Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 5

Small-group Interaction and Gender

ation and brainstem cholinergic suppression. Journal of Considering gender in a broader context draws
Neuroscience 11: 3200–17 attention to how the interactional patterns it entails
Steriade M, McCarley R W 1990 Brainstem Control of Wake- are both similar to and different from those that
fulness and Sleep. Plenum Press, New York
characterize other systems of difference and inequality
such as those based on race, ethnicity, or wealth.
J. A. Hobson
While interaction occurs between the advantaged and
the disadvantaged on each of these forms of social
difference, the rate of interaction across the gender
divide is distinctively high. Gender divides the popu-
Small-group Interaction and Gender lation into social categories of nearly equal size; it
cross-cuts kin and households and is central for
When people interact together in groups that are small reproduction. Each of these factors increases the
enough to allow person-to-person contact (2 to 20 frequency with which men and women interact and the
people), regular patterns of behavior develop that intimacy of the terms on which they do so. Fur-
organize their relations. The study of gender in this thermore, research on social cognition has shown that
context examines how these patterns of behavior are people automatically sex categorize (i.e., label as male
affected by members’ social locations as men or women or female) any concrete person with whom they
and the consequences this has for beliefs about gender interact. As a consequence, gender is potentially at
differences and for gender inequality in society. play in all interaction and interactional events are
likely to be important for the maintenance or change
of a society’s cultural beliefs and practices about
1. The Emergence of the Field gender. West and Zimmerman (1987) argue that for
gender to persist as a social phenomenon, people must
The systematic, empirical study of small group in- continually ‘do gender’ by presenting themselves in
teraction and gender (hereafter, gender and inter- ways that allow others to relate to them as men or
action) developed during the 1940s and 1950s out of women as that is culturally defined by society.
the confluence of a general social scientific interest in Recognition of these distinctive aspects of gender
small groups and structural-functionalist theorizing has increased attention to the role that gendered
about the origin of differentiated gender roles for men patterns of interaction play in gender inequality.
and women. Parsons and Bales (1955) argued that Interaction mediates the process by which people form
small groups, like all social systems, must manage social bonds, are socially evaluated by others, gain
instrumental functions of adaptation and goal at- influence, and are directed towards or away from
tainment while also attending to expressive functions positions of power and valued social rewards. Inter-
of group integration and the well-being of members. action also provides the contexts in which people
Differentiated instrumental and expressive gender develop identities of competence and sociality. To the
roles develop to solve this problem in the small group extent that gender moderates these interaction pro-
system of the family. Children internalize these func- cesses, it shapes the outcomes of men and women as
tionally specialized roles as personality traits that well as shared beliefs about gender.
shape their behavior in all groups.
Although eventually discredited on logical and
2. Current Theories
empirical grounds, the functional account of gender
roles stimulated broader empirical attention to gender Four theoretical approaches predominate. Two, social
and interaction, not only within the family but also in role theory and expectation states theory, single out a
task-oriented groups such as committees and work society’s cultural beliefs about the nature and social
groups. Evidence accumulated that gender’s effects on value of men’s and women’s traits and competencies
interaction are complex and quite context specific (see as primary factors that create gendered patterns of
Aries 1996; Deaux and LaFrance 1998). This evidence interaction in that society. The theories conceptualize
is inconsistent with the view that gender is best these beliefs in slightly different terms (i.e., as gender
understood as stable individual traits that affect men stereotypes or gender status beliefs) but are in sub-
and women’s behavior in a consistent manner across stantial agreement about their explanatory impact on
situations. interaction through the expectations for behavior that
These findings on interaction contributed to a develop when these beliefs are evoked by the group
gradual transformation of social scientific approaches situation. The remaining theories take somewhat
to gender. From an earlier view of gender as a matter different approaches.
of individual personality and family relations, social
science has increasingly approached gender as a broad
2.1 Social Role Theory
system of social difference and inequality that is best
studied as part of social stratification as well as Eagly’s (1987) social role theory argues that widely
individual development and family organization. shared gender stereotypes develop from the gender

14185
Small-group Interaction and Gender

division of labor that characterizes a society. In to accept or resist influence from others, creating a
western societies, men’s greater participation in paid behavioral influence hierarchy that usually advantages
positions of higher power and status and the dis- men over women in the group.
proportionate assignment of nurturant roles to women In mixed sex groups with a gender-neutral task, the
have created stereotypes that associate agency with theory predicts that men will participate more asser-
men and communion with women. In addition, the tively and be more influential than women. If the
gendered division of labor gives men and women group task or context is culturally linked to men, their
differentiated skills. When gender stereotypes are influence advantage over women will be stronger. If
salient in a group because of a mixed sex membership the task or context is associated with women’s cultur-
or a task or context that is culturally associated with ally expected competencies, however, the theory pre-
one gender, stereotypes shape behavior directly dicts that women will be somewhat more assertive and
through the expectations members form for one influential than men. There should be no gender
another’s behavior. When group members enact social differences in assertive influence behavior between
roles that are more tightly linked to the context than men and women in same sex groups with a gender-
gender, such as manager and employee in the work- neutral task, since gender status beliefs should not be
place, these more proximate roles control their beha- salient.
vior rather than gender stereotypes. Even in situations
where gender stereotypes do not control behavior,
however, men and women may still act slightly
differently due to their gender differentiated skills. 2.3 Structural Identity Theories
Social role theory has a broad scope that applies to
A set of symbolic interactionist theories, including
interaction in all contexts and addresses assertive,
Heise and Smith-Lovin’s affect control theory and
power related behaviors as well as supportive or feeling
Burke’s identity theory forms the structural identity
related behaviors (called socioemotional behaviors).
approach (see Ridgeway and Smith-Lovin (1999) for a
The explanations offered by the theory are not highly
review). It, too, emphasizes shared cultural meanings
specific or detailed, however. The theory predicts that
about gender but focuses on the identity standards
women will generally act more communally and less
those beliefs create for individuals in groups. People
instrumentally than men in the same context, that
learn cultural meanings about what it is to be
these differences will be greatest when gender is highly
masculine or feminine and these meanings become a
salient in the situation, and that gender differences will
personal gender identity standard that they seek to
be weak or absent when people enact formal, institu-
maintain through their actions. Identity standards act
tional roles.
like control systems that shape behavior. If the context
of interaction causes a person to seem more masculine
or feminine than his or her gender identity standard,
the person reacts with compensatory behaviors (e.g.,
2.2 Expectations States Theory warm behaviors to correct a too masculine impres-
sion). Consequently, different actions serve to express
Another major approach, Berger and co-worker’s
and maintain gender identities in different situational
expectation states theory, offers more detailed ex-
contexts.
planations within a narrower scope. The theory
Since people automatically sex categorize one an-
addresses the hierarchies of influence and esteem that
other, this approach assumes that gender identity
develop among group members in goal-oriented con-
standards affect behavior in all interaction, although
texts and makes predictions about when and how
the extent of their impact varies with gender’s salience
gender will shape these hierarchies due to the status
in the context. Gender is often a background identity
value gender carries in society (see Ridgeway 1993;
that modifies other, more situationally prominent
Wagner and Berger 1997). It does not address socio-
identities, such as woman judge. Unlike the other
emotional behavior. Gender status beliefs are cultural
theories, the predictions of structural identity theories
beliefs that one gender (men) is more status worthy
focus primarily on the behavioral reactions gender
and generally more competent than the other (women)
produces to events in small groups.
in addition to each having gender specific competen-
cies. When gender status beliefs become salient due to
the group’s mixed sex or gender associated context,
they create implicit expectations in both men and
2.4 Two-cultures Theory
women about the likely competence of goal oriented
suggestions from a man compared to those from a Maltz and Borker’s (1982) two cultures theory, popu-
similar woman. These often unconscious expectations larized by Tannen (1990), takes a different approach.
shape men and women’s propensity to offer their ideas It limits its scope to informal, friendly interaction.
to the group, to stick with those ideas when others People learn rules for friendly conversation from peers
disagree, to positively evaluate the ideas of others, and in childhood, it argues. Since these peer groups tend to

14186
Small-group Interaction and Gender

be sex-segregated and because children exaggerate polarized than the dominant beliefs find that gender
gender differences in the process of learning gender differences in interaction are also less for these
roles, boys and girls groups develop separate cultures populations (Filardo 1996).
that are gender-typed. Girls learn to use language to
form bonds of closeness and equality, to criticize in
nonchallenging ways, and to accurately interpret the
intentions of others. Boys learn to use speech to
3.2 Empirical Patterns in North American Groups
compete for attention and assert positions of domi-
nance. In adult mixed sex groups, these rules can cause Taking these methodological concerns into account,
miscommunication because men and women have narrative and meta-analytic reviews of research sug-
learned to attribute different meanings to the same gest several provisional conclusions about gender and
behavior. Men and women’s efforts to accommodate interaction in groups governed by the dominant
each other in mixed-sex interaction, however, modifies culture of North American society. Aries (1996),
their behavior slightly, reducing gender differences. In Deaux and La France (1998), and Ridgeway and
same sex interaction, gendered styles of interaction are Smith-Lovin (1999 provide reviews of the research on
reinforced. Thus, two cultures theory predicts greater which these conclusions are based.
gender differences in behavior between men and Gender differences in behavior do not always occur
women in same-sex groups than in mixed sex groups. in small groups and vary greatly by context. Beha-
The theory has been criticized for ignoring status and vioral expectations associated with the specific focus
power differences between men and women and and institutional context of the small group (e.g., the
oversimplifying childhood interaction patterns (see workplace, a committee, a friendship group, a student
Aries 1996). group) generally are more powerful determinants of
both men’s and women’s behavior than gender. When
gender differences occur, they tend to be small or
moderate in effect size, meaning that there is usually at
3. Research Findings least a 70 percent overlap in the distributions of men’s
and women’s behavior.
The body of systematic evidence about men and When men and women are in formal, prescribed
women’s behaviors in small group interaction is large roles with the same power and status, there are few if
and growing. Several methodological concerns must any differences in their behavior. Research has shown
be kept in mind in order to interpret this evidence and that men and women in equivalent leadership or
infer general patterns. managerial roles interact similarly with subordinates
of either sex. On the other hand, when women are
gender atypical occupants of positions of power, they
are sometimes perceived by others as less legitimate in
3.1 Methodological Issues
those roles and elicit more negative evaluations when
Interaction in small groups is an inherently local they behave in a highly directive, autocratic way than
phenomenon that is embedded within larger socio- do equivalent men. These findings are in accord with
cultural structures and affected by many aspects of the predictions of social role theory and expectations
those structures besides gender. Three methodological states theory.
problems result. First, care is required to ascertain that Influence over others and assertive, goal-directed
behavioral differences between men and women in a behavior such as participation rates, task suggestions,
situation are indeed due to their gender and not to and assertive patterns of gestures and eye gaze are
differences in their other roles, power, or statuses in associated with power and leadership in small groups.
the situation. Second, reasonably large samples of In mixed sex groups with a gender-neutral task, men
specific interactional behaviors are necessary to infer have moderately higher rates of assertive behaviors
gendered patterns in their use. Third, attention must and influence than do women who are otherwise their
be paid to the specific cultural context within in which peers. When the group task or context is culturally
the group is interacting. At present, almost all sys- linked to men, this gender difference increases. When
tematic research has been based on small groups in the the task or context is one associated with women,
US composed predominately of white, middle class however, women’s rates of assertive behaviors and
people. Since several theories emphasize the import- influence are slightly higher than men’s. When per-
ance of cultural beliefs about gender in shaping formance information clearly demonstrates that
interaction, researchers must be alert to subcultural women in a mixed sex group are as competent as the
and cross-cultural variations in these beliefs and men, gender differences in assertiveness and influence
appropriately condition their empirical generaliza- disappear. In same sex groups, there are no differences
tions about gendered patterns of interaction. The between men’s and women’s rates of assertive beha-
available studies that compare US populations such viors or influence levels. These patterns closely match
African–Americans whose gender beliefs are less the predictions of expectations states theory, are

14187
Small-group Interaction and Gender

consistent with social role theory, and inconsistent and power. Small group interaction is an arena in
with two cultures theory. They suggest that gender which the appearance of gender differences is con-
status beliefs in society and the expectations for tinually constructed through power and status rela-
competence in the situation that they create are an tions and identity marking in the socioemotional
important determinant of gender differences in power realm.
and assertiveness in groups, independent of men and Theory and research on gender and interaction have
women’s personalities or skills. They indicate as well focused on the way cultural beliefs about gender and
that both men and women act assertively or deferen- structural roles shape interaction in ways that confirm
tially depending on the situational context. the cultural beliefs. New approaches investigate the
Men, like women, show higher rates of socioemo- ways that interactional processes may perpetuate or
tional behaviors when they are in subordinate rather undermine gender inequality in a society as that society
than superordinate positions in groups. These are undergoes economic change. If the cultural beliefs
verbal and nonverbal behaviors that support the about gender that shape interaction change more
speech of others, express solidarity, and show active, slowly than economic arrangements, people inter-
attentive listenership. In mixed sex groups, women acting in gendered ways may rewrite gender inequality
engage in slightly more socioemotional behavior than into newly emerging forms of socioeconomic organi-
men. However, women engage in the highest rates of zation in society. On the other hand, rapidly changing
socioemotional behaviors, and men the lowest, in socioeconomic conditions may change the constraints
same sex groups. The latter findings are the only ones on interaction between men and women in many
in partial accord with the two-cultures theory. That contexts so that people’s experiences undermine con-
theory, however, does not account for the partial sensual beliefs about gender and alter them over time.
association of socioemotional behaviors with lower
status positions. See also: Androgyny; Cultural Variations in Inter-
Status factors alone, however, do not explain personal Relationships; Feminist Theory; Gender
women’s increased socioemotional behaviors in fe- and Feminist Studies in Psychology; Gender and
male groups. Assertive, instrumental behaviors appear Feminist Studies in Sociology; Gender and Language:
to reflect power, competence, and status equally for Cultural Concerns; Gender Differences in Persona-
men and women and, thus, do not reliably mark
lity and Social Behavior; Gender Ideology: Cross-
gender identity for the actor. To the extent that people
signal gender identity consistently across interaction cultural Aspects; Gender-related Development;
contexts, they appear to do so primarily through Groups, Sociology of; Interactionism: Symbolic;
socioemotional behaviors that are less associated with Interpersonal Attraction, Psychology of; Language
instrumental outcomes. and Gender; Male Dominance; Masculinities and
Femininities; Social Networks and Gender; Social
Psychology: Sociological; Social Psychology, Theories
of; Social Relationships in Adulthood; Stereotypes,
Social Psychology of
4. Conclusions
Both the gender division of labor and gender in-
equality in a society depend on its cultural beliefs
about the nature and social value of gender differences Bibliography
in competencies and traits. Such taken for granted
Aries E 1996 Men and Women in Interaction: Reconsidering the
beliefs allow actors to be reliably categorized as men Differences. Oxford University Press, New York
and women in all contexts and understood as more or Deaux K, LaFrance M 1998 Gender. In: Gilbert D T, Fiske S T,
less appropriate candidates for different roles and Lindzey G (eds.) The Handbook of Social Psychology, 4th edn.
positions in society. For such cultural beliefs to persist, McGraw-Hill, Boston, Vol. 1, pp. 788–827
people’s everyday interactions must be organized to Eagly A H 1987 Sex Differences in Social Behaior: A Social-
support them. The empirical evidence from North Role Interpretation. Earlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ
America suggests that unequal role and status rela- Filardo E K 1996 Gender patterns in African-American and
tionships produce many differences in interactional white adolescents’ social interactions in same-race, mixed-sex
behavior that are commonly attributed to gender. groups. Journal of Personal Social Psychology 71: 71–82
Network research suggests that most interactions Maltz D N, Borker R A 1982 A cultural approach to male-
female miscommunication. In: Gumperz J J (ed.) Language
between men and women actually occur within the
and Social Identity. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge,
structural context of unequal role or status relations UK, pp. 196–256
(see Ridgeway and Smith-Lovin 1999). These points Parsons T, Bales R F 1955 Family, Socialization, and Interaction
together may account for the fact that people perceive Process. Free Press, Glencoe, IL
gender differences to be pervasive in interaction, while Ridgeway C L 1993 Gender, status, and the social psychology of
studies of actual interaction show few behavioral expectations. In: England P (ed.) Theory on Gender\Feminism
differences between men and women of equal status on Theory. A de Gruyter, New York

14188
Smith, Adam (1723–90)

Ridgeway C L, Smith-Lovin L 1999 The gender system and Philosophy, Hutcheson succeeded him in 1729. A
interaction. Annual Reiew of Sociology 25: 191–216 great stylist, Hutcheson lectured in English (rather
Tannen D 1990 You Just Don’t Understand: Women and Men in than Latin) 3 days a week on classical sources and 5
Conersation. Morrow, New York
days on Natural Religion, Morals, Jurisprudence, and
Wagner D G, Berger J 1997 Gender and interpersonal task
behaviors: Status expectation accounts. Sociological Perspec- Government. As Dugald Stewart was to observe,
ties 40: 1–32 Hutcheson’s lectures contributed to diffuse, in
West C, Zimmerman D 1987 Doing gender. Gender and Society Scotland, the taste for analytical discussion, and a
1: 125–51 spirit of liberal enquiry.
It is believed that Smith graduated from Glasgow in
C. L. Ridgeway 1740 and known that he was elected to the Snell
Exhibition (scholarship) in the same year. He matric-
Copyright # 2001 Elsevier Science Ltd. ulated in Balliol College on July 7, and did not return
All rights reserved. to Scotland until 1746—the year that ended the
Jacobite Rebellion and saw the death of Hutcheson.
The 6 years spent in Oxford were often unhappy.
Smith, Adam (1723–90) Smith had a retiring personality, his health was poor,
and the College was pro-Jacobite and ‘anti-Scottish.’
Adam Smith was born in Kirkcaldy, in the County of Some members were also ‘unenlightened’; a fact
Fife, and baptized on June 5, 1723 (the date of birth is confirmed by the confiscation by one tutor of David
unknown). He was the son of Adam Smith, Clerk to Hume’s Treatise on Human Nature.
the Court-Martial, later Comptroller of Customs in And yet Smith was to write to the Principal of
the town, and of Margaret Douglas of Strathendry. Glasgow University (Archibald Davidson), on the
occasion of his election as Lord Rector (letter 274,
dated November 16, 1787):
1. Early Years No man can owe greater obligations than I do to the
Smith entered Glasgow University in 1737—at the not University of Glasgow. They educated me, they sent me to
Oxford, soon after my return to Scotland they elected me one
uncommon age of 14. He was fortunate in his mother’s
of their members, and afterwards preferred me to another
choice of University and in the period of his at- office, to which the abilities and virtues of the never to be
tendance. The old, nonspecialised, system of ‘regent- forgotten Dr Hutcheson had given a superior degree of
ing’ had been replaced in the late 1720s by a new illustration.
arrangement whereby individuals professed a single
subject. There is little doubt that Smith benefited from The reference to Oxford was no gilded memory.
the teaching of Alexander Dunlop (Greek) and of Balliol had one of the best libraries in Oxford.
Robert Dick (Physics). Smith’s occasional ill-health may be explained by his
But two men in particular are worthy of note in view enthusiastic pursuit of its riches. It has been assumed
of Smith’s later interests. The first is Robert Simson that Smith developed an interest in Rhetoric and
(1687–1768) whom Smith later described as one of the Belles Lettres during this period, although it now
‘greatest mathematicians that I have ever had the seems likely, in view of his later career, that he also
honour to be known to, and I believe one of the two further developed longer standing interests in litera-
greatest that have lived in my time’ (TMS, III.2.20). ture, science, ethics, and jurisprudence.
Smith could well have acquired from Simson and
Matthew Stewart (father of Dugald) his early and
continuing interest in mathematics. Dugald Stewart 2. Professor in Glasgow
recalled in his memoir (Stewart 1977) that Smith’s
favorite pursuits while at university were mathematics Smith returned to Kirkcaldy in 1746 without any fixed
and natural philosophy. plan. But his wide-ranging interests must have been
Campbell and Skinner noted that ‘Simson’s interests known to his friends, three of whom arranged a
were shared generally in Scotland. From their stress program of public lectures which were delivered in
on Greek geometry the Scots built up a reputation for Edinburgh between 1748 and 1751. The three friends
their philosophical elucidation of Newtonian fluxions, were Robert Craigie of Glendoick, James Oswald of
notably in the Treatise on Fluxions (1724) by Colin Dunnikier, and Henry Home, Lord Kames.
Maclaurin (1698–1746), another pupil of Simson’s The lectures were of an extramural nature and
who held chairs of mathematics in Aberdeen and delivered to a ‘respectable auditory.’ They probably
Edinburgh (1982, p. 20). also included material on the history of science
But important as it undoubtedly was Simson’s (astronomy), jurisprudence, and economics.
influence upon Smith pales by comparison with that The success of Smith’s courses in Edinburgh no
exerted by Francis Hutcheson (1694–1746). A student doubt led to his appointment to the Glasgow Chair of
of Gerschom Carmichael, the first Professor of Moral Logic and Rhetoric in 1751—where once again he

14189

International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences ISBN: 0-08-043076-7

You might also like