Delima 2009

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Aquaculture Research, 2009, 41, 53^60 doi:10.1111/j.1365-2109.2009.02302.

Activity pattern of the marine shrimp Litopenaeus


vannamei (Boone 1931) in laboratory as a function of
different feeding frequencies

Patr|¤ cia Pereira de Lima, Cibele Soares Pontes & Maria de FaŁtima Arruda
Departamento de Fisiologia, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte, Natal, RN, Brazil

Correspondence: P P de Lima, Departamento de Fisiologia, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte, Caixa Postal 1511, Campus
UniversitaŁrio, 59078-970 Natal, RN, Brazil. E-mail: pattlyma@yahoo.com.br

Abstract has increased markedly after the introduction of the


species Litopenaeus vannamei (Boone 1931) in the
Management practices on most Brazilian shrimp
1980s (Bueno 1989). Its excellent adaptation to var-
farms have resulted in high expenses for producers
ious culture conditions, especially to temperature
and damage to the environment. Applied ethology
and water salinity, has made it the main cultivated
could provide information on the pattern of shrimp
shrimp species in the country (Wainberg & Ca“mara
activity, enabling more e⁄cient feeding frequencies.
1998).
Behavioural activities of the shrimp Litopenaeus
Despite the growth in activity, management (in-
vannamei were recorded during di¡erent feeding
cluding feeding management) on most farms is still
frequencies. The shrimp were kept in aquariums
carried out empirically. The feeding frequency used,
on a 12:12 h light/dark cycle. They were fed commer-
feeding times and the number of trays per hectare
cial ration three (at 6:00, 12:00 and 18:00 hours),
are established by the producer based on traditional
four (at 6:00, 10:00, 14:00 and 18:00 hours) or
practices that can result in unnecessary expenditure
seven times per day (at 6:00. 8:00, 10:00, 12:00, 14:00,
because it is likely that not all the ration o¡ered will
16:00 and 18:00 hours). We observed animals at
be consumed. As a result, an accumulation of feed
15 min 1 aquarium 1 time periods, recording feed-
could occur, causing water and soil deterioration in
ing, substrate exploration, swimming and inactivity.
the pond (Nunes, Goddard & Gesteira 1996). This
Feeding and exploration were the highest for shrimp
could also lead to eutrophization problems in the
fed three times per day, swimming was greatest when
pond itself, as well as in adjacent areas where the ef-
animals received four feedings, whereas inactivity
£uents will be discharged.
was higher for shrimp fed seven times per day. There
Given that feeding is part of the behavioural reper-
was greater food ingestion between 12:00 and 14:00
tory of any animal species and considering the im-
hours for animals fed three and four times per day,
portance of activity patterns over a 24-h period,
whereas swimming was high mainly at 18:00 hours
understanding these aspects is critical for developing
for shrimp fed three and seven times per day. The re-
suitable management policies on shrimp farms. This
sults indicate higher forage-related activities (ex-
can be achieved through greater knowledge of
ploration/ingestion) when feed was o¡ered three or
shrimp behaviour, because ethological studies are
four times, suggesting optimization in the pattern of
now widely being used to improve the cultures of dif-
shrimp activities based on these feeding frequencies.
ferent animal species (Wechsler, Fr˛hlich, Oester,
Oswald, Troxler, Weber & Schmid 1997; Sambraus
Keywords: applied ethology, shrimp, feeding fre-
1998; Millman, Duncan, Stau¡acher & Stookey
quency, Litopenaeus vannamei
2004; Ashley 2007). Furthermore there is a need for
more studies related to the feeding frequency to be
Introduction
adopted in shrimp culture that could enable the
Shrimp culture has been gaining prominence optimization of shrimp activity. An additional advan-
throughout the world. In Brazil, commercial activity tage could be lower feed loss to the environment and

r 2009 The Authors


Journal Compilation r 2009 Blackwell Publishing Ltd 53
Feed o¡er and activities pattern in L. vannamei P P de Lima et al. Aquaculture Research, 2009, 41, 53^60

consequently greater bene¢t to the producers in this paper is to study the activity patterns of the mar-
terms of sustainable culture. Greater knowledge of ac- ine shrimp L. vannamei as a function of di¡erent feed-
tivity patterns may lead to reduced arti¢cial feed costs ing frequencies.
(ration), which has accounted for the highest produc-
tion costs of shrimp farms (Sedgwick 1979; Velasco,
Material and methods
Lawrence & Castille 1999; Molina, Cadena & Orellana
2000; Smith, Burford,Tabrett, Irvin & Ward 2002). We used juvenile shrimp of L. vannamei species after 2
Part of the knowledge of shrimp activities has been months of culture and a mean weight of 8.04  1.5 g
acquired from studies quantifying activity versus in- obtained from a commercial shrimp farm in the state
activity of the animal through the use of photocells, of Rio Grande do Norte (NE of Brazil). The tempera-
without discriminating between the di¡erent activ- ture was maintained at 28  1 1C and salinity at
ities performed by these animals (Rodriguez & Naylor 34 g L 1 throughout the experiment.
1972; Hindley 1975; Reynolds & Casterlin 1979; Vance The experimental units consisted of glass aqua-
1992; Guerao & Ribera 1996). Ethological studies ap- riums (0.3  0.5  0.4 m) containing 30 L of sea-
plied to shrimp farming are rare (Primavera & Lebata water, a closed water recirculation system, constant
1995, 2000). With L. vannamei speci¢cally, some stu- aeration and continuous ¢ltration through a 3 cm
dies have been developed that show their daily activ- high substrate of gravel and crushed oyster shells.
ity pattern di¡erentiating each one (Pontes & Arruda Five animals were placed in each aquarium (41
2005a, b; Pontes, Arruda, Menezes & Lima 2006). shrimps m 2) although during the observations
This had led to greater knowledge of the behavioural only four of these animals were observed, owing to
repertory of this species. However, many questions the observation capacity of the researchers. In order
related to shrimp activity remain unanswered. to recognize each animal individually during the ob-
One important aspect is the study of the interfer- servations, we placed di¡erent-coloured silicone
ence of food in inducing other shrimp activities. rings on the ocular peduncle. The observations began
According to Nunes (2000), low concentrations of or- 7 days after the animals were placed in the aqua-
ganic composites in the water, such as amino acids, riums to enable them to acclimatize to the experi-
sugars and nucleotides in the extracts and meals pro- mental conditions.
duced from ¢sh, molluscs or crustaceans when added The shrimp were subjected to a 12:12 hour light^
to arti¢cial feed, function as chemical attractants for dark cycle, with behavioural observations performed
the shrimp stimulating foraging activity in these an- from 6:00 to 18:00 hours. Illumination in the light
imals. Accordingly, in addition to arti¢cial feed inges- phase was provided by 12 timer-controlled white
tion, substrate exploration, swimming and inactivity £uorescent light bulbs (32 W) distributed across the
seem to be in£uenced by the presence of food in the laboratory ceiling. The last observation (18:00 hours)
surrounding environment. was performed under individual aquarium illumina-
A number of studies using di¡erent feeding fre- tion, consisting of one red incandescent light (15 W)
quencies (Sedgwick 1979; Robertson, Lawrence & (Rodriguez & Naylor 1972; Hindley 1975). The white
Castille 1993; Josekutty & Jose 1996; Velasco et al. £uorescent lights generated a mean luminosity of
1999; Smith et al. 2002; Tacon, Cody, Conquest, Diva- 180 lx for each aquarium while the red incandescent
karan, Forster & Decamp 2002; Carvalho & Nunes light provided a mean luminosity of 1lx.
2006) do not consider the in£uence of arti¢cial feed During the entire experiment, the shrimp were fed
on the animal’s response to this stimulus or on the an amount equivalent to 10% of the biomass of each
pattern of other activities. It is important to point aquarium using pelleted shrimp ration with 35%
s
out that in the natural environment, feeding beha- crude protein (Camaronina 35, Purina , Agribrands
viour is related to the search for this resource during do Brasil S. A., Sao Paulo, Brazil), according to the
the performance of other activities. treatment adopted: three, four or seven daily por-
Moreover, other factors such as light intensity, tions. The same amount of ration was o¡ered inde-
water and soil quality of the ponds, availability of nat- pendent of the feed frequency adopted; only the
ural food, time of day, larval stage and shrimp size number of daily feedings varied. The ration was
may in£uence arti¢cial feed consumption and conse- o¡ered on transparent acrylic trays (4  3  2 cm),
quently the other activities performed by these ani- left for 1h and removed at the end of this period.
mals (Molina-Poveda; Escobar; Gamboa-Delgado; The animals were subjected to the following feed-
Cadena; Orellana & Pina 2002). Thus, the purpose of ing frequencies: three feedings per day every 6 h

r 2009 The Authors


54 Journal Compilation r 2009 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Aquaculture Research, 41, 53^60
Aquaculture Research, 2009, 41, 53^60 Feed o¡er and activities pattern in L. vannamei P P de Lima et al.

(6:00, 12:00 and 18:00 hours); four feedings per day The shrimp that were fed three times a day were ob-
every 4 h (6:00, 10:00, 14:00 and 18:00 hours); or se- served ingesting arti¢cial feed more frequently than
ven feedings per day every 2 h (6:00, 8:00, 10:00, those that received four or seven feedings (Fig. 1a).
12:00, 14:00, 16:00 and 18:00 hours). Independent of As was the case with feeding, substrate explora-
the treatment adopted, the ¢rst feeding was per- tion behaviour showed signi¢cant di¡erences
formed immediately after the onset of the light phase between all feeding frequencies used [Kruskal^
(6:00 hours) and the last after the onset of the dark Wallis, H (2; 8570) 5 380.974; Po0.05], with greater
phase (18:00 hours). exploratory activity observed in the animals fed three
The observations of each aquarium followed the times per day (Fig. 1b).
feeding regime adopted, starting immediately after Swimming showed di¡erences as a function of the
introduction of the ration and lasting 15 min, consid- three feeding frequencies adopted [Kruskal^Wallis,
ering the following behavioural activities: (a) feeding H (2; 8570) 5 82.314; Po0.05], the highest levels
^ introduction of arti¢cial feed (ration) in the oral cav- being found in shrimp that received four daily feed-
ity with subsequent ingestion; (b) exploration of the ings (Fig.1c).
substrate ^ continuous insertion and removal of che- There were also di¡erences in inactivity as a func-
lated pereiopods with the cephalothorax inclined tion of the three feeding frequencies [Kruskal^Wallis,
slightly downward; (c) swimming ^ continuous verti- H (2; 8570) 5 394.235; Po0.05], with higher inactiv-
cal or horizontal displacement or suspension in the ity recorded in the animals fed seven times a day than
water column through movement of the pleopods; those fed three or four times a day (Fig. 1d).
and (d) inactivity ^ stationary with or without loco-
motor appendage movement. The behaviours were
recorded using the instantaneous focal method every
Behavioural activities as a function of feeding
60 s (Martin & Bateson 2007).
time and di¡erent feed frequencies: feeding,
A total of seven replicate tanks were applied for
substrate exploration, swimming and
each treatment; the duration of each repetition was
inactivity.
33 consecutive days, with four weekly observations.
The experiment included 84 animals and 490 obser- There were di¡erences in feeding according to feed-
vation hours. Given the non-adherence of the data to ing times for the animals fed three (Friedman, w2
normal distribution and the heterogeneity of the fac- (2;600) 5 32.077; Po0.05), four (w2 (3;600) 5 90.774;
tor variances shown by the Shapiro^Wilks and Po0.05) or seven times a day (w2 (6;600) 5 74.912;
Levene tests, respectively, we used non-parametric Po0.05). The shrimp that received three daily feed-
statistical analysis. To analyse behavioural activities ings consumed the most at 12:00 hours; those that
as a function of feeding frequency, we used the Krus- received four feedings ingested feed mainly at 14:00
kal^Wallis test while analysis of activities at di¡erent hours. For the shrimp receiving seven daily feedings,
feeding times was performed using the Friedman arti¢cial feed was ingested more frequently at 8:00 up
test. For signi¢cant di¡erences, we applied the post to 14:00 hours (Fig. 2).
hoc Mann^Whitney U-test (activity versus feeding Exploratory activity as a function of feeding times
frequency) or the Wilcoxon test (activities versus feed- showed no di¡erence for the animals fed three times
ing times). The signi¢cance level was set at 5%, and a day [Friedman, w2 (2;600) 5 1.247; P40.05]. How-
the results in graph form were based on median va- ever, there were di¡erences for the animals that re-
lues and inter-quartile range (75^25%). Di¡erent let- ceived four (w2 (3;600) 5 59.740; Po0.05) or seven
ters indicate a statistically signi¢cant di¡erence. feedings (w2 (6;600) 5 86.983; Po0.05). Exploration
activity of the shrimp fed four times a day was high
at 6:00, 10:00 and 14:00 hours, decreasing only dur-
Results ing the last hour. The shrimp that were fed seven
times a day displayed greater exploratory activity of
Behavioural activities as a function of feeding
the 8:00 up to 14:00 hours (Fig. 3).
frequency: feeding, substrate exploration,
The shrimp showed di¡erences in swimming when
swimming and inactivity
they were fed three (Friedman, w2 (2;600) 5 20.742;
We observed signi¢cant di¡erences in feeding as a Po0.05) or seven times a day (w2 (6;600) 5 54.206;
function of the three di¡erent feeding frequencies Po0.05). When receiving three feedings, the shrimp
[Kruskal^Wallis, H (2; 8570) 5 312.043; Po0.05]. swam more at 12:00 and 18:00 hours. The animals

r 2009 The Authors


Journal Compilation r 2009 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Aquaculture Research, 41, 53^60 55
Feed o¡er and activities pattern in L. vannamei P P de Lima et al. Aquaculture Research, 2009, 41, 53^60

(a) (b)
Median 25%-75% Min-Max Median 25%-75% Min-Max
16 16

Substrate Exploration
13 13
(episodes/15 min.)

(episodes/15 min.)
10 10
Feeding

a b
7 7
c
4 a 4
b
c 1
1
–2 –2
3 4 7 3 4 7
Feeding frequency Feeding frequency

(c) Median 25%-75% Min-Max


(d) Median 25%-75% Min-Max
16 16
13 13 b
(episodes /15 min.)

(episodes /15 min.)


a
Swimming

10 10
Inactivity
a
7 7
b
4 a 4
c
1 1
–2 –2
3 4 7 3 4 7
Feeding frequency Feeding frequency

Figure 1 Behavioural activities: feeding (a), substrate exploration (b), swimming (c) and inactivity (d) of the shrimp
Litopenaeus vannamei (Boone 1931) as a function of feeding frequency (three, four or seven times per day). Di¡erent letters
indicate a signi¢cant di¡erence at a level of 5%.

(a) (b) (c)


Median 25%-75% Min-Max Median 25% -75% Min-Max Median 25% -75% Min-Max
16 16 16
(episodes/15 min.)

13 13 13
Feeding

10 10 10
7 b 7 7
a b
4 c 4 a a 4 a b b b
c b c d
1 1 1
–2 –2 –2
6 12 18 6 10 14 18 6 8 10 12 14 16 18
Hours Hours Hours

Figure 2 Feeding of the shrimp Litopenaeus vannamei (Boone 1931) as a function of feeding times and di¡erent frequen-
cies: (a) three times per day 5 6:00,12:00 and18:00 hours; (b) four times per day 5 6:00,10:00,14:00 and18:00 hours; and
(c) seven times per day 5 6:00, 8:00, 10:00, 12:00, 14:00, 16:00 and 18:00 hours. Di¡erent letters indicate a signi¢cant dif-
ference at a level of 5%.

that received seven daily feedings also showed more mals fed four times were more inactive at 18:00
swimming activity at 18:00 hours. There were no sig- hours. The shrimp that received seven feedings were
ni¢cant di¡erences in the animals fed four times a day more inactive at 6:00 and 16:00 hours (Fig. 5).
(w2 (3;600) 5 6.198; P40.05) (Fig. 4).
There were di¡erences in the inactivity of the ani-
mals when they received three (Friedman, w2
Discussion
(2;600) 5 34.585; Po0.05), four (w2 (3;600) 5 59.352;
Po0.05) or seven feedings (w2 (6;600) 5 75.338; The behaviours of feeding, substrate exploration,
Po0.05). When fed three times a day, the shrimp swimming and inactivity varied as a function of the
were more inactive at 6:00 and 18:00 hours; the ani- di¡erent feeding frequencies.

r 2009 The Authors


56 Journal Compilation r 2009 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Aquaculture Research, 41, 53^60
Aquaculture Research, 2009, 41, 53^60 Feed o¡er and activities pattern in L. vannamei P P de Lima et al.

(a) (b) (c)

Substrate exploration
Median 25%-75% Min-Max Median 25% -75% Min-Max Median 25% -75% Min-Max
16 16 16

(episodes /15 min.)


13 13 13
10 10 a a a 10
b b b b b
7 7 7 c a
a
4 4 4
1 1 1
–2 –2 –2
6 12 18 6 10 14 18 6 8 10 12 14 16 18
Hours Hours Hours

Figure 3 Substrate exploration of the shrimp Litopenaeus vannamei (Boone 1931) as a function of feeding times and dif-
ferent frequencies: (a) three times per day 5 6:00, 12:00 and 18:00 hours; (b) four times per day 5 6:00, 10:00, 14:00 and
18:00 hours; and (c) seven times per day 5 6:00,8:00,10:00,12:00,14:00,16:00 and18:00 hours. Di¡erent letters indicate a
signi¢cant di¡erence at a level of 5%.

(a) (b) (c)


Median 25%-75% Min-Max Median 25% -75% Min-Max Median 25% -75% Min-Max
16 16 16
(episodes/15 min.)

13 13 13
Swimming

10 10 10
7 7 7
b b d e
4 a 4 4 a ab cd bcd ac
1 1 1
–2 –2 –2
6 12 18 6 10 14 18 6 8 10 12 14 16 18
Hours Hours Hours

Figure 4 Swimming activity of the shrimp Litopenaeus vannamei (Boone1931) as a function of feeding times and di¡erent
frequencies: (a) three times per day 5 6:00, 12:00 and 18:00 hours; (b) four times per day 5 6:00, 10:00, 14:00 and 18:00
hours; and (c) seven times per day 5 6:00, 8:00, 10:00, 12:00, 14:00, 16:00 and 18:00 hours. Di¡erent letters indicate a sig-
ni¢cant di¡erence at a level of 5%.

(a) Median 25%-75% Min-Max (b) Median 25% -75% Min-Max (c) Median 25% -75% Min-Max
16 a
16 16 b b b bd a bc
(episodes/15 min.)

13 13 c
a b 13
a a
Inactivity

10 b 10 a 10
7 7 7
4 4 4
1 1 1
–2 –2 –2
6 12 18 6 10 14 18 6 8 10 12 14 16 18
Hours Hours Hours

Figure 5 Inactivity of the shrimp Litopenaeus vannamei (Boone1931) as a function of feeding times and di¡erent frequen-
cies: (a) three times per day 5 6:00,12:00 and18:00 hours; (b) four times per day 5 6:00,10:00,14:00 and18:00 hours; and
(c) seven times per day 5 6:00, 8:00, 10:00, 12:00, 14:00, 16:00 and 18:00 hours. Di¡erent letters indicate a signi¢cant dif-
ference at a level of 5%.

The ingestion of arti¢cial feed was the highest in Corroborating our results, Smith et al. (2002)
the shrimp fed three times a day, indicating greater assessed the e¡ect of feeding frequency on the
food consumption by the animals when feeding is growth of the black tiger shrimp Penaeus monodon
more evenly spaced throughout the day. This re- (Fabricius 1798) when they were fed at di¡erent
sponse could be more advantageous to the shrimp frequencies (three, four, ¢ve or six times per day),
producer in cost^bene¢t terms. and found no signi¢cant di¡erences in growth, food

r 2009 The Authors


Journal Compilation r 2009 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Aquaculture Research, 41, 53^60 57
Feed o¡er and activities pattern in L. vannamei P P de Lima et al. Aquaculture Research, 2009, 41, 53^60

conversion and animal survival. Based on these re- Exploratory activity as well as arti¢cial feed inges-
sults, the authors recommend that once a nutrition- tion showed higher values for the shrimp fed three
ally suitable feed is used, frequencies of more than times a day. This may be explained by the direct rela-
three times a day provide no additional bene¢t to the tion between these two behaviours because when
animal. searching for food in general, exploration is an im-
Carvalho & Nunes (2006) pointed out that the portant component. Exploration occurred through-
number of feedings adopted on shrimp farms varied out the day and the animals fed four or seven times
as a function of the cultivation method and manage- daily showed a predominance of exploratory activity
ment system used. These authors subjected L. vanna- during the light phase. The lowest activity was re-
mei to di¡erent feeding frequencies (two, three, four, corded at 18:00 hours, in contrast to the animals that
¢ve or six times per day) in a grow-out pond received three daily feedings, for which no di¡er-
and found that when feed rations were adjusted ences were observed in relation to time.
weekly only, using estimated shrimp biomass as the Pontes et al. (2006), observing L. vannamei juve-
criterion, more than two daily feedings were not niles fed once a day at random times, showed that
advantageous. Only in cases where feeding occurs substrate exploration occurred both in the light and
exclusively in trays, or when low nutritional and phy- in the dark phase, suggesting that the search for food
sical quality feeds are used, should multiple feedings occurs in both phases. The animals reached higher
be considered as a strategy for reducing the food con- peaks 7 h after the onset of the light phase, although
version rate. our data point to increased exploration up to 8 h after
With respect to preferred feeding times at di¡erent the lights are turned on, after which time exploratory
feeding frequencies, there was greater ingestion activity decreases.
between 12:00 and 14:00 hours for the animals that Swimming was more frequent among the shrimp
received three or four daily feedings. Animals fed that received four feedings. This behaviour showed
seven times a day consumed more feed at 8:00 up high values, independent of the feeding frequency
to 14:00 hours. This shows that feeding of shrimp, in adopted (three, four or seven times) at 18:00 hours,
addition to undergoing alterations in the number of the only observation time in the dark phase. Pontes
feedings, is also in£uenced by the times at which the et al. (2006), studying the daily behavioural pattern
feed is made available. of L. vannamei over a 24-h period, found that they
Molina et al. (2000) obtained similar results swam predominantly in the dark phase.
when they tested L. vannamei kept in aquariums and With respect to inactivity, it was observed that the
o¡ered arti¢cial feed twice a day at di¡erent feeding shrimp were more inactive when they were fed seven
times (8:00 and 16:00, 10:00 and 18:00, 12:00 and times a day. The shrimp that received three or seven
20:00 or 14:00 and 22:00 hours). They found that feeds remained stationary at 6:00 hours, becoming
the shrimp fed at 12:00 or 14:00 hours consumed more active over the course of the day until they once
more feed and had a higher ¢nal biomass, gradually again registered high values for this behaviour at the
ingesting less food until 24:00 hours. They also end of the day. Owing to large energy expenditures
observed that the peak of amylase, lipase and pro- during swimming, the shrimp seem to alternate be-
tease enzymes occurred at 14:00 hours, independent tween moments of activity and inactivity. This may
of feeding time. Thus, the authors suggested feeding be due to the relation between activity/inactivity
the shrimp larger amounts at12:00 hours, the time at and oxygen consumption.
which greater food ingestion was observed and fol- Dall (1986) related shrimp activity with oxygen
lowed by higher digestive enzyme activity. consumption in his study performed with Penaeus es-
A lower amount of arti¢cial feed was consumed at culentus (Haswell 1879) to measure the metabolic
18:00 hours independent of the feeding frequency routine of shrimp through oxygen consumption. He
used (three, four or seven times per day). Because found small £uctuations in O2 consumption during
18:00 hours was the only dark phase time recorded the day when the shrimp were more inactive. At
in our study, our results agree with those obtained night, when they become more active, oxygen con-
by Pontes & Arruda (2005a). These authors o¡ered ju- sumption triples.
venile L. vannamei ration four times a day (8:00, Primavera & Lebata (2000), based on 25 hourly
16:00, 20:00 and 4:00 hours) and found that feeding laboratory observations, also found higher activity
occurred more intensely during the light phase than levels between Metapenaeus ensis (De Haan 1844),
it did in the dark phase. Penaeus latisulcatus (Kishinouye1896) and Fennerope-

r 2009 The Authors


58 Journal Compilation r 2009 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Aquaculture Research, 41, 53^60
Aquaculture Research, 2009, 41, 53^60 Feed o¡er and activities pattern in L. vannamei P P de Lima et al.

naeus merguiensis (De Man 1888) juveniles during the Dall W. (1986) Estimation of routine metabolic rate in a
dark phase. M. ensis and P. latisulcatus juveniles dis- penaeid prawn, Penaeus esculentus Haswell. Journal of
played a pattern of burrowing themselves into the Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 96, 57^74.
substrate during the day and emerging at night. Diur- Guerao G. & Ribera C. (1996) Locomotor activity patterns
nal burrowing was greater in Metapenaeus than in and feeding habits in the prawn Palaemon serratus (Pen-
nant 1777) (Decapoda, Palaemonidae) in the Alfacs Bay,
Penaeus while activities above the substrate (clean-
Elbro Delta, Spain. Crustaceana 69, 101^112.
ing, feeding, sheltering, inactivity, swimming and
Hindley J.P.R. (1975) E¡ects of endogenous and some exo-
crawling) were more frequent in Penaeus than in Me- genous factors on the activity of juvenile banana prawn
tapenaeus species. This pattern was less pronounced Penaeus merguiensis. Marine Biology 29, 01^08.
in F. merguiensis, which is equally active in the light Josekutty P.A. & Jose S. (1996) Optimum ration size and feed-
and dark phases. ing frequency for rearing of Penaeus monodon (Fabricius).
Thus, it can be a⁄rmed that feeding frequency FisheriesTechnological Society (India) 33, 16^20.
in£uenced the pattern of shrimp activities. The Martin P. & Bateson P. (eds.) (2007) The recording medium.
highest feeding and exploration levels resulted from In: Measuring Behaviour: An Introductory Guide, 3rd edn,
three feedings per day, followed by four feedings. pp. 62–71. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge,
UK.
When fed seven times a day, the animals explored less
Molina C., Cadena E. & Orellana F. (2000) Alimentac|¤ on
and were more inactive. Given that exploration is
de camarones en relac|¤ on a la actividad enzimaŁtica como
an activity related to the search for food, the less
una repuesta natural al ritmo circadiano y ciclo de muda.
frequent feedings (three or four times a day in the In: Proceedings of the V Simposium Internacional de
light phase) seem to optimize activity patterns Nutric|¤ on Acu|¤ cola (ed. by L.E. Cruz-Suaréz, D. Ricki-
directed towards both searching for and consuming Marie, M. Tapia-Salazár, M.A. Olvera-Novoa & R.
food, suggesting that the shrimp should receive three Civera Cerecedo), pp. 358–380. Mérida, Yucatán,
daily feedings. Three feedings per day is bene¢cial México.
both to the animal and to the producer, who can Molina-Poveda C., EscobarV., Gamboa-Delgado J., Cadena E.,
improve management and achieve better business Orellana F. & Pina P. (2002) Estrategia de alimentacio¤n de
results. acuerdo a la demanda ¢siolo¤gica del juvenil Litopenaeus
vannamei (Boone). In: Avances en Nutricio¤n Acu|¤ cola VI.
Memorias del VI Simposium Internacional de Nutricio¤n
Acu|¤ cola. 3 al 6 de Septiembre del 2002 (ed. by L.E. Cruz-
Acknowledgments Suárez, D. Ricque-Marie, M. Tapia-Salazar, M.G.
Gaxiola-Cortés & N. Simoes), pp. 98–113. Cancún,
We would like to thank the Conselho Nacional de
Quintana Roo, México.
Desenvolvimento Cient|¤ ¢co e Tecnolo¤gico (CNPq),
Millman S.T., Duncan I.J.H., Stau¡acher M. & Stookey J.M.
Coordenacao de Aperfeicoamento de Pessoal de N|¤ vel (2004) The impact of applied ethologists and the Interna-
Superior (Capes), the Papeba shrimp farm and the tional Society for Applied Ethology in improving animal
Department of Physiology of the Universidade Feder- welfare. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 86, 299^311.
al do Rio Grande do Norte for their support in devel- Nunes A.J.P. (2000) Manual Purina de Alimentac ao para Ca-
oping this study. maroes Marinhos. Agribrands Purina do Brasil Ltda., Paul
|¤ nia, Brazil, 40pp.
Nunes A.J.P., Goddard S. & Gesteira T.C.V. (1996) Feeding ac-
tivity patterns of the Southern brown shrimp Penaeus
References
subtilis under semi-intensive culture in NE Brazil. Aqua-
Ashley P.J. (2007) Fish welfare: Current issues in aquacul- culture 144, 371^386.
ture. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 104,199^235. Pontes C.S. & Arruda M.F. (2005a) Comportamento de Lito-
Bueno S.L.S. (1989) Te¤ cnicas, procedimentos e manejo para a penaeus vannamei (Boone) (Crustacea, Decapoda, Penaei-
produc ao de po¤s-larvas de camaroes pene|¤ deos: experieŒncia dae) em funcao da oferta do alimento arti¢cial nas fases
vivida pela Maricultura da Bahia S.A. Bras|¤ lia. Comissao clara e escura do per|¤ odo de 24 horas. Revista Brasileira
Interministerial para os Recursos do Mar (CIRM), de Zoologia 22, 648^652.
Bras|¤ lia, Brasil,107pp. Pontes C.S. & Arruda M.F. (2005b) Acesso ao alimento arti¢-
Carvalho E.A. & Nunes A.J.P. (2006) E¡ects of feeding fre- cial e enchimento do trato digestivo de juvenis do camar-
quency on feed leaching loss and grow-out patterns of ao marinho Litopenaeus vannamei (Boone) (Crustacea,
the white shrimp Litopenaeus vannamei fed under a diur- Decapoda, Penaeidae) durante as fases clara e escura do
nal feeding regime in pond enclosures. Aquaculture 252, per|¤ odo de 24 horas. Revista Brasileira de Zoologia 22,
494^502. 1039^1043.

r 2009 The Authors


Journal Compilation r 2009 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Aquaculture Research, 41, 53^60 59
Feed o¡er and activities pattern in L. vannamei P P de Lima et al. Aquaculture Research, 2009, 41, 53^60

Pontes C.S., Arruda M.F., Menezes A.A.L. & Lima P.P. (2006) Smith D.M., Burford M.A., Tabrett S.J., Irvin S.J. & Ward L.
Daily activity pattern of the marine shrimp Litopenaeus (2002) The e¡ect of feeding frequency on water quality
vannamei (Boone 1931) juveniles under laboratory condi- and growth of the black tiger shrimp (Penaeus monodon).
tions. Aquaculture Research 37,1001^1006. Aquaculture 207,125^136.
Primavera J.H. & Lebata J. (1995) Diel activity patterns in Tacon A.J.G., Cody J.J., Conquest L.D., Divakaran S., Forster
Metapenaeus and Penaeus juveniles. Hydrobiologia 295, I.P. & Decamp O.E. (2002) E¡ect of culture system on the
295^302. nutrition and growth performance of Paci¢c White
Primavera J.H. & Lebata M.J.H.L. (2000) Size and diel di¡er- shrimp Litopenaeus vannamei (Boone) fed di¡erent diets.
ences in activity patterns of Metapenaeus ensis, Penaeus la- Aquaculture Nutrition 8, 121^137.
tisulcatus and P. merguiensis. Marine and Freshwater Vance D.J. (1992) Activity patterns of juvenile penaeid
Behaviour Physiology 33, 173^185. prawns in response to arti¢cial tidal and day^night cy-
ReynoldsW.W. & Casterlin M.E. (1979) Diel activity of the pink cles: a comparison of three species. Marine Ecology Pro-
shrimp Penaeus duorarum. Hydrobiologia 66, 223^226. gress Series 87, 215^226.
Robertson L., Lawrence A.L. & Castille F.L. (1993) E¡ect of Velasco M., Lawrence A.L. & Castille F.L. (1999) E¡ect of var-
feeding frequency and feeding time on growth of Penaeus iations in daily feeding frequency and ration size on
vannamei (Boone). Aquaculture and Fisheries Management growth of shrimp, Litopenaeus vannamei (Boone), in
24,1^6. zero-water exchange culture tanks. Aquaculture 179,
Rodriguez G. & Naylor E. (1972) Behavioural rhythms in littor- 141^148.
al prawns. Journal of Marine BiologyAssociation 52, 81^95. Wainberg A.A. & Ca“mara M.R. (1998) Brazilian shrimp
Sambraus H.H. (1998) Applied ethology ^ it’s task and limits farming ^ It’s growing, but is it sustainable? World Aqua-
in veterinary practice. Applied Animal Behaviour Science culture 29, 26^30.
59, 39^48. Wechsler B., Fr˛hlich E., Oester H., Oswald T.,Troxler J.,We-
Sedgwick R.W. (1979) E¡ect of ration size and feeding ber R. & Schmid H. (1997) The contribution of applied
frequency on the growth and food conversion of juvenile ethology in judging animal welfare in farm animal hous-
Penaeus merguiensis De Man. Aquaculture 16, 279^298. ing systems. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 53, 33^43.

r 2009 The Authors


60 Journal Compilation r 2009 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Aquaculture Research, 41, 53^60

You might also like