Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

International Journal of Psychology, 2014

DOI: 10.1002/ijop.12110

The importance of autonomy support and the mediating


role of work motivation for well-being: Testing
self-determination theory in a Chinese work organisation
Youyan Nie1 , Bee Leng Chua1 , Alexander Seeshing Yeung2 , Richard M. Ryan2,3 ,
and Wai Yen Chan1
1 Psychological Studies Academic Group, National Institute of Education, Nanyang
Technological University, Singapore
2 Insitute for Positive Psychology and Education, Australian Catholic University, Strathfield,

NSW, Australia
3 Department of Clinical and Social Sciences in Psychology, University of Rochester, Rochester

NY, USA

W e examine relations between perceived organisational autonomy support and different types of work motivation
and well-being outcomes in 266 teachers from two government schools in China. We hypothesised that greater
autonomy support would be associated with more autonomous forms of employee motivation, and that teacher motivation
would in turn mediate the effects of autonomy support on indicators of work well-being (i.e., job satisfaction, work stress
and physical ill symptoms). Results generally supported the hypothesised relations between perceived autonomy support
and SDT’s five types of motivations. Findings also showed that perceived autonomy support predicted job satisfaction
directly and indirectly through the mediating roles of intrinsic motivation, identified regulation, introjected regulation and
external regulation. Perceived autonomy support predicted work stress directly and indirectly through the mediating roles
of external regulation and amotivation. Autonomy support also predicted illness symptoms via the mediating roles of
intrinsic motivation, introjected regulation and amotivation. The current findings highlight how perceived organisational
support for autonomy relates to motivational differences in a Chinese work context, and the potential relevance of
autonomy support for employee well-being.

Keywords: Self-determination theory; Motivation; Well-being.

Work is a very important part of life, consuming much sense of guilty or obligation). This theory has generated
time in one’s lifespan and for many, much investment much empirical research in organisational literature,
of energy and concern. As such, the role of work in with findings showing that more autonomous forms of
contributing to individuals’ well-being and quality of motivation are generally associated with more positive
life is gaining increasing attention in research (e.g., outcomes (e.g., self-regulation, persistence, commitment,
Vansteenkiste, Lens, de Witte, & Feather, 2005). The job satisfaction) and that autonomy support can promote
purpose of the current study is to contribute to the under- autonomous forms of motivation (e.g., Fernet, Guay,
standing of employees’ job satisfaction and well-being Senécal, & Austin, 2012).
within a Chinese work organisation using motivation The variability in values and behaviours across dif-
constructs based in self-determination theory (SDT; ferent cultures has been well observed and this has led
Ryan & Deci, 2000). researchers to consider social integration, motivation and
SDT is a motivation theory that focuses on people’s well-being issues from cultural perspectives. The notion
volitional motivation—specifically the degree to which of autonomy support bringing forth beneficial work out-
they experience their actions as autonomous (i.e., acting comes has been an area that attracts researchers across
based on choice, interest, pleasure or values) versus cultures. Some researchers argue that, culturally valued or
controlled (i.e., acting based on rewards, punishment, not, behaviour based on more autonomous motives will

Correspondence should be addressed to Youyan Nie, Psychological Studies Academic Group, National Institute of Education, Nanyang Technolog-
ical University, Singapore 639798, Singapore. (E-mail: youyan.nie@nie.edu.sg).

© 2014 International Union of Psychological Science


2 NIE ET AL.

foster greater persistence, performance and well-being are initiated and maintained by contingencies external
outcomes than controlled motives (e.g., Chirkov, Ryan, to the person such as rewards or threats of punishments.
Kim, & Kaplan, 2003). Autonomy support is therefore External regulation is characterised by low autonomy,
expected to lead to a range of positive outcomes. In con- and has been related to negative wellness outcomes (Ryan
trast, some scholars have suggested that because peo- & Deci, 2000). Somewhat less controlled is introjected
ple from collectivist cultures value interdependence they regulation, which is represented by behaviours driven by
will place lower value on individual autonomy, and may internal rather than external rewards and punishments.
not realise the same benefits from autonomy support that In introjected regulation, the person is motivated to
have been identified in Western samples (e.g., Kitayama, avoid self-conscious emotions such as shame, guilt and
Snibbe, Markus, & Suzuki, 2004; Markus, Kitayama, self-critical feelings, and to obtain positive self-related
& Heiman, 1996; Schwartz, 1994). The current study affects and appraisals. A more autonomous form of
aims to contribute to the current research by testing motivation would be identified regulation, in which the
the relations among autonomy support, motivation and person is motivated because the behaviour is congruent
well-being in a Chinese (mainland) cultural context. with the individual’s personal goals and values. In short,
Chinese culture is chosen in the current study because SDT specifies five types of motivation (intrinsic motiva-
China is a fast developing country with a long history tion, identified regulation, introjected regulation, external
of Confucianism with values emphasising group orienta- regulation and amotivation) in a descending order of
tion, interpersonal harmony, acceptance of authority and relative autonomy or self-determination.
benevolence (e.g., Liem & Nie, 2008) but not auton- There has been considerable evidence of the ordered
omy in any explicit way. It is therefore unclear whether nature of these motives along a continuum of autonomy.
SDT is applicable to Chinese organisational contexts with This evidence has primarily come from domains such as
adult employees. Accordingly this study examines the sport and education (Ryan & Connell, 1989), and has
five different types of motivation specified within SDT included samples from Asian contexts (e.g., Yamauchi
in a Chinese organisational context. More specifically & Tanaka, 1998). Nonetheless there has been a paucity
we examine a model based on SDT in which perceived of studies in Chinese contexts, especially Chinese work
organisational autonomy support is associated with both organisations.
autonomous forms of motivation and well-being; and In the current study, we thus investigated if this con-
whether the different types of motivation mediate the rela- tinuum of autonomy manifests in the context of teacher
tions between perceived autonomy support and employee employees in two large Chinese government-run schools.
well-being. As predicted by SDT, we expected a consistent trend
depicting gradual changes in the correlations between
these five types of motivation with both perceived
Self-determination and different types autonomy support from managers and with employee
of motivation well-being outcomes. For example, there should, for
most wellness-related outcome variables, be a gradual
According to SDT, there are distinct types of motivation
correlation change from more to less positive relations
that vary in terms of their degree of self-determination.
between autonomy support and identified, introjected
The theory began with investigations of intrinsic motiva-
and external regulation, respectively.
tion, which is characterised by a high degree of auton-
omy. Intrinsic motivation refers to behaviours arising
from inherent interest or enjoyment of a task itself. Perceived autonomy support, employee
This type of motivation is reflected in people’s nat- motivation and well-being
ural tendency to seek novelty and challenges, and to
explore and learn for its own sake. As a prototypically Besides distinguishing different types of motivation in
autonomous form of motivation, it represents the top end terms of their degree of autonomy, SDT also highlights
of the self-determination continuum. At the lowest end of the importance of an autonomy supportive environment in
the self-determination continuum is amotivation, which facilitating or undermining different types of motivation
refers to a state of lacking any intention to act. Other and personal well-being (Ryan & Deci, 2000). In gen-
types of motivation lie in between. These other types of eral, perceived autonomy support should facilitate inter-
motivation can be categorised as extrinsic motivation, in nalisation and thus promotes more autonomous forms
that unlike intrinsic motivation, the motive to act is oper- of behavioural regulation. A great deal of research has
ationally separable from the action itself. According to shown the importance of autonomy support in promoting
SDT, extrinsic motives vary in terms of the level of auton- autonomous motivation and well-being (Chirkov & Ryan,
omy entailed in behaviour regulation. 2001; Deci et al., 2001; Gagné, Ryan, & Bargmann, 2003;
The most controlled form of extrinsic motivation is Kins, Beyers, Soenens, & Vansteenkiste, 2009; Pelletier,
labelled external regulation, and concerns behaviours that Fortier, Vallerand, & Brière, 2001; Taylor & Ntoumanis,

© 2014 International Union of Psychological Science


MOTIVATION AND WELL-BEING 3

2007). However, few studies have explored how perceived symptoms) which have been widely used as well-being
autonomy support is related to the five different types indicators. These provide adequate coverage of the
of motivation. Extending previous studies, the current well-being construct (i.e., positive and negative aspects,
research will examine the differential relations between as well as psychological and physical aspects) (e.g., Ng
perceived autonomy support and the five types of moti- et al., 2012). We hypothesised that intrinsic motivation
vation described above. In addition, we investigate how will show the strongest relations with well-being (i.e.,
autonomy support directly and/or indirectly predicts job positively related to positive indicators and negatively
satisfaction, work stress and illness symptoms. related to negative indicators of well-being), followed by
Studies have shown that the more autonomous one’s identified regulation. External regulation and amotivation
motivation is, the more the person is likely to experience will show negative relations with well-being indicators
greater well-being. Many of these empirical studies have (i.e., negatively related to the positive indicators and pos-
combined different types of motivation into a single index itively related to the negative indicators in well-being).
(i.e., autonomous index) or further classified them into Finally, introjected regulation will be associated with
two categories—autonomous motivation (the combina- both positive consequences and negative consequences.
tion of intrinsic motivation and identified regulation), and In practical terms, research on the relative associations
controlled motivation (the combination of introjected and between each motivation type with outcomes may pro-
external regulation) (e.g., Fernet et al., 2012; Kins et al., vide insights regarding how specific types of motivation
2009; Vansteenkiste et al., 2005). These studies are valu- may enhance or undermine specific well-being outcomes.
able in their depiction of the importance of autonomous
motivation in learning and work organisations. Yet these Motivations as mediators in the Chinese context
findings often provide limited information on how the
distinct types of motivation influence well-being. This dif- In the current study, we also explore the psychological
ferentiation is important because first, intrinsic motivation mechanisms that may account for the hypothesised rela-
and identified motivation, which are usually classified as tion between perceived autonomy support and well-being.
autonomous motivations, could have differential effects Since previous studies have shown that perceived auton-
on well-being and performance. Intrinsic motivation omy support is the antecedent of different types of moti-
has been found to predict well-being whereas identified vation and different types of motivation also have differ-
motivation predicted academic performance. Second, ent consequences for wellness(e.g., Pelletier et al., 2001;
introjected motivation and external motivation, which Richer, Blanchard, & Vallerand, 2002), it is reasonable
are usually classified as controlled motivation, could also to hypothesise that different types of motivation may
show differential effects on well-being. Research shows mediate the relations between autonomy support and
that external regulation consistently has positive relations well-being. However, empirical studies testing the media-
with maladaptive outcomes (e.g., depression, anxiety) tion effects of motivation on perceived autonomy support
and negative relations with adaptive outcomes (e.g., pos- and well-being are scarce in Chinese contexts. Hence the
itive affect, quality of life). Most importantly, introjected current study also attempts to fill this gap in the literature.
regulation, which represents regulations that are inter- SDT postulates that an autonomy supportive envi-
nalised and yet not well integrated, have shown positive ronment should have beneficial effects on motivation
correlations with both negative affect and positive affect and well-being across different cultural contexts because
(see Ng et al., 2012 for a meta-analysis). Third, identified fundamental psychological needs (e.g., needs for auton-
and introjected regulations have different relations with omy) are universal (Chua, Wong, & Koestner, 2014).
decision-making strategies, emotions and behaviours Some studies in Chinese learning contexts have sup-
after controlling for each other’s variance (partial corre- ported the beneficial effects of motivation on learning
lation) (Koestner, Losier, Vallerand, & Carducci, 1996). (e.g., Zhou, Ma, & Deci, 2009).Yet this claim is con-
Thus while both identification and introjection are asso- troversial because cross-cultural studies have shown that
ciated with effort (Ryan & Connell, 1989), introjection cultures vary greatly in their explicit valuing of autonomy
has been associated with some undesirable outcomes, (Schwartz, 1994) and, in some views, support for auton-
whereas identification tends to predict positive outcomes omy may be important only in cultures in which auton-
(e.g., Pelletier et al., 2001). Therefore, in the current omy is explicitly valued. Therefore, a driving question of
study we attempt to examine the predictive relations of current study is whether perceived autonomy support is
all five types of motivation with well-being, treating them still desirable or beneficial in a cultural context that may
as distinct types of motivation with their own unique not explicitly value the importance of autonomy. Specif-
predictive relationships on a range of outcomes. ically, the current study extends research by examining
Based on our review of numerous studies on well- whether perceived autonomy support and the five types
being (e.g., Baard, Deci, & Ryan, 2004; Vansteenkiste of motivation show beneficial relations with employee
et al., 2005), we selected three indicators of work-related well-being in a Chinese work context using a sample of
well-being (i.e., job satisfaction, work stress, illness teachers in a Chinese government school organisation.

© 2014 International Union of Psychological Science


4 NIE ET AL.

Significance of the present study TABLE 1


Fit indices for 5-factor, 3-factor and 1-factor models for teacher
The present study will contribute to the existing liter- motivation scale
ature by providing a deeper understanding of the dif- 5-Factor model 3-Factor model 1-Factor model
ferentiated impact of different types of motivation in
well-being. Specifically, the present study will examine χ2 155.09 705.56 963.31
N 266 266 266
the differential relations of all five types of work motiva-
df 76 87 90
tion specified within the SDT continuum with well-being TLI .95 .673 .554
both at the univariate and multivariate levels, which has CFI .97 .729 .618
been seldom reported in previous studies. In addition RMSEA .06 .164 .191
this research focuses on the positive impact of perceived
autonomy support on employees’ wellness and job satis-
faction. Finally, by focusing on teacher employees within
a Chinese government school organisation, this research one-factor structure provided a good fit for the data, χ2 (59,
further examines the applicability of SDT-derived con- N = 266) = 144.20, TLI = .97, CFI = .97, RMSEA = .07.
structs to Asian cultural contexts. The scale showed good internal consistency reliability,
α = .96
METHOD

Participants Teacher work motivation scale

Participants were 266 teachers from two government mid- Teacher work motivation scale was adapted from the
dle schools in Hebei province, China. Ages ranged from work tasks motivation scale for teachers (Fernet, Senécal,
23 to 60 (median = 33 years). Participants included 76 Guay, Marsh, & Dowson, 2008). There were 5 subscales
males and 188 females (two teachers did not indicate gen- to measure intrinsic motivation, identified regulation,
der). Of these teachers, 85.7% held a Bachelor’s degree. introjected regulation, external regulation and amoti-
vation. Each scale had three items. We compared the
5-factor model with theoretically plausible 3-factor and
Procedures 1-factor CFA models, and found that the 5-factor model
provided the best fit for the data. These fit indices are
Two trained researchers administered the surveys to the
presented in Table 1. The internal consistency was good
teachers from the two schools in their respective school
(α = .92 for intrinsic motivation, α = .82 for identified
auditoriums during working hours. Each teacher took
regulation, α = .77 for introjected regulation, α = .75 for
about 15 minutes to complete the survey. In accordance
external regulation, α = .85 for amotivation).
with ethics procedures, participants were told that their
responses would be confidential and anonymous and that
they were allowed to withdraw from the study at any point
Work-related well-being
without penalty.
Work-related well-being was measured with three
Measures scales (i.e., job satisfaction, work stress, illness symp-
toms) adapted from Pettegrew and Wolf (1982). These
All items were rated on 5-point Likert scales (1 = strongly scales were all constructed relevant to teachers’ school
disagree to 5 = strongly agree). All measures on the work. Job satisfaction is a domain-specific satisfaction
questionnaire were translated from English into Chinese about work. The job satisfaction scale included 4 items
through a translation and back-translation procedure coor- (e.g., I am satisfied with my job). The work stress scale
dinated by the first author. included 8 items about the stress related to work in
the school (e.g., trying to complete the school work
on time causes me a lot of stress). The illness symp-
Perceived work autonomy support
toms scale included 4 items about the general illness
A 13-item scale assessed participants’ perceived work symptoms related to work (e.g., I have trouble getting
autonomy support from their direct supervisor/manager. to sleep or staying asleep due to the work problem).
This scale was adapted from Baard et al. (2004)’s work A three-factor confirmatory factor analysis provided a
climate questionnaire. The original scale consisted of good fit for the data, (96, N = 266) = 138.49, TLI = .96,
15 items. Two items were dropped due to low factor CFI = .97, RMSEA = .04. The internal consistency was
loadings. Confirmatory factor analysis was conducted good (α = .75 for job satisfaction, α = .81 for work stress,
to examine the factorial structure of the construct. A α = .82 for ill symptoms).

© 2014 International Union of Psychological Science


MOTIVATION AND WELL-BEING 5

Test the presence of common method direct and indirect relations among the perceived auton-
variance omy support, different types of teacher motivation, and
well-being indicators.
When all data are self-reported and collected through Multivariate normality test (Mardia’ coefficient) was
the same questionnaire during the same period of time, conducted and the results showed that kurtosis = 32.882,
common method variance may be a concern. To test critical value = 18.985. Based on a cut-off value of 5 (e.g.,
whether a substantial amount of common method vari- Byrne, 2010), the assumption of multivariate normality
ance is present, the unrotated principal component analy- is rejected. Therefore, bootstrapping was applied using
sis (Harman’s one-factor test) and principal component the MPlus software to obtain estimates of standard errors
analysis with promax rotation were conducted for 44 as bootstrap approach is less biased when the underlying
items. Both revealed the presence of 9 distinct factors distribution is non-normal (e.g., Preacher & Hayes, 2008).
with eigenvalue greater than 1.0, rather than a single fac- The number of bootstrap samples was set as 200 using
tor. The 9 factors together accounted for 68.46% of the Maximum Likelihood estimator.
total variance; the first (largest) factor did not account
for a majority of the variance (33.70%). Thus, no gen-
eral factor is apparent. Moreover, the confirmatory fac- Descriptive statistics and zero-order
tor analysis showed that the single-factor model did not correlations
fit the data well, χ2 (594, N = 266) = 3212.4, p < .001,
CFI = .612; TLI = .589; RMSEA = .129. Although the Descriptive statistics and zero-order correlations among
results of these analyses do not preclude the possibility the variables are presented in Table 2. Consistent with
of common method variance, they suggest that common SDT, the correlation coefficients for five types of moti-
method variance was limited. vation with autonomy support and job satisfaction show
that (a) intrinsic motivation has the highest positive
correlations with autonomy support and job satisfaction,
Control variables (b) amotivation had highest negative correlations with
them, and (c) there was a gradual correlation change
A number of work-related variables and demographics from positive to negative with identified, introjected
were considered as control variables, including gender and external regulation, respectively. Based on the
(0 = female; 1 = male), age, educational level, occupa- degree of self-determination from highest to lowest,
tional position level, teaching experience and school the rank order was as follows: Intrinsic motivation with
(one school was coded as 0 and the other was coded as the highest positive correlation with perceived auton-
1). However, only gender and school were controlled in omy support (r = .56), identified regulation (r = .48),
subsequent analyses because (a) gender was correlated introjected regulation (r = .42), external regulation
with introjected regulation external regulation and (b) with a negative correlation (r = −.38), and amotivation
school was correlated with perceived autonomy support, with the greatest negative correlation (r = −.49). The
intrinsic motivation, identified regulation, external regu- reversed pattern was obtained for work stress and ill
lation, amotivation, job satisfaction and gender. The other symptoms.
potential control variables were not significantly related
with perceived autonomy support, work motivation or
the well-being outcomes. Mediation analysis

Mediation analysis was performed using the causal steps


DATA ANALYSIS AND RESULTS recommended by MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman,
West and Sheets (2002). In the first step, we demon-
Data analysis strated significant direct effects of perceived autonomy
support for all three outcomes of job satisfaction, work
We calculated the means for all the variables to construct stress and ill symptoms, when all indirect paths are con-
the measured latent variables.1 Descriptive statistics strained to zero. In the second step, we demonstrated that
and zero-order Pearson correlation coefficients were perceived autonomy support significantly predicted the
computed and inspected. Causal steps recommended five mediators. In the final step, we allowed all indirect
by MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West, and Sheets paths to be freely estimated and demonstrated significant
(2002) were performed to demonstrate mediation. Finally, indirect effects. At the same time, the direct effects are
Path analysis was conducted to examine the significant considerably weaker. Table 3 shows the results of the

1 It is recommended to use summated scale scores rather than factor scores if generalisability is desired because it can be easily replicated across

studies (Hair, Black, Babin, Anderson, & Tatham, 2006).

© 2014 International Union of Psychological Science


6 NIE ET AL.

TABLE 2
Descriptive statistics and zero-order correlations

Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

1 Perceived work autonomy support 3.33 .78 —


2 Intrinsic motivation 3.61 .79 .56** —
3 Identified regulation 3.76 .67 .48** .60** —
4 Introjected regulation 3.38 .74 .42** .45** .48** —
5 External regulation 2.75 .74 −.38** −.30** −.31** −.07 —
6 Amotivation 2.56 .78 −.49** −.53** −.50** −.36** .46** —
7 Job satisfaction 3.51 .67 .53** .66** .56** .47** −.34** −.50** —
8 Work stress 3.04 .66 −.36** −.31** −.30** −.12 .36** .41** −.28** –
9 Ill symptoms 2.42 .82 −.20** −.30** −.15* −.03 .19** .32** −.22** .45** —
10 Gender −.03 .04 .04 .13* .16* .09 .03 .02 .04 —
11 School .18** .17** .21** .10 −.12* −.16* .18** .04 −.04 −.22** —
* p < .05. ** p < .01.

mediation analysis and the test of direct effects, total stress. In addition, perceived autonomy support also
indirect effects and specific indirect effects. indirectly predicted work stress through external regu-
lation and amotivation. Perceived autonomy support did
not directly predict illness symptoms, but it indirectly
Path analysis predicted it through introjected regulation, amotivation
and (negatively) intrinsic motivation.
The path analysis was conducted and all the signifi-
cant regression coefficients were kept in the path model
and all the non-significant regression coefficients were DISCUSSION
removed from the model (i.e., constrained to zero in
the model). The final path model fits the data well, In this study we examined the relations of five types
χ2 (23, N = 266) = 24.934, Bootstrap p = .354, TLI = .995, of motivation, specified with SDT (Ryan & Deci, 2000)
CFI = .998, RMSEA = .018. The path coefficients for the as lying along a continuum of relative autonomy, with
final path model are presented in Figure 1. Key observa- perceived work climate and well-being outcomes in a
tions of the model are highlighted below. Chinese work organisation. Data from teachers in two
First, perceived autonomy support predicted employee public schools revealed a systematic pattern in correla-
work motivation. The results showed that perceived tions. The more autonomous the type of motivation, the
autonomy support was a positive predictor of intrinsic stronger its positive relations with both perceived auton-
motivation, identified regulation and introjected regu- omy support and job satisfaction. Conversely, there is a
lation. Perceived autonomy support was, in contrast, reversed progressing pattern of correlations to both work
negatively associated with external regulation, and stress and illness symptoms. This pattern has provided
amotivation. empirical evidence to support SDT’s claim of five types of
Second, teachers’ work motivation was associated motivation varying in terms of autonomy, as well as their
with work-related well-being in generally expected ways. relations with important workplace outcomes. These find-
For job satisfaction, intrinsic motivation, identified reg- ings extend previous research by delineating in a Chinese
ulation and external regulation were significant positive work organisation how each motivation type relates at a
predictors. Introjected regulation was unexpectedly pos- univariate level to various outcomes. The results also sup-
itively associated with these wellness outcomes as well. ported our hypotheses concerning the meditational role of
For work stress, both external regulation and amotivation these motives in helping to explain the relations between
were predictors. Illness symptoms were predicted by both autonomy supportive work environments and employee
introjected motivation and amotivation, and were nega- outcomes, which replicates findings conducted in West-
tively related with intrinsic motivation. ern teacher (e.g., Pelletier et al., 2001) and employee (e.g.,
Third, perceived autonomy support predicted well- Richer et al., 2002) settings.
being directly and indirectly through the mediating
role of teacher work motivation. Perceived autonomy
support directly predicted job satisfaction. In addition, The beneficial roles of autonomy support
perceived autonomy support also indirectly predicted
job satisfaction through intrinsic motivation, identified The present findings support the beneficial roles of
regulation, introjected regulation and external regulation. autonomy support in promoting internalised forms of
Perceived autonomy support directly predicted work motivation (intrinsic motivation, identified regulation and

© 2014 International Union of Psychological Science


MOTIVATION AND WELL-BEING 7

TABLE 3
Mediation analysis using bootstrap standard errors

Standardised effect p-Valuea

Step 1
Direct effects
Perceived autonomy support → Job Satisfaction .522 <.001
Perceived autonomy support → Work Stress −.374 <.001
Perceived autonomy support → Ill Symptoms −.214 <.001
Step 2
Mediations regressed on predictors
Perceived autonomy support → Intrinsic Motivation .554 <.001
Perceived autonomy support → Identified Regulation .482 <.001
Perceived autonomy support → Introjected Regulation .420 <.001
Perceived autonomy support → External Regulation −.376 <.001
Perceived autonomy support → Amotivation −.494 <.001
Step 3
Direct effects
Perceived autonomy support → Job Satisfaction .123 .058
Perceived autonomy support → Work Stress −.144 .030
Perceived autonomy support → Ill Symptoms −.014 .867
Indirect Effects of Perceived Autonomy Support → Job Satisfaction .408 <.001
Perceived autonomy support → Intrinsic Motivation → Job Satisfaction .212 <.001
Perceived autonomy support → Identified Regulation → Job Satisfaction .066 .038
Perceived autonomy support → Introjected Regulation → Job Satisfaction .062 .007
Perceived autonomy support → External Regulation → Job Satisfaction .033 .167
Perceived autonomy support → Amotivation → Job Satisfaction .035 .301
Indirect Effects of Perceived Autonomy Support → Work Stress −.195 <.001
Perceived autonomy support → Intrinsic Motivation → Work Stress −.032 .523
Perceived autonomy support → Identified Regulation → Work Stress −.049 .270
Perceived autonomy support → Introjected Regulation → Work Stress .044 .138
Perceived autonomy support → External Regulation → Work Stress −.063 .046
Perceived autonomy support → Amotivation → Work Stress −.107 .015
Indirect Effects of Perceived Autonomy Support → Ill Symptoms −.181 .001
Perceived autonomy support → Intrinsic Motivation → Ill Symptoms −.142 .006
Perceived autonomy support → Identified Regulation → Ill Symptoms .035 .405
Perceived autonomy support → Introjected Regulation → Ill Symptoms .061 .039
Perceived autonomy support → External Regulation → Ill Symptoms −.011 .665
Perceived autonomy support → Amotivation → Ill Symptoms −.124 .002
a p-values computed using bootstrap standard errors (number of bootstrap samples = 200).

introjected regulation) as well as job satisfaction as an out- This result may reflect the Confucian cultural context,
come. Autonomy support was also directly or indirectly and represent perhaps a nuanced difference that exists
negatively associated with external regulation, amoti- between Asian and Western organisational contexts. It
vation, work stress and illness symptoms. Thus despite is noteworthy, however, that some Western studies have
some claims that autonomy is not highly relevant in also shown a similarly modest relation of autonomy
Asian cultures, SDT hypotheses concerning the positive support to introjections, displaying a similar pattern of
impact of autonomy support on motivation were largely correlations. For example, Pelletier et al. (2001) showed
supported in this Chinese organisational context. These a similar descending pattern of correlations between
findings are also consistent with research conducted perceived coach autonomy support and elite-level Cana-
in other cultures supporting the applicability of SDT dian swimmers’ intrinsic, identified, introjected, external
(Chirkov & Ryan, 2001; Deci et al., 2001; Gagné et al., and amotivational motivations, within which introjection
2003; Pelletier et al., 2001; Taylor & Ntoumanis, 2007). was positively associated with autonomy support and
Although the results generally conformed to expec- short-term persistence. It is also worth noting in interpret-
tations, the positive correlation between perceived ing the current findings that, although introjection was not
autonomy support and introjected regulation was par- directly related to any negative outcomes at the zero-order
ticularly intriguing given the study context. It seems level, in our SEM model it was positively related to both
likely that the more supportive the perceived work cli- positive and negative outcomes (i.e., illness symptoms),
mate, the more Chinese teachers were apt to endorse suggesting in line with SDT predictions that introjection
motives of “not disappointing” supervisors or leaders. is a form of internal motivation that can have internal

© 2014 International Union of Psychological Science


8 NIE ET AL.

Antecedent variable Criterion variable


Mediator
.31
Intrinsic
Motivation
.56** .34**
.23
.53
Identified .16**
.42**
Regulation Job
Satisfaction
.12*
.13*
.24
-.16** -.10*
Perceived
Autonomy Work
Support Stress
.16**
.40** .20**
.19

-.36** Introjected
Regulation -.20** .13
.14* Ill
.16
-.50** Symptoms
External .27**
Regulation

.24

Amotivation

Figure 1. The path analysis results for the direct and indirect (mediating) relations among the perceived autonomy support, different types of teacher
motivation, and well-being indicators.

costs. Nonetheless, both the positive relations of introjec- externally regulated teachers tend to experience more
tion to job satisfaction and autonomy support suggest that stress in work. Further down the continuum of autonomy,
the meaning and role of introjection within Chinese and amotivation predicted both work stress and illness symp-
other Asian work contexts warrants further investigation. toms, indicating that teachers’ amotivation is associated
with poor vocational well-being.
The most interesting finding is about introjected
Differential predictive relations between work regulation. The path analysis found that it positively pre-
motivation and well-being dicted job satisfaction, but also illness symptoms. This
finding suggests that introjected regulation may have
The path analysis results showed that different types of both positive consequences and negative consequences
motivation have different consequences after controlling in terms of its psychological functioning. On one hand,
for other predictors. Intrinsic motivation seems to be the the people with higher introjected regulation are likely
most beneficial, demonstrating clear positive predictive to experience higher job satisfaction, but on the other
relations with job satisfaction and negative relations to hand, introjected regulation may at the same time bring
illness symptoms. That is, if employees are more intrin- ill health. This seems to be consistent with the classic
sically motivated, they are more likely to experience job study conducted by Ryan and Connell (1989) that found
satisfaction and have less symptoms of illness such as introjected regulation to be positively related to positive
sleeping problems, headaches and stomach upsets. Iden- coping strategies, effort and enjoyment but also to anxiety
tified regulation is also positively related with job satis- amplification and cognitive anxiety. Similarly, Koestner
faction. Yet in this model it was not associated with any et al. (1996) also found that introjection was positively
negative consequences (i.e., work stress and illness symp- related to both expected pleasant emotions and expected
toms) after controlling for intrinsic motivation and other unpleasant emotions. These findings suggest that intro-
types of motivation. This suggests that identified regu- jected regulation may have some special consequences
lation is not as protective as intrinsic motivation when in psychological functioning, different from those asso-
considering vocational ill-being. This was not unexpected ciated with external regulation (controlled) and identified
because intrinsic motivation is the most autonomous regulation (more autonomous) (see Ryan & Deci, 2000),
type of motivation in comparison to the other types of which differ in terms of the degrees of internalisation
motivation being investigated (e.g., Ryan & Connell, according to the theory. Introjected regulation involves the
1989; Vallerand, 1997) partial internalisation of external values; people may take
The results also support the negative roles played in the values but not fully accept it as their own. There-
by external regulation and amotivation in well-being. fore, it is plausible that such partial internalisation leads
Consistent with the predictions of SDT, external regu- to introjected regulation displaying some of the benefits
lation positively predicted work stress. This means that of identified regulation, as well as some of the negative

© 2014 International Union of Psychological Science


MOTIVATION AND WELL-BEING 9

consequences of external regulation. Practitioners should and well-being. In addition, although the results of Har-
hence be cautious when triggering introjected regulation. man’s one-factor test and the single-factor confirmatory
Even though it has positive relations with satisfaction factor analysis suggest that common method variance is
or pleasant emotion, the cost (the positive relations not of great concern, future studies may consider minimis-
with negative outcomes, e.g., ill symptoms, unpleasant ing this potential problem through procedural remedies,
emotion) should also be taken into consideration. such as obtaining measures of the predictor and criterion
In this sense, many SDT studies using controlled and variable from different sources, or conducting experimen-
autonomous classification of motivation have overlooked tal studies with manipulations of independent variables
the special functioning of different types of motivation, (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, 2003).
especially introjected regulation. The research on psy- Third, the current study measured a limited set of indi-
chological functioning of introjected regulation is still cators of well-being related to work (i.e., job satisfaction,
limited. Given the mixed consequences it may bring to work stress, illness symptoms). Future research may con-
individuals, further studies are needed to uncover the sider examining a wider range of indicators of well-being,
psychological functioning of introjected regulation. such as positive affect, mood.
Fourth, the current focused on psychological percep-
tions at a personal level. Thus the findings do not allow
The mediating roles of motivation
generalisation to the organisational level. Further stud-
The current study found support for the hypothesis that ies should attempt to measure organisational cultures and
motivation mediates the relations between perceived climates through the use of multi-level sampling and ana-
autonomy support and vocational well-being. Intrin- lytical techniques.
sic motivation, for example, significantly mediated the Fifth, the current study found that introjected regula-
relations between perceived autonomy support and job tion positively predicted both job satisfaction and illness
satisfaction as well as the relation between perceived symptoms. While this finding is consistent with Ng et al.
autonomy support and illness symptoms. These findings (2012) showing that introjected regulation may have both
highlight how the psychological climate created in organ- positive and negative impacts, we need to ask whether
isations (i.e., perceived autonomy support), is linked to such impacts are culture specific, and when and where
workers’ motivation, which in turn predicts their voca- introjected regulation may be adaptive or maladaptive. In
tional well-being, in addition to their engagement and essence, the underlying processes and outcomes related
productivity. From a theoretical perspective, the findings to introjected regulation need to be further studied.
support SDT by showing that intrinsic motivation and Sixth, the current study found that male employ-
identified regulation, the two most autonomous types of ees, who comprised less than a third of the total sam-
motivation, predict well-being in a vocational context, ple, showed higher introjected and external regulation
not only in terms of quantity of motivation, but more compared with female employees, and thus gender was
importantly, in terms of the quality of their influences in entered as a control variable in the primary analyses.
driving individual behaviour. Nonetheless, future studies might further examine gender
differences.
Seventh, although the current research findings support
Limitations and future directions the applicability of SDT within a Chinese organisa-
tional context, it is still unclear whether the model
Some limitations of the current study need to be con- proposed could be generalised to any other types of
sidered. First, the participants of the current study were contexts or whether any boundary conditions (e.g.,
teachers in two government schools in a medium sized individualism/collectivism, independent/interdependent
city in the Hebei province of China. Given the diverse self-construal) may moderate the results. For example,
geographical locations of the major cities within the huge Chirkov et al. (2003) have highlighted that autonomy
country of China, different regions and career diversities is largely orthogonal to both independence and indi-
may have differences in the modernization and globalisa- vidualism, and therefore more empirical studies are
tion processes, social economic development and open- needed to further clarify the relations between autonomy,
ness to Western cultures. Hence generalisation of the independence and individualism and how they may
results to other areas and other vocations in China and jointly (additively or interactively) relate to motivation,
elsewhere should be done with caution. Future studies well-being and other outcomes.
should replicate this model in other work settings.
Second, the study was cross-sectional in nature, and
thus our path models did not allow us to draw causal CONCLUSION
conclusions. Further studies may consider using a lon-
gitudinal design to examine the dynamic changes in the Our findings have several important implications for moti-
relations among perceived autonomy support, motivation vation in the workplace and well-being. With regard to

© 2014 International Union of Psychological Science


10 NIE ET AL.

theory, the findings support the application of SDT in the Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(8), 930–942.
organisational setting of a Chinese government school. In doi:10.1177/0146167201278002.
particular, the results highlight the importance of auton- Fernet, C., Guay, F., Senécal, C., & Austin, S. (2012). Pre-
omy support, even in the context of Chinese culture that is dicting intraindividual changes in teacher burnout: The
role of perceived school environment and motivational
often described as unsupportive of the individual’s auton-
factors. Teaching and Teacher Education, 28(4), 514–525.
omy in work. Furthermore, the current findings also sug-
doi:10.1016/j.tate.2011.11.013.
gest that the function of introjected regulation needs to be Fernet, C., Senécal, C., Guay, F., Marsh, H., & Dowson,
further considered and clarified, especially with respect to M. (2008). The work tasks motivation scale for teachers
its implications with different cultural contexts. (WTMST). Journal of Career Assessment, 16(2), 256–279.
With regard to practical implications, the core mes- doi:10.1177/1069072707305764.
sage from the current study is that work motivation plays Gagné, M., Ryan, R. M., & Bargmann, K. (2003). Auton-
an important role in employees’ well-being, with more omy support and need satisfaction in the motivation and
autonomous forms of motivation being more beneficial. well-being of gymnasts. Journal of Applied Sport Psychol-
Intrinsic motivation was especially associated with more ogy, 15(4), 372–390. doi:10.1080/714044203.
positive well-being (e.g., higher job satisfaction, lower Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., Anderson, R. E., & Tatham,
R. L. (2006). Multivariate data analysis (6th ed.). Upper
ill symptoms). In contrast, amotivation is the most mal-
Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.
adaptive motivation because it is related to higher work
Kins, E., Beyers, W., Soenens, B., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2009).
stress and more illness symptoms. In addition, from our Patterns of home leaving and subjective well-being in
findings, motivations variously mediated the relations emerging adulthood: The role of motivational processes
between autonomy support and well-being outcomes. and parental autonomy support. Developmental Psychology,
Strategies that support autonomy may be critical in foster- 45(5), 1416–1429. doi:10.1037/a0015580.
ing intrinsic motivation and identified regulation, which Kitayama, S., Snibbe, A. C., Markus, H. R., & Suzuki, T.
are forms of motivation most characterised by commit- (2004). Is there any “free” choice? Self and dissonance
ment and engagement, and employee well-being. in two cultures. Psychological Science, 15(8), 527–533.
doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00714.x.
Koestner, R., Losier, G. F., Vallerand, R. J., & Carducci, D.
Manuscript received February 2014 (1996). Identified and introjected forms of political inter-
Revised manuscript accepted October 2014 nalization: Extending self-determination theory. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(5), 1025–1036.
REFERENCES doi:10.1037/0022-3514.70.5.1025.
Liem, A. D., & Nie, Y. (2008). Values, achievement goals, and
Baard, P. P., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2004). Intrinsic individual-oriented and social-oriented achievement motiva-
need satisfaction: A motivational basis of perfor- tions among Chinese and Indonesian secondary school stu-
mance and well-being in two work settings. Journal dents. International Journal of Psychology, 43(5), 898–903.
of Applied Social Psychology, 34(10), 2045–2068. doi:10.1080/00207590701838097.
doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2004.tb02690.x. MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S.
Byrne, B. M. (2010). Structural equation modeling with Amos: G., & Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test
Basic concepts, applications, and programming (2nd ed.). mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psycholog-
New York, NY: Taylor and Francis Group. ical Methods, 7(1), 83. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.7.1.83.
Chirkov, V., & Ryan, R. M. (2001). Parent and teacher Markus, H. R., Kitayama, S., & Heiman, R. J. (1996). Culture
autonomy-support in Russian and U.S. Adolescents: and “basic” psychological principles. In E. T. Higgins & A.
Common effects on well-being and academic motivation. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32(5), 618–635. principles (pp. 857–913). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
doi:10.1177/0022022101032005006. Ng, J. Y., Ntoumanis, N., Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C., Deci, E.
Chirkov, V., Ryan, R. M., Kim, Y., & Kaplan, U. (2003). L., Ryan, R. M., Duda, J. L., & Williams, G. C. (2012).
Differentiating autonomy from individualism and indepen- Self-determination theory applied to health contexts: A
dence: A self-determination theory perspective on inter- meta-analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(4),
nalization of cultural orientations and well-being. Jour- 325–340. doi:10.1177/1745691612447309.
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(1), 97–110. Pelletier, L. G., Fortier, M. S., Vallerand, R. J., & Brière, N. M.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.1.97. (2001). Associations among perceived autonomy support,
Chua, S. N., Wong, N., & Koestner, R. (2014). Autonomy forms of self-regulation, and persistence: A prospec-
and controlling support are two sides of the same coin. tive study. Motivation and Emotion, 25(4), 279–306.
Personality and Individual Differences, 68(0), 48–52. doi:10.1023/a:1014805132406.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.04.008. Pettegrew, L. S., & Wolf, G. E. (1982). Validating measures
Deci, E. L., Ryan, R. M., Gagné, M., Leone, D. R., Usunov, J., of teacher stress. American Educational Research Journal,
& Kornazheva, B. P. (2001). Need satisfaction, motivation, 19(3), 373–396. doi:10.2307/1162720.
and well-being in the work organizations of a former Eastern Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J.-Y., & Podsakoff, N.
bloc country: A cross-cultural study of self-determination. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research:

© 2014 International Union of Psychological Science


MOTIVATION AND WELL-BEING 11

A critical review of the literature and recommended reme- Individualism and collectivism: Theory, methods, and
dies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(5), 879–903. application (pp. 85–119). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
doi:10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879. Taylor, I. M., & Ntoumanis, N. (2007). Teacher motivational
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resam- strategies and student self-determination in physical educa-
pling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects tion. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(4), 747–760.
in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, doi:10.1037/0022-0663.99.4.747.
40(3), 879–891. doi:10.3758/brm.40.3.879. Vallerand, R. J. (1997). Toward a hierarchical model of intrinsic
Richer, S. F., Blanchard, C., & Vallerand, R. J. (2002). A and extrinsic motivation. Advances in Experimental Social
motivational model of work turnover. Journal of Applied Psychology, 29, 271–360.
Social Psychology, 32(10), 2089–2113. Vansteenkiste, M., Lens, W., de Witte, H., & Feather, N. T.
Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of (2005). Understanding unemployed people’s job search
causality and internalization: Examining reasons for act- behaviour, unemployment experience and well-being:
ing in two domains. Journal of Personality and Social A comparison of expectancy-value theory and self-
Psychology, 57(5), 749–761. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.57. determination theory [Article]. British Journal of Social Psy-
5.749. chology, 44(2), 269–287. doi:10.1348/014466604x17641.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory Yamauchi, H., & Tanaka, K. (1998). Relations of autonomy,
and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social develop- self-referenced beliefs, and self-regulated learning among
ment, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55(1), 68–78. Japanese children. Psychological Reports, 82(3), 803–816.
doi:10.1037//0003-066X.55.1.68. Zhou, M., Ma, W. J., & Deci, E. L. (2009). The importance of
Schwartz, S. H. (1994). Beyond individualism/collectivism: autonomy for rural Chinese children’s motivation for learn-
New cultural dimensions of values. In H. C. T. U. ing. Learning and Individual Differences, 19(4), 492–498.
Kim, C. Kagitcibasi, S. C. Choi, & G. Yoon (Eds.), doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2009.05.003.

© 2014 International Union of Psychological Science

You might also like