Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Journal of Counseling Psychology Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1996, Vol.43, No. 1,90-98 0022-0167/%/$3.OO

The Relationships of Self-Efficacy for the Holland Themes to Gender,


Occupational Group Membership, and Vocational Interests

Nancy E. Betz Lenore W. Harmon


The Ohio State University University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign
Fred H. Bqrgen
Iowa State University

The studies presented in this article resulted in the findings that gender differences in
self-efficacy for the Holland themes are consistent with previous findings regarding gender
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

differences in Holland interest patterns, although the gender differences are less pronounced
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

in employed adults than in college students. Within occupational group, the self-efficacy
profiles of women and men were very similar and closely corresponded to the Holland
interest code of the occupation. Confidence for and interest in a theme were moderately
correlated, and recommendations for the counseling use of joint combinations of interests and
self-efficacy are made.

One major trend in both the theory and practice of career ior to which self-efficacy theory has been applied include
counseling is the inclusion of the concept of career self- task-specific occupational self-efficacy (Osipow, Temple,
efficacy as a major individual differences variable. Career & Rooney, 1993; Rooney & Osipow, 1992), mathematics
self-efficacy, based on Bandura's (1977, 1986) theory of self-efficacy (Betz & Hackett, 1983), career decision-mak-
self-efficacy expectations as a major mediator of both be- ing self-efficacy (Taylor & Popma, 1990), self-efficacy with
havior and behavior change, was first investigated by Betz respect to academic milestones (Lent, Brown, & Larkin,
and Hackett (1981), who reported that college students' 1984) and scientific and technical careers (Lent et al., 1984),
beliefs about their educational and occupational capabilities and career search efficacy (Solberg et al., 1994).
were significantly related to the nature and range of career One major career domain for which self-efficacy measures
options they considered. Their findings have been replicated have both theoretical and applied utility is that represented by
in other samples (Layton, 1984; Zilber, 1988), age groups the six interest types of Holland's (1973, 1985a) theory; these
(Post-Kammer & Smith, 1985), and cultures (Matsui & six types, Realistic, Investigative, Artistic, Social, Enterprising,
Tsukamoto, 1991). More generally, meta-analyses and re- and Conventional (henceforth abbreviated as RIASEC), have
views (Hackett & Lent, 1992; Lent, Brown, & Hackett, been among the major individual differences variables used in
1994; Multon, Brown, & Lent, 1991) strongly support the career theory, assessment, and counseling. Although a primary
role of self-efficacy as a predictor of academic performance use of the RIASEC model has been in guiding individuals to
and persistence and career decision-making intentions and careers compatible with their interests and personality dispo-
behaviors. sitions, inclusion of perceptions of self-efficacy with respect to
Since the emergence of research attention to career self- these domains should also contribute to theory explication as
efficacy, measures of a number of specific career-related well as career counseling.
domains have been developed (it should be recalled that In addition to the need for measures of self-efficacy with
Bandura's concept of perceived self-efficacy is in reference respect to the RIASEC dimensions, such measures would
to a specific behavioral domain, e.g., mathematics, driving facilitate research on the relationship between self-efficacy
a car, or skydiving). To date, domains of vocational behav- and interest development, a recent focus of research atten-
tion. Lent et al. (1994) proposed a social cognitive theory of
Nancy E. Betz, Department of Psychology, The Ohio State career and academic interest, choice, and performance in
University; Lenore W. Harmon, Department of Educational Psy- which perceived self-efficacy plays a major role in interest
chology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign; and Fred H. development and is also related, both through interests and
Borgen, Department of Psychology, Iowa State University. independently, to career choice goals, actions, and perfor-
We would like to thank Karla Klein, Allison Kaplan, and Beth mance attainments. The Lent et al. theory is testable using
Parsons of The Ohio State University; Dawn Schauer of the any relevant domain of vocational interests, and one of the
University of Illinois; and David Donnay and Lori Jones of Iowa most interesting questions will involve its differential ap-
State University for their contributions to this research. We also
plicability across interest domains. Given the centrality of
acknowledge financial support from, the assistance of, and coop-
eration from the staff of Consulting Psychologists' Press. the RIASEC typology to our conceptualization of voca-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to tional interests, testing of the social cognitive theory using
Nancy E. Betz, Department of Psychology, The Ohio State Uni- the RIASEC types is an obvious, needed step.
versity, 137 Townshend Hall, 1885 Neil Avenue Mall, Columbus, In terms of applicability to counseling, both Lent et al.
Ohio 43210-1222. (1994) and Betz (1993) have stressed the crucial role of
90
SELF-EFFICACY FOR HOLLAND'S THEMES 91
restricted experiential opportunity in interpreting low inter- abilities. She used 14 abilities items and 30 skills items (5
est scores. Not only may lack of experience prevent the per theme). Reliabilities of the 5-item skills scales ranged
development of strong self-efficacy beliefs as well as inter- from .68 (Investigative) to .84 (Realistic and Social).
ests, but also the avoidance behavior that results from low Most recently, Lenox and Subich (1994) developed mea-
efficacy expectations would prevent even initial exploration sures of self-efficacy with respect to Holland's themes. Six
of interests in those areas. Betz (1993) contended that with- 5-item scales were developed using activities representing
out the moderate level of self-efficacy necessary for "ap- each theme. Pilot data indicated adequate levels of internal
proach" behavior, and thus experiential contact, we cannot consistency reliability for the Realistic, Investigative, and
adequately evaluate an individual's potential for interest Enterprising scales. Lenox and Subich did not report the
development. Lent et al.'s Proposition 1G states that: "Gen- results for the Social, Conventional, and Artistic themes
der and racial/ethnic differences in interests and in interest/ because of extreme restriction of range at the lower end of
goal relations arise largely through differential access to the scale.
opportunities, supports, and socialization processes" (Lent Thus, although some previous researchers have included
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

et al., 1994, p. 108). (It should be noted that the positions self-efficacy relative to the Holland themes as a variable, or
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

taken by Betz (1993) and Lent et al. (1994) are not new, as have begun to develop such measures (i.e., Lenox & Subich,
illustrated, for example, by Cole and Hanson's (1975) "op- 1994), the present series of studies was designed to use
portunity dominance" hypothesis.) Instead, joint consider- large-sample methods to develop and validate a measure of
ation of interests and perceived efficacy may assist in the self-efficacy with respect to the six Holland themes and to
interpretation of vocational interest patterns and, in some examine the relationships of self-efficacy in RIASEC areas
cases, suggest the design of counseling interventions to to gender, occupational group membership, and RIASEC
increase exposure to the four sources of efficacy informa- interest measures.
tion.
Finally, although they do not use the concept of self-
efficacy expectations per se, other researchers (Campbell, Pilot Study: Instrument Development
1992; Swanson, 1993) have suggested the use of a related
concept, self-rated skills and abilities, in interest inventory Scale Construction
interpretation and career counseling, and Holland, in his
Self-Directed Search (1985b), has long included a section The basis for scale construction was a construct-based
on self-rated RIASEC competencies. approach emphasizing scale homogeneity, and content and
Thus, for both theoretical and practical reasons, further construct validity (Nunnally, 1978). For each of the six
development of measures of perceived self-efficacy with Holland themes, items reflecting activities and school sub-
respect to the six Holland themes is needed. Research to this jects related to that theme were generated by the scale
point has provided some useful information. Lapan, Boggs, authors. Activities items (e.g., build a dollhouse) were in-
and Morrill (1989) asked college students to rate their cluded because they are one of the most commonly used
self-efficacy with respect to occupations representing the stimuli for the assessment of self-efficacy (for example, see
six Holland theme areas and to the occupational group as a Lenox & Subich, 1994; Rooney & Osipow, 1992). School
whole (e.g., "jobs which involve science and science-related subjects (e.g., art) were also included because they have
activities" for Investigative; Lapan et al., 1989, p. 177). A been found to be the most useful subscale from the Betz and
total score for each theme was calculated by combining the Hackett (1983) Mathematics Self-Efficacy Scale. To con-
separate occupations and occupational group ratings. Al- struct the initial pool of items, each author developed 15-25
though self-efficacy was found to play a major role in items per theme based on his or her understanding of the
mediating gender differences in Investigative and Realistic Holland RIASEC system, including published descriptions
interests, reliability data and plans for further development of the themes. These author-generated pools were combined
of this measure were not reported. and examined for duplication or overlap. In consultation
Rooney (1991) cluster-analyzed the items of the Task among all three authors, agreement of at least two was
Specific Occupational Self-Efficacy Scale (TSOSS; Rooney required to retain the item for pilot testing. In this way, an
& Osipow, 1992) and was able to assign 225 of the 230 initial pool of 195 items was derived.
items to one of the six Holland theme areas. Number of Assessment of self-efficacy beliefs originally utilized a
items per cluster ranged from 18 (Enterprising) to 56 (Re- "level" (yes or no) response and a "strength," or confidence,
alistic). No reliability data for the clusters was provided, nor rating. Following research suggesting the high correlation
was it suggested that these clusters be used in further between level and strength-confidence, Lent et al. (1984)
research. Research evaluating the TSOSS (Osipow et al., and others have suggested that use of confidence ratings
1993; Rooney & Osipow, 1992) has used a four-factor was sufficient. Thus, respondents herein were asked to
structure (Verbal, Interpersonal Skills; Quantitative, Logi- indicate their degree of confidence in their ability to per-
cal, Business, and Scientific Skills; Physical Strength and form that activity or task; responses were obtained on a
Agility; and Aesthetic Skills). 5-point scale ranging from 1 no confidence at all to 5
Swanson (1993) reported correlations ranging from .29 to complete confidence. For school subjects items (e.g., alge-
.46 between RIASEC interests and self-rated skills, and bra, art, botany), respondents were asked to indicate their
from .38 to .46 between RIASEC interests and self-rated degree of confidence in completing the course successfully.
92 BETZ, HARMON, AND BORGEN

Responses were obtained using the same 5-point continuum Hansen, Borgen, & Hammers, 1994, p. 109). More specific de-
used with the activities items. To facilitate accessibility and scriptive characteristics of particular occupational norm groups
face validity in uses with research participants and, eventu- (e.g., mean age, years of experience, and educational background)
are contained in Appendix A of the SII manual (Harmon et al.,
ally, counseling clients, the resulting inventory was called
1994). Of the individuals participating in the 1994 revision of the
the Skills Confidence Inventory (SCI). Strong Interest Inventory (from which the employed adults studied
herein were sampled), 5% were ethnic and racial minority group
members. Of these, 22% were African American, 27% were Asian
Scale Refinement American, 23% were Latino, Latina, or Hispanic, 5% were Amer-
ican Indian, and 18% were minority group members but did not
A heterogeneous group of 73 participants (49 women and specify one of these groups. Generally, the minority group mem-
24 men) constituted the initial pilot sample for empirical bers were distributed fairly evenly across the occupational groups
item evaluation and refinement. The sample included work- included in the normative sample.
ing adults and graduate and undergraduate students repre- Members of these occupational groups who had expressed an
senting two public universities and one business organiza- interest in participating in additional research beyond the initial
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tion. Participants were administered the 195-item version of norming of the 1994 SII were mailed a copy of the SCI along with
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

the scale. the $1 token payment to thank them for their participation. Ques-
Electing to use an approach to scale construction that tionnaires were mailed to 1,525 individuals, and completed forms
would yield relatively homogeneous measures of RIASEC were received from 1,147 (585 men and 562 women) for a return
rate of 75%. Return rates across gender and occupation combina-
confidence (see Nunnally, 1978, for a description of item
tions ranged from 50% (male computer programmers) to 89%
homogeneity approaches to scale construction), we used the (male marketing executives).
pilot data to eliminate the poorest items from the standpoint Three samples of college students, a total of 706, also partici-
of item-own scale correlations in comparison with item- pated in the research. These samples were (a) 236 female and 157
other scale correlations. Because we wanted the initial form male students enrolled in introductory psychology courses at The
for broad scale study to include about 25 items for each Ohio State University, (b) 148 female and 84 male students en-
RIASEC scale, it was possible to eliminate any item not rolled in introductory psychology classes at Iowa State University,
correlating at least .45 with its own scale and, in addition, to and (c) 61 female and 20 male students enrolled in a measurement
eliminate those items having one or more correlations with course at Iowa State University. All students were administered the
SCI. In addition, the first 250 students in the Ohio State sample
other scales within .15 of or higher than its correlation with
were also administered the 1985 version of the SII (Hansen &
its own scale. For example, the item "Start a conversation Campbell, 1985). (These data were collected in the summer and
with a stranger," correlated .59 with Social (the scale for autumn of 1993, before the 1994 Revision of the Strong Interest
which it was written) and .47 with Enterprising. The school Inventory was available.) Administration of the SII to only the first
subject electronics was correlated .73 with Realistic (its own 250 participants was done to limit costs. Of the 250 participants,
scale) and .65 with Investigative. These items were elimi- 248 provided usable data.
nated. Of the total sample of 706 college students, 47% were freshmen,
Following the elimination of these items, the revised 34% were sophomores, 11% were juniors, and 7% were seniors.
version of the SCI consisted of 151 items. The number of One percent listed "other" as their class rank. Of the Ohio State
items per scale ranged from 23 to 27. Initial analyses of the sample of 393, 76% were Caucasian, 5% were African American,
scales indicated promising levels of internal consistency 6% were Asian American, 2% were Hispanic, 1 % was American
Indian, and 10% were international students. Forty-nine percent
reliability—values of coefficient alpha were .94 (Realistic
were freshmen, and 30%, 12%, and 8% were sophomores, juniors,
and Investigative), .93 (Conventional), .92 (Social), and .91 and seniors, respectively.
(Enterprising and Artistic). After construction of the origi- Instruments. The 151 ^tem version of the SCI was adminis-
nal 151-item form, a much larger sample was used to tered in this study. Based on the data obtained from the 1,147
develop and validate a shorter version of the scale and to adults and 706 college students (a total of 1,853 respondents), the
examine its relationships with gender, occupational group 151 item research version was shortened to 60 items (10 for each
membership, and vocational interests. RIASEC theme). We selected the best 10 items for each scale
based on three criteria: (a) high correlations (r > .45) with own
scale (total scale score minus the item score in question); (b)
Study 1 correlations with other scales that were at least .15 and usually .20
to .40 lower than their own-scale correlations; and (c) content, so
Method that the major substantive meanings of a Holland theme were
included in the item content. In some cases, content considerations
Participants and procedures. Participants in this study in-
cluded 1147 adults and 706 college students. The adult sample was
1
obtained from the database collected in 1993 for use in the 1994 The occupations included, in alphabetical order, were actuary,
revision of the Strong Interest Inventory (SII). Twenty-one occu- advertising executive, architect, auto mechanic, bookkeeper, busi-
pational groups' selected to be representative of both RIASEC ness education teacher, chemist, community service organization
areas and occupational level were selected. Members of the occu- director, computer programmer/systems analyst, elementary
pational groups, like those in previous noonings of the SII, met school teacher, flight attendant, forester, gardener or grounds-
criteria of reporting adequate job satisfaction, having at least three keeper, life insurance agent, marketing executive, medical records
years' experience on the job, performing the typical job duties of technician, parks and recreation coordinator, physicist, police of-
that occupation, and being between 25 and 60 years old (Harmon, ficer, and school administrator.
SELF-EFFICACY FOR HOLLAND'S THEMES 93
took priority over criterion b; for example, the school subject accomplishments necessary to build confidence. Interventions de-
agriculture was correlated .50 with Realistic (its designated scale) signed to increase efficacy, possibly structured by the counselor,
and .45 with Investigative, but its content was deemed essential to may be necessary to build confidence. Similarly, we postulate that
the Realistic content area. the low interest-high confidence pattern is more likely to occur in
Finally, if the above criteria were met and more than ten items gender-stereotypical areas, where people may have received con-
remained, those with the most extreme gender differences were siderable exposure to and thus competence and confidence in, but
eliminated. The t value for the significance of gender differences are not particularly interested in, that domain.
in item means was calculated for each item, and t values (absolute
values) greater than 3.5 (p < .001) represented items that were
eliminated to arrive at the desired ten. Even though plans for the Results
final scale included the use and interpretation for counseling
purposes of same-sex normative scores as well as raw scores, Values of coefficient alpha for the revised 10-item scales
elimination of at least some of the items with more extreme gender indicated adequate internal consistency reliability, with val-
differences in response was thought desirable. To the extent that ues of coefficient alpha ranging from .84 to .87 in the
gender differences are minimized in initial scale construction, raw student sample and from .84 to .88 in the employed adult
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

and same-sex standard scores yield more similar results. sample. Within this range, the Enterprising scale had the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Examples of Activities and School Subjects items for each lowest alpha (.84 in both samples), and the Realistic scale
theme are as follows: build a doll house, industrial arts (R); the highest alpha (.87 and .88, in the student and adult
perform a scientific experiment, calculus (I); design sets for a play, samples, respectively).
art (A); meet new people, counseling methods (S); sell a product Gender comparisons in raw scores within each sample are
to a customer, public speaking (E); organize systems for filing shown in Table 1.2 Because of the large number of statis-
information, accounting (C). Two scores were calculated for each
tical tests performed, we decided to use an alpha of .01 as
Holland confidence theme—a simple sum of the 1 to 5 confidence
the critical value for statistical significance. These data
ratings over the ten items and a standard T score (M = 50, SD =
10) based on the entire normative group of 1,853 individuals. For indicate differences largely in accordance with previous
later use in counseling, same-sex normative scores were also research on gender differences in RIASEC interests using
calculated. raw scores, but the differences are more pronounced in the
The General Occupational Themes (GOT) scores from the 1985 college student versus adult sample. Significant gender dif-
Strong Interest Inventory are highly reliable, with values of coef- ferences in the college sample were found on all but the
ficient alpha ranging from .83 to .91, and 30-day test-retest reli- Artistic theme, with college men scoring higher on the
ability ranging from .84 to .92. In addition, they correlate very Realistic, Investigative, Enterprising, and Conventional
highly with same-named GOT scores on the 1994 SII; correlations Confidence scales and college women scoring higher only
range from .93 (C) to .98 (A). on the Social theme.
Analysis of data. Means, standard deviations, and t tests for Within the adult sample, gender differences overall were
gender comparisons were computed separately for each participant evident only for the Realistic and Enterprising themes,
sample (college students and employed adults). In addition, a
toward which men reported greater confidence. There was
comparison of the sample groups was also made. Two-way mul-
tivariate analysis of variance was used to analyze score differences no female advantage in Social confidence among employed
as a function of gender and the 21 occupational groups, and adults. Within-gender comparisons of employed adults ver-
following significant multivariate Fs, post-hoc tests were used to sus college students indicated that employed men reported
examined the significance of differences. significantly more confidence with respect to the Enterpris-
Correlations between GOT interest scores from the 1985 SII ing and Conventional themes (both at p < .001) than did
confidence scores for the subsample of 248 college students were college men. Employed women were significantly more
calculated separately for men and women. In addition, a frequency confident than college women on the Realistic, Investiga-
distribution of the joint combinations of interest and confidence tive, Enterprising, and Conventional themes (p < .001), and
scores was produced. Because for eventual counseling purposes it on the Social theme (p < .01).
was postulated that high interest-low confidence and low interest- Multivariate analyses of variance of RIASEC confidence
high confidence would offer the most challenging information to a
scores as a function of occupational group and gender
counselor (vs. high interest-high confidence and low interest-low
confidence), their frequency of occurrence was determined by indicated statistically significant Fs for occupational group
defining high interest as any GOT score that would have resulted (Wilks' lambda = .20, F(120, 6365) = 17, p < .001),
in the interpretive comment "very high," "high," or "moderately gender (Wilks' lambda = .87, F(6,1100) = 27.1,p < .001),
high" within that gender, thus likely causing that theme to be and the group by gender interaction (Wilks' lambda = .87,
suggested for exploration. Confidence scores of 3.5 or more were F(120, 6365) = 1.3, p < .05). Although individual means
also defined as high—a score of 3.5 was selected because it is and profiles within each male and female occupational
halfway between 3 (moderate confidence), and 4 (much confi- group are too extensive to be provided here,3 in all cases the
dence). Scores on both the GOT and the SCI not categorized as highest confidence mean was congruent with the Holland
"high" were categorized as "low" for present purposes.
We postulated that the high interest-low confidence pattern is
most likely to occur in gender-nontypical RIASEC areas. More 2
Combined sex and within-sex standard (7) scores and percen-
specifically, the use of same-sex norms on inventories such as the tiles are available from Nancy E. Betz.
SII is intended to highlight potential interests in RIASEC areas 3
Score means and standard deviations for the 21 occupational
associated with the socialization experiences more typical of the groups, both male and female, can be obtained from Nancy E.
other gender, but same-sex norms cannot provide the performance Betz.
94 BETZ, HARMON, AND BORGEN

Table 1
Gender Comparisons of Scores on the Skills Confidence Inventory for College Students
and Employed Adults
College student raw scores Employed adult raw scores
Men Women Men Women
(n = 261) (n == 445) (n = 585) (n =•• 5 6 2 )
Theme M SD M SD t M SD M SD t
Realistic 3.5 0.74 2.9 0.74 -10.9** 3.6 0.81 3.3 0.83 -6.4**
Investigative 3.4 0.73 3.0 0.76 -6.7** 3.5 0.84 3.5 0.88 -0.2
Artistic 2.9 0.74 3.0 0.80 1.8 3.0 0.80 3.1 0.76 2.0
Social 3.4 0.64 3.7 0.61 6.0** 3.5 0.65 3.5 0.67 1.7
Enterprising 3.3 0.68 3.1 0.69 -3.4** 3.6 0.70 3.4 0.76 -5.0**
Conventional 3.4 0.70 3.1 0.74 -5.9** 3.8 0.72 3.7 0.76 -1.2
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

**p < .001.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

interest code of the occupation. For example, the highest the 1985 edition of the SII (the remaining students were
confidence scores of both male and female architects were administered only the SCI). As shown in Table 2, the
Artistic, Realistic, and Investigative—the Holland code for within-theme correlations between interests and confidence
architects in the SII norm groups (male and female) is, range from .46 (Social) to .74 (Artistic) among women;
likewise, Artistic, Realistic, and Investigative. Other strik- values shown for men range from .39 (Enterprising) to .68
ing profile similarities included confidence peaks on Artis- (Artistic). Most of the off-diagonal correlations that are
tic and Enterprising among male and female advertising statistically significant reflect a Holland-theory-predicted
executives, also an Artistic-Enterprising occupation on the similarity between adjacent (on the Hexagon) Holland
SII. Male and female chemists (Investigative-Realistic) re- themes; for example, r = .37 between Realistic interests and
ported greatest confidence on Investigative, and male and Investigative confidence in women and r = .36 between
female auto mechanics (Realistic) reported their highest Enterprising interests and Social confidence in men. Al-
confidence on Realistic. though moderate in magnitude, these correlations between
Table 2 shows the interest-efficacy correlations for the adjacent themes are lower than the confidence-interest cor-
subset of 158 college women and 90 college men who took relations within a theme. However, among both men and
women, Conventional confidence is significantly correlated
with Investigative interests. Among men, Investigative in-
Table 2 terests are significantly correlated with four of the six con-
Correlations Between Skills Confidence Inventory and fidence scores, and among women, Investigative interests
General Occupational Theme (GOT) Scores From the are related to three confidence scores.
1985 Strong Interest Inventory (SII) for Undergraduate
Women (n - 158) and Men (n = 90) Table 3 shows the number of college students obtaining
off-quadrant score patterns, that is, high interests-low con-
GOT interests (SII) fidence or vice versa. As predicted, patterns of high
Confidence
(self-efficacy) R I A S E C interest-low confidence were most common for themes
associated with the other gender. Sixty-six percent of the
Realistic
Women .63** .29** .34** .19 .12 .06 occurrences among women, versus 42% for men, occurred
Men .62** .28** .23 .00 .04 -.02 in Realistic, Investigative, Enterprising, or Conventional. In
Investigative contrast, 29% of those for men, but only 8% among women,
Women .37** .71** .08 .12 .00 .14 were in the Social theme. For high confidence-low interest,
Men .32* .60** .19 -.04 .06 .07 8 1 % of those for men, versus 5 1 % for women, were in
Artistic
Women .20* .14 .74** .13 .07 -.18 Realistic, Investigative, Enterprising, and Conventional. In
Men .11 .21 .68** .12 .04 -.15 contrast, 32% of those for women, versus 16% for men,
Social were in Social. All of these gender differences were statis-
Women -.11 .09 .10 .47:** .17 -.06 tically significant (p < .001) in pairwise comparisons using
Men .06 .24 .35** .51!** .36** .14
Enterprising the z test for tests of two proportions (Glass & Stanley,
Women -.02 .09 .10 .21* .46** .19 1984).
Men .15 .28* .25 .21 .39** .15
Conventional
Women .15 . 2 5 * •-.17 .08 .27** .62**
Men .20 .28* .18 .22 .06 .41** Study 2
Note. To reduce the possibility of experiment-wise error, the
criterion for statistical significance was set at p < .01. R = Method
Realistic; I = Investigative; A = Artistic; S = Social; E =
Enterprising; C = Conventional. Participants and procedures. Participants were 110 undergrad-
*p < .01. **p < .001. uate students, 29 men and 81 women, enrolled in a career devel-
SELF-EFFICACY FOR HOLLAND'S THEMES 95

Table 3 significance, respectively. Given the stated criterion of .01


Number of Students Falling Into the Off-Quadrants of a for a, only values of r of .43 achieve statistical significance
Confidence-Interest Fourfold Classification in the sample of men.
High interest and Higll confidence and Given this, it may be noted that confidence and interests
low confidence low interest within a Holland theme are correlated from .38 (Social) to
Women Men Women Men .59 (Conventional) among women and from .14 to .67
(» = 158) (/> =•• 9 0 ) (n = 158) (n = 90) among men. Most other significant correlations are those
between confidence and interests in adjacent themes (Real-
Holland theme N % N % N % N %
istic confidence and Investigative interests among women
Realistic 5 13 1 4 23 11 44 32 and Artistic confidence and Social interests among men). In
Investigative 2 5 0 0 34 16 20 14
Artistic 8 21 7 29 36 17 4 3
addition to a correlation with Realistic, Conventional con-
Social 3 8 7 29 70 32 22 16 fidence is correlated with interests in every area except
Enterprising 10 26 6 25 26 12 17 12 Artistic among women.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Conventional 8 21 3 13 27 13 32 23
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Total instances 38 24 216 139


Note. Based on the 1985 Strong Interest Inventory. The highest
General Occupational Theme (GOT) interest score was used if no Discussion
high or very high GOT was obtained. Each respondent, there-
fore, had at least one high interest score, and potentially up to The present studies resulted in the development and eval-
six. Thus, column totals can be over the actual number of partic- uation of a measure of perceived self-efficacy or confidence
ipants. Percentages reflect the proportion of occurrences within with respect to the six Holland theme areas. The 10-item
gender. scales were shown to have respectable levels of internal
consistency reliability. Evidence for validity was provided
by score patterns consistent with predicted group and occu-
pational differences, and within-occupation score profiles
opment class at a large midwestern university. The 1994 version of consistent with the Holland interest codes of each occupa-
the SII (Harmon et al., 1994) and the SCI were administered as tion. Moderate correlations with the analogous Holland
part of the course requirements. The SII was administered and interest score, obtained from the General Occupational
interpreted early in the semester, and the SCI was administered at
mid semester.
Instruments. The 60-item version of the SCI, as described in
Study 1, was administered. The GOT scores from the 1994 SII
were also obtained. The revision of the GOT's for the 1994 SII Table 4
began with an analysis of how well the original 20 items in each Correlations Between Skills Confidence Inventory and
scale were working, based on item-scale correlations. Weak items General Occupational Theme (GOT) Scores From the
were deleted and new, stronger items were added. Items were also 1994 Strong Interest Inventory (SII; Study 2)
examined for (low) correlations with other scales and for content.
Thus, internal consistency reliability was increased while main- GOT interests (SII)
taining content and construct validity. The resulting GOT scales Confidence
(self-efficacy) R I A S E c
varied in number of items per scale (from 20 for Investigative to 33
for Artistic), but all six had values of coefficient alpha equal to or Realistic
above .90 (from .90 for Social, Enterprising, and Conventional to Women .53** .36** .16 .24 -.04 .10
.94 for Artistic). The normative sample for these reliability anal- Men .14 .06 .09 .08 -.28 -.21
yses was 18,951 employed adults. Investigative
Women .16 .51** .08 .22 -.17 .00
Men -.09 .38 .29 .09 -.27 -.14
Artistic
Results Women .30** .12 .69** .02 .00 -.23
Men -.05 .12 .51** .48** -.20 -.23
Gender comparisons of score means indicated significant Social
Women .13 .14 .23 .38** -.07 -.10
differences (p < .05) only on Realistic, in favor of men Men -.02 .00 .31 .67** .04 -.15
(M = 3.3 vs. 2.9) and on Social, in favor of women (M = Enterprising
3.8 vs. 3.4). Scores on the other four themes were compa- Women .18 -.06 .17 .25 .49** .25
rable for the two sexes. Men -.17 -.07 -.06 .07 .21 .36
Table 4 presents the correlations between SCI scores and Conventional
Women .31* .34*: - . 2 0 .37** .32* .59**
the GOT scores from the 1994 SII. In examining the data Men -.35 -.05 -.38 -.18 -.01 .52*
shown in Table 4, it should be recalled that they are based Note. N = 81 for women; N = 29 for men. To reduce the
on only 29 men, and thus should not be considered as possibility of experiment-wise error, the criterion for statistical
generalizable, as would correlations obtained in a larger significance was set at p < .01. R = Realistic; I = Investigative;
sample. In addition, with an N of 29, values of r of .37 and A = Artistic; S = Social; E = Enterprising; C = Conventional.
.43 are needed to reach the .05 and .01 levels of statistical *p<.01. **p<.001.
96 BETZ, HARMON, AND BORGEN

Theme scores of the 1985 and 1994 editions of the SII, In addition to the moderate correlations between confi-
provided additional validity evidence. dence and interests within a theme, some unexpected cor-
Significant gender differences in Realistic confidence relations were also found. For example, the tendencies of
were found in all samples studied herein, the college sam- Conventional confidence scores to correlate (positively or
ples of Studies 1 (N = 706) and 2 (N = 110) and the sample negatively) with nonadjacent interest themes and for Inves-
of employed adults. College women reported significantly tigative interests in men to be correlated with several con-
more Social confidence than did college men, but this fidence scores need further study.
difference was not found in adults. In the large sample of Given the very promising nature of the present findings,
college students, men were also significantly more confi- however, we suggest that the SCI be used in further research
dent than were women on Enterprising, Investigative, and and theory testing. We also believe that it is useful in career
Conventional, and the gender difference on Enterprising counseling. We suggest that at a descriptive level, confi-
also occurred in the adult sample. These overall gender dence scores can be interpreted as the individual's confi-
differences are largely consistent with previous research on dence in her or his ability to perform activities and in
the Holland typology, suggesting that Realistic and Social
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

academic subjects related to each RIASEC theme area.


are the Holland interest themes on which gender differences
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Higher levels of confidence indicate greater comfort with


in favor of women have been most persistent when either
one's capabilities in an area and, theoretically, greater like-
raw scores or combined sex norms are used (cf. Betz, 1993;
lihood of "approaching" versus avoiding that behavioral
Gottfredson, Holland, & Gottfredson, 1975; Prediger &
domain. Lower confidence scores suggest a pattern of either
Hanson, 1976). The higher confidence of college men with
past failure or lack of experience, either one of which is
respect to the Conventional theme may reflect the heavier
computing and quantitative emphasis of the Conventional predictive of continued avoidance of that behavioral domain
confidence scale, which was designed to parallel the en- unless interventions are introduced.
hanced emphasis on these areas in the Conventional GOT of If we assume, as the Lent et al. (1994) model implies, that
the 1994 revision of the SII. both interests and some feeling of confidence or self-effi-
cacy are necessary before a client will pursue a career
On the other hand, there was a relative absence of gender option, then joint interpretation of interests and confidence
differences within occupational groups—for example, male may prove useful. Holland theme areas of both high interest
and female architects reported almost identical confidence and high confidence provide fairly obvious general areas to
profiles. Those few within-occupation gender differences
explore, and the high confidence scores may serve to
that were found were on RIASEC themes other than those
strengthen suggestions based on high interests. Low
characterizing the interest code of the occupation. Thus,
interest-low confidence themes can generally be eliminated
women successfully employed in an occupation appear to
from further consideration unless the profile suggests few or
be remarkably similar to men employed in that occupation,
at least in terms of confidence patterns. It should be noted, no other options. Areas of high interest-low confidence may
however, that the findings from the adult sample are not have been rejected by the client, but if efficacy-increasing
generalizable to all employed adults, much less to adults in interventions can be structured by the counselor (see Betz,
general. Because these employed adults were satisfied and 1992), that RIASEC area may be re-opened for the client.
relatively experienced in their jobs, they are not represen- We postulated that the high interest-low confidence pat-
tative of all employed people. Furthermore, the employed tern is most likely to occur in gender-nontypical RIASEC
men may be more similar to college-educated men in gen- areas. More specifically, the use of same-sex norms on
eral than the employed women are to college-educated inventories such as the SII is intended to highlight potential
women, because many more of the latter take significant interests in RIASEC areas associated with the socialization
amounts of time out of the labor force (e.g., see Arnold, experiences more typical of the other gender. Examination
1995). Overall, these employed adults possess characteris- of the frequency of occurrence of off-quadrant patterns for
tics consistent with higher scores on the SCI, in comparison the 248 students who were administered the SII as well as
to other groups. the SCI indicated that women were significantly more likely
Correlations between same-theme confidence and inter- than men to obtain high interest-low confidence patterns in
ests indicate overlap but also some distinctiveness, ranging traditionally male RIASEC areas, and men were signifi-
from .39 to .71 in Study 1 using the 1985 SII, and from .38 cantly more likely than women to obtain this pattern in
to .69 in women and .14 to .67 in the 29 men in Study 2, Holland's Social theme. The reverse pattern was true with
using the 1994 SII. Moderate correlations are consistent the low interest-high confidence pattern.
with both theoretical expectation and some previous re- Same-sex as well as combined sex norms for the SCI are
views and research (e.g., Lenox & Subick, 1994; Lent et al., available (footnote 2), but the use of the SCI to alert the
1994), but many herein are higher than Rooney's (1991) counselor to areas in which lack of confidence may be
correlations of .20 to .45 with Vocational Preference Inven- preventing exploration of possible interest areas suggests
tory interests. The present study does not address the issue the desirability of interpreting confidence scores in an ab-
of causality, for example, as postulated in the Lent et al. solute, that is "raw score" sense. Use of SCI raw scores is
(1994) social cognitive model. Structural models or, better consistent with the use on the SII of same-sex norms in that
yet, intervention studies, are needed to address this question. both are intended to have the result of broadening options
SELF-EFFICACY FOR HOLLAND'S THEMES 97

beyond those suggested by traditional gender role socializa- Gottfredson, G. D., Holland, J. L., & Gottfredson, L. S. (1975).
tion. Scores are only a means to an end in counseling, in this The relation of vocational aspirations and assessments to em-
case broadened exploration, so our endorsement of SCI raw ployment reality. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 7, 15-148.
scores and SII (GOT) within-sex normative scores is not Hackett, G., & Lent, R. W. (1992). Theoretical advances and
inconsistent. Additional research on the effects of using raw current inquiry in counseling psychology. In S. D. Brown &
versus same-sex normative scores is also needed to address R. W. Lent, Jr. (Eds.), Handbook of counseling psychology (2nd
ed., pp. 419-452). New York: Wiley.
questions of their differential effects.
Hansen, J. C , & Campbell, D. P. (1985). Manual for the
Although the applied focus of the SCI and its underlying
SVIB-SCII (4th ed.). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists
theory suggest interventions capable of increasing expecta- Press.
tions of self-efficacy or confidence, Lent et al.'s (1994) Harmon, L. W., Hansen, J. C , Borgen, F. H., & Hammer, A. L.
theory would also suggest that in expanding confidence we (1994). Strong Interest Inventory: Applications and technical
can expand interests, because facilitating approach behavior guide. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
should facilitate interest exploration. People may have in- Holland, J. L. (1973). Making vocational choices: A theory of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

terest potentials that are never developed or are underdevel- careers. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

oped because they were never exposed to the activity—for Holland, J. L. (1985a). Making vocational choices (2nd ed.).
many reasons, this may be more likely to happen in activity Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
areas traditional for the other sex. As there is a theoretical Holland, J. L. (1985b). Professional manual for the Self-Directed
basis for increasing self-efficacy via the four sources of Search. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
efficacy information and therefore approach behavior, it Lapan, R. T., Boggs, K. R., & Morrill, W. H. (1989). Self-efficacy
may be possible to start with confidence and see under what as a mediator of Investigative and Realistic general occupational
conditions interest development follows. themes on the Strong Interest Inventory. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 36, 176-182.
More research is needed on the correlates and utility of
Layton, P. L. (1984). Self-efficacy, locus of control, career sa-
the SCI, but we suggest that use of a confidence measure
lience, and women's career choice. Unpublished Ph.D. disser-
along with a measure of Holland's interests has consider- tation, Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota,
able potential to increase the educational and career options Minneapolis.
of individual clients and to stimulate the design of effective Lenox, R. A, & Subich, L. M. (1994). The relationship between
career interventions. We especially recommend research on self-efficacy beliefs and inventoried vocational interests. Career
the effectiveness of interventions based on efficacy theory Development Quarterly, 42, 302-313.
and on the interrelationships of efficacy, interests, and ap- Lent, R. W., Brown, S. D., & Hackett, G. (1994). Toward a uni-
proach versus avoidance behavior. fying social cognitive theory of career and academic interest,
choice, and performance. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 45,
79-122.
Lent, R. W., Brown, S. D., & Larkin, K. C. (1984). Relation of
References self-efficacy expectations to academic achievement and persis-
tence. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 31, 356-362.
Arnold, K. D. (1995). Lives of promise. San Francisco: Jossey Matsui, T. M., & Tsukamoto, S. (1991). Relation between career
Bass. self-efficacy measures based on occupational titles and Holland
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of codes and model environment. Journal of Vocational Behavior,
behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191-215.
38, 78-92.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action.
Multon, K. D., Brown, S. D., & Lent, R. W. (1991). Relation of
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
self-efficacy beliefs to academic outcomes: A metaanalytic in-
Betz, N. E. (1992). Counseling uses of career self-efficacy theory.
Career Development Quarterly, 41, 22-26. vestigation. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38, 30-38.
Betz, N. E. (1993). Issues in the use of ability and interest Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric theory. New York: McGraw
measures with women. Journal of Career Assessment, 1, 217- Hill.
232. Osipow, S. H., Temple, R. D., & Rooney, R. A. (1993). The Short
Betz, N. E., & Hackett, G. (1981). The relationship of career- Form of the Task Specific Occupational Self-Efficacy Scale.
related self-efficacy expectations to perceived career options in Journal of Career Assessment, 1, 13-20.
college women and men. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 28, Post-Kammer, P., & Smith, P. L. (1985). Sex differences in career
399-410. self-efficacy, consideration, and interests of eighth and ninth
Betz, N. E., & Hackett, G. (1983). The relationship of mathematics graders. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 32, 551-555.
self-efficacy expectations to the selection of science-based col- Prediger, D., & Hanson, G. (1976). Holland's theory of careers
lege majors. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 23, 329-345. applied to men and women: Analysis of implicit assumptions.
Campbell, D. (1992). Campbell Interest and Skill Survey. Minne- Journal of Vocational Behavior, 8, 167-184.
apolis, MN: National Computer Systems. Rooney, R. A. (1991). The relationship of task-specific occupa-
Cole, N., & Hanson, G. (1975). Impact of interest inventories on tional self-efficacy and career interests in college women and
career choice. In E. E. Diamond (Ed.), Issues of sex bias and sex men. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Psychol-
fairness in career interest measurement. Washington, DC: Na- ogy, The Ohio State University, Columbus.
tional Institute of Education. Rooney, R. A., & Osipow, S. H. (1992). Task-specific occupa-
Glass, G. V., & Stanley, J. (1984). Statistical methods in psychol- tional self-efficacy scale: The development and validation of a
ogy and education. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. prototype. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 40, 14-32.
98 BETZ, HARMON, AND BORGEN

Solberg, V. S., Good, G. E., Nord, D., Holm, C , Hohner, R., Zima, CDMSE to vocational indecision. Journal of Vocational Behav-
N., Hefferman, M., & Malen, A. (1994). Assessing career search ior, 37, 17-31.
expectations: Development and validation of the Career Search Zilber, S. M. (1988). The effects of sex, task performance, and
Efficacy Scale. Journal of Career Assessment, 2, 111-124. attributional styles on task and career self-efficacy expectations.
Swanson, J. L. (1993). Integrated assessment of vocational inter- Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Psychology, The
Ohio State University, Columbus.
ests and self-rated skills and abilities. Journal of Career Assess-
ment, 1, 50-65. Received May 30, 1995
Taylor, K. M., & Popma, J. (1990). Construct validity of the career Revision received August 14, 1995
decision-making self-efficacy scale and the relationship of Accepted August 21, 1995 •
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Statement of Ownership, Management, and Circulatior


fBm POSTAL SERVICE™
I. pm*™».Ti«. kMogiMi
EJUM and N u n of Oroulrton Atiml No. Copt., of Btigu !•
»• .
D«,lr,ll',.i.= , , 3 , l l , s . , M PuUHhM N W H I U Flbig D.U

M-M-l I M 10,199 10,701


Quarterly • i ;• ' , ' i n s t .

"°-
-0-

— • - — 9,1** 9,398
9,144 9,398

109 113

-0- -0-

109 113

Clara E. K i l l , Ph.D.
9,2^3 9,511

(i) on™ mi. M M I . Soaud 946 1,190

-CI- -0-

j£jH£»S£S3 l O , 199

U.I 98.«
, 1 P 1, 1 1

OtktiSwrlylMinOMtiggiit k t n g l P « « r y M « . o l T o U I — — - " — • . « -

,0»»

as a gonorjf or rsquHlei' publcatron. Ihil Statement ol OrmofAOip. Management, ar-

puCtahod during Oclobec

'ssssssss D
(SalSlttoSllIrl^IIS^aw.'wison

You might also like