Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 16

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/265412921

Athlete perceptions of intra-group conflict in sport teams

Article  in  International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology · September 2014


DOI: 10.53841/bpssepr.2014.10.3.4

CITATIONS READS

39 4,062

3 authors:

Kyle F Paradis Albert Carron


Ulster University The University of Western Ontario
64 PUBLICATIONS   406 CITATIONS    79 PUBLICATIONS   6,684 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Luc J Martin
Queen's University
124 PUBLICATIONS   1,221 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Intricacies of the friendship-cohesion relationship View project

Athletes' positive experiences with youth sport officials View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Kyle F Paradis on 08 September 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Original Article
Athlete perceptions of intra-group
conflict in sport teams
Kyle F. Paradis, Albert V. Carron & Luc J. Martin

The study of conflict has been of primary interest in various fields such as organisational psychology for
decades (e.g. Barki & Hartwick, 2004). In sport psychology, however, conflict research has been almost non-
existent (Lavoi, 2007) with few exceptions (e.g. Holt et al., 2012; Sullivan & Feltz, 2001). The importance
of understanding conflict in sport and in groups, however, has been acknowledged because it has potentially
serious implications for group outcomes (Lavoi, 2007). The present study investigated competitive sport
athletes’ perceptions of intra-group conflict in sport. Ten intercollegiate athletes: (N=5 males, N=5 females;
Mage=25.00, SD=2.87) participated in semi-structured interviews. Athletes perceived the nature of conflict
to manifest itself in several ways including: (a) disagreements; (b) negative emotions; and (c)
interference/antagonistic behaviors. In addition, conflict episodes were perceived to arise in task and social
situations. The findings are discussed in terms of their contributions to current perspectives on intra-group
conflict in sport.
Keywords: sport psychology; group dynamics; social psychology; conflict.

G
ROUP DYNAMICS has been to emerge (Lavoi, 2007). This is surprising
described as ‘a field of inquiry dedi- considering the presence of conflict is
cated to advancing knowledge about believed to be inevitable in any group
the nature of groups, the laws of their devel- (Robbins & Judge, 2010). Lavoi (2007) also
opment, and their inter-relations with indi- noted that conflict is an inevitable part of life
viduals, other groups, and larger institutions’ and relationships, and, thus, is an important
(Cartwright & Zander, 1968, p.19). In the phenomenon to understand in sport teams.
field of sport psychology, the study of group To date, certain research projects have
dynamics has increased our understanding referenced conflict in sport – including
with regard to the nature and measurement studies of athlete participation and involve-
of various group constructs including: cohe- ment in sport teams (Holt & Sparkes, 2001;
sion (e.g. Carron, Widmeyer & Brawley, Holt et al., 2008), friendships in youth sport
1985); role ambiguity (e.g. Eys et al., 2005); (Weiss & Smith, 1999; Weiss, Smith &
the coach-athlete relationship (e.g. Jowett & Theebom, 1996), motivational climate
Ntoumanis, 2004); coaching leadership (e.g. (Ntoumanis & Vazou, 2005; Vazou,
Chelladurai & Saleh, 1980); athlete (peer) Ntoumanis & Duda, 2005) and the coach-
leadership (e.g. Loughead, Hardy & Eys, athlete relationship (Jowett, 2003). The
2006); athlete satisfaction (e.g. Riemer & forms of conflict in these studies, however,
Chelladurai, 1998); collective efficacy (e.g. were assessed to gain a better understanding
Short, Sullivan & Feltz, 2005); and team attri- of other phenomena (i.e. motivational
butions (e.g. Greenlees et al., 2005). One climate, youth sport settings, adherence).
construct in sport, however, that has gener- One study that directly assessed the pres-
ated relatively minimal research attention is ence of conflict in sport examined the
intra-group conflict. For example, in a conflict-cohesion relationship in ice hockey
search of subject indices in various sport teams (Sullivan & Feltz, 2001). The assess-
psychology textbooks, the term conflict failed ment involved the Group Environment

4 Sport & Exercise Psychology Review, Vol. 10 No. 3


© The British Psychological Society 2014
Perceptions of conflict

Questionnaire (cohesion; Carron et al., culture among others. The utility of the find-
1985) and an untitled ‘conflict style ings from the body of research emanating
measure’ (conflict; Canary, Cunningham & from organisational psychology however is
Cody, 1988) which consisted of seven dimen- limited for two reasons.
sions labelled integrative tactics, topic shifting, One reason is the variety of constitutive
personal criticism, showing anger, personal definitions (and by extension, operational
sarcasm, semantic focus, and denial. The results definitions) used as the bases for investiga-
indicated topic shifting (a ‘negative conflict tions. In one perspective for example,
style’) was negatively related to task cohesion conflict is represented by interference
and social cohesion whereas integrative tactics behaviour reflected by incompatibilities and
(a ‘positive conflict style’) was positively interpersonal interference or obstruction
related to social cohesion. (e.g. Alper, Tjosvold & Law, 2000). Charac-
A second study that examined conflict in teristic of this perspective is the definition of
sport focused on female intercollegiate conflict by Wall and Callister (1995) as
athletes’ perceptions of the sources of team- ‘a process by which one party perceives that
mate conflicts (Holt, Knight & Zukiwski, its interests are being opposed or negatively
2012). Through semi-structured interviews, affected by another party’ (p.517). Wilmot
the results indicated the presence of conflict and Hocker (2001) also defined conflict as
relating to performance (i.e. task) and rela- ‘an expressed struggle between at least two
tionships (i.e. social). In addition, with persons who perceive incompatible goals,
regard to conflict resolution, the partici- scarce resources, and interference from
pants suggested that conducting team others in achieving their goals’ (p.41).
building early in the season, addressing Although these definitions are useful, this
conflict early, having mediators, and holding perspective in and of itself is not sufficient to
structured interviews could help to manage define conflict. Individuals may attempt to
conflict. prevent each other from attaining their goals
Despite the previous two studies, the lack (e.g. two athletes that are competing for the
of research attention directed towards intra- same position on a team) but may not be in
group conflict in sport is particularly disagreement or hold negative feelings
disheartening when contrasted with the towards each other.
extensive research conducted in other fields In another perspective, conflict has been
of inquiry. For example, in organisational defined by the existence of negative emotions
psychology, there has been more than 70 (e.g. tension, jealousy, anxiety, frustration,
years of research on conflict (Barki & anger, friction, hostility) in task and social
Hartwick, 2004). At first glance, it might situations (e.g. Bodtker & Jameson, 2001;
seem reasonable to assume that research Jehn, 1994). Characteristic of this perspective
assessing work groups can inform sport is the definition advanced by Pelled, Eisen-
psychologists about the nature and corre- hardt and Xin (1999): ‘a condition in which
lates of conflict in sport teams. Work groups group members have interpersonal clashes
and sport teams do possess some strong simi- characterised by anger, frustration, and other
larities (Barker, Rossi & Puhse, 2010). For negative feelings’ (p.2). While conflict will
example, outcomes such as individual and undoubtedly spurn negative emotions in
group productivity, member satisfaction, and individuals, standing alone, this perspective
so on are of principal concern in work does not describe conflict entirely. For
groups and sport teams. Also, there is a example, the persons involved may dislike
common interest in group dynamics each other based on personality, work ethic,
constructs that might influence these personal attributes, or past experiences, but
outcomes – cohesion, leadership, role clarity, may not be in any specific disagreement or
role acceptance, role satisfaction, and group interference with one another.

Sport & Exercise Psychology Review, Vol. 10 No. 3 5


Kyle F. Paradis, Albert V. Carron, & Luc J. Martin

From a third and perhaps most common individual awareness of it, nor affective
perspective, conflict has been viewed as states, nor its over manifestations, nor its
disagreement. Dahrendorf (1958) defined residues of feelings, precedents, or structure,
conflict as ‘all relations between sets of indi- but all these taken together’ (p.319).
viduals that involve an incompatible differ- Using their literature summary as a basis,
ence of objective…’ (p.135). More recently – Barki and Hartwick (2004) suggested that
and representative of this perspective – is the for an interaction between two parties to be
work of Jehn and her colleagues (e.g. Jehn, considered a conflict, it must contain not
1995, 1997; Jehn et al., 2008; Jehn & only a disagreement, but also negative
Bendersky, 2003; Jehn & Chatman, 2000; emotions, and interference behaviours to
Jehn & Mannix, 2001; Jehn, Northcraft & fully represent the in-depth, complex, multi-
Neale, 1999). Jehn (1995) defined conflict as dimensional nature of the construct. Consis-
perceptions by group members that they tent with this suggestion, they defined
hold discrepant views or have interpersonal conflict as ‘a dynamic process that occurs
incompatibilities. between interdependent parties as they
A limitation in any definition that treats experience negative emotional reactions to
conflict as simply a disagreement is that it perceived disagreements and interference
might underrepresent the construct. Good with the attainment of their goals’ (p.234).
friends can have discrepant views about The second (related) reason why the
issues or beliefs but not necessarily be in utility of the findings from organisational
conflict with each other. In non-profit organ- psychology is limited in its utility for sport
isational settings, people deemed ‘conflict,’ lies in its conceptual underpinnings (or lack
too strong a word to describe disagreements thereof). Historically, the most promising
or differences of opinion (Hamm-Kerwin, conceptualisation – one that has had the
Doherty & Harman, 2011). Undoubtedly, greatest impact on research over the past two
disagreement is at the root of conflict, in that decades – was advanced by Jehn (1995). This
disagreements can escalate into conflict at conceptualisation formed the basis for an
which point it becomes far more complex operational definition that has been used
and in-depth that just a disagreement on its extensively by Jehn and her colleagues to
own. study the effects of conflict on various group
More recently, Barki and Hartwick outcomes (e.g. Jehn, 1995, 1997; Jehn &
(2004) conducted a comprehensive Bendersky, 2003).
summary and evaluation of the research, Jehn’s (1995, 1997) conceptual model is
constitutive and operational definitions, and founded on the assumption that three types
conceptualisations advanced in organisa- of intra-group conflict are possible: task, rela-
tional psychology. They suggested that after tionship, and process. According to Jehn, task
the 70+ years of scientific scrutiny, a gener- conflict exists when disagreements (over
ally accepted constitutive and operational performance issues) among group members
definition for conflict is still disputed. They occur about the content of tasks being
also highlighted common problems with performed including differences in view-
many constitutive (and by extension) opera- points, ideas, and opinions. Relationship
tional definitions. For example, statements conflict exists when disagreements and inter-
such as ‘arises from’, ‘occurs when’, or ‘exists personal incompatibilities (e.g. tension,
when’, risk simply providing descriptions of animosity) are present among group
the antecedents of conflict and/or the members. Finally, process conflict exists when
conditions under which it can occur without disagreements arise in regard to the manner
describing its fundamental nature. As Pondy in which tasks should be accomplished (e.g.
(1967) elaborated ‘the term conflict refers delegation). Despite the overall comprehen-
neither to its antecedent conditions, nor to siveness of the conceptualisation advanced

6 Sport & Exercise Psychology Review, Vol. 10 No. 3


Perceptions of conflict

by Jehn, it does possess one critical short- directly related to performance. As such, the
coming – the perception that conflict is aforementioned literature supported the
synonymous with disagreement – which risks adoption of a two-dimensional model of task
under-representing the construct. and social conflict as a conceptual starting
A conceptualisation or typology of intra- point for the present study.
group conflict was also advanced by Barki Assessing conflict in this manner is also
and Hartwick (2004). Their first main gaining attention in the broader sport litera-
component – the one introduced above – is ture. Mellalieu, Shearer and Shearer (2013)
founded on the assumption that interper- assessed interpersonal conflict within an
sonal conflict contains cognitive, behavioural, entire sport organisation (i.e. athletes,
and affective components. These are repre- coaches, management, and support staff)
sented by disagreement, negative emotions, and from various national teams representing
interference behaviour respectively. The second the UK at major sporting competitions (i.e.
main component is a distinction between the Olympics, World Championships). The
task and the interpersonal relationship authors used the Barki and Hartwick (2004)
contexts of conflict. Given that the conflict definition as a framework for their research
literature has focused on these two main and assessed the frequency, intensity, and
contexts (i.e. task and relationship), they duration of conflict in these national teams,
have proposed a two-dimensional framework along with the cognitive, behavioural, and
comprised of the aforementioned disagree- affective components. The importance of
ments, interference, and negative emotions assessing conflict in groups is an important
experienced in the: (a) task processes; and endeavour due to the inevitability of conflict
the (b) interpersonal relationship contexts (e.g. Robbins & Judge, 2010) and potential
respectively. performance and relationship implications
The inclusion of both task and relation- (e.g. De Dreu & Weingart, 2003). The overall
ship considerations in any conceptual model influence of conflict has been well repre-
for conflict seems reasonable. Historically, sented in organisational psychology as in the
there has been a longstanding recognition study from Mellalieu et al. (2013). In fact,
among group dynamics theoreticians on the they found that within the organisational
need to acknowledge both the task and the structure, athletes reported the highest
social orientations of groups (e.g. Cartwright number of conflicts. Thus, the intricate
& Zander, 1968; Fiedler, 1967; Hersey & nature of conflict between athletes within a
Blanchard, 1969). From the perspective of team remains an important area to further
the study of conflict in work groups, this task investigate due to its implications for team
and social orientation also has been broadly functioning and was the primary interest of
supported (e.g. Amason & Sapienza, 1997; the present study.
De Dreu & Weingart, 2003; Jehn & The adoption of Barki and Hartwick’s
Bendersky, 2003; Jehn & Chatman, 2000; (2004) conceptualisation for a sport setting
Jehn & Mannix, 2001; Rahim, 2002). Finally, seems to make intuitive sense. As such, we
this distinction also has had support in terms look to add to the sport psychology conflict
of the types of conflicts that arise in a sport literature by adopting this approach. Thus,
setting (e.g. Holt et al., 2012). Holt and his the purpose of the present study was to
colleagues found that performance (i.e. task) improve our understanding of the nature of
conflict is a product of issues centered on intra-group conflict in sport through the
practice or competition concerns, and perceptions of competitive level athletes.
playing time, whereas relationship (i.e. social) The relative paucity of attention paid to
conflict is a product of issues reflecting inter- intra-group conflict led to some decisions
personal disputes or disagreements and about the protocol adopted. First, a qualita-
personality clashes – issues that are not tive methodology was used. This provided

Sport & Exercise Psychology Review, Vol. 10 No. 3 7


Kyle F. Paradis, Albert V. Carron, & Luc J. Martin

the opportunity to use participants as active tenure of 4.0 years with their respective
agents in the research process. Second, the teams (Canadian intercollegiate athletes
conceptualisation advanced for intra-group typically have five years of eligibility to play at
conflict by Barki and Hartwick (2004) was the intercollegiate level). The first and third
used as a deductive frame-of-reference. authors recruited and contacted athletes
Specifically, participants’ responses were directly to participate in the study.
analysed with a view to determining whether Certain athletes competed in multiple
intra-group conflict in sport teams: (1) sports and thus drew from these experi-
occurs in both task and relationship (i.e. ences. Specifically, Athlete Seven competed
social) contexts; and (2) contains cognitive in track and field and rugby and Athlete
(i.e. disagreement), affective (i.e. negative Nine in hockey and lacrosse. The five male
emotions), and behavioural components participants (Mage =25.0, Mtenure =4.4 years)
(i.e. interference). were Athletes Four (golf), Six, (ice hockey),
Seven (track and field and rugby), Eight
Method (volleyball), and Nine (hockey and lacrosse).
Participants The five female participants (Mage =25.0;
Our goal was not to obtain sport- or gender- Mtenure =3.6 years) were Athletes One (rugby),
specific insights into the nature of conflict as Two (volleyball), Three (curling), Five
we wanted to obtain a generalisable assess- (rowing), and Ten (dance).
ment from athletes of both genders from
multiple sports. Thus we set out to gain Procedure
insights regarding the nature of conflict in Approval was obtained from the lead
sport using a purposeful sampling approach author’s institutional research ethics board.
(Patton, 2002). Four specific sampling The semi-structured interviews were con-
criteria were established to recruit partici- ducted in an informal lab/office setting at
pants who could provide independent, the convenience of the athlete. The first and
insightful, and potentially heterogeneous third authors conducted the interviews,
responses. The first was to recruit an equal which lasted approximately 20 to 40 minutes
number of male and female athletes to (20.12 to 39.55; M~30 minutes). A semi-
obtain perspectives from both genders. The structured interview guide was used
second was to recruit athletes with a following general recommendations from
minimum of two years tenure on their Rubin and Rubin (2011). The set-up of qual-
respective teams to ensure extensive compet- itative protocols used in past group dynamics
itive experiences. The third was to ensure research (e.g. Eys et al., 2009; Martin et al.,
that only one athlete representative from any 2011) served as a general template to
given team participated; this ensured that develop the interview questions for the
awkward situations attending within teams present study.
were avoided. Finally, athletes were The outline for the semi-structured inter-
purposely recruited from a heterogeneous views contained four sections: introductory
sample of teams and sports. The four criteria questions, transition questions, key questions, and
were used as a basis for the selection of the concluding questions. The goal of the introduc-
sample to increase generalisability of results. tory questions was to obtain demographic
Intercollegiate athletes that met the above information from the athletes as well as to
criteria were recruited individually. ‘break the ice’ and begin the interview
Participants were 10 current and former process (e.g. ‘What sport do you play?’ and
intercollegiate athletes (N=5 males, N=5 ‘How long have you played?’). The transition
females) from Canadian universities. They questions sought to direct attention towards
ranged in age from 21 to 30 years the notion of conflict and conflict experi-
(Mage =25.00, SD=2.87) and had a mean ences within the team (e.g. ‘How often

8 Sport & Exercise Psychology Review, Vol. 10 No. 3


Perceptions of conflict

would conflict arise in your team?’ ‘What (b) the cognitive, behavioural, and affective
sort of conflict would arise in your team?’ components of conflict. As Munroe-
and ‘What form did the conflict take?’). The Chandler et al. (2007) noted, researchers
key questions – which represented the main always have some initial hypothesis based on
inquiries of the interview – were designed to previous theory.
gain insight into the athlete’s perceptions of To enhance the trustworthiness and
the nature, antecedents, and consequences credibility of our findings; self-reflective
of conflict in teams (e.g. ‘How would you bracketing was undertaken before
define conflict?’ ‘What are some typical indi- conducting any interviews (e.g. Benson et
cators that a team has conflict?’ and ‘What al., 2013). This strategy allowed the first
goes on in a group with conflict?’ the present author to reflect on his own experiences of
study however only deals with issues conflict as a former intercollegiate athlete
surrounding the nature of conflict). Finally, (e.g. Dale, 1996) and to acknowledge any
concluding questions were used to obtain preconceptions or biases held about the
any final thoughts, to clarify any issues that topic to become more aware on how such
were discussed, and to conclude the inter- biases might influence the data collection
view (e.g. ‘Is there anything that you would process or data analysis process (e.g. Giorgi,
like to add?’ and ‘Is there anything you did 2009). Member checks were also performed
not get a chance to say?’). after the interviews whereby participants
could add or omit information, all of which
Analysis served to corroborate the data. With regard
Each interview was audiotaped and the lead to data analysis, the first and second authors
author transcribed the interviews verbatim. coded the transcripts together and reached
This process resulted in 145 typed (double- 100 per cent agreement before item cate-
spaced) transcript pages that were uploaded gorisation (e.g. Lincoln & Guba, 1985;
into the Nvivo 9 qualitative statistical soft- Maxwell, 2002; Sparkes, 1998). Finally, as a
ware program where data were categorised means of analyst triangulation (Patton,
and coded. Specifically, meaning units, 2002), critical review of meaning units and
which Tesch (1990) defined as ‘a segment of an expert audit review were performed
text that is comprehensible by itself and whereby all three members of the research
contains one idea, episode, or piece of infor- team reached a triangular consensus
mation’ (p.116) were created. Thus, a (Sparkes, 1998), which confirmed the cate-
meaning unit could reflect a word, phrase, gorisation of results.
sentence, or paragraph of text.
A thematic analysis was used and the Results
coding was carried out using a combination Responses were categorised on the basis of
of an inductive (e.g. Côté et al., 1993) and task versus social – the context in which the
deductive approaches (e.g. Munroe-Chan- conflict occurred. Responses were cate-
dler et al., 2007). An inductive analysis is gorised based on their reference to cognitive
grounded in the data whereas a deductive (e.g. disagreement), affective (e.g. negative
analysis is guided by a theoretically informed emotions), and behavioural manifestations
framework (Patton, 2002; Tesch, 1990). The (e.g. interference) of conflict. In the
inductive approach involved the identifica- sections that follow, the results pertaining to
tion and classification of emerging themes the nature of conflict are presented initially
from the data while the deductive approach (i.e. where a distinction between task and
was based on a conceptual understanding; in social contexts was not apparent) followed
the present study this consisted of: (a) the by a presentation of the nature of conflict in
task versus social distinction of conflict; and task and social contexts.

Sport & Exercise Psychology Review, Vol. 10 No. 3 9


Kyle F. Paradis, Albert V. Carron, & Luc J. Martin

The nature of conflict (a hockey/lacrosse player): ‘[conflict is] not


Cognitive. A common reference in the just a typical disagreement you might have
discussions of the nature of conflict was to with someone else; conflicts just go deeper
disagreement. For example, Athlete 8 (a than that and you’re going to have worse
volleyball player) suggested that: ‘in the arguments and more intense conflicts’.
broad context, I would think of it [conflict]
as a general disagreement on one topic or Affective. Consistent with the Barki and
potentially one goal… where people’s views Hartwick (2004) conceptualisation, athletes
don’t align with one another’. Athlete 4 referenced the emotional aspects of conflict.
(a golfer) advanced a similar viewpoint: ‘it’s Athlete 8 (a volleyball player) stated:
definitely some kind of disagreement about ‘conflict is a lot more [intense], the
a viewpoint or a certain way things should be emotional connection in the conflict is a lot
done’. Athlete 2 (a volleyball player) also more.’ Athlete 10 (a dancer) also referenced
discussed disagreement in her view of the negative emotions associated with her
conflict: ‘I guess I would see conflict as some- example of a conflict episode: ‘I did feel a
thing negative, so generally a clash of ideas little bit of resentment towards the group at
or personalities… or two sides not agreeing times or at least towards particular individ-
on a certain concept’. Athlete 10 (a dancer) uals… it wasn’t an enjoyable experience at
also commented that: ‘conflict could be all.’ Athlete 3 (a curler) also recalled a
anything really revolving around a disagree- conflict situation with her coach where
ment between individuals or groups’. emotions escalated: ‘The coach was
Athlete 7 (a track/rugby player) viewed extremely upset with me and kind of freaked
conflict as: ‘disagreements between two out and yelled at me about the situation and
groups or two entities on a certain aspect’. almost threatened me in a way like ‘if you do
Finally, Athlete 9 (a hockey player) said that that again, either you or me is not going to
conflict in a team sport setting was: be on this team anymore’, so it kind of made
‘A disagreement between two players on the me worry.’ It was also apparent that athletes
same team… who don’t necessarily see eye to felt that the situation becomes emotionally
eye and… rub each other the wrong way.’ charged when individuals are in conflict situ-
It was apparent that disagreement was ations. Many recalled that it took a toll on
one of the first things that came to mind them and often made them contemplate
when athletes thought of intra-group quitting the sport as Athlete 2 (a volleyball
conflict because they stated it explicitly in player) noted ‘There were situations and
their responses. As a caveat, however, it was times were I was like I don’t want to do this
apparent that athletes did not consider anymore, this is not fun, I’m not getting
conflict to be solely represented by disagree- anything out of this… and I know a lot of
ment. For example, Athlete 2 (a volleyball girls who didn’t come back’.
player) suggested that conflict is of a greater
severity than just disagreement: ‘The situa- Behavioural. In discussing the conflict situa-
tion has to be pretty severe to call it conflict. tions they had observed or in which they
I don’t think it’s something as simple as were involved, athletes repeatedly made
disagreeing on something… it has to really references to specific behaviours that they
divide people and keep them divided… It felt made the interaction a conflict. For
has to be of a greater severity… like war, example, Athlete 5 (a rower) recalled that:
some huge kind of outbreak… like aggres- ‘in the boat, people would yell and scream
sion, disagreement, all those things and there was lots of swearing and a lot
combined together, I don’t see it as just a of frustration, so people would slam their
difference of opinion.’ A similar view of the oars down or fall out of the boat
nature of conflict was also stated by Athlete 9 on purpose.’ The conflict situation Athlete 9

10 Sport & Exercise Psychology Review, Vol. 10 No. 3


Perceptions of conflict

(a hockey/lacrosse player) recalled was char- For example, Athlete 6 (a hockey player)
acterised by: ‘lots of snide remarks, lots of thought of it as: ‘When team members are
sarcasm, lots of patronising… it’s pretty cruel not getting along off the ice’. Other athletes
stuff’. A similar observation was made by thought of social conflict as being more than
both Athlete 6 (a hockey player): ‘There simply not getting along. For example,
would be verbal sarcasm, silent treatment Athlete 8 (a volleyball player), seemed to
and someone might ignore somebody if they touch on various aspects of social conflict:
are pissed off’ and Athlete 7 (a track/rugby ‘In the broad context, I would think of it as a
player): ‘it was a prime example of a negative general disagreement about one topic (e.g.
culture… the bad mouthing and trash disagreement)… and I think it has a really
talking continued from the senior players negative connotation associated with it’ He
and trickled down to the rookies and then went on to elaborate…’ People are
continues on every year’. being defensive (e.g. behavioural) and it’s
like I’m getting attacked [personally] for
Nature of task conflict this? I really have to stick up for myself or
When athletes were queried about the nature I just lose face in front of a lot of people’
of conflict, a clear element of task conflict (e.g. affective).
arose. In addition to this emergence, an As well as confrontations, social conflict
important over-riding perception was that it also manifests itself through isolation and
(task conflict) is inevitable in competitive exclusion. We debated whether these behav-
sport. For example, Athlete 8 (a volleyball iours were consequences of conflict or
player) observed: ‘If there is not some kind of whether they reflected the nature of conflict.
conflict… than you’re not that invested in it.’ The context advanced by the athletes as well
Also, typically, when athletes discussed as the long-term nature of the isolation and
conflict in task situations, its multidimen- exclusion led us to conclude that these were
sional nature (i.e. cognitive, affective, behav- a manifestation of the perceived nature of
ioural) was referenced. Athlete 5 (a rower) conflict.
stated: ‘If one person kind of screws up a The behavioural act of isolating or
little bit, the whole thing is ruined… so… I’d excluding teammates could accompany
be so angry [with them]’ (e.g. affective). disagreement as Athlete 1 (a rugby player)
Also, Athlete 9 (a hockey/lacrosse suggested: but also could be viewed as
player) pointed out: ‘When there’s conflicts serious interference behaviours in social
around the task it’s usually two guys that are conflict. Athlete 1 made this clear when she
passionate and want the same results, but stated: ‘lack of inclusion of others, I think
they are just going about achieving that that was our primary issue, exclusion was
result in different ways. They have different definitely one of the main conflict issues.’
ideas on how they would go about it’ (e.g. She went on to describe a specific situation
behavioural). Athlete 9 went on to say: ‘Guys that addressed this, ‘There was a group that
just want to be heard and a lot of times guys began to isolate themselves more and have a
are getting mad at other guys because they [year end] party and purposely not invite
don’t see their point of view or don’t see certain people on the team to our final
their reasoning or thought process behind party… that was a big issue on the team.’
certain ideas or decisions’ (e.g. cognitive). (e.g. behavioural)
Athlete 10 (a dancer) reflected on the
Nature of social conflict social conflict in her team and felt the
It appears that social conflict was also tension just permeated through the group,
perceived by the athletes differently from as well as the interference she felt from her
their experiences. One of these can be classi- teammates attempting to exclude her from
fied as poor relations away from the sport. social events and the negative emotions she

Sport & Exercise Psychology Review, Vol. 10 No. 3 11


Kyle F. Paradis, Albert V. Carron, & Luc J. Martin

felt towards them: ‘In terms of social conflict, stop at just disagreement. This notion also
I could definitely tell that there were cliques supports the findings from volunteer sport
in the group. Whether they were meant to organisations that conflict goes beyond mere
happen or not [I don’t know]. I definitely disagreement (Hamm-Kerwin et al., 2011).
felt excluded at times from particular cliques Athletes also highlighted the affective
and especially because I did really enjoy component of conflict through references to
everyone… I had a few really close friends on negative emotional states, feelings of resent-
the team but I didn’t want to talk to anyone ment, jealousy, anger, frustration, and irrita-
anymore and there were times I felt like I tion. In addition, athletes reported the
couldn’t talk to certain people on the team presence of ‘heavy’ emotional investment
and I know I was not the only person that felt and ‘heated’ emotional debates surrounding
that way.’ various issues. This perception of height-
Athlete 9 (a hockey/lacrosse player), ened emotion-as-conflict also has support in
reflected on a social conflict situation he the organisational psychology literature (e.g.
experienced firsthand when significant Amason, 1996; Amason & Sapienza, 1997).
others got involved: ‘I’ve seen personal Finally, athletes also identified a behav-
things get involved too, and it’s a shame ioural component of conflict. The behav-
when it does, because you know normally its ioural component reflected various actions
stuff that is totally unrelated [to the sport] or behaviours that the athletes undertook
but I mean, there’s conflicts on teams over that led to the perception of the presence of
girls and stuff like that and that’s when it gets conflict. Some behaviours mentioned
a little bit harder to resolve because as team- included interference with attainment of
mates you want to help whoever is involved goals, the presence of negative body
in the conflict to sort it out but in a situation language, avoidance behaviour, silent treat-
where there’s external forces like girlfriends ment, and verbal and physical fighting. The
or something like that, where can you step perception of behaviours-as-conflict has
in? You really can’t… so it’s definitely not been highlighted in the work of Alper et al.
limited to things within the team.’ (2000). Also, Dyer and Song (1997) consid-
ered conflict to be the interference with
Discussion respect to the attainment of goals.
The present study explored which percep- When athletes defined conflict, their
tions members of competitive sport teams response(s) typically included a reference to
held about the nature of intra-group one or more of the Barki and Hartwick
conflict. Athletes explained how they (2004) components – cognitions (e.g.
perceived conflict and what occurs during a disagreement), affect (e.g. emotion) and
typical conflict. Several examples supported behaviour (e.g. interference). All three
the Barki and Hartwick (2004) suggestion components however were not referenced
that conflict contains cognitive, behavioural, simultaneously in a single response but
and affective components. Insofar as the rather in multiple responses over the course
cognitive component is concerned, the term of the interview. This also could have been
‘disagreement’ was mentioned several times due to the nature of the questions asked. As
with conflict being described as a disagree- mentioned above, much of the organisa-
ment, a difference of opinion, or differing tional psychology literature has defined
viewpoints. This perspective of disagree- conflict-as-disagreement (e.g. Jehn, 1995,
ment-as-conflict is consistent with the organ- 1997). Jehn (1997) proposed that all
isational psychology literature (e.g. Jehn, conflicts have some degree of emotionality,
1997). There is no disputing that disagree- but this proposal is not reflected in her defi-
ment is at the heart of any conflict; however, nition. In the present study, several athletes
as the athletes noted, it typically does not alluded that conflict goes beyond just

12 Sport & Exercise Psychology Review, Vol. 10 No. 3


Perceptions of conflict

disagreement. These athletes’ perspectives strategies, player delegations, decisions on


support the Barki and Hartwick (2004) how things are done); the athletes typically
proposal that conflict is a concurrent combi- have little to no input. In such an autocratic
nation of three components. environment, there may be minimal oppor-
Another point for discussion relates to tunity for process conflict to emerge.
the support found in the present results for Competition between teammates also
the (deductive) categorisation of conflict seemed to emerge as a possible type of intra-
into task and social components based on: group conflict. Boardley and Jackson (2012)
(1) the conceptual model advanced by Jehn examined intra-group moral behaviour
(1997); and (2) the findings highlighting when teammates are viewed as rivals. One
performance and relationship conflict in finding from their study was that high task
sport (Holt et al., 2012). Our results are cohesion (specifically attraction to group-
consistent with the Holt et al. (2012) find- task) predicted greater antisocial behaviour.
ings and partially consistent with Jehn’s The authors attributed the finding to some
(1997) conceptualisation of conflict. Jehn of the potential disadvantages of high task
(1997) indicated that ‘there is an apparent cohesion such as communication problems
distinction between task and relationship and reduced social relations (Hardy, Eys &
[conflict] similar to other organisational Carron, 2005). Considering that high task
theories that distinguish between task and cohesion was associated with poor communi-
interpersonal dimensions of organisational cation, reduced social relationships, greater
life’ (p.531). As was suggested earlier, this anti-social behaviour, perhaps elements of
task and social distinction is well supported task conflict may also be associated with such
from various group dynamics theoreticians findings. Researchers could assess the rela-
with regards to the orientation of groups tionship between cohesion, conflict, and
(e.g. Carron et al., 1985; Cartwright & moral behaviour within competitive sport
Zander, 1968; Fiedler, 1967). teams to better understand the influence of
Where our findings differ from Jehn’s these group processes on overall group func-
(1997) conceptualisation is the limited tioning.
support for process conflict in sport – which The importance of understanding intra-
was an integral component of Jehn’s (1997) group conflict in sport teams also lends to
conceptualisation. The notion of process the importance of learning the causes and
conflict in sport also did not seem to emerge implications that could stem from such
in the results of the Holt et al. (2012) study. conflict. In terms of possible causes, Jehn et
One possible explanation is that process al. (1999) found in a comparison of homo-
issues are subsumed under the general cate- geneous vs. heterogeneous groups that
gory of task conflict. Recently, Bendersky et diversity or differences among group
al. (2010) revised Jehn’s original three- members in values, goals, personality,
dimensional conflict conceptualisation (i.e. ethnicity, and socio-economic status can all
task, relationship, and process) and lead to conflict. In addition, Jehn and
proposed that conflict was two-dimensional – Bendersky (2003) found that individual
task and relationship conflict – and that differences could contribute to conflict.
process conflict was actually perceived as a Future research should further investigate
form of task conflict. Another possible expla- the possible antecedents of conflict in sport.
nation for the absence of process conflict is In terms of possible implications of
the fundamental nature of elite level sport conflict, the De Dreu and Weingart (2003)
teams (which contrasts with the fundamental meta-analysis, found that both types of
nature of work groups). Typically, in elite- conflict (i.e. task and relationship) had nega-
level sport, the coach is responsible for estab- tive relationships with performance and
lishing and dictating process (i.e. game satisfaction. Similarly, in sport, popular

Sport & Exercise Psychology Review, Vol. 10 No. 3 13


Kyle F. Paradis, Albert V. Carron, & Luc J. Martin

outcomes that have been assessed are athlete conflict is acceptable and encouraged, the
satisfaction (e.g. Riemer & Chelladurai, ability to address and resolve the conflict
1998) and performance success (e.g. Carron early becomes much more likely.
et al., 2002). Considering the importance of Another recommendation advanced
both performance and satisfaction in the from previous sport literature was to address
context of sport, a worthwhile endeavour for conflict early (Holt et al., 2012). Previous
future research would be the assessment of organisational research has also echoed this
the conflict-performance and conflict-satis- point that high performing teams should
faction relationships. deal with conflict in the early stages of their
Another point that warrants discussion is formation (Greer, Jehn & Mannix, 2008).
the possibility that positive outcomes may be Due to the effects of both task and social
associated with conflict. Some athletes conflict on both team performance and satis-
suggested that task conflict, if resolved early, faction (e.g. De Dreu & Weingart, 2003), the
could be beneficial. Athletes also indicated importance of understanding how conflict
that this type of conflict can be a growing may evolve and grow over time would
moment and can help direct focus. The certainly motivate those involved to resolve it
potential for positive outcomes has both quickly. The long lasting impact of conflicts
been refuted (e.g. De Dreu & Weingart, that go unresolved would be detrimental for
2003) and supported (e.g. Jehn & Mannix, any high performance team (Greer et al.,
2001) in previous literature. In the present 2008; Holt et al., 2012).
study, however, and consistent with previous Finally, from a sport perspective, a
research (e.g. Jehn, 1995; De Dreu & Wein- preventative measure for avoiding conflict
gart, 2003), there were no perceived positive may be the implementation of team building
outcomes derived from social conflict. interventions early in the season (Holt et al.,
Future research in sport should continue to 2012). As many of the athletes in the present
assess the potential for positive outcomes. study noted, low cohesion and the presence
Considering the inevitability of conflict, the of cliques often were issues associated with
ability for a team to derive positive outcomes task and social conflict. For example, a focus
from conflict would be profound. on group norms could be a useful tool here
to establish structure. The development of
Practical implications and future directions agreed upon group norms establishes a set
The following are some practical implica- of behavioural guidelines for athletes to
tions that coachers and/or sport psychology abide by which could go a long way to
practitioners might consider when trying to avoiding conflict issues (Paradis & Martin,
manage or resolve conflict within their 2012).
teams. First, conflict can arise around Carron and Spink (1993) also outlined in
performance (i.e. task) or relationships (i.e. their conceptual model of team building
social) issues – a finding also reported in the that having athletes engage in sacrifice
Holt et al. (2012) study. Sport psychology behaviours could be beneficial for
practitioners can identify and isolate the improving the group’s processes. Individual
context(s) in which the conflict occurred, so sacrifices that help others and are for the
that situation-specific strategies can be devel- good of the team could help increase
oped and used to address these issues. Sport perceptions of cohesion and enable athletes
psychology practitioners should proactively to gain an appreciation of each other
develop (i.e. before the first presence of (Martin et al., 2013). Holt et al. (2012) also
conflict) group norms that encourages the supported and advanced the recommenda-
discussion of conflict openly (Jehn et al., tion of using team building interventions to
2008). If athletes are in an environment aid in creating a cohesive atmosphere.
where the open constructive discussion of Researchers and practitioners alike could

14 Sport & Exercise Psychology Review, Vol. 10 No. 3


Perceptions of conflict

assess the effectiveness of such team building The Authors


interventions on the impact of conflict Kyle F. Paradis
prevention. University of Western Ontario.
Some limitations of the present study
should be addressed. First, our sample was Albert V. Carron
composed solely of intercollegiate athletes University of Western Ontario.
and thus, results may not be generalisable to
other sport populations (e.g. children, Luc J. Martin
youth, masters). Future research could University of Lethbridge
explore intra-group conflict in these
domains. Second, the qualitative process has Correspondence
only captured a snapshot of conflict from ten Kyle F. Paradis
athletes who reflected on their specific School of Kinesiology,
sporting experiences. Future research could Thames Hall/3M Centre,
assess a larger more diverse sample to verify University of Western Ontario,
themes from the present study. 1151 Richmond Street,
Overall, the results supported a conclu- London, Ontario,
sion that intercollegiate athletes viewed Canada, N6A 3K7.
conflict as dynamic and complex in nature Email: kparadis@uwo.ca
principally encompassing cognitive, behav-
ioural, and affective components regarding
task and social issues. The present study has
served as a starting point for what is certainly
a new discussion on the nature of conflict in
sport. Future research could also utilise our
results to aid in the development quantitative
measures as it has been suggested that meas-
urement is fundamental to the advancement
of knowledge (Carron, Eys & Martin, 2012).

Sport & Exercise Psychology Review, Vol. 10 No. 3 15


Kyle F. Paradis, Albert V. Carron, & Luc J. Martin

References
Alper, S., Tjosvold, D. & Law, K.S. (2000). Conflict Carron, A.V., Widmeyer, W.N. & Brawley, L.R. (1985).
management, efficacy, and performance in The development of an instrument to assess
organisational teams. Personnel Psychology, 53, cohesion in sport teams: The Group Environment
625–642. Questionnaire. Journal of Sport Psychology, 7,
Amason, A.C. (1996). Distinguishing the effects of 244–266.
functional and dysfunctional conflict on strategic Cartwright, D. & Zander, A. (1968). Group dynamics:
decision making: Resolving a paradox for top Research and theory. New York: Harper & Row.
management teams. Academy of Management Chelladurai, P. & Saleh, S.D. (1980). Dimensions of
Journal, 39, 123–148. leader behaviour in sports: Development of a
Amason, A.C. & Sapienza, H. (1997). The effects of Leadership Scale. Journal of Sport Psychology, 2,
top management team size and interaction norms 34–45.
on cognitive and affective conflict. Journal of Côté, J., Salmela, J. H., Baria, A. & Russell, S. J. (1993).
Management, 23, 496–516. Organising and interpreting unstructured
Barker, D., Rossi, A. & Puhse, U. (2010). Managing qualitative data. The Sport Psychologist, 7, 127–137.
teams: Comparing organisational and Dahrendorf, R. (1958). Toward a theory of social
sport psychological approaches to teamwork. conflict. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 2, 170–183.
Scandinavian Sport Studies Forum, 1, 115–132. Dale, G.A. (1996). Existential phenomenology.
Barki, H. & Hartwick, J. (2004). Conceptualising the Emphasising the experience of the athlete in
construct of interpersonal conflict. International sport psychology research. The Sport Psychologist, 7,
Journal of Conflict Management, 15, 216–244. 307–321.
Bendersky, C., Behfar, K., Weingart, L., Todorova, G., De Dreu, C.K.W. & Weingart, L.R. (2003). Task versus
Bear, J., Jehn, K. (2010). Revisiting the dimensions relationship conflict, team performance, and
of intra-group conflict: Theoretical and psycho- team member satisfaction: A meta-analysis. Journal
metric construct refinement. IACM Paper No. of Applied Psychology, 88, 741–749.
11–03. Dyer, B. & Song, X.M. (1997). The impact strategy of
Benson, A.J., Eys, M.A., Surya, M., Dawson, K. & conflict: A cross-national comparative study of US
Schneider, M. (2013). Athletes’ perceptions of and Japanese firms. Journal of International Business
role acceptance in interdependent sport teams. Studies, 28, 467–493.
The Sport Psychologist, 27, 269–281. Eys, M.A., Carron, A.V., Beauchamp, M.R. & Bray, S.R.
Boardley, I.D. & Jackson, B. (2012). When teammates (2005). Athletes’ perceptions of the sources of
are viewed as rivals: A cross-sectional investigation role ambiguity. Small Group Research, 36, 383–403.
oh achievement goals and intra-team moral Eys, M.A., Loughead, T.M., Bray, S.R. & Carron, A.V.
behaviour. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 34, (2009). Perceptions of cohesion by youth sport
503–524. participants. The Sport Psychologist, 23, 330–345.
Bodtker, A.M. & Jameson, J.K. (2001). Emotion in Fiedler, F.E. (1967). A theory of leadership effectiveness.
conflict formation and its transformation: New York: McGraw-Hill.
Application to organisational conflict manage- Giorgi, A. (2009). The descriptive phenomenological
ment. International Journal of Conflict Management, method in psychology: A modified Husserlian approach.
12, 259–275. Pittsburgh, PA: Stanford University Press.
Canary, D.J., Cunningham, E.M. & Cody, M.J. (1988). Greenlees, I., Lane, A., Thelwell, R., Holder, T. &
Goal types, gender, and locus of control in Hobson, G. (2005). Team-referent attributions
managing interpersonal conflict. Communication among sport performers. Research Quarterly for
Research, 15, 426–446. Exercise and Sport, 76, 477–487.
Carron, A.V., Colman, M.M., Wheeler, J. & Stevens, D. Greer, L.L., Jehn, K.A. & Mannix, E.A. (2008).
(2002). Cohesion and performance in sport: Conflict transformation: A longitudinal investiga-
A meta-analysis. Journal of Sport & Exercise tion of the relationships between different types
Psychology, 24, 168–188. of intra-group conflict and the moderating role of
Carron, A.V. & Eys, M.A. (2012). Group dynamics in conflict resolution. Small Group Research, 39,
sport (4th ed.). Morgantown WV: Fitness 278–302.
Information Technology. Hamm-Kerwin, S. & Doherty, A. (2010). Intra-group
Carron, A.V., Eys, M.A. & Martin, L.J. (2012). conflict in non-profit sport boards. Journal of Sport
Cohesion. In G. Tenenbaum, R. Eklund & A. Management, 24, 245–271.
Kamata (Eds.), Measurement in sport and exercise Hamm-Kerwin, S., Doherty, A. & Harman, A. (2011).
psychology (pp.411–422). Champaign, IL: Human ‘Its not conflict, its differences of opinion’:
Kinetics. An in-depth examination of conflict in non-profit
Carron, A.V. & Spink, K.S. (1993). Team building in boards. Small Group Research, 42, 562–594.
an exercise setting. The Sport Psychologist, 7, 8–18.

16 Sport & Exercise Psychology Review, Vol. 10 No. 3


Perceptions of conflict

Hardy, J., Eys, M.A. & Carron, A.V. (2005). Exploring Lavoi, N.M. (2007). Interpersonal communication
the potential disadvantages of high cohesion in and conflict in the coach-athlete relationship.
sports teams. Small Group Research, 36, 166–187. In S. Jowett & D. Lavallee (Eds.), Social psychology
Holt, N.L., Black, D.E., Tamminen, K.A., Fox, K.R. & in sport (pp.29–40). Champaign, IL: Human
Mandigo, J.L. (2008). Levels of social complexity Kinetics.
and dimensions of peer experiences in youth Lincoln, Y. & Guba, E. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry.
sport. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 30, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
411–431. Loughead, T.M., Hardy, J. & Eys, M.A. (2006). The
Holt, N.L., Knight, C.J. & Zukiwski, P. (2012). Female nature of athlete leadership. Journal of Sport
athletes’ perceptions of teammate conflict in Behaviour, 29, 142–158.
sport: Implications for sport psychology Martin, L.J., Carron, A.V., Eys, M.A. & Loughead, T.M.
consultants. The Sport Psychologist, 26, 135–154. (2011). Children’s perceptions of cohesion.
Holt, N.L. & Sparkes, A. (2001). An ethnographic Sport & Exercise Psychology Review, 7, 11–23.
study of cohesiveness in a college soccer team over Martin, L.J., Paradis, K.F., Eys, M.A. & Evans, M.B.
a season. The Sport Psychologist, 20, 348–367. (2013). Cohesion in sport: New directions for
Jehn, K.A. (1994). Enhancing effectiveness: An practitioners. Journal of Sport Psychology in Action,
investigation of advantages and disadvantages of 4, 14–25.
value-based intra-group conflict. International Maxwell, J. (2002). Understanding and validity in
Journal of Conflict Management, 5, 268–294. qualitative research. In A.M. Huberman & M.B.
Jehn, K.A. (1995). A multi-method examination of the Miles (Eds.), The qualitative researcher’s companion
benefits and detriments of intra-group conflict. (pp.37–64). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 40, 256–282. Mellalieu, S., Shearer, D.A. & Shearer, C. (2013).
Jehn, K.A. (1997). A qualitative analysis of conflict A preliminary survey of interpersonal conflict at
types and dimensions in organisational groups. a major games and championships. The Sport
Administrative Science Quarterly, 42, 530–557. Psychologist, 27, 120–129.
Jehn, K.A. & Bendersky, C. (2003). Intragroup conflict Mortensen, M. & Hinds, P.J. (2001). Conflict and
in organisations: A contingency perspective on shared identity in geographically distributed
the conflict-outcome relationship. Research in teams. International Journal of Conflict Management,
Organisational Behaviour 25, 187–242. 12, 212–238.
Jehn, K.A., Chadwick, C. & Thatcher, S. (1997). Munroe-Chandler, K.J., Hall, C.R., Fishburne, G.J. &
To agree or not to agree: The effects of value Strachan, L. (2007). Where, when, and why young
congruence, individual demographic dissimilarity, athletes use imagery. An examination of
and conflict on workgroup outcomes. International developmental differences. Research Quarterly for
Journal of Conflict Management, 8, 287–305. Exercise and Sport, 78, 103–116.
Jehn, K.A. & Chatman, J.A. (2000). The influence of Ntoumanis, N. & Vazou, S. (2005). Peer motivational
proportional and perceptual conflict composition climate in youth sport: Measurement, develop-
on team performance. International Journal of ment, and validation. Journal of Sport & Exercise
Conflict Management, 11, 56–73. Psychology, 27, 432–455.
Jehn, K.A., Greer, L., Levine, S. & Szulanski, G. (2008). Paradis, K.F. & Martin, L.J. (2012). Team building in
The effects of conflict types, dimensions, and sport: Linking theory and research to practical
emergent states on group outcomes. Group application. Journal of Sport Psychology in Action, 3,
Decision and Negotiation, 17, 465–495. 159–170.
Jehn, K.A. & Mannix, E.A. (2001). The dynamic Patton, M.Q. (2002). Qualitative research and education
nature of conflict: A longitudinal study of methods (3rd ed.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
intragroup conflict and group performance. Pearson, A.W., Ensley, M.D. & Amason, A.C. (2002).
Academy of Management Journal, 44, 238–251. An assessment and refinement of Jehn’s intra-
Jehn, K.A., Northcraft, G.B. & Neale, M.A. (1999). group conflict scale. International Journal of Conflict
Why differences make a difference: A field study Management, 13, 110–126.
of diversity, conflict and performance in Pelled, L.H., Eisenhardt, K.M. & Xin, K.R. (1999).
workgroups. Administrative Science Quarterly, 44, Exploring the black box: An analysis of work
741–763. group diversity, conflict, and performance.
Jowett, S. (2003). When the honeymoon is over: A case Administrative Science Quarterly, 44, 1–28.
study of a coach-athlete dyad in crisis. The Sport Pondy, L.R. (1967). Organisational conflict: Concepts
Psychologist, 17, 446–462. and models. Administrative Science Quarterly, 12,
Jowett, S. & Ntoumanis, N. (2004). The Coach-Athlete 296–320.
Relationship Questionnaire (CART-Q): Develop- Rahim, M. (2002). Toward a theory of managing
ment and initial validation. Scandinavian Journal organisational conflict. International Journal of
of Medicine and Science in Sports, 14, 245–257. Conflict Management, 13, 206–235.

Sport & Exercise Psychology Review, Vol. 10 No. 3 17


Kyle F. Paradis, Albert V. Carron, & Luc J. Martin

Riemer, H.A. & Chelladurai, P. (1998). Development Tesch, R. (1990). Qualitative research analysis types and
of the Athlete Satisfaction Questionnaire (ASQ). software tools. New York: Falmer.
Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 20, 127–156. Vazou, S., Ntoumanis, N. & Duda, J. (2005). Peer
Robbins, S.P. & Judge, T.A. (2010). Organisational motivational climate in youth sport: A qualitative
behaviour (14th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: inquiry. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 6, 497–516.
Prentice Hall. Wall, J.A. & Callister, R.R. (1995). Conflict and its
Rubin, H.J. & Rubin, I.S. (2011). Qualitative management. Journal of Management, 21, 515–558.
interviewing: The art of hearing data (3rd ed.). Weiss, M.R. & Smith, A.L. (1999). Quality of youth
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. sport friendships: Measurement development and
Short, S.E., Sullivan, P. & Feltz, D.L. (2005). validation. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 21,
Development and preliminary validation of the 145–166.
Collective Efficacy Questionnaire for Sports. Weiss, M.R., Smith, A.L. & Stuntz, C.P. (2008). Moral
Measurement in Physical Activity and Exercise Science, development in sport and physical activity.
9, 181–202. In T. Horn (Ed.), Advances in sport psychology
Simons, T.L. & Peterson, R.S. (2000). Task conflict and (3rd ed., pp.187–210). Champaign, IL: Human
relationship conflict in top management teams: Kinetics.
The pivotal role of intragroup trust. Journal of Weiss, M.R., Smith, A.L. & Theeboom, M.M. (1996).
Applied Psychology, 85, 102–111. ‘That’s what friends are for’: Children’s and
Sparkes, A.C. (1998). Validity in qualitative inquiry teenagers’ perceptions of peer relationships in the
and the problem of criteria: Implications for sport sport domain. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology,
psychology. The Sport Psychologist, 12, 363–386. 18, 347–379.
Sullivan, P.J. & Feltz, D.L. (2001). The relationship Wilmot, W.W. & Hocker, J.L. (2001). Interpersonal
between intra-team conflict and cohesion in conflict (6th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
hockey teams. Small Group Research, 32, 342–355.

18 Sport & Exercise Psychology Review, Vol. 10 No. 3

View publication stats

You might also like