Drewnowski 1998

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Fond Qualilyand PreferenceVol. 9, No. l/2, pp.

13-20, 1998
0 1998 Published by Elsevier Science Ltd
All rights reserved. Printed in Great Britain
ELSEVIER PII: 50950-3293(97)00017-7 0950-3293/98 $19.00+ .oo

REPIACINGSUGARAND FATINCOOKIES:lMPACTON
PRODUCTQUALITYANDPREFERENCE
Adam Drewnowski,a* Kristina Nordenstenb & Johanna DwyerC
“Human Nutrition
Program, School of Public Health, University of Michigan, USA
bBeth Israel Hospital, Boston, USA
Tufts University Schools of Medicine and Nutrition, Jean Mayer Human Nutrition Research Center on Aging at Tufts
University, Frances Stern Nutrition Center, New England Medical Center Hospital, 750 Washington St, Boston, MA, USA

(Accepted 16 April 1997)

of cookies. Reducing fat content of sweet baked goods


ABSTRACT would reduce calories even further, while preserving the
sensory quality and the acceptability of the product.
This study was designed to test the hypothesis that
Sugar and fat are two key ingredients of cookies and other reducing fat content of cookies would have less impact on
baked desserts. Food writers frequently advise reducing the product quality than reducing their sugar content. A sub-
amount of sugar in cookie recipes as a method for arriving at sidiary hypothesis was that the sensory panel would
a lower-calorie product. An alternative strategy, reducing the immediately perceive a reduction in the sweetness of
fat content of cookies, may reduce calories even further, while cookies, but would be less sensitive to corresponding
maintaining high overall product acceptability levels. This manipulations of Fat content. These two hypotheses were
study showed that sweetness was the key sensory attribute that directly suggested by past studies on sensory aspects of
sugar/fat mixtures (Drewnowski, 1987a; Drewnowski and
determined preferences for cookies. Acceptability ratings for
Schwartz, 1990). However, those early studies had an
cookies dropped sharpb following a 25% reduction in recipe
important limitation, since they had been conducted only
sugar levels, but were relatively una$ctedfollowing a 25%
with dairy fats and had been restricted to liquid and semi-
reduction in recipe fat content. 0 1998 Elsevier Science Ltd.
solid foods. More recent studies conducted by other inves-
All rights reserved. tigators continued to use dairy fats as the stimuli of choice
(Mela, 1988; Warwick and Schiffman, 1990; Monneuse et
Keywords: Sugar; fat; cookie recipes; product acceptability; al., 1991; Bacon et al., 1994; Crystal et al., 1995). One aim
sensory attributes; texture; jlavor; sweetness. of the present study was to examine whether hedonic
responses profiles that had been obtained with a range of
sweet milkshakes, cake frostings, and ice cream would also
hold for a selection of baked cookies. Only a few studies
INTRODUCTION
had compared consumer responses to sugar and fat con-
tent in liquid and in solid foods (Drewnowski et al., 1989).
Many cookies and other sweet baked goods contain large It is well known that consumers are highly sensitive to
amounts of sugar and fat (Pennington, 1994). According even small variations in sweetness, particularly in water
to the USDA Food Guide Pyramid (United States solutions (Moskowitz et al., 1974; Drewnowski, 19876).
Department of Agriculture & United States Department Though individual sweetness preferences may vary,
of Health and Human Services, 1992) and the more group hedonic responses for sugar solutions and sweet-
recent Dietary Guidelines for Americans (USDA and ened beverages typically show a sharply defined opti-
DHHS, 1995), dietary sugars and fats should be con- mum point or breakpoint at approximately 10% sucrose
sumed only in moderation. In translating dietary guide- wt/vol (Moskowitz et al., 1974). Sensory evaluations of
lines into food recipes for the general public, the overall sweet milkshakes, sweetened cream cheese, cake frostings
emphasis has been on reducing the fat content of foods. or ice cream also showed a well-defined breakpoint for
However, when it comes to cookies, food writers suggest sweetness (Drewnowski and Greenwood, 1983; Drew-
that decreasing the amount of added sugar in cookie nowski et al., 1987u,b, 1989; Drewnowski and Schwartz,
recipes is the best way of obtaining a healthier and lower- 1990). Similar results were obtained in studies using
calorie product. A better alternative, for both theoretical sweetened fromage blanc, a creamy dessert type white
and practical reasons, may be to decrease the fat content cheese (Martin et al., 1989; Monneuse et al., 1991; Simon
et al., 1993). In all cases, preferences declined sharply for
*To whom correspondence should be addressed. those products that were judged as being too sweet. The

13
14 A. Drewnowski et al.

sugar content of the best-liked product varied depending Reducing fat in foods has become a public health issue.
on the model food system employed, and was generally Among health goals listed in the Healthy People 2000
higher for solid as opposed to liquid foods (Moskowitz et Report (US DHHS 1991) was the creation of some 5000
al., 1974; Drewnowski and Schwartz, 1990). brand name products that are reduced in fat and satu-
In contrast, consumers were far less sensitive to the rated fat. The food industry has risen to the challenge in
oral sensation of fat in foods (Drewnowski, 1987; developing a number of low-fat or fat-free cookies. For
Drewnowski et al., 1989; Warwick and Schiffman, 1990; the most part, the oral sensation of fat in such products
Mela and Sacchetti, 1991). One problem with sensory was supplied by a variety of other ingredients, notably
evaluation of fat content is that the presence of fat in modified starches and gels, fibers and gums, maltodex-
foods is associated with a wide range of textural charac- trim, and of course simple sugar. In some cases, replacing
teristics, from smooth and creamy, to chewy, crunchy fat with starch has led to the creation of a fat-free cookie
and brittle. Fats provide texture and bulk to foods, hold that was higher in calories than the original recipe.
water, provide a mechanism for heat transfer at high In designing stimuli for the present study, we decided
temperatures, and act as a carrier for fat-soluble flavor against the use of fat substitutes. The use of modified star-
molecules. As a result, no single textural attribute can be ches and gels is outside the scope of a typical home kitchen.
unambiguously associated with the fat content of foods Furthermore, most recipes for reduced calorie cookies
(Drewnowski, 1987a). In liquid dairy products, where fat call for reducing the amount of sugar and fat, with the
is present in the form of microglobules, smoothness, major emphasis on reducing sugar content. Yet from the
creaminess and viscosity generally serve as the principal standpoint of cookie recipes, reducing fat content might
sensory cues for fat content as well as a measure of prod- have less effect on taste, texture, appearance and overall
uct quality (Cooper, 1987). In solid foods, fats have been acceptability than could be achieved by a comparable
associated with the tenderness and juiciness of meat, reduction in sugar. From the nutritional standpoint,
crispness of potato chips, and with the melting quality of lower-fat cookies would also be lower in saturated fat and
chocolate. However, it is unclear what sensory charac- cholesterol, and proportionately higher in carbohydrate.
teristics are associated with the fat content of solid cook- Appetite for fats is a characteristic feature of human
ies and whether these qualities are common or differ obesity. Children’s preferences for fat in foods are influ-
between one cookie recipe and another. Another ques- enced, moreover by parents’ fatness indices (Fischer and
tion is how the perceived fat content of cookies is related Birch, 1995). One concern regarding the use of low-fat or
to product quality and to overall food acceptance. low-sugar foods been that a caloric deficit at one meal
Oral perception of fat in solid foods tends to be would simply be compensated for at the next meal, or
unreliable, and subjects are sometimes unable to distin- later on during the day. Studies in which subjects con-
guish between high- and low-fat versions of the same sumed either high- or low-fat preloads indeed showed
food (Warwick and Schiffman, 1990). In one study, that active young males compensated for the missing
(Drewnowski et al., 1989), subjects rated low-fat and energy by consuming extra carbohydrates, with a result-
high-fat versions of sweetened cream cheese mixtures as ing shift in the carbohydrate to fat ratio (Rolls and
approximately the same. In other studies, conducted Hammer, 1995). In contrast, young women presented
with mixtures of milk, cream and sugar, hedonic response with low-sugar breakfasts compensated for only a frac-
profiles ascended only gradually with increasing fat con- tion of the missing calories, while overweight and dieting
tent or remained flat (Drewnowski and Greenwood, 1983; women failed to compensate altogether (Drewnowski,
Drewnowski et al., 1989). Warwick and Schiffman (1990) 1995). In general, studies on sugar substitutes (aspar-
observed that the elderly, in particular, were relatively tame) and on fat replacements (olestra) showed that
insensitive to the fat content of foods, suggesting that there was a net advantage to using lower-energy foods
they might find reduced-fat foods entirely acceptable. (Rolls and Hammer, 1995). Since dieters typically avoid
These findings suggested to us that reducing the amount sweet and high fat foods such as cookies, creating accep-
of fat in foods might have less of an impact on overall table low-energy recipes would be one step toward
hedonic ratings than reducing sugar content. However, improving the control of body weight, without compro-
with some exceptions (Mela and Sacchetti, 1991) most mising quality of life.
previous studies on preferences for sugar/fat mixtures
have been carried out with liquid or semi-liquid products,
such as milkshakes, cake frostings, ice cream, or sweetened
METHODSAND PROCEDURES
mixtures of dessert-type soft cheese (fromage blanc)
(Drewnowski et al., 1987a,b; Mela, 1988; Drewnowski and
Holden-Wiltse, 1992). Fewer studies attempted to manip- Subjects
ulate the sugar or fat content of solid foods (Moskowitz et
al., 1974; Mela and Sacchetti, 1991). We therefore Members of the sensory panel were 16 young normal-
examined the impact of reducing sugar, fat, or both on weight men and women recruited by advertising on the
sensory profiles and preference ratings for a number of Tufts University Health Sciences Campus (eight men
popular cookies, as judged by two taste panels. and eight women). Mean age was 32.2 years for men and
Replacing Sugar and Fat in Cookies 15

28.0 years for women. Body mass indices (BMI = kg/m’) a 25% reduction in added sugarsugar), a 25%
(75%
were 21.2 for men and 23.5 for women. The subjects reduction in added fat (75% reduction in
fat), a 25%
were non-restrained as ascertained by the restraint scale both sugar and fat (75% sugar/fat), and a 50% reduc-
(Herman, 1978), were free of eating disorders as ascer- tion in fat (50% fat). In all cases, reduction in sugar and
tained by the EAT scale (Garner and Garfinkel, 1979), fat content was achieved simply by reducing the amount
and were not following a weight-reducing or a vegetarian of sugar or butter expressed in common household
diet. All subjects were regular consumers of cookies and measures per batch of cookies, as is normally done in
other sweet baked goods. The subjects were divided into food preparation in the home. There was no attempt to
two panels of eight persons each, approximating trained replace the sugar or fat with intense sweeteners, bulking
sensory panels. The study had been approved by the agents, or any of the available fat replacement products.
Tufts-New England Medical Center Human Investiga- Whereas ingredient substitution is the usual case in coo-
tion Review Committee and the subjects signed informed kie preparation in the food industry, it is not in the
consent forms. Subjects received modest monetary com- home. The recipes are summarized in Table 1.
pensation for completing all study protocols. The present study design approximated the full fac-
Subjects were required to come to the laboratory for a torial design of previous studies (Drewnowski et al.,
total of four 1 h visits. Tasting sessions were regularly 1987a,b; Drewnowski and Schwartz, 1990). However, for
held between 11 am and 1 pm. The subjects were asked culinary and baking reasons, some reduced sugar and fat
to eat their usual breakfast on the day of testing and to recipes did not yield an acceptable cookie-like product
avoid consuming any snacks between that meal and the and could not be used. Furthermore, reducing sugar and
tasting session in order to eliminate major variations in fat by 25-50% more closely approximated the suggested
hunger or satiety. culinary practices in the home. Of course, as fat or sugar
were removed and no replacement products were used,
Stimuli the proportions of other ingredients had to increase.
Thus, the proportion of sugar (wt/wt) in toll house
The stimuli were six different types of cookies, including cookies rose from 2 1% in the control recipe to 23% in
biscotti (two types), brownies, oatmeal cookies, peanut the 50% fat reduction recipe.
butter cookies and tollhouse cookies. The recipes for each The cookies were prepared in a test kitchen by two
cookie can be obtained from the second author (K.N.). professional chefs on the day before the testing sessions.
Six different conditions were used for each type of cookie: The recipes and baking techniques had been standardized
two sets of the original recipe (Standard recipe 1 and 2), to minimize differences in quality between successive

TABLE 1. Summary of Cookie Recipes

lllgedimts Tollhouse Peanut butter Oatmeal Brownies Biscotti


cookies cookies cookies land2

g Household g Household g Household g Household g Household


measures measures measures measures measures

Sugar
Control 259 1; c 172 lc 172 lc 400 2c 200 1c
75% sugar 194 1 c 2 tbs. 130 fC 130 iC 300 1; c 150 $C

Butter
Control 227 1c 113 ;C 113 +C 227 1c 170 ?C

75% butter 170 ZC 85 6 tbs. 85 6 tbs. 170 3C 113 fC

50% butter 113 $2 57 4 tbs. 57 4 tbs. 113 fC 85 6 tbs.

Flour 281 2+c 125 lc 125 lc 125 lc 375 3c


Eggs, beaten 88 2 44 1 44 1 176 4 176 4
Baking powder -- 5 1 tsp.
Baking soda 5 1 tsp. 2.5 4 tsp. 5 1 tsp. 14 3 tsp.
Salt 6 1 tsp. 3 f tsp. 6 1 tsp. 1.5 ; tsp. 3 4 tsp.
Vanilla extract 5 1 tsp. 2.5 4 tsp. 5 1 tsp. 5 1 tsp. 7 1; tsp.
Other
Chocolate -- ~ 114 4 0s
Choc. chips 340 2c
Peanut butter -- - 256 lc
Rolled oats 81 IC

Milk -- 15 1 tbs.
Almond extract -- - - 7 1; tsp.
16 A. Drewnowski et al.

batches of the same product. Each batch of cookies was Acceptability


wrapped in plastic and then placed in a sealed plastic
bag labeled with an individual code letter. The cookies 7

were stored overnight in the refrigerator at 4°C and were


brought to room temperature before testing.
1 T T

Since the aim of the study was to compare the reduced


added sugar and fat products with standard recipes, we
employed a repeated measures design, The original
recipes were tested twice for a total of 36 presentations.
The subjects thus participated in four 1 h experimental
sessions, tasting different variations of each cookie per
session. The stimuli were presented in a randomized
order, and the subjects rinsed their mouths with plain
water between tasting successive samples.

Sensory evaluations 100% 75%


Fat
50% 100% 75%
.SlJg;lr
7-5!75%
FNSU~

The subjects were asked to taste each cookie in turn and FIG. 1. Mean overall acceptability ratings (and SEM bars) as a
function of recipe sugar and fat content. The data are averaged
rate it along a number of g-point category scales. The
over cookie types.
first scale rated overall hedonic preference. It ranged
from “dislike extremely” to “like extremely” with a
neutral point at 5 (“neither like nor dislike”). The sub- of stimulus sugar and fat content. The statistical com-
jects then rated their liking for the texture, flavor, and parison was between the higher-rated of the two standard
appearance of each cookie, again using g-point hedonic recipes and the three stimuli with lower sugar and fat
preference scales. Ratings of preference were followed by contents. Reducing fat content by 25% had no impact on
ratings of taste and texture attributes. The subjects rated overall product acceptability, which declined only when
the cookies using g-point unipolar attribute scales, fat was reduced by 50%. In contrast, reducing sugar
including sweet, buttery, moist, soft, chewy, and tender, content of the cookies, even by 25%, had an immediate
as well as greasy, crumbly, mealy and dry. The scales and adverse impact on overall acceptability ratings.
were anchored at each extreme with “not at all ” Analysis of variance showed a significant main effect of
and “extremely _“. Similar procedures had been used sugar, F( 1,15) = 10.1; p < 0.01, but no main effect of fat,
in previous studies (Brandt et al., 1963; Drewnowski, 1987). F( 1,15) < 1; ns. There was no significant main effect of
cookie type, F(5,75) = 1.40; ns. However, a significant
Statistical analyses cookie by sugar interaction, F(5,75) = 2.57; pcO.05,
suggested that the adverse effects of removing sugar from
Hedonic preference and sensory evaluation data were a recipe might vary from one type of cookie to another.
analyzed using SPSS for Windows (SPSS Inc, Chicago, Multiple regression analyses showed that the overall
IL). Analysis of variance for repeated measures, with acceptability of the cookies was almost entirely predicted
cookie type, and the added sucrose and fat levels as by liking of flavor, then texture, and to a much lesser
within-subject variables was based on two levels of added extent by liking of appearance. As seen in Table 2, these
sucrose (75% and 100%) and two levels of added fat three variables accounted for over 80% of the variance in
(75% and 100%). In addition, multiple regression anal- overall acceptability ratings. Subsequent analyses of
yses were conducted on the full data set, pooled over the variance addressed the impact of fat and sugar levels on
six cookie types, to determine the contribution of the preference ratings for the texture and flavor of cookies.
liking for texture, flavor, and appearance to the overall Sensory acceptability was sharply reduced by reducing
acceptability of the product. Additional multiple regres- the sugar content of cookie recipes, but remained unaf-
sion analyses examined the contribution of selected taste fected by lowering the recipe fat content. These data,
and texture attributes, including sweetness, to the overall averaged over the six types of cookies, are shown in Fig. 2,
ratings of product acceptability. again as a function of sugar and fat content of the cook-
ies. Reducing fat content by 25% or even 50% had little
impact on the liking for flavor. In contrast, reducing
RESULTS sugar content by 25% led to a reduction in ratings.
Analysis of variance showed a strong main effect of
sugar, F( 1,15) = 41.3; p < 0.001, but no main effect of
Preference ratings fat, F( 1,15) < 1; ns, nor of cookie type, F(5,75) = 1.40;
ns. However, the presence of interactions, including coo-
Overall acceptability ratings averaged over the six kie by sugar, F(5,75) = 2.74; p < 0.05, and cookie by fat,
types of cookies, are presented in Fig. 1, as a function F(5,75) = 2.58; p < 0.05, again suggested that the effects
Replacing Sugar and Fat in Cookies 17

TABLE 2. Multiple Regressions of Overall Acceptability on Liking for Product Texture, Flavor and Appearance. Shown are
Standardized Regression Coefficient (beta) and Significance of T-statistic

Standard Standard 100% fat/ 75% fat/ 75% fat/ 50% fat/
recipe 1 recipe 2 75% sugar 100% sugar 75% sugar 100% sugar

beta sig T beta sig T beta sig T beta sig T beta sig T beta sig T
(PC) @<I @<I @<) @<I @<I

Texture 0.24 0.00 0.38 0.00 0.37 0.00 0.32 0.00 0.38 0.00 0.29 0.00
Appearance 0.08 0.11 0.00 0.97 -0.07 0.15 0.00 0.90 0.00 0.89 0.12 0.05
Flavor 0.67 0.00 0.61 0.00 0.66 0.00 0.69 0.00 0.60 0.00 0.59 0.00

Multiple R 0.91 0.92 0.91 0.93 0.92 0.91


1-2 0.83 0.84 0.83 0.86 0.84 0.83

of varying fat and sugar content of cookie recipes did than others. Notably, brownies were perceived as sweeter
vary with the type of cookies. than either biscotti or peanut butter cookies.
The acceptability of cookie texture might be expected In contrast to sweetness ratings, texture-related attri-
to be influenced by both sugar and fat content, since butes showed no systematic effects of reduced butter
both determine the optimal texture of cookies. However, content in cookie recipes. Ratings on the scales of ‘but-
as shown in Fig. 3, liking for texture was not affected by tery’, ‘moist’, and ‘tender’ showed no significant main
reducing either sugar or fat content in this selection of effects of fat content. However, the presence of significant
cookies. There were no main effects of cookie type, or the main effects of cookie type (p < 0.001) suggested that
recipe sugar or fat content. These data suggest that reducing the butter content of cookies may have had
reducing the amount of butter in cookie recipes, at least more adverse effects for some cookies than for others.
within the present limits, had no marked impact on
reported preferences for the texture of cookies. Sweetness and acceptability

Sweetness and texture Regression analyses were conducted to determine the


impact of taste- and texture-related sensory attributes,
Sweetness ratings, averaged over the six types of cookies, including sweetness, on the overall acceptability of the
are shown in Fig. 4. It can be seen that reducing the present selection of cookies. To make the results more
sugar content of cookie recipes led to lower ratings of generalized, the analyses were conducted with the full
sweetness. As expected, the main effect of sugar was data set, pooled over the six cookie types.
highly significant, F( 1,15) = 29.3; p < 0.001. The main The data are summarized in Table 3. ‘Sweetness’ was
effect of cookie type was also significant, F(5,75) = 3.62; the only attribute that was consistently and significantly
p < 0.001, that is, some cookies were perceived as sweeter linked to ‘overall preference’, regardless of cookie recipe.

Liking for flavor Liking for texture

b
> 5

:
P
324
3

100% 100% 75% 75l75%


‘F”, 5g% Sugar FNSUg~

FIG. 2. Mean ratings of preferences for flavor (and SEM bars) FIG. 3. Mean ratings of preferences for texture (and SEM
as a function of recipe sugar and fat content. The data are bars) as a function of recipe sugar and fat content. The data are
averaged over cookie types. averaged over cookie types.
18 A. Drewnowski et al.

The key sensory attribute for this class of food products


was sweetness. Analyses of sensory attribute data showed
7
that ‘overall acceptability’ and ‘liking’ for cookie texture
and flavor all declined with reduced sugar content. In
8
contrast, ‘overall acceptability’ and taste preference rat-
ings remained relatively unaffected following a 25%
reduction in the fat content of cookie recipes. Reducing
fn 5
the sugar content of cookies is likely to have an adverse
impact on acceptance ratings.
fn 4 One explanation may be that consumers are more
sensitive to small variations in sugar content. In contrast,
fat is often difficult to detect in solid foods (Drewnowski,
3
1987; Mela, 1988). No single attribute can be unam-
biguously associated with fat content. The present study
2 employed a range of cookies and a variety of texture
100% 75% 50% 100% 75% 15/75%
attributes, ranging from ‘tender’, ‘chewy’, and ‘soft’ to
Fat sugar F&6UgW
‘greasy’ and ‘moist’, as well as ‘dry’ and ‘mealy’. The
FIG. 4. Mean sweetness intensity ratings (and SEM bars) as a cookies tested included a variety of types, from brownies
function of recipe sugar and fat content. The data are averaged
to biscotti, and the judgment of optimum texture was
over cookie types.
most likely influenced by cookie type. Numerous cookie
by sugar and cookie by fat interactions attest to major
Other desirable attributes that were associated with differences by cookie type. However, sweetness was a
overall liking were ‘buttery’, ‘tender’, and ‘not greasy’, desirable attribute throughout, and sweetness ratings
though the associations were less consistent. It appears were positively linked to overall product acceptability,
that sweetness is the key attribute for this class of food regardless of the type of cookies tested.
products. The data clearly indicate that any variation in These data have some implications for designing
cookie recipes that significantly reduced product sweetness acceptable lower-energy cookies. First, sweetness is the
had a negative impact on overall acceptability ratings. most desirable sensory attribute in this group of food
products. As a result, any manipulations that reduce the
sweetness level of cookies will result in diminished

DISCUSSION acceptance. Reducing the sugar content of cookies tends


to lower their appeal. Replacing sucrose with less sweet
sugars such as sorbitol or polydextrose is likely to have
The study showed that reducing the fat content of six the same detrimental effects. On the other hand, redu-
popular cookie recipes affected overall product quality cing the fat content in some cookies will reduce both total
less than did a comparable reduction in sugar content. and fat calories, while preserving the sensory appeal of
At the same time, the decreased butter content of cookie the final product. The fact that some lower-fat cookies
recipes allowed for equal or greater calories savings than are slightly higher in their sugar content may in fact add
those achieved with reduced-sugar cookies. to their appeal.

TABLE 3. Multiple Regression of Overall Acceptability on Sensory Attributes of Different Cookie Recipes. Shown are Standardized
Regression Coefficient (beta) and the Significance of T-statistic

Standard Standard 100% fat/ 75% fat/ 75% fat/ 50% fat]
recipe 1 recipe 2 75%sugar 100% sugar 75%sugar 100% sugar

beta sig T beta sig T beta sig T beta sig T beta sig T beta sig T
@<) @<) @<I @<) @<I (P<)
Sweet 0.23 0.04 0.43 0.00 0.34 0.00 0.21 0.03 0.50 0.00 0.28 0.02
Buttery 0.34 0.03 0.23 0.05 0.07 0.55 0.07 0.60 0.22 0.05 0.11 0.39
Moist 0.10 0.62 -0.03 0.85 0.27 0.08 0.23 0.18 -0.04 0.75 0.17 0.28
Greasy -0.43 0.00 -0.20 0.08 -0.28 0.02 -0.23 0.07 -0.27 0.00 -0.17 0.13
Soft -0.06 0.68 0.27 0.07 0.18 0.20 0.13 0.30 0.09 0.48 -0.12 0.42
Chewy 0.07 0.54 -0.04 0.63 -0.15 0.20 0.04 0.67 -0.15 0.08 0.13 0.23
Crumbly 0.22 0.10 0.16 0.16 0.21 0.08 0.18 0.10 0.08 0.41 0.09 0.40
Dry 0.13 0.38 -0.08 0.58 -0.12 0.39 -0.08 0.63 -0.17 0.15 -0.16 0.22
Mealy -0.02 0.85 -0.04 0.71 -0.08 0.48 -0.07 0.45 0.07 0.44 0.05 0.59
Tender 0.26 0.04 -0.10 0.50 -0.14 0.28 0.29 0.02 0.08 0.49 0.28 0.03
Multiple R 0.55 0.60 0.56 0.66 0.47 0.62
r2 0.30 0.35 0.32 0.43 0.41 0.39
Replacing Sugar and Fat in Cookies 19

Indeed, in some industry recipes for fat-free cookies, Drewnowski, A. (19876) Sweetness and obesity. In Sweetness,
fat is sometimes replaced by simple sugars, and the final ed. J. Dobbing. ILSI-Nutrition Foundation Symposium,
product is as sweet or sweeter than the original recipe. Springer-Verlag, Berlin.

The present data provide a theoretical rationale for this Drewnowski, A. (1995) Intense sweeteners and the control of
appetite. Nutrition Reviews 53, l-7.
approach. As long as the sweetness of cookies is main-
Drewnowski, A. and Greenwood, M. R. C. (1983) Cream and
tained, consumers will tolerate some deviations from the
sugar: human preferences for high-fat foods. Physiology and
standard recipe in both texture and flavor.
Behavior 30, 629-633.
Dietary guidelines typically recommend consuming Drewnowski, A. and Holden-Wiltse, J. (1992) Taste responses
only moderate amounts of foods rich in sugars and fat. and food preferences in obese women: effects of weight
Whereas sugar was viewed as the main culprit in the cycling. International Journal of Obesity 16, 6399648.
1970’s, the chief emphasis over the past several years has Drewnowski, A. and Schwartz, M. (1990) Invisible fats: sensory
been on restricting dietary fats. Reducing the consump- assessment of sugar/fat mixtures. Appetite 14, 203-217.
tion of fats and saturated fats were the key recommen- Drewnowski, A., Bellisle, F., Aimez, P. and Remy, B. (1987a)
dations of both the Nutrition and Health (1988) and the Taste and bulimia. Physiology and Behavior 41, 62 l-626.

Diet and Health (1989) reports. However, messages to Drewnowski, A., Halmi, K. A., Pierce, B., Gibbs, J. and Smith,
G. P. (19876) Taste and eating disorders. American Journal of
consumers regarding reduced sugar consumption persist,
Clinical Nutrition 46, 4422450.
and have been included in both the Food Guide Pyramid
Drewnowski, A., Shrager, E. E., Lipsky, C., Stellar, E. and
(1992) and in the Dietary Guidelines for Americans
Greenwood, M. R. C. (1989) Sugar and fat: sensory and
( 1995). Although most sets of dietary guidelines promote hedonic evaluations of liquid and solid foods. PhysioloQ and
dietary variety, consumers concerned with diet and Behavior 45, 1777183.
health tend to eliminate sweet and high fat foods, Fischer, J. 0. and Birch, L. L. (1995) Fat preferences and fat
including cookies, sweets and other desserts from their consumption of 3 to 5 year old children are related to parental
diets. However, eliminating whole categories of foods obesity. Journal of the American Dietetic Association 95, 759-764.
from the habitual diet is neither practical nor particu- Garner, D. M. and Garfinkel, P. E. (1979) The Eating Atti-
larly beneficial. All foods can fit into a healthy diet, pro- tudes Test: an index of the symptoms of anorexia nervosa.
vided they are consumed in moderate amounts and are Psychological Medicine 9, 273-279.
Herman, C. P. (1978) Restrained eating. Psychiatr Clin North Am
part of a varied diet. We conclude that reducing fat as
1, 595-607.
opposed to reducing sugar in cookie recipes is the pre-
Martin, C. and Bellisle, F. (1989) Eating attitudes and taste
ferred method for obtaining a reduced-calorie product.
responses in young ballerinas. Physiology and Behavior 46, 223-
227.
Mela, D. .J. (1988) Sensory assessment of fat content in fluid

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS dairy products. Appetite 10, 37-44.


Mela, D. J. and Sacchetti, D. A. (1991) Sensory preferences for
fats: relationships with diet and body composition. American
Journal of Clinical Nutrition 53, 908-9 15.
Supported by a grant from the Sugar Association to
Monneuse, M. F., Bellisle, F. and Louis-Sylvestre, J. (1991)
Johanna Dwyer. The assistance of Andrea Scarpa Nero,
Impact of sex and age on sensory evaluation of sugar and fat
MS, RD is acknowledged with thanks. Kay Coleman,
in dairy products. Physiology and Behavior 50, 111 l-l 117.
Ph.D., Jean Rainey and Charles Baker, Ph.D. provided Moskowitz, H. R., Kluter, R. A., Westerling, J. and Jacobs,
helpful suggestions in developing this project. H. L. (1974) Sugar sweetness and pleasantness: evidence for
different psychophysical laws. Science 184,583-585.
National Academy of Sciences (1989) Diet and Health. Commit-
tee on Diet and Health, Food and Nutrition Board, National
REFERENCES Academy Press, Washington, DC.
Pennington, J. A. T. (1994) Bowes and Church’s Food Values of
Portions Commonly Used. J. B. Lippincott Company, Philadel-
Bacon, A., Miles, J. S. and Schiffman, S. S. (1994) Effect of phia.
race on perception of fat alone and in combination with Rolls, B. J. and Hammer, V. A. (1995) Fat, carbohydrate, and
sugar. Physiology and Behavior 55, 603-606. the regulation of energy intake. American Journal of Clinical
Brandt, M. S., Skinner, E. Z. and Coleman, J. A. (1963) Nutrition 62(Suppl.), 1086S-95s.
Texture profile method. Journal of Food Science 28, 404-409. Simon, Y., Bellisle, F., Monneuse, M. O., Samuel-Lajeunesse,
Cooper, H. (1987) Texture in dairy products and its sensory B. and Drewnowski, A. (1993) Taste responsiveness in anor-
evaluations. In Food Texture, ed. H. R. Moskowitz, pp. 217- exia nervosa. British Journal of Psychiatry 162, 244246.
250. Marcel Dekker, New York. United States Department of Agriculture (1992) USDA’s
Crystal, S., Frye, C. A. and Kanarek, R. B. (1995) Taste Food Guide Pyramid. Human Nutrition Information Service. Home
preferences and sensory perceptions in female varsity swim- and Garden Bulletin No 249. USDA and USDHHS,
mers. Appetite 24, 25-36. Washington, DC.
Drewnowski, A. (1987a) Fats and food texture: sensory and United States Department of Agriculture (1995) Nutrition &
hedonic evaluations. In Food Texture, ed. H. R. Moskowitz, Your Health: Dietary Guidelines for Americans. Home and Garden
pp. 251-272. Marcel Dekker, New York. Bulletin No 232. USDA and USDHHS, Washington, DC.
20 A. Drewnowski et al.

United States Department of Health and Human Services the egg(s) and other wet ingredients. Mix dry ingredients
(1988) The Surgeon General’s Report on Nutrition and Health. and add to the butter/sugar mixture. Blend until smooth.
DHHS (PHS) Publication No. 88-50210. US Government Drop onto ungreased cookie sheet using a No. 70 scoop
Printing Office, Washington, DC. (2x9 tsp.). For peanut butter cookies roll the same
United States Department of Health and Human Services amount into balls and press flat with a fork. Place about
(1991) Healthy People .?,O#. US Government Printing Office,
2 inches apart. Bake l&l2 min or until lightly brown in
Washington, DC.
preheated oven 190°C (375°F). Let cool on sheet pan.
Warwick, Z. and Schiffman, S. (1990) Sensory evaluations of
fat-sucrose and fat-salt mixtures: relationship to age and Yields are 30-28-36 cookies respectively.
weight status. Physiology and Behavior 48, 633436. To prepare biscotti: cream butter, sugar and extracts,
then add the eggs one at a time. Mix dry ingredients
together and add to the butter/sugar mixture. Form
dough into 2 loaves, lf inch wide and the length of the
APPENDIX greased cookie sheet. Bake at 176°C (350°F) for 35-
40 min. Remove from oven and let cool. Slice loaves
Cookie recipes diagonally into 9 inch slices. Place cut side down on coo-
kie sheet and return to oven at 190°C (375°F) for 10min
Fat and sugar manipulations in cookie recipes were or until toasted and crisp. Yield is 40 slices.
combined to produce a total of five formulations. The To prepare brownies melt butter and chocolate toge-
formulations were: standard fat/standard sugar; standard ther and let cool. Beat eggs, sugar, salt and vanilla
fat/75% sugar; standard sugar/75% fat; standard sugar/ extract and combine with chocolate/butter mixture. Fold
50% fat; and 75% sugar/75% fat. in the sifted flour. Bake in a greased 9~ 13 inch pan
To prepare tollhouse cookies, peanut butter cookies, about 40min. Cut 4x5 squares when cool. Yield is 20
and oatmeal cookies: cream butter and sugar, then add squares.

You might also like