Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Aggression and Violent Behavior 34 (2017) 166–177

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Aggression and Violent Behavior

Youth exposure to violence in the community: Towards a theoretical


framework for explaining risk and protective factors
Maria João Lobo Antunes a,⁎, Eileen M. Ahlin b
a
Department of Sociology, Anthropology and Criminal Justice, Towson University, 8000 York Road, Towson, MD 21252, United States
b
School of Public Affairs, Criminal Justice Program, Penn State Harrisburg, 777 W. Harrisburg Pike, Middletown, PA 17057, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Exposure to community violence (ETV-C) negatively impacts youth development and is associated with many
Received 2 February 2016 negative outcomes. Although attention has been paid to examining risk and protective factors that promote or
Received in revised form 11 October 2016 reduce ETV-C, many of the studies in this growing body of literature do not place predictive models within a the-
Accepted 18 January 2017
oretical framework. In this review, we argue that the routine activity theory and lifestyles perspectives (RAT/LS)
Available online 20 January 2017
within an ecological framework is a useful strategy for examining how a series of behaviors and choices enacted
Keywords:
by youth in their everyday lives affects their ETV-C. By focusing on the role of target suitability and capable guard-
Exposure to violence ianship within the neighborhood, family, peers, and individual levels of the mesosystem, we suggest scholars can
Criminological theory examine the relative salience of these various components to determine whether they serve to increase youth's
Ecological framework ETV-C or buffer against such experiences. We propose that the RAT/LS perspectives can not only be placed in an
Youth violence ecological framework, but it also provides effective tenets with which to explore ETV-C.
Routine activity theory © 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Contents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 167
1.1. Defining exposure to violence in the community . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 167
1.2. The need for theoretical perspectives . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 167
2. Rationale and utility of routine activity theory (RAT)/lifestyles (LS) perspectives . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 168
2.1. Relevance of RAT/lifestyles in the study of youth exposure to community violence . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 168
2.2. Contextualizing RAT/LS and youth ETV-C: A mesosystem approach . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 169
3. Applying an ecological framework to youth exposure to community violence . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 170
3.1. Community . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 171
3.1.1. Structural characteristics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 171
3.1.2. Collective efficacy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 172
3.2. Family and parenting . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 172
3.2.1. Family characteristics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 172
3.2.2. Parenting strategies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 172
3.3. Peers . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 173
3.3.1. Unstructured socializing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 173
3.3.2. Peer deviance . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 173
3.4. Individual characteristics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 174
3.4.1. Race/ethnicity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 174
3.4.2. Gender . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 174
3.4.3. Age . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 174
3.4.4. Personality characteristics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 175
4. Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 175
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 176

⁎ Corresponding author.
E-mail addresses: mantunes@towson.edu (M.J.L. Antunes), ema105@psu.edu (E.M. Ahlin).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2017.01.015
1359-1789/© 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
M.J.L. Antunes, E.M. Ahlin / Aggression and Violent Behavior 34 (2017) 166–177 167

1. Introduction 1.1. Defining exposure to violence in the community

Violence among youth continues to be a public health problem, and The concept of exposure to violence can have a variety of interpreta-
despite a decrease in murder rates in the United States since 1995 tions and meanings depending on the experience under scrutiny
(Federal Bureau of Investigation, 2015), assault (including homicide) (Guterman, Cameron, & Staller, 2000; Lynch, 2003; van Dulmen,
remains the second leading cause of death among 15- to 24-year olds Belliston, Flannery, & Singer, 2008). Exposure to violence in different
(Murphy, Xu, & Kochanek, 2012). While such direct violence is un- contexts often results in different outcomes (see Mrug, Loosier, &
doubtedly detrimental to youth health and psychological well-being, Windle, 2008; Mrug & Windle, 2010; Slopen, Fitzmaurice, Williams, &
there are also significant risks that plague the large number of youth Gilman, 2012), elevating the importance of concrete conceptualization.
who witness violence. Further, evidence also indicates that exposure The heterogeneity surrounding the definition of ETV-C deserves atten-
to violence in the community (ETV-C), direct and indirect victimization tion. What is meant by ETV-C, and how it differs from other forms of vi-
outside of the home, can have devastating consequences (see Osofsky, olence that may be experienced by youth, is addressed prior to
Wewers, Hann, & Fick, 1993; Salzinger, Feldman, Stockhammer, & examining the literature on the topic.
Hood, 2002). Unsurprisingly, the prevalence of ETV-C for 14–17 year The definition of ETV-C has certainly varied across research en-
olds, particularly among urban minority males (Buka, Stichick, deavors. Guterman et al. (2000) address issues of definition and include
Birdthistle, & Earls, 2001), is greater than individual experiences with a discussion of what constitutes “community” and “violence”. For the
direct victimization (Finkelhor, Turner, Shattuck, Hamby, & Kracke, current review, ETV-C includes violent events (for example being shot
2015). at, robbed, beaten, threatened, assaulted) experienced in the communi-
Dahlberg (1998) identified key risk factors of youth violence (perpe- ty, that specifically locates victimization, witnessing, or hearing of such
tration and victimization), and highlighted components consistent with events within this particular context. We move beyond direct victimiza-
an ecological framework: neighborhood, family, peers, and individual tion and include secondary ETV-C as the witnessing of violence directed
factors. Scholarly investigation of the multiple facets of ETV-C often em- against someone else and/or hearing about someone's victimization. In
braces an ecological framework by investigating the relevance of a com- essence, community violence, as studied here, follows definitions used
bination of these contexts. However, the application of an ecological by Selner-O'Hagan and colleagues (Selner-O'Hagan, Kindlon, Buka,
model is often piecemeal, as only some, but not all, of the various con- Raudenbush, & Earls, 1998), Gardner and Brooks-Gunn (2009), and
textually relevant factors are examined in empirical models. Although Gibson, Morris, and Beaver (2009) as violent victimization or violence
there is ample research on ETV-C, including several review articles, a that occurs in the neighborhood/community as is specifically identified
unifying theoretical base is often lacking and there is a paucity of re- by the youth as having happened within that particular context. We
search that has explored the utility of certain theoretical perspectives draw from Gibson et al. (2009) to include the “hearing of violence” in
in explaining youth experiences with neighborhood violence. Often- the neighborhood as a form of secondary ETV-C. However, contrary to
times investigations of ETV-C have included elements of particular the- others who have studied ETV-C, we do not include perpetration of vio-
ories like the routine activity theory/lifestyles perspective (RAT/LS), lence (Selner-O'Hagan et al., 1998) or burglary (Schwartz, Hopmeyer-
theoretical explanations of victimization, but have not specifically test- Gorman, Toblin, & Abou-ezzeddine, 2003). A key element of ETV-C is
ed whether these theories can help explain youth experiences with that it occurs outside of the home. By comparison, other forms of vio-
neighborhood violence. We propose that the RAT/LS perspective can lence such as intimate partner violence, family violence, and child mal-
not only be placed within an ecological framework, we also suggest treatment are more generally cloistered behind closed doors and less
that it provides effective tenets with which to explore ETV-C. accessible to the public. While these instances of violence are worthy
The present review covers studies that apply an ecological frame- of scientific inquiry, focusing on youth ETV-C is particularly imperative.
work – whether explicit or implicit – to explain the role of context in Neighborhoods are an important factor in shaping youth development,
youth ETV-C, as well as exploring RAT/LS as a theoretical basis for and engagement with the community increases as youth age, become
explaining youth ETV-C in multicontextual models. While not a system- more independent from their families, and form stronger ties with
atic review or meta-analysis of the literature, the current piece proposes their peers. This increased mobility and exposure to their neighborhood
a theoretical platform upon which to place investigation into the causes and surrounding communities increases the probability that youth will
of youth ETV-C. Essentially, this review summarizes research on several encounter environments where violence may occur, especially if they
mesosystem layers of the ecological framework (neighborhood, family, lack sufficient guardianship to protect them against ETV-C or have risk
peers, and the individual) as they pertain to youth ETV-C, and examines factors that inflate their target suitability for such harmful experiences
the current status of knowledge in this area, in addition to the relevance (Jensen & Brownfield, 1986; Miethe & Meier, 1994; Wilcox, Land, &
of RAT/LS theories within these layers. By summarizing the literature on Hunt, 2003). Youth are more likely to experience violence in their com-
youth ETV-C it is hoped that future studies will broaden their focus and munity, either as direct victims or by witnessing or hearing about
incorporate multiple contexts to explain ETV-C and approach the study others' victimization, than in their homes (see Finkelhor et al., 2015)
of youth ETV-C theoretically. The studies included examining exposure thereby elevating the need to focus on risk and protective factors of
to violence as an outcome variable and focus on children and youth.1 ETV-C among youth.

1.2. The need for theoretical perspectives

While there has been much research devoted to studying the dam-
1
We place children and adolescents under the unifying term - youth. Wilson and aging consequences of youth exposure to violence, including communi-
Rosenthal (2003) suggest such practice may be problematic given developmental differ- ty violence, there is also a growing body of literature focusing on ETV-C
ences between preadolescent and adolescent youth. However, unrestricted access to the
as an outcome variable though findings regarding what increases or at-
neighborhood is itself a developmental milestone marking strides in achieving autonomy.
Studies that look at ETV-C (Gibson et al. (2009) for example) often use samples that cap- tenuates risk have been mixed (Gardner & Brooks-Gunn, 2009; Lobo
ture differences in access to the community as a way of controlling for developmental dif- Antunes, 2012). There is substantial depth to the literature on the con-
ferences. We recognize that particular behaviors are certainly the product of sequences of youth ETV-C which highlights a multitude of negative out-
developmental pathways (e.g., friendships, unstructured socializing) and that younger comes across a variety of contexts. This in-depth focus of ETV-C as a
children are less likely to be exposed to community violence or have deviant friends. How-
ever, the results of survey research suggest that, by the middle years of childhood, “most
predictor of various dependent variables overshadows the need to ex-
inner-city children have already had firsthand encounters with serious acts of violence” amine risk and protective factors that explain ETV-C as an outcome.
(Schwartz et al., 2003, p. 39). Some scholars have begun to explore various neighborhood, family,
168 M.J.L. Antunes, E.M. Ahlin / Aggression and Violent Behavior 34 (2017) 166–177

peer, and individual contextual elements as buffers or predictors of RAT is often distilled to its three main components - motivated of-
youth ETV-C (see Gardner & Brooks-Gunn, 2009; Gibson et al., 2009; fender, suitable target, and lack of capable guardianship. A classic as-
Selner-O'Hagan et al., 1998), though these studies often focus on only sumption of the RAT is that motivated offenders are readily present
one or two mesosystem variables and lack a cohesive and unified theo- and provided with a steady supply of victims who are suitable targets
retical framework. We propose that the lack of consistent results stems and devoid of capable guardianship. Assuming motivated offenders
from the dearth of theoretical rationales and frameworks used in exam- are ubiquitous, what makes a target suitable for experiencing direct or
ining ETV-C. Furthermore, oftentimes variables used to predict youth indirect violent victimization in the community? Suitable targets in-
victimization and ETV-C are not placed within useful theoretical per- clude inanimate objects such as lightweight property available for
spectives which could help clarify processes and explain why some theft, the portable goods described by Cohen and Felson (1979). But in-
youth, compared to others, are more likely to experience violence in dividuals whose routines place them in the same environment as a mo-
their community. Maybe one of the greater difficulties has been to dis- tivated offender capable of violence and who view such goods as “target
entangle context from consequence. The utility of understanding the attractive” are equally enticing as targets to those who commit crime.
consequences of ETV-C is undeniable, but in order to better comprehend What characterizes individuals as suitable or attractive targets will de-
the phenomena that is exposure to violence outside of the home, espe- pend, in great part, on the intentions of the motivated offender, the
cially for youth, it is critical that studies explore in greater breadth and levels of guardianship, and the circumstances of the crime. There is
depth the causal mechanisms and pathways that predict ETV-C. Thus, agreement throughout the literature that certain individual attributes,
while consequences matter, we need a better grasp of the processes as suggested by lifestyles theory (Hindelang et al., 1978), make people
that predict youth experiences with neighborhood violence. By focusing more or less susceptible to victimization, especially when these same
on the relevant risk and protective factors in addition to the application characteristics are considered in conjunction with guardianship (Ahlin
of a theoretical framework, the design and creation of policies and prac- & Lobo Antunes, 2017). Target suitability is said to be diminished
tices that emphasize protective strategies, and in turn minimize risk fac- when capable guardians, including informal (e.g., neighbors) and for-
tors, can contribute to reducing not only risk but also the often-studied mal (e.g., police, security guards) social controls and environmental de-
harmful effects of ETV-C. terrents (e.g., closed-circuit television), are unavailable to monitor the
environment. Thus, like target suitability, guardianship can be activated
at different levels, from community (Wilcox, Madensen, & Tillyer, 2007)
2. Rationale and utility of routine activity theory (RAT)/lifestyles to the individual (Zimmerman & Messner, 2013) and encompasses a
(LS) perspectives wide range of behaviors like collective efficacy, parental supervision,
and self-control. Guardianship is the ability, or inability, to attenuate
The scarcity of studies that have used theory to explain youth ETV-C or limit exposure to noxious environments, events, and even individ-
stems, perhaps, from the overemphasis on correlates of victimization uals. According to Cohen and Felson (1979, p. 590) “guardianship is im-
and isolated explanatory mechanisms rather than the lack of a suitable plicit in everyday life and usually is marked by the absence of
theoretical framework. We posit that the RAT/LS perspective is one of violations”, but could also be the active prevention and/or protection
the most useful theories with which to explore ETV-C. Although this against crime and victimization.
perspective has not often been directly tested within such a scenario For a crime, and therefore victimization, to occur the motivated of-
several investigations of youth victimization and in some cases ETV-C fender, suitable target, and lack of guardianship must meet within a par-
have LS or elements from the perspective/LS or elements from the per- ticular time frame and geographical space (Cohen & Felson, 1979). This
spective (Ahlin & Lobo Antunes, 2017; Averdijk & Bernasco, 2015; theoretical proposition has inspired several reformulations and may
Finkelhor & Asdigian, 1996; Nofziger & Kurtz, 2005; Spano, Freilich, & prove to be a useful foundation upon which to study youth ETV-C. We
Bolland, 2008; Taylor, Freng, Esbensen, & Peterson, 2008; Tillyer, argue ETV-C which is a form of victimization, is the product of a series
Tillyer, Miller, & Pangrac, 2011). We start by highlighting the theoretical of behaviors and choices enacted by individuals in the pursuit of their
foundations of RAT/LS, beginning with the lifestyles theory of everyday, routine existence. It is these elements of RAT/LS that can be
victimization. useful as a framework for explaining ETV-C as an outcome and clarifying
The lifestyles theory (Hindelang, Gottfredson, & Garofalo, 1978) fo- critical risk and protective factors that impact youth ETV-C, particularly
cuses on how activities in which individuals engage during the course considering the implied or assumed role of the motivated offender.
of their everyday lives contribute to the likelihood of victimization.
Such activities include going to bars, spending time away from the 2.1. Relevance of RAT/lifestyles in the study of youth exposure to community
home, and working outside the home. Furthermore, demographics violence
like age, marital status, race/ethnicity, and gender seem to shape these
daily routine activities. For instance, Miethe and Meier (1994, p. 32) The utility of the RAT/LS framework in studying youth ETV-C rests
point out, that these characteristics are often associated with certain most specifically on the relevance of concepts like target suitability
patterns of behavior and the formation of relationships that can invari- and guardianship. ETV-C, whether it is from direct victimization or vi-
ably “enhance one's exposure to risky and vulnerable situations”. This carious victimization such as hearing about or witnessing of a violent
exposure, therefore, translates to higher probabilities of victimization event, may be clarified using a framework that incorporates an ecolog-
as well as witnessing of violent events. Lifestyles can, even if only to a ical perspective. Miethe, Stafford, and Long (1987, p. 185) posit that “in-
certain extent, help explain differences in risks of ETV-C. Individuals dividuals who spend more time away from home should have higher
who spend more time away from their home engage in unstructured so- risks of victimization because of their greater suitability as a target
cializing with their peers, and who interact with deviant friends, are (i.e., greater visibility and accessibility) and decreased guardianship”.
more susceptible to ETV-C. Susceptibility is enhanced because of Further, the capacity to exert guardianship or even minimize target suit-
lowered guardianship and increased target suitability. Hindelang and ability rests often on the actions and lifestyles of the youth themselves,
colleagues emphasized the more micro-individual routine activities ap- who they spend their time with, and whether they are out in the neigh-
proach, but it was Cohen and Felson's (1979) description of how “rou- borhood without adult supervision and monitoring.
tine activities which include formalized work as well as the provision When a youth experiences direct or vicarious victimization several
of standard food, shelter, sexual outlet, leisure, social interaction, learn- mechanisms operated simultaneously for the exposure to actually
ing, and childrearing” (p. 593) could influence victimization rates that come to fruition. Firstly, the exposure necessitates, in most cases, a mo-
placed RAT on the horizon as a major theoretical explanation of tivated offender without which there would be no incidence of violence.
victimization. The very fact that the motivated offender is a given, shifts attention
M.J.L. Antunes, E.M. Ahlin / Aggression and Violent Behavior 34 (2017) 166–177 169

towards target suitability and guardianship, the characteristics, and offenders (Cohen & Felson, 1979). Even though Hindelang et al.
contexts related to those who are victimized and exposed to violence. (1978) proposed a much more individual-level approach for explaining
Secondly, youth can enhance their target suitability by making con- victimization in examining how features of one's lifestyle may place an
scious choices regarding who to spend time with and where to socialize. individual at more or less risk for violence, in the end both perspectives
And finally, the idea of guardianship can be thought as having multiple are somewhat unidimensional and do not incorporate cross-level risk or
levels with external capable guardians (for example the neighborhood, protective factors to explain victimization. There has not been much in-
family, and peers) inhibiting ETV-C coupled with the youth's ability to vestigation dedicated to unravelling the layers within the RAT/LS per-
protect themselves against victimization or witnessing of violent spective, nor demonstrating that, essentially, guardianship and target
events. suitability may be exerted at several levels in the mesosystem. The
Which brings us to the question: What specifically about youth is same is true of the presence of motivated offenders. Certain neighbor-
more or less likely to put them at risk of ETV-C? One explanation is hoods may provide more or less opportunity for these individuals, espe-
that youth are often left unsupervised during the hours between school cially in neighborhoods lacking in guardianship and social control.
and when parents arrive home from work. The literature on afterschool Neighborhoods that lack informal social controls such as collective effi-
victimization is extensive (Flannery, Williams, & Vazsonyi, 1999; cacy convey to motivated offenders that guardianship is absent or atten-
Gottfredson, Gottfredson, & Weisman, 2001; Snyder & Sickmund, uated when physical and social disorder such as graffiti, vice, and
1999). It is during these hours that youth are freer to associate with general unkemptness remain unchecked (Sampson & Raudenbush,
their friends, and are away from the protective supervision (guardian- 2001; Sampson, Raudenbush, & Earls, 1997).
ship) that parents and caregivers can provide. Their daily routine activ- Using an ecological framework (Bronfenbrenner, 1979), we argue
ities and their lifestyles enhance opportunities and therefore risk for that RAT/LS can be reframed into a mesosystemic context whereby
ETV-C not only because of their suitability as a target and lack of capable the motivated offender, guardianship, and target suitability are exam-
guardianship, but also because of the relationships the youths them- ined at distinct levels with respect to ETV-C. Susceptibility to ETV-C
selves cultivate with each other. For instance, while there is much should be considered from a multifaceted lens that encompasses the
known about peer deviance and how it impacts youth engagement in multitude of contexts and situations that may influence not only the
crime and delinquency (Kirk, 2006; Maimon & Browning, 2010; youth's activities, behaviors, and choice-driven actions but also environ-
Osgood & Anderson, 2004), we also know that these associations may ments which put them in direct contact with motivated offenders. An
place children and youth in more vulnerable situations with respect to ecological framework encompasses the interconnections of various set-
violent events (Averdijk & Bernasco, 2015). These deviant relationships tings – such as the neighborhood, family, peers, and individual traits –
simultaneously increase occasions where youth become suitable targets that influence how individuals conduct their daily lives and respond
while also hindering capable guardianship. In essence, opportunities for to situations. Referred to as microsystems within this framework, to-
unstructured socializing (Maimon & Browning, 2010; Osgood & gether these settings comprise the larger mesosystem, or multiple con-
Anderson, 2004; Osgood, Wilson, O'Malley, Bachman, & Johnston, texts, in which people live (Bronfenbrenner, 1979, 1992). This
1996), time spent with friends without a set agenda or the protective mesosystem represents the linkages between various microsystems
monitoring and supervision of adults and caregivers, is more likely to and evidence suggests that these contexts influence youth ETV-C. Indi-
expose these youth to motivated offenders given that “supervision is a viduals are nested in social contexts (Bronfenbrenner, 1979) and their
protective factor and participation in deviant lifestyles is a risk factor experiences and choices are situated in the environment (Hitlin &
for violent victimization” (Spano et al., 2008, p. 383). Elder, 2007). Therefore, concepts central to RAT/LS are placed within
Just as peer relationships comfortably fit into a RAT/LS perspective four tiered levels - the neighborhood, the family, peers, and the youth
and can contribute to explaining youth ETV-C, parenting plays a critical themselves. As Ahlin and Lobo Antunes (2017, p. 64) suggest, “individ-
role in enhancing guardianship and reducing target suitability such that uals are nested in a variety of social contexts and their routine activities
youth are protected from violent events occurring within the communi- and lifestyle choices are influenced by multiple systems”. This is espe-
ty. Time youth spend away from the home and beyond the watchful cially valid when examining the mechanisms that exacerbate or attenu-
eyes of parents and caregivers provides them with the unregulated op- ate youth ETV-C.
portunity to explore their surroundings and forge relationships within The very idea of capable guardianship can be extended to the neigh-
the neighborhood. Thus, parental management strategies designed to borhood, family, peers, and even youth, beyond what has been usually
organize a youth's activities outside the home as well as rules regarding thought of as individual-level characteristics. Concepts like neighbor-
unfettered access to the community serve to strengthen guardianship, hood collective efficacy (Sampson et al., 1997), a community's willing-
limit access to motivated offenders, and diminish opportunities where ness and ability to enforce social control and nurture social cohesion
the youth may become a suitable target in terms of victimization prob- certainly serves a guardianship function in not only fostering protection
ability and secondary exposure to violence. and prevention but also exerting or attempting to exert control over
Youth also have considerable control over their environments by motivated offenders. Communities high in collective efficacy will, there-
enacting a variety of choices such as where and with whom to associate fore, not want for capable guardianship, which itself should impact
and spend time that affect their routine activities and lifestyle. These in- youth ETV-C and serve to buffer youth against such exposure. Moreover,
dependent predictors of ETV-C demonstrate the various mesosystem community conditions can themselves inform parents and guide
factors (neighborhood, family, peers, and individual) that influence choices made regarding management practices and active protection
youths' risk of direct and indirect victimization in the community, from environments deemed potentially harmful, or conversely, less re-
while also demonstrating the utility of RAT/LS as a unifying theoretical strictive parenting in community seen as less risky (Fig. 1).
perspective for multilevel models that integrate various risk and protec- The family and youths' peers are present in the mesosystem layer
tive factors of ETV-C. Before examining the extant literature on ETV-C as between community and youth. Similar to communities, parenting
an outcome variable, we explore the theoretical underpinnings of RAT/ and family management strategies act as another level of guardianship
LS and their fit within an ecological framework capable of explaining or protection, and also function to limit the target suitability of youth.
ETV-C. Family management is the host of strategies, practices, and activities
that parents engage in or organize to structure their children's daily
2.2. Contextualizing RAT/LS and youth ETV-C: A mesosystem approach lives in and outside the home (Eccles, 1992). Furstenberg, Cook,
Eccles, Elder, and Sameroff (1999) divide these practices into those
Traditionally, RAT has emphasized the macro-level nature and utility that apply within the home and those outside the home. Cohen and
of guardianship, target suitability, and the presence of motivated Felson (1979, p. 594) argued that “routine activities performed within
170 M.J.L. Antunes, E.M. Ahlin / Aggression and Violent Behavior 34 (2017) 166–177

Fig. 1. Ecological model of routine activity and lifestyles theory perspectives.

or near the home and among family members or other primary groups framework. We then conclude with suggestions for embracing RAT/LS
entail lower risk of criminal victimization because they enhance guard- as a theoretical framework for multilevel explanations of youth ETV-C.
ianship capabilities”. Of particular interest to youth ETV-C, management
outside the home underscores the importance of controlling youth ac- 3. Applying an ecological framework to youth exposure to commu-
cess to the community, monitoring practices when children are away nity violence
from the home, and curtailing relationships with deviant peers. Interac-
tions with peers can also contribute to reduced levels of guardianship To fully explore and examine the several processes that impact
and increased target suitability, particularly among youth who spend youth ETV-C a multilevel, ecological approach should be employed. In
time with deviant peers and engage in unstructured socializing. Deviant recent years more attention has been given to the role neighborhoods
peers may not serve as capable guardians to buffer against direct play in youth outcomes and more studies have been dedicated to inves-
and vicarious victimization as they are involved in delinquent and tigating youth experiences with victimization and violence in the neigh-
criminal activities themselves. Such relationships and involvement in borhood, especially from an ecological perspective (Ahlin & Lobo
unsupervised and aimless activities can also increase target suitability Antunes, 2017; Gardner & Brooks-Gunn, 2009; Gibson et al., 2009).
by placing youth in risky environments prone to victimization The very nature of ETV-C necessitates an ecological analysis that is com-
(Maimon & Browning, 2010; Osgood & Anderson, 2004; Osgood et al., prised of mesosystemic levels that differentially influence ETV-C. Neigh-
1996). borhood context, family relations, peers, and even individual
Finally, we suggest that youth can and often do serve as their own characteristics have been demonstrated to significantly predict youth
guardian, participating in behaviors and developing traits that can min- ETV-C (Ahlin & Lobo Antunes, 2017; Lobo & Ahlin Antunes, 2015;
imize ETV-C. For example, the development of self-control as a product Salzinger et al., 2002). Moreover, each context makes unique contribu-
of parenting (Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990) or the establishment of an tions to explaining why some individuals are more likely to experience
internal locus of control (Ahlin & Lobo Antunes, 2015) serve to inform victimization and/or witness violence in their community. However, not
choices youth make in their daily lives. Moreover, youth may also many studies have sought to use an ecological framework, especially
have control over their own susceptibility as a target through peer inter- not a RAT/LS ecological perspective for examining ETV-C.
actions and unstructured socializing. The choice of peer relationships To date there have been two comprehensive reviews of youth ETV
will inevitably influence guardianship and target suitability through ex- (Jonson-Reid, 1998; Salzinger et al., 2002). Both use an ecological frame-
posure to motivated offenders - which are often the peers themselves work to review the research on youth violence, youth ETV, and/or the
(Averdijk & Bernasco, 2015; Richards et al., 2004) which will, in turn, af- consequences of ETV. In her 1998 review piece, Jonson-Reid incorporat-
fect the likelihood of ETV-C. Moreover, family management practices ed both youth violence perpetration and ETV-C. Rather than emphasize
themselves influence the youth and the establishment of individual the contexts for each, the author specifically examines research on the
guardianship mechanisms like locus of control or self-control (Ahlin & relationship between violence perpetration and three separate domains
Lobo Antunes, 2015) but may also shape the decisions youth make of violence exposure: child maltreatment, domestic violence, and com-
with regard to friends and time spent away from the home. munity violence. Interestingly, Jonson-Reid (1998) organizes the review
These various layers of the mesosystem have been examined in the by microsystem and macrosystem, but without considering any possi-
extant literature on youth ETV-C, often as covariates of the various ad- ble overlap between the two. For example, when discussing the litera-
verse outcomes of ETV-C outlined earlier, though more recently as pre- ture on child maltreatment (exposure to violence within the home)
dictors of ETV-C. In the next section, we review the evidence of she includes the role of poverty at the micro-level (parental socioeco-
neighborhood, family, peers, and individual factors that fit an ecological nomic status) but does not examine what effect macro-level poverty/
M.J.L. Antunes, E.M. Ahlin / Aggression and Violent Behavior 34 (2017) 166–177 171

concentrated disadvantage may have on child maltreatment. Elder indicators of poverty, concentrated disadvantage, and ethnic diversity
(1994) and Furstenberg et al. (1999) note that parenting and parent- are used (Buka et al., 2001; Selner-O'Hagan et al., 1998). While the liter-
child relationships may vary according to the economic affluence of a ature cited here, as well as other studies, describe community risk fac-
community above and beyond poverty at the individual level. Thus, in tors that should be taken into account when studying ETV-C they do
addition to individual-level SES, neighborhood SES itself may affect not adequately address questions of why contextual factors matter.
child maltreatment. With respect to community violence, Jonson-Reid In an urban sample of African American youth (low income and mid-
(1998) looks at outcomes of ETV and how these, along with exposure it- dle class) Fitzpatrick and Boldizar (1993) reported that environmental
self, influence violence perpetration. Her approach to the ecological characteristics like percentage of female-headed households predicted
framework is unique in that she considers the contextual mesosystemic higher levels of post-traumatic stress disorder among youth experienc-
location of where exposure and violence perpetration occur rather than ing ETV-C. Relatedly, youth living in areas high in concentrated disad-
what mesosystemic domains influence these outcomes. The review, vantage (areas high in percentage of residents living below poverty,
nonetheless, provided one of the first discussions on how youth vio- on public assistance, unemployed, less than 18 years of age, and African
lence, whether perpetration or exposure, should be examined within a American) have a statistically significantly higher risk of ETV-C (Lobo
multilevel context, even if it did not include literature on predicting or Antunes & Ahlin, 2015). However, understanding why these contextual
explaining ETV-C, or theories that may help scholars understand these variables are important and why these factors increase risk for ETV-C
relationships. and detrimental sequelae resulting from ETV-C remains an issue for fu-
Salzinger et al. (2002) build on the Jonson-Reid study to examine lit- ture research. From a RAT/LS perspective we could argue that such areas
erature that predicts and explains ETV-C by considering both risk for ex- are more inviting to the motivated offender and that in conditions of
posure and the effects exposure has on youth within five distinct poverty parents are required to maintain several jobs which may hinder
contexts - neighborhood, family, parents, peers, and youth characteris- guardianship (Simons, Lorenz, Wu, & Conger, 1993) thus increasing tar-
tics. The authors find that there is much evidence for direct and indirect get suitability. Similarly, the proportion of female-headed or
influences of these domains on youth violence and victimization. The monoparental households within a community can impede the forma-
focus on ETV-C as well as the consequences of said violence lays impor- tion of friendship networks and community bonds that in turn reduce,
tant foundational precedence for the current piece as Salzinger et al. according to Zimmerman and Messner (2010) and Sampson and
(2002) adopt a multilevel, ecological framework in their review of the Lauritsen (1994), informal social control, a form of guardianship. Yet,
literature. Further, they begin at the most distal point, the neighbor- in many of the studies that incorporate structural characteristics, the
hood; an approach later seen in other studies of ETV-C (Ahlin & Lobo causal mechanisms and the processes that may explain why these char-
Antunes, 2017; Gardner & Brooks-Gunn, 2009; Gibson et al., 2009). acteristics are important are often overlooked and not placed within a
The authors move from outside the mesosystem (macro-level) to the specific theoretical framework.
inside (micro-level) and conclude that “there is overwhelming evidence The nexus between neighborhood structural characteristics and
that the effects of exposure cannot be understood without taking into youth ETV-C may rest on the potential influence these conditions
account the context - environmental and person - in which the expo- have, therefore, on parental and community ability to exert social con-
sure occurs” (p. 445). trol, which in our view, ties in with a RAT/LS perspective and the idea
In the following sections, we extend this argument and update the that guardianship is simply another way of defining social control
literature, bridging a 14-year gap, by proposing that ETV-C itself must (Ahlin & Lobo Antunes, 2017). For example, Gibson et al. (2009) expand
also be understood within a multilevel, ecological framework that con- on the ETV-C literature by investigating risk factors for secondary (hear-
siders how neighborhood, family, peers, and individual characteristics ing and witnessing) exposure to violence. Using data from the Project
serve to enhance, predict, and explain youth experiences with neigh- on Human Development in Chicago Neighborhoods (PHDCN) the au-
borhood violence. We take this approach one step further in examining thors examine neighborhood structural characteristics of disadvantage,
these relationships from a RAT/LS approach and placing the literature immigrant concentration, and residential stability but also include col-
with this theoretical framework. lective efficacy and parenting measures. They find that youth living in
communities with higher levels of poverty and immigrant concentra-
3.1. Community tion experienced higher incidences of secondary ETV-C. Moreover,
Gibson et al. (2009) conclude that neighborhood characteristics influ-
Much of the literature on youth ETV-C has investigated the predic- ence ETV-C even after controlling for family and individual risk factors.
tive effects of family and individual-level characteristics. As Buka et al. Of note, was the lack of significant findings regarding parental supervi-
(2001) among others find, less is understood about how neighborhood sion. We discuss the role of parenting further on in this review, though
conditions may influence youth ETV-C, and there are several plausible we posit that if parental guardianship measures like supervision are not
explanations as to why this is less prominent in the current literature. contextualized, meaning placed within the home or outside the house-
Firstly, the paucity of data permitting a true ecological analysis of hold, we may be missing important causal mechanisms and failing to
youth experiences with neighborhood violence has impeded its prog- understand how RAT/LS can be a useful theoretical perspective.
ress. Secondly, neighborhood research examining community effects Along with the same structural characteristics used by Gibson et al.
on youth development has been inconclusive with some studies (2009), Gardner and Brooks-Gunn (2009) examine how the presence
reporting a direct influence of neighborhood characteristics on youth of youth community organizations impact youth ETV-C. Their study
behaviors while others show an indirect effect (Leventhal & Brooks- controls not only for structural characteristics but also for community
Gunn, 2000). Further, some studies have found no neighborhood effects violence. The authors report that youth ETV-C is less frequent in neigh-
at all (Kellam, Ling, Merisca, Brown, & Ialongo, 1998; Kupersmidt, borhoods with a greater variety of youth organizations and activities
Griesler, DeRosier, Patterson, & Davis, 1995; Lynam et al., 2000; and make an interesting point concerning why this may be the case.
Spencer & Dornbusch, 1990). Those studies that do incorporate neigh- The popular assumption is that participation in such organizations de-
borhood level variables focus on various structural characteristics and creases youth unstructured socializing, an activity we argue could in-
collective efficacy. crease youth target suitability. Instead, Gardner and Brooks-Gunn
(2009) suggest that neighborhood youth organizations inhibit violence
3.1.1. Structural characteristics at the neighborhood level which therefore decreases youth ETV-C. This
There has been scant investigation, in particular studies that look is congruent with the multi-mesosystemic RAT/LS approach we advo-
specifically at youth ETV-C, extending neighborhood conditions beyond cate for here. The variety and availability of youth organizations denotes
the traditional neighborhood structural characteristics. Oftentimes a level of investment in and commitment to the community, which we
172 M.J.L. Antunes, E.M. Ahlin / Aggression and Violent Behavior 34 (2017) 166–177

contend, may also be considered a protective factor that deters crime at with family characteristics and is followed by a review of the parenting
the neighborhood level (Gardner & Brooks-Gunn, 2009), and serve as a strategies literature.
type of neighborhood-level guardianship and social control. There is, as
the authors point out, a limiting of opportunities for violent crime which
3.2.1. Family characteristics
also curbs opportunities for exposure to said violence. Gardner and
Family characteristics have been used to evaluate youth exposure to
Brooks-Gunn's (2009) investigation illustrates how criminal opportuni-
violence (Gorman-Smith, Henry, & Tolan, 2004). Female headed house-
ty structures can occur at different levels of the mesosystem and its ef-
holds, living arrangements, and family SES are some of the more com-
fects can cascade downward from community to individual. Examining
monly examined variables (Buka et al., 2001; Jonson-Reid, 1998).
how concepts of RAT/LS can shape these opportunity structures not
Richters and Martinez (1993) reported that whether youth lived in
only provides a theoretical foundation for studying youth experiences
houses versus apartments had a significant predictive effect on youth
with neighborhood violence but also for establishing inter-level pro-
ETV-C, as youth who lived in houses were more likely to witness vio-
cesses within an ecological framework.
lence. From a theoretical standpoint, the RAT/LS perspective provides
a basis upon which to explain these findings. Guardianship, especially
3.1.2. Collective efficacy
informal guardianship, may be more pervasive in an apartment com-
The macro level perspective of collective efficacy blends informal so-
plex where neighbors may be more attuned to their surroundings
cial control and social cohesion to explain reductions in crime rates in
than among single family homes which are less interdependent struc-
communities (see Sampson et al., 1997). Although collective efficacy is
turally and perhaps socially, even if only informally. Financial hardship
not explicitly a component of the ecological framework, it would fall
and family SES have also been demonstrated to have a negative effect
under Bronfenbrenner's (1979) idea of the mesosystem and how child
on youth witnessing of violence (Crouch, Hanson, Saunders, Kilpatrick,
experiences, including ETV-C, are influenced by the community. While
& Resnick, 2000). Crouch et al. (2000) used family income as a measure
the use of an ecological framework is not clearly stated in research ex-
of SES and showed that youth, especially White youth, from families
amining the influence of collective efficacy on ETV-C, coupled with the
with higher SES witnessed fewer incidences of violence, assault, and
RAT/LS theoretical perspectives it does provide a community-level
harsh disciplining. Bell and Jenkins (1993) demonstrated that family
guardianship variable (see Ahlin & Lobo Antunes, 2017). Collective effi-
characteristics like parental structure impacted youth outcomes. The
cacy serves an external guardianship role in the community where
authors concluded that youth from homes with fewer caregivers pres-
neighbors watch over each other's children and provide informal social
ent (i.e., female-headed households) were more likely to be involved
controls over their behaviors to monitor truancy, vandalism, and other
in violence. Similarly, Esbensen and Huizinga (1991) found that youth
delinquent acts (Kirk, 2009; Sampson, Morenoff, & Raudenbush, 2005;
living in two-parent households were less likely to be victimized, espe-
Sampson et al., 1997). Collective efficacy is theoretically a buffer against
cially compared to those living in single-parent homes, suggesting from
ETV-C, and is one of the most robust macro level predictors of crime
a RAT/LS perspective that guardianship may be reduced in
(Pratt & Cullen, 2005).
monoparental homes.
Sampson et al. (1997) introduce the concept of collective efficacy
Like much of the literature on victimization and exposure to vio-
and demonstrate that higher levels of collective efficacy are significantly
lence, there are studies that find no effects of family characteristics.
associated with less neighborhood violence. Such reductions in violent
For example, in their study of pre-teen and teen African-American and
crime rates in neighborhoods with high collective efficacy may translate
Latino males, Gorman-Smith and Tolan (1998) failed to uncover signif-
to fewer opportunities for youth to experience violence in their commu-
icant effects of family organization and other family characteristics on
nities. Few studies examine the relationship between collective efficacy
youth experiences with violence. It is possible that for these populations
and ETV-C, and most research in support of collective efficacy suggests it
neighborhood social processes play a greater factor in ETV-C than family
is a significant predictor of violence at the macro level, but not necessar-
characteristics. These investigations serve to emphasize the need to es-
ily at the micro-level. Building on the original collective efficacy study,
tablish theoretical linkages between family characteristics and process-
Morenoff, Sampson and Raudenbush (2001) determined that homicide
es, especially for those populations at higher risk for ETV-C. Family
rates were higher in neighborhoods with lower levels of collective effi-
characteristics like SES may convey that the financial resources with
cacy and high concentrated disadvantage. The authors also investigate
which parents can actively protect their children from dangerous envi-
the relationship between the availability of local organizations such as
ronments are scarce and limit, to some extent, familial ability to exert
religious institutions, political groups, and community councils finding
guardianship over youth.
that the presence of these associations increase collective efficacy
through the formation of social ties. What is missing from these corner-
stone blocks upon which collective efficacy is built is an explicit theoret- 3.2.2. Parenting strategies
ical framework to explain the causal mechanism between collective Parenting strategies take place within the home and beyond
efficacy, neighborhood violence, and micro-level experiences. The utili- household walls, and limiting our studies to the traditional measures
ty of collective efficacy, a macro level variable, to reduce individual's of supervision, attachment, and even discipline fails to capture the
ETV-C, a micro level variable, has yet to be established. Collective effica- full breadth of parenting practices employed by caregivers to protect
cy is not predictive of youth's exposure to violent peers (Zimmerman & their children. Averdijk and Bernasco (2015, p. 159) conclude
Messner, 2011) or ETV-C (Ahlin & Lobo Antunes, 2017). that “guardianship by parents is often operationalized in one of
two ways - attachment to parents and parental control - the latter
3.2. Family and parenting being the more direct measure”. However, according to
Furstenberg et al. (1999), community conditions will inform paren-
The relationship between family characteristics, parenting strate- tal strategies, underscoring the importance of examining a wider
gies, and youth ETV-C has been studied throughout the literature as breadth of family management strategies that reach outside of the
these tend to be the most common focus of inquiries on ETV-C risk home. The use of protective parenting strategies at home and while
and protective factors. Undeniably, family plays a critical role in attenu- youth are in the community can guard youth from negative neigh-
ating or exacerbating youth ETV-C. Often, the research has been divided borhood influences. In essence, when examining youth ETV-C it
into studies that examine family characteristics like marital status and makes sense to place parenting strategies within the community
SES and those that focus on parenting strategies like supervision, mon- context because managing the outside world and neighborhood in-
itoring, and discipline (Bacchini, Miranda, & Affuso, 2011; Buka et al., fluences is particularly important as youth mature and venture out
2001; Selner-O'Hagan et al., 1998; among others). This section begins into the community.
M.J.L. Antunes, E.M. Ahlin / Aggression and Violent Behavior 34 (2017) 166–177 173

To date there has been little agreement on what role the traditional 3.3. Peers
measures of parenting strategies have in explaining ETV-C. Gibson et al.
(2009) find no statistically significant effect of supervision and parental Using an ecological framework, Salzinger et al. (2002) identified five
warmth on youth secondary ETV-C. Interestingly, Tillyer et al. (2011) ETV-C risk domains – community, family, parents/caregivers, peers, and
found that parental control actually increased victimization risk, validat- personal characteristics – and noted that much youth victimization in
ing the results of Miller, Wasserman, Neugebauer, Gorman-Smith, and the neighborhood is perpetrated by peers. From a RAT/LS perspective,
Kamboukos (1999). In a replication of Gorman-Smith and Tolan (1998), the role of peers in assessing victimization risk is essential. Clearly,
Gorman-Smith et al. (2004) uncovered no influence of parental supervi- choosing ones friends and engaging in various activities with those indi-
sion, discipline, and attachment to parents on youth ETV-C. Findings re- viduals will affect how much opportunity youth have to experience vic-
garding parental strategies have clearly been mixed. Understanding timization while also determining the quality and quantity of
why youth differentially experience ETV-C may be a function of how par- guardianship over peer interactions. As such, risk of victimization may
ents choose to guard or limit access to the community itself rather than operate through peer interactions (see Schreck, Fisher, & Miller, 2004)
practices clearly limited to the home and requires additional exploration. such as unstructured socializing and association with deviant peers.
Recent studies have expanded the use of parenting strategies to in- From a RAT/LS perspective, these interactions with peers can impact
clude those that reach outside of the home and assess their relationship target suitability by influencing risk of direct victimization (Maimon &
with youth ETV-C. For example, setting limits for youth and increasing Browning, 2010). Less is known, however, about whether these vari-
monitoring for the times when youth are not at home has been shown ables influence ETV-C.
to decrease youth risk for ETV-C (Lobo Antunes, 2012; Dishion &
McMahon, 1998). Lobo Antunes and Ahlin (2015) and Ahlin and Lobo 3.3.1. Unstructured socializing
Antunes (2015, 2017) reported that youth whose parents actively re- Unstructured socializing, sometimes referred to as unstructured lei-
strict their unsupervised time in the neighborhood are less likely to ex- sure time (Larson, 2001), generally consists of youth peer interactions
perience ETV-C. Additionally, Ahlin and Lobo Antunes (2017) suggest that have no set agenda or plans (e.g., going to parties, riding in cars
that youth experiences with community violence should be not only ex- for fun, hanging out with friends) that lack supervision by parents or
amined within an ecological framework but also from a RAT/LS perspec- other authority figures (Osgood et al., 1996). “Unstructured activities
tive, as parents actively engage in guardianship over their children. are considered risky because they leave time available for deviant activ-
Parents who supervise within the home, restrict unfettered access to ities. Therefore, the link between unstructured time and victimization”
the community, and refrain from harsh discipline can not only attenuate (Averdijk & Bernasco, 2015, p. 157) may be understood through the
instances of ETV-C, they also reduce youth unstructured socializing and role of peers and guardianship. By affecting routine activities, primarily
association with deviant peers – leading to a reduction in target suitabil- through a reduction in capable guardianship, unstructured socializing
ity and enhancement of guardianship which is significantly predictive of also alters how much ETV-C youth experience. Parents who restrict
less youth ETV-C (Ahlin & Lobo Antunes, 2015; Lobo Antunes & Ahlin, their children's unsupervised access to the community reduce the
2015). amount of ETV-C experienced by youth (see Ahlin & Lobo Antunes,
Harsh disciplining practices, family conflict, and family violence 2017). At the neighborhood level, poor guardianship and meager at-
have also been shown to increase risk for ETV-C (Ahlin & Lobo tempts at informal social control permits instances of youth unstruc-
Antunes, 2017; Osofsky et al., 1993). Perhaps indirectly, these factors tured socializing. The literature also suggests that youth who are
influence the likelihood that youth will associate with deviant peers unsupervised in the neighborhood and are involved in unstructured so-
and spend time in unstructured activities (Richards et al., 2004; cializing with their peers are more likely to be exposed to community
Zimmerman & Messner, 2013), both of which increase youth target suit- violence, though this relationship may differ across racial/ethnic catego-
ability (Ahlin & Lobo Antunes, 2017; Richards et al., 2004; Schreck, ries (see Lobo Antunes & Ahlin, 2015; Zimmerman & Messner, 2013).
Wright, & Miller, 2002). Observing curfews and rules regarding whether For example, Richards et al. (2004) examined the influence of risk and
children can spend time unsupervised in the neighborhood may, at least protective situational factors over ETV-C, including measures of both di-
to some extent, reflect the quality of a youth's relationship with their rect victimization and witnessing violent acts. According to their analy-
parent/caregiver. Too much harsh discipline, or family conflict or vio- sis, more time spent in a risky situational context, assessed as
lence, may negatively shape the choices youth make. Some evidence percentage of time spent in unstructured activities with peers, increased
suggests that certain family processes can influence youth anger regula- youth ETV-C, while increased time with family or engaged in structured
tion, particularly with respect to parental support and cohesion. These and supervised activities acted as protective factors which decreased
in turn influence ETV-C (Houltberg, Henry, & Morris, 2012) through su- ETV-C. More specifically, compared to socializing at home or school,
pervision; reinforcing the idea that guardianship itself can extend be- youth who engage in public outdoor companionship in the neighbor-
yond supervision and monitoring. hood with older peers experience greater risk for ETV-C (Goldmann et
Embracing an ecological framework, youth development research al., 2011).
has sought to examine how neighborhood characteristics can interact
with parenting to explain various outcomes (Lobo Antunes, 2012; 3.3.2. Peer deviance
Elliott et al., 2006; Furstenberg et al., 1999). Sheidow, Gorman-Smith, Like unstructured socializing, peer deviance also influences ETV-C
Tolan, and Henry (2001, p. 347) investigated the protective effects of and there is substantial literature to support the salience of peer devi-
parenting on youth ETV-C, suggesting that “past studies have failed to ance over youths' risk of ETV-C. Deviant peers can influence youth
consider (whether) the effect of family functioning may vary depending ETV-C in multiple ways. Most directly, having deviant peers can increase
on neighborhood characteristics”. The authors conclude that family youth's own victimization (Ahlin & Lobo Antunes, 2017; Schreck &
functioning (which included parenting practices, parent-child relation- Fisher, 2004; Schreck et al., 2002; Zimmerman & Messner, 2013).
ship, and clustering within neighborhoods) is especially important for From the RAT/LS perspective, risk of ETV-C can also be affected through
youth who live in highly disadvantaged neighborhoods. Inefficient fam- the presence of deviant peers by increasing opportunity and reducing
ily functioning predicted greater levels of ETV-C for youth in these com- capable guardianship. Similar to the perspective adopted by Haynie
munities. This resonates with the argument recently made by Ahlin and and Osgood (2005; see also Miethe & Meier, 1994) who argue that de-
Lobo Antunes (2017) and Lobo Antunes (2012) that when resources are linquent behavior is influenced by opportunity resulting from time
particularly scarce, parenting is one of the most important protective spent with delinquent peers, increased risk for victimization is also a
factors in reducing youth ETV-C by increasing guardianship and function of opportunity which is heightened when youth associate
diminishing youth target suitability. with deviant peers. In terms of aggravating the risk of ETV-C, Averdijk
174 M.J.L. Antunes, E.M. Ahlin / Aggression and Violent Behavior 34 (2017) 166–177

and Bernasco (2015) also argue that adolescent youth are reaching their that situating race and ethnicity within an ecological framework may
peak levels of risk for engagement in crime, and thus hanging out with explicate its relationship to ETV-C.
peers may also expose youth to a stable cadre of motivated offenders,
potentially increasing ETV-C.
Stepping outside the peers' direct mesosystem level and highlight- 3.4.2. Gender
ing the need for an ecological approach, there are also individual charac- Tremendous research has been dedicated to understanding gender
teristics that may mitigate the risk of ETV-C posed by deviant peers. differences not only in delinquency but also victimization and ETV-C.
While youth with low levels of self-control are at increased risk of Many studies have found that boys are at greater risk for ETV-C (Ahlin
ETV-C (Zimmerman & Messner, 2013), high levels of self-control can & Lobo Antunes, 2017; Selner-O'Hagan et al., 1998; Singer, Anglin,
buffer this risk through reduced contact with deviant peers (see Song, & Lunghofer, 1995). Similarly, Lobo Antunes and Ahlin (2015)
Schreck et al., 2002). Further, youth with higher street efficacy, defined and Ahlin and Lobo Antunes (2017) show that, compared to females,
as the ability to successfully avoid negative interactions in one's neigh- males are approximately 40% more likely to experience violence in the
borhood, are less likely to associate with delinquent peers (see Sharkey, neighborhood. However, Zimmerman and Messner (2010) demon-
2006), which may in turn attenuate ETV-C. These individual character- strate that the gender gap in violent offending narrows due to exposure
istics reduce the instances of ETV-C suggesting that youth may act as to peer violence and community concentrated disadvantage. Essential-
their own capable guardian (see Ahlin & Lobo Antunes, 2017). ly, the authors find that the negative effect of exposure to violent
peers is enhanced for girls in conditions of greater neighborhood pover-
3.4. Individual characteristics ty, suggesting perhaps that gender interactions between peer associa-
tions and violence merit closer examination within a multilevel RAT/
The RAT/LS perspectives offer a macro-level explanation of victimi- LS framework.
zation risk, which often highlights the demographic factors of groups Frequently, research on the gender gap in ETV-C has argued that dif-
most likely to experience victimization, such as ETV-C. Such individual ferences lie in variations of parental monitoring and supervision (Lobo
characteristics are the most central mesosystem level to youth within Antunes, 2012; LaGrange & Silverman, 1999). As previously stated, in
an ecological framework, and how individuals interact with their com- impoverished neighborhoods parents may lack the financial and social
munities and structure their daily routine activities and particular life- resources to effectively protect their children from neighborhood condi-
style may be a function of their individual attributes. Until now, far tions and deviant peers which may lead to increased target suitability
too little attention has been paid to the relevance of individual charac- and exposure to motivated offenders (Ahlin & Lobo Antunes, 2017).
teristics beyond traditional demographic variables and how they may The gendered relationship between neighborhood poverty and differ-
serve to attenuate or mitigate youth ETV-C. We examine the relevance ences in youth ETV-C may, just like age and race, be the result of paren-
of the conventional individual characteristics, race/ethnicity, gender, tal and neighborhood interaction and the inability of parents to exert
and age, for explaining ETV-C and also extend the discussion to person- guardianship and attenuate youth risk for ETV-C. The gendered selec-
ality characteristics which have more recently been explored in the tion of parenting mechanisms may influence routine activities and life-
literature. styles and thereby differentially affect target suitability and
guardianship for females, depending on whether parents and caregivers
3.4.1. Race/ethnicity can and are willing to enact capable guardianship.
Race and ethnicity are common correlates of victimization risk and a
considerable amount of research has been dedicated to this topic. In ra-
cial comparisons, scholars have shown that Blacks are more likely than 3.4.3. Age
Whites to experience indirect, or secondary, ETV-C (Lobo Antunes & At what age does ETV-C become a risk for children? This is an im-
Ahlin, 2015; Gibson et al., 2009; Selner-O'Hagan et al., 1998). When ex- portant question, and is perhaps best answered by considering vari-
amining race and ethnicity, there is consensus among social scientists ous elements of the ecological framework that we suggest are
that Black and Hispanic youth are more likely than Whites to experience fundamental for addressing ETV-C. For youth to experience violence
ETV-C (Crouch et al., 2000; Martin, Gordon, & Kupersmidt, 1995). In a in their community they must spend time there and compared to
recent study, Zimmerman and Messner (2013) note that compared to younger children, teens are much more likely to spend time in
White youth, the odds of being exposed to community violence for their neighborhood where they form peer relationships and engage
Hispanic and Black youth are 74% and 112% higher, respectively. These in community activities (Lobo Antunes, 2012; Ward & Laughlin,
racial and ethnic disparities persist across informant types, youth, 2003). Perhaps this is why studies have consistently established
primary caregiver, or parent, though youth self-reports of ETV-C tend that older youth are at greater risk for experiencing and witnessing
to be higher (Kuo, Mohler, Raudenbush, & Earls, 2000; see Buka et al., violence in their neighborhood (Ahlin & Lobo Antunes, 2017; Lobo
2001). Antunes & Ahlin, 2015; Selner-O'Hagan et al., 1998; Singer et al.,
While Hispanic youth experience more ETV-C than their White 1995). From a RAT/LS perspective, increased youth engagement
peers, either as a direct victim or through witnessing or hearing about with their community, often without the watchful eye of a parent
violence perpetrated against others (Crouch et al., 2000; Gibson et al., or caregiver, is akin to a reduction in capable guardianship and, in
2009; Martin et al., 1995; Zimmerman & Messner, 2013), more current turn, a source of enhancement of target suitability.
attention has focused on the role immigrant generational status (e.g., In a series of studies using the PHDCN data, Lobo Antunes (2012), Lobo
first, second, third generation immigrant) has on ETV-C. Studies control- Antunes and Ahlin (2014, 2015), and Ahlin and Lobo Antunes (2017)
ling for immigrant generational status suggest that ETV-C among Latino found that the older cohorts in their sample, those in their mid- to late
youth is a function of whether they (first generation), or their parents teens reported higher risk for ETV-C. The authors demonstrate that youn-
(second generation), or grandparents (third generation) are foreign- ger youth are subjected to higher levels of parental control and guardian-
born, with second and later generation Hispanic youth experiencing ship, which means they are less likely to spend time unsupervised in their
more ETV-C (Eggers & Jennings, 2014; Gibson & Miller, 2010). Unlike community and more likely to be monitored at home. Parental guardian-
these scholars, MacDonald and Saunders (2012) report no differences ship also translates to fewer associations with deviant peers and time
in ETV-C among Hispanics who are first-generation immigrants com- spent in what Richards et al. (2004) call “risky” activities (i.e., unstruc-
pared to Latinos born in the US. In fact, controlling for neighborhood tured socializing), reducing their chances of experiencing ETV-C. Guard-
structural characteristics in addition to collective efficacy and social dis- ianship and target availability can explain, therefore, why it is that
order, first-generation immigrants experienced less ETV-C, suggesting younger youth are less likely to experience ETV-C.
M.J.L. Antunes, E.M. Ahlin / Aggression and Violent Behavior 34 (2017) 166–177 175

3.4.4. Personality characteristics mesosystem levels - neighborhood, family, peers, and youth - but
Individuals can increase their own risk of victimization through per- should also consider applying a concrete theory to explain why
sonal decision-making about where and with whom to associate youth experience violence in the community.
(Cohen & Felson, 1979; Gibson et al., 2009; Hindelang et al., 1978; The focus here was primarily on ETV-C as an outcome. In essence, a
Miethe & Meier, 1994), thereby increasing risk of victimization through majority of the articles reviewed specifically investigated predictors of
the selection of situations and peers (see Schreck et al., 2002). Certain per- youth ETV-C. We have found that there seems to be more literature ded-
sonality characteristics can assist prosocial decisions to avoid ETV-C or icated to the deleterious sequelae of ETV-C than explaining how youth
constrain good choices and increase ETV-C inviting its detrimental effects are at differential risk for ETV-C. We also demonstrate that these studies
on youth development. One much studied personality factor is low self- often fail to consider the multilevel nature of ETV-C or explicitly use the-
control. Low self-control is characterized as lacking inhibitory control, ory to explain these outcomes. This is certainly a direction for future re-
being present oriented, preferring immediate gratification, sensation search. Moreover, as van Dulmen et al. (2008) suggest, witnessing
seeking, and a lack of persistence in tasks (Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990). violence and being the victim of violence are conceptually different
Individuals with low self-control are more likely to place themselves in measures. It is reasonable then to argue that causal processes may also
precarious situations, perhaps because they fail to see the long-term con- be different. Gibson et al. (2009) do explore the issue of secondary, or
sequences of ETV-C, or they are engaged in criminal behavior themselves indirect, exposure to community violence; however, as many of the
(see Schreck, 1999). Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990, p. 157) suggest youth studies included here they do not propose a definitive theoretical
with low self-control “gravitate to the street”. As such, compared to youth framework. Future efforts should be directed at exploring these
with higher levels of self-control, individuals with less self-control are at a avenues.
higher risk of experiencing ETV-C (Higgins, Jennings, Tewksbury, & Lynch (2003) makes an important point regarding self-reported expe-
Gibson, 2009; Zimmerman & Messner, 2013). riences of community violence versus rates of community violence, sug-
Low self-control may also place individuals in contexts that are gesting that rates themselves could negatively influence youth
prone to victimization due to a lack of guardianship and increased outcomes or alter guardianship. The parenting literature (Lobo Antunes,
opportunities (see Forde & Kennedy, 1997), through delinquent 2012; Elliott et al., 2006; Furstenberg et al., 1999) shows that neighbor-
peer associations (Schreck et al., 2002). Compared to delinquent hood conditions shape family management practices. Communities rid-
peers, low self-control has a stronger effect over risk of direct victim- dled with violence may prompt parents to enhance guardianship. While
ization, though high self-control has not been shown to act as a pro- this may seem the best course of action, research on adolescent develop-
tective factor (Schreck et al., 2002). While low self-control and ment highlights the need for teens to strive for autonomy and seek the
impulsivity appear to predict increased direct violent victimizations freedom to make their own choices (Roche & Leventhal, 2009; Tobler,
(Gibson, 2012), less is known about its relationship to indirect ETV- Komro, & Maldonado-Molina, 2009) and serve as their own guardians
C. However, impulsivity, one component of low self-control, does in- over ETV-C (see Ahlin & Lobo Antunes, 2017). As youth mature, “parents
crease ETV-C (Wilcox, Tillyer, & Fisher, 2009), while having an inter- will reinforce and stimulate this process of growing autonomy, self-deter-
nal locus of control (a sense of ownership over outcomes resulting mination, and independence” (Eccles et al., 1993, p. 97). It is conceivable
from one's behaviors) is associated with a reduction in ETV-C then, yet unexplored, that too much guardianship can have a negative ef-
(Ahlin & Lobo Antunes, 2015). The literature on the relevance of per- fect on youth behavior and possibly on ETV-C as youth seek to break the
sonality characteristics, though quickly developing, is still in its na- protective grip established by parents (see Miller et al., 1999; Tillyer et al.,
scent period. Within an ecological framework, and by applying the 2011). This in turn may lead to increased suitability as a target and ulti-
RAT/LS perspective, we believe that these and other personality mately an increase in the likelihood of ETV-C.
characteristics could be explored as explanations of ETV-C, particu- In a similar vein, research may consider the differences in reporting
larly as moderators of existing known predictors of ETV-C. mechanisms when examining youth ETV-C (Kuo et al., 2000). Parental,
teacher, and even peer reports of violence may vary from those made by
4. Conclusions the youth themselves. Youth perceptions of neighborhood conditions
and environment, perceptions that guide decisions and choices, are
The current review examines the literature on youth experiences not necessarily the same as those held by family, peers, and neighbor-
with ETV-C, placing the studies analyzed within a multileveled con- hood residents. Such considerations in future studies could shed light
text while also exploring the utility of a RAT/LS perspective. We find on the complex processes involved in neighborhood violence and
that in order to better comprehend the mechanisms that influence more specifically youth ETV-C.
the likelihood a youth will be exposed to community violence we Within a policy paradigm, gains in knowledge regarding what can be
should shy away from looking at risk and protective factors using a done at the community, family, peer, and youth level to ameliorate vio-
silo approach but rather apply an ecological framework that con- lence in the neighborhood by reducing ETV-C can also help foster posi-
siders all levels of the mesosystem. This review also underscores tive community relations and youth development. Dahlberg (1998)
the paucity of studies that have actively sought to incorporate theory examined violence prevention programs but noted that many tended
in their analyses. ETV-C includes both victimization and witnessing to focus on only one level of the mesosystem. Tackling each level as if
of violent events in the community and we contend that the RAT/ in a vacuum negates the multidimensional relationship that exists be-
LS perspective is well-suited to explaining why there is a greater pro- tween community, families, peers, and youth. For instance, Gardner
pensity towards ETV-C for some youth (see Ahlin & Lobo Antunes, and Brooks-Gunn (2009) demonstrate the utility of youth organizations
2017; Averdijk & Bernasco, 2015). The RAT/LS model is placed, in deterring violence at the neighborhood level. As Eccles and Gootman
here, within a mesosystemic framework and research included in (2002) argue, programs targeting youth should be implemented from a
this review has been evaluated using the key concepts of guardian- multifaceted perspective, directed at the community, family, and peers,
ship and target suitability. Whether intentional or not, several of and we suggest including the individual. Therefore, when considering
the articles use these in their investigations, albeit sometimes the damaging consequences of youth ETV-C, the ecological RAT/LS
under different conceptualizations. Oftentimes guardianship is mea- framework within which ETV-C occurs, policymakers should take into
sured as social control, supervision, and monitoring, whereas target account a multicontextual model that focuses on increasing guardian-
suitability is assessed via peer relationships and “risky” activities ship at all levels of the mesosystem – neighborhood, family, peers, and
(Averdijk & Bernasco, 2015; Richards et al., 2004). Regardless, what individual – while also promoting strategies that minimize target suit-
these studies show is that researchers should not only contextualize ability of those who live in and explore the neighborhood on a daily
youth ETV-C by placing the risk and protective factors at different basis.
176 M.J.L. Antunes, E.M. Ahlin / Aggression and Violent Behavior 34 (2017) 166–177

References Gottfredson, M. R., & Hirschi, T. (1990). A general theory of crime. Stanford, CA: Stanford
University Press.
Ahlin, E. M., & Lobo Antunes, M. J. (2015). Locus of control orientation: Parents, peers, and Gottfredson, D. C., Gottfredson, G. D., & Weisman, S. A. (2001). The timing of delinquent
place. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 44, 1803–1818. behavior and its implications for after-school programs. Criminology & Public Policy,
Ahlin, E. M., & Lobo Antunes, M. J. (2017). Levels of guardianship in protecting youth 1, 61–86.
against exposure to violence in the community. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, Guterman, N. B., Cameron, M., & Staller, K. (2000). Definitional and measurement issues
15, 62–83. in the study of community violence among children and youths. Journal of
Averdijk, M., & Bernasco, W. (2015). Testing the situational explanation of victimization Community Psychology, 28, 571–587.
among adolescents. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 52, 151–180. Haynie, D. L., & Osgood, D. W. (2005). Reconsidering peers and delinquency: How do
Bacchini, D., Miranda, M. C., & Affuso, G. (2011). Effects of parental monitoring and expo- peers matter? Social Forces, 84, 1109–1130.
sure to community violence on antisocial behavior and anxiety/depression among Higgins, G. E., Jennings, W. G., Tewksbury, R., & Gibson, C. L. (2009). Exploring the link be-
adolescents. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 26, 269–292. tween low self-control and violent victimization trajectories in adolescents. Criminal
Bell, C. C., & Jenkins, E. J. (1993). Community violence and children on Chicago's southside. Justice and Behavior, 36, 1070–1084.
Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes, 56, 46–54. Hindelang, M., Gottfredson, M., & Garofalo, J. (1978). Victims of personal crime: An empir-
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard ical foundation for a theory of personal victimization. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger.
University Press. Hitlin, S., & Elder, G. H., Jr. (2007). Time, self, and the curiously abstract concept of agency.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1992). Ecological systems theory. Jessica Kingsley Publishers. Sociological Theory, 25, 170–191.
Buka, S. L., Stichick, T. L., Birdthistle, I., & Earls, F. J. (2001). Youth exposure to violence: Houltberg, B. J., Henry, C. S., & Morris, A. S. (2012). Family interactions, exposure to vio-
Prevalence, risks, and consequences. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 71, 298–310. lence, and emotion regulation: Perceptions of children and early adolescents at risk.
Cohen, L. E., & Felson, M. (1979). Social change and crime rate trends: A routine activity Family Relations, 61, 283–296.
approach. American Sociological Review, 44, 588–608. Jensen, G. F., & Brownfield, D. (1986). Gender, lifestyles, and victimization: Beyond rou-
Crouch, J. L., Hanson, R. F., Saunders, B. E., Kilpatrick, D. G., & Resnick, H. S. (2000). Income, tine activities. Violence and Victims, 1, 85–99.
race/ethnicity, and exposure to violence in youth: Results from the National Survey of Jonson-Reid, M. (1998). Youth violence and exposure to violence in childhood: An ecolog-
Adolescents. Journal of Community Psychology, 28, 625–641. ical review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 3, 159–179.
Dahlberg, L. L. (1998). Youth violence in the United States: Major trends, risk factors, and Kellam, S. G., Ling, X., Merisca, R., Brown, C. H., & Ialongo, N. (1998). The effect of the level
prevention approaches. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 14, 259–272. of aggression in the first grade classroom on the course and malleability of aggressive
Dishion, T. J., & McMahon, R. J. (1998). Parental monitoring and the prevention of child behavior in middle school. Development and Psychopathology, 10, 165–185.
and adolescent problem behavior: A conceptual and empirical formulation. Clinical Kirk, D. S. (2006). Examining the divergence across self-report and official data sources on
Child and Family Psychology Review, 1, 61–75. inferences about the adolescent life-course of crime. Journal of Quantitative
Eccles, J. S. (1992). School and family effects on the ontogeny of children's interests, self- Criminology, 22, 107–129.
perceptions and activity choice. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 40, 145–208. Kirk, D. S. (2009). Unraveling the contextual effects on student suspension and juvenile
Eccles, J., & Gootman, J. A. (Eds.). (2002). Community programs to promote youth arrest: An examination of school, neighborhood, and family controls. Criminology,
development. Washington, DC: National Academies Press. 47, 479–520.
Eccles, J. S., Midgley, C., Wigfield, A., Buchanan, C. M., Reuman, D., Flanagan, C., & Iver, D. Kuo, M., Mohler, B., Raudenbush, S. L., & Earls, F. J. (2000). Assessing exposure to violence
M. (1993). Development during adolescence. The impact of stage-environment fit on using multiple informants: Application of hierarchical linear model. Journal of Child
young adolescents' experiences in schools and in families. The American Psychologist, Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 1049–1056.
48, 90–101. Kupersmidt, J. B., Griesler, P. C., DeRosier, M. E., Patterson, C. J., & Davis, P. W. (1995).
Eggers, A., & Jennings, W. G. (2014). The effects of birth location and sociological influ- Childhood aggression and peer relations in the context of family and neighborhood
ences on violent victimization among Hispanic youth. Youth Violence and Juvenile factors. Child Development, 66, 360–375.
Justice, 12, 355–366. LaGrange, T. C., & Silverman, R. A. (1999). Low self-control and opportunity: Testing the
Elder, G. H., Jr. (1994). Time, human agency, and social change: Perspectives on the life general theory of crime as an explanation for gender differences in delinquency.
course. Social Psychology Quarterly, 57, 4–15. Criminology, 37, 41–72.
Elliott, D. S., Menard, S., Rankin, B., Elliott, A., Wilson, W. J., & Huizinga, D. (2006). Good Larson, R. W. (2001). How US children and adolescents spend time: What it does (and
kids from bad neighborhoods: Successful development in social context. Cambridge, En- doesn't) tell us about their development. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
gland: Cambridge University Press. 10, 160–164.
Esbensen, F., & Huizinga, D. (1991). Juvenile victimization and delinquency. Youth and Leventhal, T., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2000). The neighborhoods they live in: The effects of
Society, 23, 202–228. neighborhood residence on child and adolescent outcomes. Psychological Bulletin,
Federal Bureau of Investigation (2015). 2014 Crime in the United States. Washington, DC: 126, 309–337.
Federal Bureau of Investigation. Lobo Antunes, M. J. (2012). A multilevel exploration of neighborhood disorder, family man-
Finkelhor, D., & Asdigian, N. L. (1996). Risk factors for youth victimization: Beyond a life- agement and youth antisocial behavior, Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of
styles/routine activities theory approach. Violence and Victims, 11, 3–19. Maryland, College Park.
Finkelhor, D., Turner, H., Shattuck, A., Hamby, S., & Kracke, K. (2015). Children's exposure to Lobo Antunes, M. J., & Ahlin, E. M. (2014). Family management and youth violence: Are
violence, crime, and abuse: An update. Washington, DC: Office of Juvenile Justice and parents or community more salient? Journal of Community Psychology, 42, 316–337.
Delinquency Prevention. Lobo Antunes, M. J., & Ahlin, E. M. (2015). Protecting youth against exposure to violence:
Fitzpatrick, K. M., & Boldizar, J. P. (1993). The prevalence and consequences of exposure to Intersections of race/ethnicity, neighborhood, family and friends. Race & Justice: An
violence among African-American youth. Journal of the American Academy of Child and International Journal, 5, 208–234.
Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 424–430. Lynam, D. R., Caspi, A., Moffit, T. E., Wikström, P. O., Loeber, R., & Novak, S. (2000). The in-
Flannery, D. J., Williams, L. L., & Vazsonyi, A. T. (1999). Who are they with and what are teraction between impulsivity and neighborhood context on offending: The effects of
they doing? Delinquent behavior, substance use, and early adolescents' after-school impulsivity are stronger in poorer neighborhoods. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
time. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 69, 247–253. 109, 563–574.
Forde, D. R., & Kennedy, L. W. (1997). Risky lifestyles, routine activities, and the general Lynch, M. (2003). Consequences of children's exposure to community violence. Clinical
theory of crime. Justice Quarterly, 14, 265–294. Child and Family Psychology Review, 6, 265–274.
Furstenberg, F. F., Cook, T. D., Eccles, J., Elder, G. H., & Sameroff, A. (1999). Managing to MacDonald, J., & Saunders, J. (2012). Are immigrant youth less violent? Specifying the
make it: Urban families and adolescent success. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. reasons and mechanisms. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social
Gardner, M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2009). Adolescents' exposure to community violence: Are Science, 641, 125–147.
neighborhood youth organizations protective? Journal of Community Psychology, 37, Maimon, D., & Browning, C. R. (2010). Unstructured socializing, collective efficacy and
505–525. youth violent behavior among urban youth. Criminology, 48, 443–473.
Gibson, C. L. (2012). Unpacking the influence of neighborhood context and antisocial propen- Martin, S. L., Gordon, T. E., & Kupersmidt, J. B. (1995). Survey of exposure to violence
sity on violent victimization of children and adolescents in Chicago. Washington, DC: Na- among the children of migrant and seasonal farm workers. Public Health Reports,
tional Institute of Justice. 110, 268–276.
Gibson, C. L., & Miller, H. V. (2010). Crime and victimization among Hispanic adolescents: A Miethe, T. D., & Meier, R. F. (1994). Crime and its social context: Toward an integrated theory
multilevel longitudinal study of acculturation and segmented assimilation. Washington, of offenders, victims, and situations. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.
DC: National Institute of Justice. Miethe, T. D., Stafford, M. C., & Long, J. S. (1987). Social differentiation in criminal victim-
Gibson, C. L., Morris, S. Z., & Beaver, K. M. (2009). Secondary exposure to violence during ization: A test of routine activities/lifestyle theories. American Sociological Review, 52,
childhood and adolescence: Does neighborhood context matter? Justice Quarterly, 26, 184–194.
30–57. Miller, L. S., Wasserman, G. A., Neugebauer, R., Gorman-Smith, D., & Kamboukos, D.
Goldmann, E., Aiello, A., Uddin, M., Delva, J., Koenen, K., Gant, L. M., & Galea, S. (2011). Per- (1999). Witnessed community violence and antisocial behavior in high-risk, urban
vasive exposure to violence and posttraumatic stress disorder in a predominantly Af- boys. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 28, 2–11.
rican American urban community: The detroit neighborhood health study. Journal of Morenoff, J. D., Sampson, R. J., & Raudenbush, S. W. (2001). Neighborhood inequality, col-
Traumatic Stress, 24, 747–751. lective efficacy, and the spatial dynamics of urban violence. Criminology, 39, 517–559.
Gorman-Smith, D., & Tolan, P. (1998). The role of exposure to community violence and de- Mrug, S., & Windle, M. (2010). Prospective effects of violence exposure across multiple
velopmental problems among inner-city youth. Development and Psychopathology, 10, contexts on early adolescents' internalizing and externalizing problems. Journal of
101–116. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 953–961.
Gorman-Smith, D., Henry, D. B., & Tolan, P. H. (2004). Exposure to community violence Mrug, S., Loosier, P. S., & Windle, M. (2008). Violence exposure across multiple contexts:
and violence perpetration: The protective effects of family functioning. Journal of Individual and joint effects on adjustment. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 78,
Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 439–449. 70–84.
M.J.L. Antunes, E.M. Ahlin / Aggression and Violent Behavior 34 (2017) 166–177 177

Murphy, S. L., Xu, J., & Kochanek, K. D. (2012). Deaths: Preliminary data for 2010. National Sharkey, P. T. (2006). Navigating dangerous streets: The sources and consequences of
Vital Statistics Reports, 60, 1–52. street efficacy. American Sociological Review, 71, 826–846.
Nofziger, S., & Kurtz, D. (2005). Violent lives: A lifestyle model linking exposure to violence Sheidow, A. J., Gorman-Smith, D., Tolan, P. H., & Henry, D. B. (2001). Family and commu-
to juvenile violent offending. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 42, 3–26. nity characteristics: Risk factors for violence exposure in inner-city youth. Journal of
Osgood, D. W., & Anderson, A. L. (2004). Unstructured socializing and rates of delinquen- Community Psychology, 29, 345–360.
cy. Criminology, 42, 519–550. Simons, R. L., Lorenz, F. O., Wu, C. I., & Conger, R. D. (1993). Social network and marital
Osgood, D. W., Wilson, J. K., O'Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., & Johnston, L. D. (1996). Rou- support as mediators and moderators of the impact of stress and depression on pa-
tine activities and individual deviant behavior. American Sociological Review, 61, rental behavior. Developmental Psychology, 29, 368–381.
635–655. Singer, M. I., Anglin, T. M., Song, L., & Lunghofer, L. (1995). Adolescents' exposure to vio-
Osofsky, J. D., Wewers, S., Hann, D. M., & Fick, A. C. (1993). Chronic community violence: lence and associated symptoms of psychological trauma. Journal of the American
What is happening to our children? Psychiatry, 56, 36–45. Medical Association, 273, 477–482.
Pratt, T., & Cullen, F. T. (2005). Assessing macro-level predictors and theories of crime: A Slopen, N., Fitzmaurice, G. M., Williams, D. R., & Gilman, S. E. (2012). Common patterns of
meta-analysis. Crime and Justice, 32, 373–438. violence experiences and depression and anxiety among adolescents. Social
Richards, M. H., Larson, R., Miller, B. V., Luo, Z., Sims, B., Parrella, D. P., & McCauley, C. Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 47, 1591–1605.
(2004). Risky and protective contexts and exposure to violence in urban African Snyder, H. N., & Sickmund, M. (1999). Juvenile offenders and victims: 1999 national report.
American young adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, Washington, DC: Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention.
138–148. Spano, R., Freilich, J. D., & Bolland, J. (2008). Gang membership, gun carrying, and employ-
Richters, J. E., & Martinez, P. (1993). Violent communities, family choices, and children's ment: Applying routine activities theory to explain violent victimization among inner
chances: An algorithm for improving the odds. Development and Psychopathology, 5, city, minority youth living in extreme poverty. Justice Quarterly, 25, 381–410.
609–627. Spencer, M. B., & Dornbusch, S. M. (1990). Challenges in studying minority youth. In S. S.
Roche, K. M., & Leventhal, T. (2009). Beyond neighborhood poverty: Family management, Feldman, & G. R. Elliott (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing adolescent
neighborhood disorder, and adolescents' early sexual onset. Journal of Family (pp. 123–146). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Psychology, 23, 819–827. Taylor, T. J., Freng, A., Esbensen, F. A., & Peterson, D. (2008). Youth gang membership and
Salzinger, S., Feldman, R. S., Stockhammer, T., & Hood, J. (2002). An ecological framework serious violent victimization: The importance of lifestyles and routine activities.
for understanding risk for exposure to community violence and the effects of expo- Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 23, 1441–1464.
sure on children and adolescents. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 7, 423–451. Tillyer, M. S., Tillyer, R., Miller, H. V., & Pangrac, R. (2011). Reexamining the correlates of
Sampson, R. J., & Lauritsen, J. L. (1994). Violent victimization and offending: Individual-, adolescent violent victimization: The importance of exposure, guardianship, and tar-
situational-, and community level risk factors. In A. J. Reiss, & J. A. Roth (Eds.), Under- get characteristics. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 26, 2908–2928.
standing and preventing violence. Vol. 3. (pp. 1–114). Washington, DC: National Acad- Tobler, A. L., Komro, K. A., & Maldonado-Molina, M. M. (2009). Relationship between
emy Press. neighborhood context, family management practices and alcohol use among urban,
Sampson, R. J., & Raudenbush, S. W. (2001). Disorder in urban neighborhoods: Does it lead multi-ethnic, young adolescents. Prevention Science, 10, 313–324.
to crime. Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice. van Dulmen, M. H., Belliston, L. M., Flannery, D. J., & Singer, M. (2008). Confirmatory factor
Sampson, R. J., Raudenbush, S. W., & Earls, F. (1997). Neighborhoods and violent crime: A analysis of the recent exposure to violence scale. Children and Schools, 30, 93–102.
multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science, 277, 918–924. Ward, C. L., & Laughlin, J. E. (2003). Social contexts, age and juvenile delinquency: A com-
Sampson, R. J., Morenoff, J. D., & Raudenbush, S. (2005). Social anatomy of racial and eth- munity perspective. Journal of Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 15, 13–26.
nic disparities in violence. American Journal of Public Health, 95, 224–232. Wilcox, P., Land, K. C., & Hunt, S. A. (2003). Criminal circumstance: A dynamic
Schreck, C. J. (1999). Criminal victimization and low self-control: An extension and test of multicontextual criminal opportunity theory. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
a general theory of crime. Justice Quarterly, 16, 633–654. Wilcox, P., Madensen, T. D., & Tillyer, M. S. (2007). Guardianship in context: Implications
Schreck, C. J., & Fisher, B. S. (2004). Specifying the influence of family and peers on violent for burglary victimization risk and prevention. Criminology, 45, 771–803.
victimization extending routine activities and lifestyles theories. Journal of Wilcox, P., Tillyer, M. S., & Fisher, B. S. (2009). Gendered opportunity? School-based ado-
Interpersonal Violence, 19, 1021–1041. lescent victimization. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 46, 245–269.
Schreck, C. J., Wright, R. A., & Miller, J. M. (2002). A study of individual and situational an- Wilson, W. C., & Rosenthal, B. S. (2003). The relationship between exposure to communi-
tecedents of violent victimization. Justice Quarterly, 19, 159–180. ty violence and psychological distress among adolescents: A meta-analysis. Violence
Schreck, C. J., Fisher, B. S., & Miller, J. M. (2004). The social context of violent victimization: and Victims, 18, 335–352.
A study of the delinquent peer effect. Justice Quarterly, 21, 23–47. Zimmerman, G. M., & Messner, S. F. (2010). Neighborhood context and the gender gap in
Schwartz, D., Hopmeyer-Gorman, A., Toblin, R. L., & Abou-ezzeddine, T. (2003). Mutual adolescent violent crime. American Sociological Review, 75, 958–980.
antipathies in the peer group as a moderating factor in the association between com- Zimmerman, G. M., & Messner, S. F. (2011). Neighborhood context and nonlinear peer ef-
munity violence exposure and psychosocial maladjustment. New Directions for Child fects on adolescent violent crime. Criminology, 49, 873–903.
and Adolescent Development, 102, 39–54. Zimmerman, G. M., & Messner, S. F. (2013). Individual, family background, and contextual
Selner-O'Hagan, M. B., Kindlon, D. J., Buka, S. L., Raudenbush, S. W., & Earls, F. J. (1998). explanations of racial and ethnic disparities in youths' exposure to violence. American
Assessing exposure to violence in urban youth. Journal of Child Psychology and Journal of Public Health, 103, 435–442.
Psychiatry, 39, 215–224.

You might also like