
cide (Zanarini et al., 2012). Earlier studies based on this data set their attention from emotional material. Some research indicating
examining patterns of symptom remission have indicated that the that people with BPD are particularly prone to rumination, for
dysregulated behavioral patterns that are likely related to ED tend example, suggests difficulty disengaging from emotional events
to subside over time, but that enduring emotional suffering in the (Abela, Payne, & Moussaly, 2003; Smith, Grandin, Alloy, &
form of mood symptoms often persists. Some of the most persis- Abramson, 2006), and the cascade model of BPD posits that
tent symptoms, even among people who no longer meet diagnos- NSSI is particularly likely when rumination has amplified nega-
tic criteria, included mood and emotional disturbances (Zanarini tive emotional experiences to an intolerable level (Selby &
et al., 2003). Although NSSI, suicidality, affective instability, and Joiner, 2009). Findings from some studies have suggested the
serious interpersonal instability subsided relatively rapidly, emo- presence of an attentional bias toward negative and BPD-related
tional symptoms such as chronic dysphoria and intense anger stimuli (words; Kaiser, Jacobs, Domes, & Arntz, 2017); this pos-
tended to remit more slowly (Zanarini et al., 2007). Moreover, sible bias may amplify emotional responding and disrupt the abil-
general impulsivity and recklessness also tended to remit more ity of persons with BPD to disengage or distance themselves from
slowly over a 10-year period. This finding suggested that, while emotional events. The findings on attentional biases in BPD, how-
the severe, emotion-driven behaviors such as NSSI and suicide ever, are complex, sometimes inconsistent, and may depend on
attempts appear to resolve fairly quickly (Zanarini et al., 2007), the laboratory paradigms and stimuli used (Kaiser et al., 2017).
more enduring tendencies toward impulsivity may persist and
appear in various forms, again consistent with heterotypic models
Physiological aspects of ED
highlighting trait impulsivity as an enduring predisposition to
BPD (e.g., Crowell et al., 2009, 2014). Findings on physiological regulation of emotions in BPD paint a
fairly complex picture. Activity of the parasympathetic nervous
system, as indexed by respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA), has
Characteristics of ED in Adults With BPD
been theorized to be an indication of emotion regulation
With these findings on the developmental factors and periods in (Porges, 2001), with higher RSA indicating greater vagal influence
mind, it is helpful to consider what characterizes ED among on heart rate variability. Research on parasympathetic reactivity in
adults with BPD. What is the net result of the potential genetic, BPD remains fairly limited. Some studies have found decreasing
biological, and psychosocial factors operating at various points RSA among BPD patients in response to emotionally evocative
in development? Findings across several studies over the past cou- films relative to nonpatient (Austin, Riniolo, & Porges, 2007) or
ple of decades have begun to clarify the nature of ED in BPD. The clinical controls (Fitzpatrick & Kuo, 2015). Another study did
summary below is based on the conceptualization of emotions as not find differences in RSA activity among BPD participants in
systemic responses to motivationally relevant situations, consist- response to a laboratory emotional stressor, compared with non-
ing of cognitive, physiological, and behavioral components. ED, clinical controls (Weinberg, Klonsky, & Hajcak, 2009).
therefore, consists of aberrations in cognitive, physiological, or Brain imaging research has examined the connectivity of activ-
behavioral responding that impede the individual’s ability to ity in the prefrontal and limbic regions, as deficient inhibitory
achieve goals or engage in effective action in particular contexts. influence of certain prefrontal areas on the amygdala could be
I will focus here on cognitive, behavioral, and physiological factors considered an aspect or mechanism of ED. Some findings have
that are indicative of ED in BPD, rather than on the literature on indicated that participants with BPD show a combination of
emotional responding in BPD, which has been reviewed a few heightened activation of the amygdala in response to emotional
Development and Psychopathology 1151

stimuli (Donegan et al., 2003; Hazlett et al., 2012; particularly the sham condition. Findings from other research examining the
left amygdala) with lower activation of the dorsolateral prefrontal effect of painful stimulation on brain activity among BPD patients
cortex (Schulze, Schmahl, & Niedtfeld, 2016). In contrast, a meta also have suggested that pain increases the functional connectivity
analysis of 11 studies comparing BPD patients to healthy controls of prefrontal regions with the amygdala during emotional stress-
showed greater activation in the insula and posterior cingulate ors (Niedtfeld et al., 2012). Together with findings that adaptive
cortex and lower activation in the amygdala, subgenual anterior strategies such as cognitive reappraisal under-recruit prefrontal
cingulate, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Ruocco, regions associated with emotion regulation, these findings echo
Amirthavasagam, Choi-Kain, & McMain, 2013). Extant findings, what self-injuring patients with BPD often say about how letting
therefore, are mixed with respect to brain-related characteristics of go of NSSI and learning more adaptive strategies is sometimes an
ED in BPD. Further, the research generally has compared partic- uphill battle.
ipants with BPD to healthy controls in terms of responses to
unpleasant, emotionally evocative images; thus, more research is
Behavioral aspects of ED or heightened BPD features
needed to determine whether these patterns of brain activity are
specific to BPD or accounted for by elevated psychopathology, Other studies have examined the behavioral aspects of emotion
or relevant to emotional reactions to other situations or stimuli. regulation in BPD. BPD is associated with limited access to emo-
Some evidence has suggested that differences in brain activity tion regulation strategies (Glenn & Klonsky, 2009; Salsman &
during the implementation of certain emotion regulation strate- Linehan, 2012), and this would suggest that persons with BPD
gies may hamper the effectiveness of these strategies among might rely primarily on maladaptive strategies that produce
those with BPD. One study, for example, examined the effects quick relief. Some findings, however, have suggested that rather
of cognitive reappraisal among patients with BPD and past than simply relying on maladaptive emotion regulation strategies,
trauma, compared with healthy controls with and without past people with BPD may tend to engage in greater effort to their
trauma (Lang et al., 2012). Compared with non-trauma-exposed emotions across both adaptive and maladaptive strategies
healthy controls, those with BPD and past trauma showed less (Dixon-Gordon, Aldao, & De Los Reyes, 2015), with the possible
activation in the prefrontal cortex during upregulation of emo- exception that they tend to use less acceptance as an emotion reg-
tions and less activation of the anterior cingulate during downre- ulation strategy (Chapman, Dixon-Gordon, & Walters, 2013).
gulation (Lang et al., 2012). These patterns of brain activity may There also is some evidence that people with heightened BPD fea-
be due primarily to past trauma exposure, as there were no differ- tures as well as those with a diagnosis of BPD might experience
ences between trauma-exposed BPD participants and trauma- and report fewer emotional benefits from acceptance versus sup-
exposed healthy controls without PTSD (Lang et al., 2012). In pression and avoidance, both in the laboratory and in daily life
another study, BPD patients and healthy controls viewed pictures (Chapman, Rosenthal, Dixon-Gordon, Turner, & Kuppens,
depicting social interactions and either used cognitive distancing 2017). Compared with healthy controls, however, BPD patients
or simply viewed the pictures (Koenigsberg et al., 2010). find certain adaptive emotion regulation strategies, such as cogni-
Findings indicated that, while healthy controls and BPD patients tive reappraisal and distraction, to be less effective (Sauer et al.,
similarly showed increased activation of the dorsolateral and ven- 2016). This could be due to lack of skillful use of the strategies,
trolateral prefrontal cortex and posterior cingulate when distanc- lesser impact of the strategies on emotional distress (e.g., as sug-
ing, BPD patients showed less deactivation in the amygdala, gested by the research reviewed above on brain functioning in
among other areas (Koenigsberg et al., 2010). Similarly, a recent relation to cognitive reappraisal), problems with the validity of
study compared the functional connectivity of various frontal measures of emotion regulation strategies, or other factors.
regions with the amygdala during an effortful emotion regulation Overall, though, this pattern of findings tentatively suggests that
task (using emotional pictures), finding that BPD patients showed persons with BPD may (a) put forth a fair amount of effort to reg-
weaker increases in functional connectivity compared with non- ulate their emotions, and (b) find this effort largely unsuccessful.
patient controls, but not compared with clinical controls with Other behavioral aspects of emotion dyregulation include
Cluster C personality disorders (Baczkowski et al., 2017). Taken emotion-related disruptions in behavior and engagement in
together, these findings suggest that brain functioning that nor- mood-dependent actions. Consistent with clinical observations
mally facilitates emotion regulation may be impaired in BPD, and research suggesting that persons with BPD tend to engage
but it remains unclear whether these impairments are unique to in maladaptive behaviors when they experience emotional distress
BPD. (e.g., NSSI), some laboratory research has suggested that
In contrast, maladaptive emotion regulation strategies appear people with heightened BPD features or a diagnosis of BPD
to temporarily result in greater connectivity of the amygdala might have difficulty inhibiting behavior or solving social
and prefrontal regions (i.e., prefrontal inhibition of amygdala problems when they are emotionally distressed (Dixon-Gordon,
activity) among BPD patients. In a study modeling the effects Chapman, Lovasz, & Walters, 2011). In addition, a recent study
of NSSI on brain functioning, Reitz et al. (2015) randomly also found that participants with BPD demonstrated a decreased
assigned self-injuring women with BPD (and healthy controls) ability to learn from punishment following an emotional induc-
to an incision or sham condition following a stress induction. tion (Dixon-Gordon, Hackel, Tull, & Gratz, 2018). More research
The incision condition involved a shallow incision administered is needed, however, to determine whether problematic behavioral
by an experimenter with a sharp plastic device, designed as a lab- patterns in BPD are primarily mood dependent. Furthermore,
oratory analogue for NSSI. Whereas healthy controls did not high rates of behaviors appearing to serve emotion regulation
demonstrate evidence of physiological stress-reduction or functions (e.g., NSSI, suicide attempts, substance use, or harmful
increases in prefrontal connectivity in the incision condition, forms of impulsivity), along with a strong association of BPD fea-
women with BPD reported decreased aversive tension (distress), tures with avoidant coping, emotion suppression, and experiential
decreased amygdala activation, and increased prefrontal connec- avoidance more broadly (Beblo et al., 2013; Chapman, Specht, &
tivity (to the superior frontal gyrus) in the incision versus the Cellucci, 2005), could also be considered behavioral indicators of
1152 A. L. Chapman

ED. Findings also have suggested that individuals with BPD have association of parenting with negative outcomes, from shared
difficulty tolerating distress in the service of goal-directed behav- genetic factors, to proactive, evocative, and other person–environ-
ior (Gratz, Frances, Sullivan, Hurt, & Clarkin, 2006), and dissoci- ment transactions. More research is needed to refine and measure
ation in response to stress is a defining feature of BPD and has the construct of invalidation, and more longitudinal research is
been observed clinically and in laboratory and experience sam- needed to determine when (e.g., what developmental period),
pling research (Ebner-Primer et al., 2005). how, and for whom invalidation influences the development of
ED and BPD.
The emerging picture of the complex developmental processes
Conclusions
contributing to BPD and ED have important implications for pre-
Although the developmental processes contributing to the devel- vention and treatment. Beauchaine, Hinshaw, et al. (2019) have
opment of ED and BPD are complex, advances in research and outlined several key steps for the prevention and treatment of ado-
theory have begun to illuminate these processes. Influential theo- lescent NSSI and suicidal behavior, arguing in particular that pre-
ries of the development of BPD have strongly emphasized the role vention should target the invalidating environment through
of ED. Linehan’s (1993a) original biosocial theory proposed the evidence-based parenting interventions, and that treatment
transaction of an emotionally vulnerable temperament and an should address ED (which may be more influenced by environ-
invalidating environment contributes to the development of ED mental factors and easier to address) rather than trait impulsivity
and BPD. More recent extensions of this model (Crowell et al., (which could be a fairly stable tendency). Beauchaine, HInshaw,
2009, 2014) have proposed that ED emerges from the interaction et al. (2019) have argued that we know enough about key precur-
between an impulsive temperament and an invalidating environ- sors and risk factors for NSSI and suicidal behavior that preven-
ment. Findings have suggested that trait impulsivity is a strong tative interventions or treatments should target these precursors
candidate for a heritable vulnerability to BPD, and other findings before the behaviors begin to emerge.
have also suggested a role for neuroticism. It seems likely that Furthermore, findings from recent research has supported this
both factors at play confer vulnerability to the adverse environ- approach. A recent randomized clinical trial examined an adapted
mental contexts that often contribute to the development of ED version of DBT (DBT-C; consisting of individual parent training
and BPD. Broadly, an invalidating rearing environment may and skills training sessions with parents and their children, and
play an important role in socializing the vulnerable child’s emo- the availability of the therapist for phone coaching calls) for pre-
tion regulation tendencies, shaping increasing emotional lability adolescent children (N = 43; Perepletchikova et al., 2017). The
and dysregulation, culminating in the development of maladap- focus was on children with disruptive mood dysregulation disor-
tive coping behaviors that often begin to appear in adolescence der (DMRD), as DMRD is a key indicator of the risk for future
(e.g., NSSI, suicidal behavior, and substance use). In addition, childhood conduct problems and may indicate a trajectory toward
inadequate or delayed development of various social cognitive self-injury and BPD for some children. Findings indicated a much
capacities and adaptive stress response may further contribute lower dropout rate in DBT-C compared with treatment as usual,
to ED and serious behavioral problems among adolescents. as well as a higher rate of positive treatment response and a higher
These behavioral problems, in turn, contribute to and predict remission rate for DMRD. It remains to be seen, however,
the development of BPD. In adulthood, studies have highlighted whether this type of treatment might curtail the later development
various key cognitive, behavioral, and physiological components of NSSI, suicidal behavior, or BPD.
of ED, including possible differences in brain structure and func- Theory and research suggest that, among adolescents, NSSI
tioning that are likely rooted in earlier developmental processes. should be a high-priority treatment target. NSSI likely emerges
One challenge moving forward in the research is to determine earlier for most adolescents, compared with suicidal behavior,
where ED stops and BPD begins. Aspects of ED (emotional labil- and should be a key target for treatment. Prominent models,
ity in particular) are built into the criteria for BPD. It is hard to for example, suggest that suicide attempts result from a combina-
imagine the construct of BPD without ED. Is ED a causal, trans- tion of suicidal desire and the capability to enact lethal self-injury
diagnostic contributor to the array of features and behaviors (i.e., acquired capability; Joiner, 2005; Klonsky & May, 2015;
observed among those with BPD, or is it best characterized as a Witte, Gordon, Smith, & Van Orden, 2012). Within these models,
core feature of BPD? In addition, based on the notion that ED repeated NSSI increases an individual’s ability to enact suicide
consists of dysregulated emotional responding that interferes plans (Hamza, Stewart, & Willoughby, 2012; May & Victor,
with important, goal-oriented behavior, many of the problematic 2018; Willoughby, Heffer, & Hamza, 2015). The integrated
behaviors occurring among those with BPD could be considered model (Hamza et al., 2012), for example, proposes three potential
at least indirect indicators of ED. Considered in this way, ED explanations for the NSSI–suicide attempt relation: (a) NSSI and
could quickly devolve into a circular construct. The breadth and suicide are related by virtue of their shared relation with a third
reach of the construct of ED is both its major strength and limi- variable (e.g., BPD); (b) NSSI is a “gateway” behavior that facili-
tation, and this problem is not unique to BPD. tates suicide attempts; and (c) based on Joiner’s (2005) interper-
Invalidation has received considerable attention as an impor- sonal-psychological theory of suicide, NSSI is a painful/
tant socialization factor contributing to ED and BPD, and more provocative event that increases an individual’s capability to
research is needed to define and clarify the role of invalidation. enact suicidal behavior (i.e., acquired capability) by increasing
Invalidation can take many forms that do not involve trauma or fearlessness of death and habituation to pain.
abuse, and recent reviews have suggested that studies thus far Along with preventative interventions targeting at-risk groups
have failed to adequately measure and capture the construct of before the emergence of NSSI, treatment addressing NSSI when it
invalidation (Boucher et al., 2017; Musser, Zalewski, Stepp, & emerges (and ideally beforehand) in adolescence would be
Lewis, 2018). Further, it is very difficult to ascertain the specific expected to have a major impact on eventual risk for suicide
influence of parenting behaviors on the development of psycho- and the development of problems characteristic of BPD.
pathology. There are many potential explanations for any Findings from two large studies of DBT for adolescents have
Development and Psychopathology 1153

supported the efficacy of this treatment for reducing suicide Blakemore, S.-J., & Robbins, T. W. (2012). Decision-making in the adolescent
attempts, self-injury, suicidal ideation, and depression among self- brain. Natural Neuroscience, 15, 1184–1191.
injuring adolescents (McCauley et al., 2018; Mehlum et al., 2014). Bornovalova, M., Verhulst, B., Webber, T., McGue, M., Iacono, W. G., &
Hicks, B. M. (2017). Genetic and environmental influences on the codevel-
Further research is needed to determine if broader-scale preven-
opment among borderline personality disorder traits, major depression
tative interventions, such as emotion regulation training in
symptoms, and substance use disorder symptoms from adolescence to
schools, might help to curb the development and exacerbation young adulthood. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 49–65.
of ED and future BPD among adolescents. Boucher, M., Pugliese, J., Allard-Chapais, C., Lecours, S., Ahoundova, A.,
Chouinard, R., & Gaham, S. (2017). Parent-child relationship associated
Financial support. Work on this chapter was supported by a Michael Smith
with the development of borderline personality disorder: A systematic
Foundation for Health Research Career Investigator Award and a Simon Fraser
review. Personality and Mental Health, 11, 229–255.
University FASS Dean’s Research Grant.
Brown, M. Z., Comtois, K. A., & Linehan, M. M. (2002). Reasons for suicide
attempts and non-suicidal self-injury in women with borderline personality
disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 198–202.
References
Brunner, R., Henze, R., Parzer, P., Kramer, J., Feigl, N., Lutz, K., & Stieltjes, B.
Abela, J. R. Z., Payne, A. V. L., & Moussaly, N. (2003) Cognitive vulnerability (2010). Reduced prefrontal and orbitofrontal gray matter in female adoles-
to depression in individuals with borderline personality disorder. Journal of cents with borderline personality disorder: Is it disorder specific?
Personality Disorders, 17, 319–329. NeuroImage, 49, 114–120.
Ahmed, S. P., Bittencourt-Hewitt, A., & Sebastian, C. L. (2015) Neurocognitive Buckholdt, K. E., Parra, G. R., & Jobe-Shields, L. (2009). Emotion regulation as
bases of emotion regulation development in adolescence. Developmental a mediator of the relation between emotion socialization and deliberate self-
Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 11–25. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2015.07.006 harm. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 79, 482–490.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of Calkins, S. D., & Hill, A. (2007). Caregiver influences on emerging emotion
mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. regulation: Biological and environmental transactions in early development.
Amad, A., Ramoz, N., Thomas, P., Jardri, R., & Gorwood, P. (2014). Genetics In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 229–248). New
of borderline personality disorder: Systematic review and proposal of an York: Guilford Press.
integrative model. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 40, 6–19. Carpenter, R. W., & Trull, T. J. (2013). Components of emotion dysregula-
Austin, M. A., Riniolo, T. C., & Porges, S. W. (2007). Borderline personality tion in borderline personality disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports, 15,
disorder and emotion regulation: Insights from the polyvagal theory. 335–342.
Brain and Cognition, 65, 69–76. Casey, B. J., Jones, R. M., & Hare, T. A. (2008). The adolescent brain. Annals of
Baczkowski, B. M., van Zutphen, L., Siep, N., Jacob, G. A., Domes, G., Maier, S., the New York Academy of Science, 1124, 111–126.
… Arntz, A. (2017). Deficient amygdala–prefrontal intrinsic connectivity Caspi, A., Henry, B., McGee, R. O., Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1995).
after effortful emotion regulation in borderline personality disorder. Temperamental origin of child and adolescent behavior problems: From
European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 267, 551–565. age three to age fifteen. Child Development, 66, 55–68.
Baron, M., Gruen, R., Asnis, L., & Lord, S. (1985). Familial transmission of Chapman, A. L., Dixon-Gordon, K. L., & Walters, K. N. (2013). Borderline
schizotypal and borderline personality disorders. American Journal of personality features moderate emotional reactivity and emotion regulation
Psychiatry, 142, 927–934. in response to a laboratory stressor. Journal of Experimental
Bateman, A., & Fonagy, P. (2003). Health service utilization costs for border- Psychopathology, 4, 451–470.
line personality disorder patients treated with psychoanalytically oriented Chapman, A. L., Gratz, K. L., & Brown, M. Z. (2006). Solving the puzzle of
partial hospitalization versus general psychiatric care. American Journal of deliberate self- harm: The experiential avoidance model. Behaviour
Psychiatry, 160, 169–171. Research and Therapy, 44, 371–394.
Bateman, A., & Fonagy, P. (2008). 8-year follow-up of patients treated for bor- Chapman, A. L., Hope, N., & Turner, B. J. (in press). Borderline personality
derline personality disorder: Mentalization-based treatment versus treat- disorder. In C. Lejuez & K. L. Gratz (Eds.), Handbook of personality disor-
ment as usual. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 631–638. ders. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Beauchaine, T. P. (2015). Future directions in emotion dysregulation and youth Chapman, A. L., Rosenthal, M. Z., Dixon-Gordon, K. L., Turner, B. J., &
psychopathology. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 44, Kuppens, P. (2017). Borderline personality disorder and the effects of
875–896. instructed emotional avoidance or acceptance in daily life. Journal of
Beauchaine, T. P., Hinshaw, S. P., & Bridge, J. A. (2019). Nonsuicidal self- Personality Disorders, 31, 483–502.
injury and suicidal behaviors in girls: The case for targeted prevention in Chapman, A. L., Specht, M. W., & Cellucci, T. (2005). Borderline personality
preadolescence. Clinical Psychological Science. Advance online publication. disorder and deliberate self-harm: Does experiential avoidance play a role?
Beauchaine, T. P., Sauder, C. L., Derbidge, C. M., & Uyeji, L. L. (2019). Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 35, 388–399.
Self-injuring adolescent girls exhibit insular cortex volumetric abnormalities Cicchetti, D. (2014). Illustrative developmental psychopathology perspectives
that are similar to those observed in adults with borderline personality dis- on precursors and pathways to personality disorder: Commentary on the
order. Development and Psychopathology. Advance online publication. special issue. Journal of Personality Disorders, 28, 172–179.
Beblo, T., Fernando, S., Kamper, P., Griepenstroh, J., Aschenbrenner, S., Cloitre, M., Garvert, D. W., Weiss, B., Carlson, E. B., & Bryant, R. A.
Pastuszak, A., … Driessen, M. (2013). Increased attempts to suppress neg- (2014). Distinguishing PTSD, complex PTSD, and borderline personality
ative and positive emotions in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry disorder: A latent class analysis. European Journal of Psychotraumatology,
Research, 210, 505–509. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2013.06.036 5, 10.
Belsky, D. W., Caspi, A., Arseneault, L., Bleidorn, W., Fonagy, P., Cohen, P., Crawford, T. N., Johnson, J. G., & Kasen, S. (2005). The children in
Goodman, M., … Moffitt, T. E. (2012). Etiological features of borderline the community study of developmental course of personality disorder.
personality related characteristics in a birth cohort of 12-year-old children. Journal of Personality Disorders, 19, 466–486.
Development and Psychopathology, 24, 251–265. Comtois, K. A., Elwood, L., Holdcraft, L. C., Smith, W. R., & Simpson, T. L.
Bevilacqua, L., & Goldman, D. (2013). Genetics of impulsive behaviour. (2007). Effectiveness of dialectical behavior therapy in a community mental
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 368. health center. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 14, 406–414.
Biskin, R. S. (2015). The lifetime course of borderline personality disorder. Crowell, S. E., Beauchaine, T. P., & Linehan, M. M. (2009). A biosocial devel-
Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 60, 303–308. opmental model of borderline personality: Elaborating and extending
Black, D. W., Blum, N., Pfohl, B., & Hale, N. (2004). Suicidal behavior in bor- Linehan’s theory. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 495–510.
derline personality disorder: Prevalence, risk factors, prediction, and pre- Crowell, S. E., Kaufman, E. A., & Beauchaine, T .P. (2014). A biosocial model
vention. Journal of Personality Disorders, 18, 226–239. of BPD: Theory and empirical evidence. In C. Sharp & J. L. Tackett (Eds.),
1154 A. L. Chapman

Handbook of borderline personality disorder in children and adolescents (pp. Gratz, K. L., Rosenthal, M. Z., Tull, M. T., Lejuez, C. W., & Gunderson, J. G.
143–157). New York: Springer. (2006). An experimental investigation of emotion dysregulation in border-
Derbidge, C. M., & Beauchaine, T. P. (2014). A developmental model of self- line personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 850–855.
inflicted injury, borderline personality, and suicide risk. In M. Lewis & K. D. Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative
Rudolph (Eds.), Handbook of developmental psychopathology (pp. 521– review. Review of General Psychology, 2, 271–299.
542). New York: Springer Science + Business Media. Gross, J. J. (2014). Emotion regulation: Conceptual and empirical foundations.
Derks, Y., Westerhof, G., & Bohlmeijer, E. (2016). A meta-analysis on the asso- In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (2nd ed.). New York:
ciation between emotional awareness and borderline personality pathology. Guilford Press.
Journal of Personality Disorders, 31, 1–23. Guendelman, M. D., Owens, E. B., Galan, C., Gard, A., & Hinshaw, S. P.
Diamond, A. (2018). Effects of early adverse experiences on the brain. Paper (2015). Early-adult correlates of maltreatment in girls with attention-
presented at Brain Talks. University of British Columbia, Vancouver, deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Increased risk for internalizing symptoms
B.C., Canada. and suicidality. Development and Psychopathology, 28, 1–14.
Distel, M. A., Trull, T. J., Derom, C. A., Thiery, E. W., Grimmer, M. A., Gunderson, J. G. (2001). Borderline personality disorder: A clinical guide.
Martin, N. G., … Boomsma, D. I. (2008). Heritability of borderline person- Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.
ality disorder features is similar across three countries. Psychological Gunderson, J. G. (2007). Disturbed relationships as a phenotype for borderline
Medicine, 38, 1219–1229. personality disorder (commentary). American Journal of Psychiatry, 164,
Distel, M. A., Trull, T. J., Willemsen, G., Vink, J. M., Devon, C. A., 1637–1640.
Lynsey, N. G. M., & Boomsma, D. I. (2009). The five-factor model of Hamza, C. A., Stewart, S. L., & Willoughby, T. (2012). Examining the link
personality and borderline personality disorder: A genetic analysis of between nonsuicidal self-injury and suicidal behavior: A review of the liter-
comorbidity. Biological Psychiatry, 66, 1131–1138. ature and an integrated model. Clinical Psychology Review, 32, 482–495.
Dixon-Gordon, K. L., Haliczer, L. A., & Conkey, L. C. (in press). Emotion dys- Hazlett, E. A., Zhang, J., New, A. S., Zelmanova, Y., Goldstein, K. E.,
regulation and borderline personality disorder. In T. P. Beauchaine & S. E. Haznedar, M. M, … Chu, K. W. (2012). Potentiated amygdala response
Crowell (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of emotion dysregulation. Oxford: to repeated emotional pictures in borderline personality disorder.
Oxford University Press. Biological Psychiatry, 72, 448–456.
Dixon-Gordon, K. L., Aldao, A., & De Los Reyes, A. (2015). Repertoires of Hecht, K. F., Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Crick, N. R. (2014). Borderline
emotion regulation: A person-centered approach to assessing emotion reg- personality features in childhood: The role of subtype, developmental tim-
ulation strategies and links to psychopathology. Cognition and Emotion, 29, ing, and chronicity of child maltreatment. Development and
1314–1325. Psychopathology, 26, 805–815.
Dixon-Gordon, K. L., Chapman, A. L., Lovasz, N., & Walters, K. N. (2011). Homan, K. J., Sim, L. A., Fargo, J. D., & Twohig, M .P. (2017). Five-year pro-
Too upset to think: The interplay of borderline personality features, nega- spective investigation of self-harm/ suicide-related behaviors in the develop-
tive emotions, and social problem solving in the laboratory. Personality ment of borderline personality disorder. Personality Disorders, 8, 183–188.
Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 2, 243–260. Hope, N. H., & Chapman, A. L. (in press). Difficulties regulating emotions
Dixon-Gordon, K. L., Hackel, L. M., Tull, M. T., & Gratz, K. L. (2018). The mediates the associations of parental psychological control and emotion
influence of emotional state on reinforcement learning in borderline per- invalidation with borderline personality features. Personality Disorders:
sonality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 32, 433–446. Theory, Research, and Treatment. Advance online publication.
Donegan, N. H., Sanislow, C. A., Blumberg, H. P., Fullbright, R. K., Lacadie, C., Joiner, T. (2005). Why people die by suicide. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
Skudlarski, P., … Wexler, B. E. (2003). Amygdala hyperreactivity in border- University Press.
line personality disorder: Implications for emotional dysregulation. Johnston, J. A. Y., Wang, F., Liu, J., Blond, B. N., & Wallace, A., Liu, J., …
Biological Psychiatry, 54, 116–123. Blumberg, H. P. (2017). Multimodal neuroimaging of frontolimbic structure
Ebner-Priemer, U. W., Badeck, S., Beckmann, C., Wagner, A., Feige, B., and function associated with suicide attempts in adolescents and young
Weiss, I., … Bohus, M. (2005). Affective dysregulation and dissociative adults with bipolar disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 174, 667–675.
experience in female patients with borderline personality disorder: A startle Jørgensen, C. R., Kjølbye, M., Freund, C., Bøye, R., Jordet, H., & Andersen, D.
response study. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 39, 85–92. (2009). Level of functioning in patients with borderline personality disor-
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy, B. C., & der. The Risskov-I study. Nordic Psychology, 61, 42–60.
Reiser, M. (1999). Parental reactions to children’s negative emotions: Kaiser, D., Jacob, G. A., Domes, G., & Arntz, A. (2017). Attentional bias for
Longitudinal relations to quality of children’s social functioning. Child emotional stimuli in borderline personality disorder: A meta-analysis.
Development, 70, 513–534. Psychopathology, 49, 383–396.
Fitzpatrick, S., & Kuo, J. R. (2015). A comprehensive examination of delayed Kaufman, E., Puzia, M. F., Mead, H. K., Crowell, S. E., & Beauchaine, T. P.
emotional recovery in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Behavior (2017). Children’s emotion regulation difficulties mediate the association
Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 47, 51–59. between maternal borderline and antisocial symptoms and youth behavior
Ford, J. D., & Courtois, C. A. (2014). Complex PTSD, affect dysregulation, and problems over one year. Journal of Personality Disorders, 31, 170–192.
borderline personality disorder. Borderline Personality Disorder and Kleindienst, N., Bohus, M., Ludaescher, P., Limberger, M. F., Kuenkele, K.,
Emotion Dysregulation, 9, 1–9. Ebner-Priemer, U., … Schmahl, C. (2008). Motives for non-suicidal self-
Fyer, M. R., Frances, A. J., Sullivan, T., Hurt, S. W., & Clarkin, J. (1988). injury among women with borderline personality disorder. Journal of
Comorbidity of borderline personality disorder. Archives of General Nervous and Mental Disease, 196, 230–236.
Psychiatry, 45, 348–352. Klonsky, E. D., & May, A. M. (2015). The three-step theory (3ST): A new the-
Glenn, C. R., & Klonsky, E. D. (2009). Emotion dysregulation as a core feature ory of suicide rooted in the “ideation-to-action” framework. International
of borderline personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 23, Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 8, 114–129.
20–28. Koenigsberg, H. W., Fan, J., Ochsner, K., Liu, X., Guise, K. G., Pizzarello, S., …
Grant, B. F., Chou, S. P., Goldstein, R. B., Huang, B., Stinson, F. S., Saha, T. D., … Siever, L. J. (2010). Neural correlates of the use of psychological distancing
Ruan, W. J. (2008). Prevalence, correlates, disability, and comorbidity of to regulate responses to negative social cues: A study of patients with bor-
DSM-IV borderline personality disorder: Results from the Wave 2 National derline personality disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 66, 854–877.
Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Journal of Koons, C. R., Chapman, A. L., Betts, B. B., O’Rourke, B., Morse, N., &
Clinical Psychiatry, 69, 533. Robins, C. J. (2006). Dialectical behavior therapy adapted for the vocational
Gratz, K. L., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional Assessment of Emotion rehabilitation of significantly disabled mentally ill adults. Cognitive and
Regulation and Dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial Behavioral Practice, 13, 146–156.
validation of the difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale. Journal of Lancefield, K. S., Raudino, A., Downs, J. M., & Laurens, K. R. (2016).
Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26, 41–54. Trajectories of childhood internalizing and externalizing psychopathology
Development and Psychopathology 1155

and psychotic-like experiences in adolescence: A prospective population- Nock, M. K., & Prinstein, M. J. (2004). A functional approach to the assess-
based cohort study. Development and Psychopathology, 28, 527–536. ment of self- mutilative behavior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Lang, S., Katchoubey, B., Frick, C., Spitzer, C., Grabe, H. J., & Barnow, S. Psychology, 72, 885–890.
(2012). Cognitive reappraisal in trauma-exposed women with borderline Paris, J., & Zweig-Frank, H. (2001). A 27-year follow-up of patients with bor-
personality disorder. Neuroimage, 59, 1727–1734. derline personality disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 42, 482–487.
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. Oxford: Oxford University Paris, J., Zweig-Frank, H., & Guzder, J. (1994). Psychological risk factors for
Press. borderline personality disorder in female patients. Comprehensive
Leible, T., & Snell, W. (2004). Borderline personality disorder and multiple Psychiatry, 35, 301–305.
aspects of emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, Patterson, G. R., DeBaryshe, B. D., & Ramsey, E. (1989). A developmental per-
37, 393–404. spective on antisocial behavior. American Psychologist, 44, 329–335.
Lenzenweger, M. F., Lane, M. C., Loranger, A. W., & Kessler, R. C. (2007). Patterson, C. M., & Newman, J. P. (1993). Reflectivity and learning from aver-
DSM-IV personality disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey replica- sive events: Toward a psychological mechanism for syndromes of disinhibi-
tion. Biological Psychiatry, 62, 553–564. tion. Psychological Review, 100, 716–736.
Lieb, K., Zanarini, M. C., Schmahl, C., Linehan, M. M., & Bohus, M. (2004). Perepletchikova, F., Nathanson, D., Aexlrod, S. R., Merrill, C., Walker, A.,
Borderline personality disorder. Lancet, 364, 453–461. Grossman, M., … Walkup, J. (2017). Randomized clinical trial of dialectical
Lieberman, M. D., Eisenberger, N. I., Crockett, M. J., Tom, S. M., Pfeifer, J. H., behavior therapy for preadolescent children with disruptive mood dysregu-
& Way, B. M. (2007). Putting feelings into words: Affect labeling disrupts lation disorder: Feasibility and outcomes. Journal of the American Academy
amygdala activity in response to affective stimuli. Psychological Science, of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 56, 832–840.
18, 421–428. Porges, S. W. (2001). The polyvagal theory: Phylogenetic substrates of a social
Linehan, M. (1993a). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of borderline personality nervous system. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 42, 123–146.
disorder. New York: Guilford Press. Power, R. A., & Pluess, M. (2015). Heritability estimates of the Big Five person-
Linehan, M. (1993b). Skills training manual for treating borderline personality ality traits based on common genetic variants. Translational Psychiatry, 5.
disorder. New York: Guilford Press. doi:10.1038/tp.2015.96
Linehan, M. M., Bohus, M., & Lynch, T. R. (2007). Dialectical behavior therapy Reichborn-Kjennerud, T., Ystrom, E., Neale, M. C., Aggen, S. H., Mazzeo, S. E.,
for pervasive emotion dysregulation. In J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion Knudsen, G. P., … Kendler, K. S. (2013). Structure of genetic and environ-
regulation (pp. 581–605). New York: The Guilford Press. mental risk factors for symptoms of DSM-IV borderline personality disor-
Linehan, M. (2015). DBT skills training manual. New York: Guilford Press. der. JAMA Psychiatry, 70, 1206–1214.
Linehan, M. M., & Heard, H. L. (1999). Borderline personality disorder: Reitz, S., Kluetsch, R., Niedtfeld, I., Knorz, T., Lis, S., Paret, C., … Schmahl, C.
Costs, course, and treatment outcomes. In N. Miller & K. Magruder (2015). Incision and stress regulation in borderline personality disorder:
(Eds.), The cost-effectiveness of psychotherapy: A guide for practitioners, Neurobiological mechanisms of self-injurious behaviour. British Journal
researchers and policy makers (pp. 291–305). New York: Oxford of Psychiatry, 207, 165–172.
University Press. Rosenthal, M. Z., Gratz, K. L., Kosson, D. S., Cheavens, J. S., Lejuez, C. W., &
Links, P. S., Heslegrave, R., & van Reekum, R. (1999). Impulsivity: Core aspect Lynch, T. R. (2008). Borderline personality disorder and emotional respond-
of borderline personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 12, 1–9. ing: A review of the research literature. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 75–91.
Links, P. S., Steiner, M., & Huxley, G. (1988). The occurrence of borderline Roth, R. H., Tam, S.-Y., Ida, Y., Yang, J.-X., & Deutch, A. Y. (1988) Stress and
personality disorder in the families of borderline patients. Journal of the mesocorticolimbic dopamine systems. Annals of the New York Academy
Personality Disorders, 2, 14. of Science, 537, 138–147.
Liston, C., McEwen, B. S., & Casey, B. J. (2009). Psychosocial stress reversibly Ruocco, A. (2005). The neuropsychology of borderline personality disorder: A
disrupts prefrontal processing and attentional control. Proceedings of the meta-analysis and review. Psychiatry Research, 137, 191–202.
National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 106, 912–917. Ruocco, A. S., Amirthavasagan, S., Choi-Kain, L. W., & McMain, S. F. (2013).
Mauss, I. B., Levinson, R. W., McCarter, L., Wilhelm, F. H., & Gross, J. J. Neural correlates of negative emotionality in borderline personality
(2005). The tie that binds? Coherence among emotional experience, behav- disorder: An activation-likelihood-estimation meta-analysis. Biological
ior, and physiology. Emotion, 5, 175–190. Psychiatry, 73, 153–160.
May, A. M., & Victor, S. E. (2018). From ideation to action: Recent Salsman, N. L., & Linehan, M. M. (2012). An investigation of the relationships
advances in understanding suicide capability. Current Opinion in among negative affect, difficulties in emotion regulation, and features of
Psychology, 22, 1–6. borderline personality disorder. Journal of Psychopathology and
McCauley, E., Berk, M. S., Asarnow, J. R., Adrian, M., Cohen, J., Korslund, K., Behavioral Assessment, 34, 260–267.
… Linehan, M. M. (2018). Efficacy of dialectical behavior therapy for ado- Sanchez, M. M., Young, L. J., Plotsky, P. M., & Insel, T. R. (2000). Distribution of
lescents at high risk for suicide: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA corticosteroid receptors in the rhesus brain: Relative absence of glucocorticoid
Psychiatry, 75, 777–785. receptors in the hippocampal formation. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 4657–4668.
McCloskey, M. S., New, A. S., Siever, L. J., Goodman, M., Koenigsberg, H. W., Sauer, C., Sheppes, G., Lackner, H. K., Arens, E. A., Tarrasch, R., & Barnow, S.
Flory, J. D., & Coccaro, E. F. (2009). Evaluation of behavioral impulsivity (2016). Emotion regulation choice in female patients with borderline per-
and aggression tasks as endophenotypes for borderline personality disorder. sonality disorder: Findings from self-reports and experimental measures.
Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43, 1036–1048. Psychiatry Research, 242, 375–384.
Mehlum, L., Tørmoen, A. J., Ramberg, M., Haga, E., Diep, L. M., Laberg, S., … Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A., & Johstone, T. (2001). Appraisal processes in emo-
Sund, A. M. (2014). Dialectical behavior therapy for adolescents with tion: Theory, methods, research. New York: Oxford University Press.
repeated suicidal and self-harming behavior: A randomized trial. Journal Schulze, L., Schmahl, C., & Niedtfeld, I. (2016). Neural correlates of disturbed
of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 53, 1082–1091. emotion processing in borderline personality disorder: A multimodal meta-
Miller, A. B., & Prinstein, M. J. (2019). Adolescent suicide as a failure of acute analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 79, 97–106.
stress-response systems. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 5, 21–26. Selby, E. A., & Joiner, T. E. (2009). Cascades of emotion: The emergence of
Musser, N., Zalewski, M., Stepp, S., & Lewis, J. (2018). A systematic review of borderline personality disorder from emotional and behavioral dysregula-
negative parenting practices predicting borderline personality disorder: Are tion. Review of General Psychology, 13, 219–229.
we measuring biosocial theory’s “invalidating environment”? Clinical Shaw, P., Kabani, N. J., Lerch, J. P., Eckstrand, K., Lenroot, R., Gogtay, N., …
Psychology Review, 65, 1–16. Wise, S. P. (2008). Neurodevelopmental trajectories of the human cerebral
Niedtfeld, I., Kirsch, P., Schulze, L., Herpertz, S. C., Bohus, M., & Schmahl, C. cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 3586–3594.
(2012). Functional connectivity of pain-medicated affect regulation in bor- Sheppes, G., Suri, G., & Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation and psychopa-
derline personality disorder. PLOS ONE, 7, 1–9. thology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 11, 379–405.
1156 A. L. Chapman

Skodol, A. E., Siever, L. J., Livesley, W. J., Gunderson, J. G., Pfohl, B., & Winograd, G., Cohen, P., & Chen, H. (2008). Adolescent borderline symptoms
Widiger, T. A. (2002). The borderline diagnosis: II. Biology, genetics and in the community: Prognosis for functioning over 20 years. Journal of Child
clinical course. Society of Biological Psychiatry, 51, 951–963. Psychology & Psychiatry, 49, 933–941.
Smith, J. M., Grandin, L. D., Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (2006) Cognitive Witte, T. K., Gordon, K. H., Smith, P. N., & Van Orden, K. A. (2012). Stoicism
vulnerability to depression and axis II personality dysfunction. Cognitive and sensation seeking: Male vulnerabilities for the acquired capability for
Therapy and Research, 30, 609–621. suicide. Journal of Research in Personality, 46, 384–392.
Soloff, P. H., & Chiappetta, L. (2017). Suicidal behavior and psychosocial out- Wolff, S., Stiglmayr, C., Bretz, H. J., Lammers, C. H., & Auckenthaler, A.
come in borderline personality disorder at 8-year follow-up. Journal of (2007). Emotion identification and tension in female patients with border-
Personality Disorders, 31, 774–789. line personality disorder. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 46, 347–360.
Somerville, L. H. (2013). The teenage brain sensitivity to social evaluation. World Health Organization. (2016). ICD-10: International statistical classifica-
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 121–127. tion of diseases and related health problems (2016 Version, 2nd ed.). Geneva:
Stepp, S. D., Burke, J. D., Hipwell, A. E., & Loeber, R. (2012). Trajectories Author.
of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and oppositional defiant Yen, S., Shea, M. T., Battle, C. L., Johnson, D. M., Zlotnick, C., Dolan-
disorder symptoms as precursors of borderline personality disorder Sewell, R., … Zanarini, M. C. (2002). Traumatic exposure and
symptoms in adolescent girls. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, posttraumatic stress disorder in borderline, schizotypal, avoidant, and
40, 7–20. obsessive-compulsive personality disorders: Findings from the collabora-
Suvak, M. K., Litz, B. T., Sloan, D. M., Zanarini, M. C., Barrett, L. F., & tive longitudinal personality disorders study. Journal of Nervous and
Hofmann, S. G. (2011). Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120, 414–426. Mental Disease, 190, 510–518.
Swanson, E. N., Owens, E. B., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2014). Pathways to self-harm- Zanarini, M. C., Frankenberg, F. R., Duo, E. D., Sickel, A. E., Trisha, A.,
ful behaviors in young women with and without ADHD: A longitudinal Levin, A., & Reynolds, V. (1998). Axis I comorbidity of borderline person-
examination of mediating factors. Journal of Child Psychology and ality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 1733–1739.
Psychiatry, 55, 505–515. Zanarini, M., Frankenburg, F. R., Hennen, J., Reich, D. B., & Silk, K. R. (2004).
Takahashi, T., Chanen, A. M., Wood, S. J., Yücel, M., Tanino, R., Suzuki, M., Axis I comorbidity in patients with borderline personality disorder: 6-year
… McGorry, P. D. (2009). Insular cortex volume and impulsivity in teenag- follow-up and prediction of time to remission. American Journal of
ers with first-presentation borderline personality disorder. Progress in Psychiatry, 16, 2108–2114.
Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 33, 1395–1400. Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., Hennen, J., & Silk, K. R. (2003). The lon-
Taylor, S. E., Eisenberger, N. I., Saxbe, D., Lehman, B. J., & Lieberman, M. D. gitudinal course of borderline psychopathology: Six-year prospective
(2006). Neural responses to emotional stimuli are associated with childhood follow-up of the phenomenology of borderline personality disorder.
family stress. Biological Psychiatry, 60, 296–301. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 274–283.
Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition. Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., Reich, D. B., & Fitzmaurice, G. (2012).
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 25–52. Attainment and stability of sustained symptomatic remission and recovery
Torgersen, S., Lygren, S., Oien, P. A., Skyre, I., Onstad, S., Edvardsen, J., … among patients with borderline personality disorder and Axis II compari-
Kringlen, E. (2000). A twin study of personality disorders. Comprehensive son subjects: A 16-year prospective follow-up study. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 41, 416–425. Psychiatry, 169, 476–483.
Trippany, R. L., Helm, H. M., & Simpson, L. (2006). Trauma reenactment: Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., Reich, D. B., Silk, K. R., Hudson, J. I., &
Rethinking borderline personality disorder when diagnosing sexual abuse McSweeney, L. B. (2007). The subsyndromal phenomenology of borderline
survivors. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 28, 95–110. personality disorder: A 10-year follow-up study. American Journal of
Unoka, Z., & Richman, J. M. (2016). Neuropsychological deficits in BPD Psychiatry, 164, 929–935.
patients and the moderator effects of co-occurring mental disorders: A Zanarini, M. C., Gunderson, J. G., Marino, M. F., Schwartz, E. O., &
meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 44, 1–12. Frankenburg, F. R. (1988). DSM-III disorders in the families of borderline
van der Kolk, B. A., Pelcovitz, D., Roth, S., & Mandel, F. S. (1996). outpatients. Journal of Personality Disorders, 2, 292.
Dissociation, somatization, and affect dysregulation: The complexity of Zanarini, M. C., Jacoby, R. J., Frankenburg, F. R., Reich, D. B., &
adaption to trauma. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 83–93. Fitzmaurice, G. (2009). The 10-year course of social security disability
Vukasović, T., & Bratko, D. (2015). Heritability of personality: A meta-analysis income reported by patients with borderline personality disorder and axis
of behavior genetic studies. Psychological Bulletin, 141, 769–785. II comparison subjects. Journal of Personality Disorders, 23, 346.
Weinberg, A., Klonsky, E. D., & Hajcak, G. (2009). Autonomic impairment in Zanarini, M. C., Williams, A. A., Lewis, R. E., Reich, R. B., Vera, S. C.,
borderline personality disorder: A laboratory investigation. Brain and Marino, M. F., … Frankenburg, F. R. (1997). Reported pathological child-
Cognition, 71, 279–286. hood experiences associated with the development of borderline personality
Westen, D., Ludolph, P., Misle, B., Ruffins, S., & Block, J. (1990). Physical and disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 1101–1106.
sexual abuse in adolescent girls with borderline personality disorder. Zanarini, M. C., Yong, L., Frankenburg, F. R., Hennen, J., Reich, D. B.,
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 60, 55–66. Marino, M. F., & Vujanovic, A. A. (2002). Severity of reported childhood
Willoughby, T., Heffer, T., & Hamza, C. A. (2015). The Link between nonsui- sexual abuse and its relationship to severity of borderline psychopathology
cidal self-injury and acquired capability for suicide: A longitudinal study. and psychosocial impairment among borderline inpatients. Journal of
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124, 1110–1115. Nervous and Mental Disease, 190, 381–387.

You might also like