Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

Effects of Social Belonging on

Homesickness: An Application of
the Belongingness Hypothesis
Susan E. Watt
Alison J. Badger
University of New England

Belongingness theory proposes that humans possess an as distress caused by actual or anticipated separation
innate drive for a minimum number of lasting interper- from familiar or loved people or places (Thurber,
sonal relationships. On geographic relocation, people 1999). Homesick people are prone to crying and can be
leave their existing social networks. This greatly threat- apathetic and listless; severe homesickness can provoke
ens belongingness needs, and the authors propose this is thoughts of suicide. Often there are somatic responses
one cause of homesickness. Two studies investigated such as stomachache, loss of appetite, sleep distur-
whether homesickness arises in the need to belong. Study bance, and headaches. There is also evidence that home-
1 used a correlational design to test the relationship sickness disrupts concentration (Burt, 1993) and produces
between need to belong and homesickness while control- absentmindedness (Fisher & Hood, 1987, 1988). It has
ling for other variables. A significant positive relationship often been described as a form of grieving (e.g., Archer,
was found. Study 2 then used an experimental design to Ireland, Amos, Broad, & Currid, 1998; Fisher & Hood,
test for a causal effect of need to belong on homesickness, 1987, 1988; Stroebe et al., 2002), and some authors
and a significant effect was found. An additional finding have further characterized it as a reactive depression to
showed that individuals who felt accepted in the com- leaving home (Baier & Welch, 1992; Eurelings-Bontekoe,
munity were less homesick. This was independent of Vingerhoets, & Fontijn, 1994; Van Tilburg, Vingerhoets,
number of friends and demonstrates an important link & Van Heck, 1997a) or as similar to adjustment disor-
between community attitudes and adjustment. der with depressed mood (Van Tilburg, 2005). Most
Implications for belongingness theory are discussed. importantly, homesickness is accompanied by acute
longing and intrusive thoughts about home and attach-
Keywords:   need to belong; homesickness; immigration; ment objects. It is this cognitive component that distin-
acceptance; rejection; belongingness guishes it from other disorders (Thurber, 1999).
In Homer’s account of “The Odyssey,” a story that
originates in oral tradition from around 1200 bc, the
In a popular song by Bart Millard (2004), grief at a great hero Ulysses spent 10 years returning from Troy to
loved one's death is expressed as homesickness. Millard his home in Ithaka. He was crippled by homesickness,
is not the first to perceive a link between homesickness and when captured by the beautiful nymph Calypso
and grief. Indeed, homesickness has been described as a spent 7 years “looking out upon the barren ocean with
“mini-grief” that people experience when separated tears in his eyes, groaning and breaking his heart for
from those they love (Stroebe, van Vliet, Hewstone, &
Willis, 2002). In this article, we extend the idea of Authors’ Note: We gratefully acknowledge the University of New England,
homesickness as a mini-grief to ask whether homesick- Faculty of Arts Internal Research Grant No. 21214 for funding Study 1
ness is a consequence of a need for lasting bonds with and Sara Delonghi for her research assistance in that study. We also wish
to acknowledge the constructive comments made by two anonymous
other people. We investigate whether it arises in threat reviewers. Correspondence may be sent to Sue Watt, School of Behavioural,
to belongingness and use it as a testing ground for some Cognitive and Social Sciences, University of New England, Armidale, NSW,
central tenets of belongingness theory. 2351, Australia; e-mail: sue.watt@une.edu.au.
Homesickness is a common response to moving away PSPB, Vol. 35 No. 4, April 2009 516-530
from home, whether for school, college, work, and DOI: 10.1177/0146167208329695
domestic or international immigration. It can be defined © 2009 by the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

516

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016


Watt, Badger / SOCIAL BELONGING AND HOMESICKNESS   517

sorrow  . . .  his eyes ever filled with tears, and dying of social connections through physical separation from
sheer home-sickness” (translation by Samuel Butler, family and friends. This provokes distress, as well as the
1900, http://classics.mit.edu/Homer/odyssey.5.v.html). challenge of fulfilling social connection needs in the new
Several millennia later, Thijs (as cited in Van Tilburg et location. We will now elaborate on how the belonging-
al., 1997b) found that only 7.3% of adults said they had ness hypothesis is relevant to homesickness.
never experienced homesickness, and Fisher (1989)
found it was sufficient to interfere with daily activities in
The Need to Belong
10% to 15% of homesickness sufferers. Homesickness
affects men and women, adults and children alike, and Humans are profoundly social creatures, endowed
has been found to exist even 58 years after moving from with an array of attributes that enhance our ability to
the home of origin (Van Tilburg, 2005). Homesickness live in groups. This may be a truism but results in a great
is also found when people move into more favorable complexity of effects. A vast body of empirical research
situations. Fried (as cited in Fisher, 1989) found slum in psychology has examined attributes of human social-
dwellers in Chicago who were forced to move into bet- ity, and results from studies as diverse as Ainsworth’s
ter houses reacted with intense grief for home. The (1978) strange situation to Billig and Tajfel’s (1973)
lower the income, the stronger their reaction. minimal group paradigm consistently show that people
Homesickness is an underresearched phenomenon. It seek social inclusion and avoid exclusion. This has led
has received remarkably little attention in the literature, numerous theorists to argue for a fundamental impor-
and with some notable exceptions even less attention has tance to humans of affiliation, love and belonging, and
been paid to its theoretical underpinnings. However, it attachment (e.g., Bowlby, 1969, 1973; Erikson, 1963;
potentially afflicts anyone who moves location, either Freud, 1930/1946; Maslow, 1968; Rosenberg, 1979).
temporarily or permanently, and for whatever reason. Baumeister and Leary (1995) conducted an extensive
Fisher (1989) proposed a composite model where review of the empirical literature of social and personal-
homesickness results from the combined effects of sepa- ity psychology from which they proposed humans pos-
ration from the familiar environment and entry into the sess as a fundamental motivation a need to belong.
new environment. Archer et al. (1998) subsequently They describe this need as an innate (evolved) drive for
proposed that homesickness is a form of separation “a minimum quantity of lasting, positive, and signifi-
reaction to people and places. Their empirical research cant interpersonal relationships” (Baumeister & Leary,
used items derived from reactions described in studies 1995, p. 497). Satisfaction of the drive requires frequent
of grief and revealed two clear factors: disliking the new positive interactions in the context of an enduring affec-
place and attachment to home. tive bond between people.
Stroebe et al. (2002) took this further by conceptualiz- In support of an evolved need to belong, there is evi-
ing these two basic factors within a framework of grief and dence that humans possess mechanisms that allow
bereavement. Based on the dual process model of coping quick detection of danger of social rejection, allowing
with bereavement (Stroebe & Schut, 1987), they proposed restorative measures to be taken. FMRI research has
that homesickness results from the combined effects of loss shown that social pain (elicited by ostracism) results in
(loss-orientation) and adjustment to the new situation similar brain activity to that produced by physical pain
(restoration-orientation). Just as grieving people must cope (Eisenberger, Lieberman, & Williams, 2003). On the
with the loss experience and changes to their circum- basis of this and other studies of social exclusion,
stances, Stroebe et al. proposed homesick people must MacDonald and Leary (2005) proposed exclusion elicits
cope with the loss (even if temporary) of their family and pain because inclusion is important to human survival.
friends, as well as their changed circumstances. Just as pain from physical stimuli teaches us to avoid
Vingerhoets (2005) wrote about “cat-type” and danger, social pain assists us to behave in ways to avoid
“dog-type” homesickness. Just as dogs attach more exclusion. Rejection hurts, so we learn to say and do
strongly to people than places, one part of homesick- things to win the allegiance, love, and respect of others,
ness is missing people; and just as cats strongly attach ensuring social acceptance and survival.
to places and the physical environment, another part of The belongingness hypothesis predicts that because
homesickness is missing the physical place. In this our need to belong is so important, people will resist
research, we turn our attention to homesickness that the dissolution of social bonds at least as strenuously
arises in connection to people. We focus on this because as they work to create them. People throughout the
the belongingness hypothesis is relevant to this aspect of world show tears and grief on separation from those
homesickness. In the following section, we extend the they love and resist the dissolution of social bonds in
idea of homesickness as a mini-grief. We propose that the broader social network as well as within intimate
an important element of homesickness is the loss of relationships. People show distress and resistance to

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016


518   PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

breaking bonds even within transient, time-limited perceive many opportunities to develop new friendships
groups (Baumeister & Leary, 1995). It is important to will be less motivated to maintain friendships in the pre-
the current research that Baumeister and Leary (1995) vious location. This implies that people monitor the
used the grief reaction as evidence to support the environment for its potential to fulfill the need to belong.
belongingness hypothesis, stating, In line with Baumeister and Leary, we expect there will
be increased focus on contacts in the previous location
Some conceptualizations of grief portray it not as a reac- when this potential is low. We expect there will also be
tion to the loss of the person but as a reaction to the loss increased distress (homesickness) as there is less opportu-
of a linkage with another person.  . . .  As Lofland (1982) nity to satisfy belongingness needs. In the current research,
pointed out, when people die, relationships end. (p. 507) we operationalized friendship potential in the new loca-
tion as perceived social acceptance. The relation between
In line with this prediction, we suggest that homesickness social acceptance and homesickness forms the basis of
is partly composed of distress at the dissolution (even if Hypothesis 3, described later.
temporary) of social bonds. The need to belong would In summary, we suggest the belongingness hypoth-
cause us to protect and value our social bonds, and esis can help us understand the grief and distress that
physically removing oneself from these bonds on geo- manifests as homesickness. People who move away
graphic relocation can be expected to provoke distress. from home create physical separation from their most
This forms the basis of Hypothesis 1, described later. intimate relationships and broader social network. At
Of course, people show every intention not to aban- the point of arrival in the new location, migrants and
don their social bonds. One way to reduce distress on sojourners can no longer enjoy the physical presence of
relocation is to maintain old relationships by phoning the old network and may experience uncertainty about
and writing home and visiting as frequently as possible. being able to construct a new network. While they
It is an intriguing possibility that belongingness needs may be able to maintain relations with a few members
could be met by frequent contact home. Indeed, Van of the old network via phone calls and other commu-
Tilburg et al. (1999) noted, “the focus of chronic home- nications, evidence shows that these links decline over
sick persons remains directed on the old environment, time (Baumeister & Leary, 1995). Just as threat to
preventing the development of a genuine interest into other fundamental drives such as the need for food,
the new environment and sustaining homesickness” (p. drink, shelter, or safety produces stress, threat to
537). With modern communication technologies, it is belongingness posed by geographic relocation will also
now possible to contact home many times each day at be a stressor. We therefore expect homesickness is in
very little cost. This could permit people to physically part a direct response to threat to belongingness and
move away from home and yet maintain daily contact will lessen as belongingness needs are satisfied in the
with the social network back home, possibly meeting new location. This expectation forms the primary
their belongingness needs through this daily contact. hypothesis of our research.
Another prediction of the belongingness hypothesis The preceding analysis leads to three hypotheses that
is that one social bond can substitute for another. As were tested in the current research:
long as the need to belong is satiated, it should not mat-
ter who produces the satiation. In support of the substi- Hypothesis 1: Homesickness will be greater among indi-
tution hypothesis, Baumeister and Leary (1995) cited viduals experiencing higher need to belong. Previous
findings that women are more likely to seek extramarital research indicates that homesickness is partly composed
relationships when their marriage does not satisfy inti- of distress at the separation (even if temporary) from
macy needs, people are more likely to leave one intimate social bonds. The need to belong would cause us to pro-
tect and value our social bonds, and physically removing
relationship if another is likely to develop, and female
oneself from these bonds on geographic relocation can
prisoners commonly form substitute families. If home- be expected to provoke distress. Those who experience
sickness arises in the need to belong, and if one social stronger need to belong should also experience more
connection can substitute for another, we expect that distress (homesickness) on relocation.
homesickness will decline as new social connections are Hypothesis 2: Homesickness will be less among those who
formed in the new location. This forms the basis of form more close friendships in the new location, and the
Hypothesis 2, described later. relationship will be mediated by need to belong. The
substitution hypothesis indicates that one social bond
Baumeister and Leary (1995) drew an interesting fur-
can substitute for another in satisfying the need to
ther implication from the substitution hypothesis. They belong. As more friendships form, the need to belong
suggested that efforts to sustain friendships across long will be satisfied and homesickness reduced.
distances should be inversely proportional to opportuni- Hypothesis 3: People who feel accepted in the new location
ties to develop new friendships. That is, people who will experience less homesickness and will make less

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016


Watt, Badger / SOCIAL BELONGING AND HOMESICKNESS   519

effort to sustain relationships in the old location. In line Measures


with Baumeister and Leary (1995), we suggested that
the opportunity to form new friendships is an important Homesickness. The Utrecht Homesickness Scale
factor because there will be more opportunity to satisfy (Stroebe et al., 2002) was developed for a cross-cultural
the need to belong in the new location. Social acceptance comparison of homesickness in university students in
indicates more possibility to form friendships, so those two different countries (the Netherlands and the United
who perceive more acceptance should experience less Kingdom). As such, it was the most appropriate measure
threat to need to belong. Consistent with the rationale
of homesickness available for our sample, which included
that homesickness is a response to threat to belonging-
ness, individuals who feel socially accepted should there- students from many different countries. The scale includes
fore experience less homesickness. We also explore the 20 items such as “Missing your parents,” “Feeling
relation between feeling accepted in the new location lonely,” “Longing for acquaintances,” “Finding it diffi-
and maintaining contacts in the old location. Consistent cult adjusting to a new situation,” and “Having thoughts
with Baumeister and Leary, we propose that people who that an old situation was better than here and now.”
feel accepted in the new location will make less effort to Participants are asked to indicate “to what extent you
sustain relations with the old location because there is a
have experienced each of the following in the past 4
strong probability their need to belong will be met in the
new location. weeks,” rating each item on a scale where 1 = not, 2 =
weak, 3 = moderate, 4 = strong, and 5 = very strong.
The two studies presented in the following investi- Stroebe et al. (2002) reported strong interitem reliabil-
gated these hypotheses. The first used a correlational ity of α = .94. Principal component analysis of data col-
design to examine the predicted relations while control- lected in the Netherlands revealed five factors. The first
ling the effects of length of residence in Australia. The factor, Missing Family, explained 62% of the variance in
second used an experimental design to test for a causal responding, with the remaining factors, respectively,
relation between need to belong and homesickness and accounting for 15% (Loneliness), 9% (Missing Friends),
to replicate effects found in the correlational study. By 8% (Adjustment Difficulties), and 6% (Ruminations
applying belongingness theory to homesickness, the About Home). The Utrecht Homesickness Scale showed
research offers a new perspective on homesickness and very strong relationships with a self-report measure that
its reduction. In relation to belongingness theory, this directly asked students how often they had experienced
research tested whether the need to belong construct can homesickness in the past 4 weeks. Discriminant analysis
be used a priori to predict homesickness. This is impor- revealed 83% of the single-item homesickness scores
tant because it tests predictions made specifically from were correctly classified by the five factors, and the over-
belongingness theory, and support for those hypotheses all scale score correlated r = .71 with the single-item
should offer support for the theory itself. We then measure of homesickness. Very similar results were
applied a central component of belongingness theory, obtained in a British sample.
the substitution hypothesis, to homesickness. Again, In the current study, we shortened the scale to 15
support for this hypothesis in relation to homesickness items by dropping the item with the lowest factor load-
would offer support for the substitution hypothesis in ing from each factor. Acceptable reliabilities were still
belongingness theory more generally. obtained, with an overall Cronbach’s α of .95. Each
subscale also showed acceptable reliability (Missing
Family α = .91, Loneliness α = .87, Missing Friends α
STUDY 1 = .76, Adjustment Difficulties α = .89, Ruminations
About Home α = .79).
Method
Need to belong. Need to belong was operationalized
Participants
as an individual differences measure of chronic need to
In Study 1, 161 international university students belong, developed by Leary, Kelly, Cottrell, and
studying at five Australian universities (69 male and Schreindorfer (as cited in Leary, Cottrell, & Phillips,
92 female) participated. The students came from 42 2001). Participants rate 10 questions such as “I try hard
different countries, and age ranged from 18 to 45 years not to do things that will make other people avoid or
(M = 24.54, SD = 5.85). The majority of participants reject me” and “It bothers me a great deal when I am not
were single (84.5%). Length of residence in Australia included in other people’s plans” on a 5-point scale (we
was between 1 month and 10 years (M = 19.62, SD = used 1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = neutral, 4 =
19.16). All participants were assumed to be competent agree, and 5 = strongly agree for consistency with other
in the English language as they must pass English lan- scales in the questionnaire). Previous research suggests
guage tests before enrolling at university in Australia. the scale has good construct validity. Need to belong

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016


520   PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

scores have been found to correlate with acceptance- stay, and social activity. Gender and length of stay have
responsive self-esteem (Leary et al., 2001), sensitivity to both shown small but significant relations with home-
facial expression and vocal tone (Pickett, Gardner, & sickness in earlier research (Archer et al., 1998; Hojat &
Knowles, 2004), and frustration during group behavior Hermann, 1985; Stroebe et al., 2002; Tartakovsky, 2007)
(De Cremer & Leonardelli, 2003); they have also been and could also be expected to relate to other variables in
found to moderate a relationship between group size the analysis. For example, length of stay should relate to
and contributions to the group so that need to belong number of close friends in the new location because par-
was positively associated with cooperation, but only for ticipants are likely to develop more close friendships with
members of large groups (De Cremer & Leonardelli, passing time. Social activity was controlled because it
2003). Interitem reliability was acceptable in our sample provides pleasurable activity that could relieve some
(Cronbach’s α = .80). Other researchers have reported negative feelings associated with homesickness and could
similar interitem reliabilities of .82 (De Cremer & also be expected to relate to number of close friends and
Leonardelli, 2003) and .83 (Pickett et al., 2004). need to belong. In addition to the main hypotheses, we
conducted exploratory analyses to test whether need to
Contact with home. Nine items measured the number belong might moderate the relations between the other
(and duration) of personal phone calls, text messages, predictors (acceptance by Australians and number of
and e-mail messages between the students and their close friends in Australia) and the criterion.
families and friends from the home of origin during the
past 4 weeks. As described in the Results and Discussion Procedure
section, analysis focussed on just one of these items,
number of phone calls made. International student associations at five Australian
universities distributed an e-mail requesting interna-
tional students’ participation in a study of “Migrant
Social network in Australia. Participants were asked
Adjustment.” The request contained a link to the sur-
how many close friends they have in Australia. They
vey, which students completed online. Response rates
were also asked whether they have immediate family in
are not computed as the number of students on each
Australia (and how many) and whether they have a
mailing list was unknown to the researchers.
boyfriend or girlfriend in Australia.

Social activity. Participants were asked the number Results and Discussion
of times during the past 4 weeks they had gone out Homesickness
socially with Australians and with members of their
own national group or other nonnational Australians. Homesickness showed an average elevation of 2.43
The sum of these questions was computed to provide a (SD = .92), which corresponds with “weak” to “moder-
total number of outings. ate” on the 5-point scale. This is higher than the average
scores reported by Stroebe et al. (2002) for first-year
Acceptance by Australians. A measure of acceptance students in the Netherlands and the United Kingdom.
in the new location was adapted from Nesdale and Mak Of participants, 8 (5%) scored above 4, indicating
(2000). Participants rated “I feel accepted by Australians” strong to very strong homesickness; 39 (24.2%) scored
on a 5-point scale from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = between 3 and 4, indicating moderate to strong home-
strongly agree. Nesdale and Mak included number of sickness; 45 (40.4%) scored between 2 and 3, indicating
close friends in their measure of acceptance. However, weak to moderate homesickness; and 39 (28.0%) scored
we wished to analyze number of friends separately and below 2, indicating no homesickness to weak homesick-
did not include it in the measure of acceptance. As ness. Scores on the missing family subscale were highest
shown in Table 1, number of friends and perceived (M = 2.87, SD = 1.07), followed by missing friends (M
acceptance did not significantly correlate. = 2.63, SD = 1.05), loneliness (M = 2.35, SD =1.15),
ruminations (M = 2.15, SD = 1.02), and adjustment dif-
Demographics. Demographic measures included gen- ficulties (M = 2.15, SD = 1.04).
der, age, marital status, occupation, religion, country of A hierarchical multiple regression analysis was con-
birth, and level of education. ducted to examine predictors of homesickness. Gender
(coded as 1 = male, 2 = female), length of residence in
Australia, social activity, social acceptance, and number
Design
of close friends in Australia were entered on Step 1, and
Multiple regression was used to explore the aforemen- need to belong and the interactions of social acceptance
tioned hypotheses while controlling for gender, length of and number of close friends in Australia with need to

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016


Watt, Badger / SOCIAL BELONGING AND HOMESICKNESS   521

TABLE 1:   Summary Statistics and Correlations Between Variables in Study 1

Correlations

Median M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Gender (1), coded 1 = male, 2 = female 1


Length of residence in Australia (months) (2) 15.00 19.62 19.16 –.01 1
Acceptance by Australians (3) 4.00 3.50 1.05 .04 –.10 1
Need to belong (4) 3.20 3.26 0.62 .18* .02 –.17* 1
Social activity (5) 7.00 9.45 8.62 .15 –.08 .15 .06 1
Number of friends in Australia (6) 10.00 18.46 25.57 –.08 –.07 .07 –.12 .34*** 1
Homesickness (7) 2.27 2.43 0.92 .03 .03 –.36*** .43*** –.21** –.09 1
Phone calls made (8) 4.00 5.75 7.46 .13 –.12 –.07 .15 .15 .15 .27***

*p ≤ .05. **p ≤ .01. ***p ≤ .001.

belong (computed from centered variables) were entered


TABLE 2:  Predictors of Homesickness and Phone Calls Home
on Step 2. Need to belong was entered on Step 2 with in Study 1
the interaction terms in case it masked any other effects.
This was not the case, so only the results at Step 2 of the Phone
Calls
analysis are reported.
Homesickness Home at
Prior to analysis, length of residence in Australia, at Step 2 Step 3
number of close friends in Australia, social activity in of the of the
Australia, and homesickness were submitted to log 10 Analysisa Analysisb
transformation to correct positive skew and the data
β sr b sr
were screened for multivariate outliers at p < .001. The
means and standard deviations of variables in the mul- Gender .02 .02 .06 .06
tiple regression (prior to transformation) are presented Length of residence –.03 –.03 –.07 –.07
   in Australia
in Table 1. Correlations shown in the table used the log
Acceptance by –.23*** –.22*** .06 .05
transformed variables.    Australians
The regression model explained 25% of the variance Need to belong .36*** .35*** –.02 –.02
in homesickness. Need to belong was by far the strongest Social activity –.18* –.17* .20* .18*
predictor of homesickness, with higher need to belong Number of friends –.03 –.03 .16 .14
   in Australia
predicting more homesickness. This variable accounted
Need to Belong –.09 –.08 .03 .03
for a substantial 12.3% of the variance (see Table 2). The    × Acceptance
other significant predictors of homesickness were accept- Need to Belong –.09 –.08 .11 .10
ance and social outings. The more accepted participants    × Friends
felt, the less homesick, and this variable accounted for Homesickness — — .34*** .29***
Need to Belong — — .10 .09
4.7% of the variance in homesickness. A negative rela-
   × Homesickness
tionship with social outings indicated that more social Homesickness — — –.09 –.08
activity predicted less homesickness, but this accounted    × Acceptance
for just 2.9% of the variance in homesickness.
a. R = .50, R2 = .25, adjusted R2 = .20, F(8, 130) = 5.40, p < .001.
A multiple regression of the same design was then b. R = .40, R2 = .16, adjusted R2 = .09, F(11, 122) = 2.13, p = .023.
conducted for each subscale of the Utrecht Homesickness *p ≤ .05. ***p ≤ .001.
Scale (it was first necessary to log transform the Loneliness,
Adjustment Difficulties, and Ruminations subscales to not support Hypothesis 2, which was derived from the
correct positive skew). Need to belong and acceptance by substitution hypothesis in belongingness theory, that
Australians significantly predicted each subscale. In addi- one set of social connections can replace another. That
tion, social activity was a significant negative predictor of is, homesickness was not predicted by number of close
loneliness (sr = –.23, p = .002) and of missing friends friends in Australia. It was also not predicted by length
(sr = –.18, p = .02). of residence in Australia.
These results supported Hypothesis 1, that home-
sickness is greater among individuals experiencing
Contact Initiated With Home
higher need to belong, and the first part of Hypothesis
3, that people who feel accepted in the new location will The variables relating to contact home were substan-
experience less homesickness. However, the results did tially skewed. While most students initiated a moderate

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016


522   PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

amount of contact home in the 4-week period, with a Hypothesis 3 predicted that people who felt less
median of 4 phone calls made, 2 mobile phone text mes- accepted in the new location would experience more
sages sent, and 6 e-mail messages sent, some students distress (homesickness) and would make more effort to
showed extremely high frequency of contact, with a sustain relationships in the old location. The previously
maximum of 50 phone calls made, 1,000 mobile phone reported results showed that those who felt less accepted
text messages sent, and 130 e-mails sent. There was also experienced more homesickness, and those who experi-
a significant portion of students who did not make any enced more homesickness also made more phone calls,
phone calls (15.6%), send any mobile phone text mes- but there was no direct relationship between acceptance
sages (42.2%), or send any e-mails (11.1%). The large and number of phone calls made. We tested whether
number of students who did not send mobile phone text such a relationship might be moderated by need to
messages may indicate low use of mobile phone tech- belong, but this was not the case.
nology. E-mail contact could also be problematic, as it
is possible that some but not all people in the home of
STUDY 2
origin have access to e-mail. We therefore decided to use
number of phone calls made because we considered this
Study 1 provided a preliminary test of our hypothe-
is likely the most reliable technology by which students
ses, however the correlational nature of the design does
could initiate contact with home.
not allow causal inferences to be drawn. The primary
We then tested the second part of Hypothesis 3, that
purpose of Study 2 was to extend Study 1 by using an
people who feel accepted in the new location will make
experimental design to investigate whether need to
less effort to sustain relationships in the old location.
belong exerts a causal effect on homesickness.
A hierarchical multiple regression analysis was con-
The study also provided an opportunity to replicate
ducted to examine predictors of phone calls home
elements of Study 1. As well as manipulating need to
(phone calls home was first submitted to log 10 trans-
belong in people who had recently relocated, we meas-
formation to correct positive skew). Gender, length of
ured the number of close friendships in the new location
residence in Australia, social activity, social accept-
and included this as a predictor of homesickness. This
ance, and number of close friends in Australia were
furnished a second test of Hypothesis 2. We also tested
entered on Step 1, and need to belong and the interac-
whether the relationship found in Study 1 between
tions of social acceptance and number of close friends
acceptance in the new location and homesickness was
in Australia with need to belong were entered on Step
replicated in Study 2 and whether people who feel
2. Homesickness and the interaction of homesickness
accepted in the new location will make less effort to sus-
and need to belong were then entered on a third step,
tain relationships in the old location (Hypothesis 3).
in case homesickness masked any other effects. This
was not the case, so only the results at Step 3 of the
Experimental Design
analysis are reported. All interactions were computed
on centered variables. New intake students at the University of New England
The regression model at Step 3 explained 16% of the (UNE) participated in this experiment. The university is
variance in phone calls made, with social activity and located in Armidale, New South Wales (Australia).
homesickness the only significant predictors (see Table Armidale is in a rural location, and most students must
2). The strongest predictor was homesickness, which leave home to attend the university. Consistent with
explained 8.4% of the variance in a positive direction. Stroebe et al. (2002), this study was conducted 6 weeks
Social activity was the only other significant predictor, into the semester, at a time when students would be past
explaining 3.2% of the variance. the initial novelty of being at university and most prone
It is possible that the positive relationship between to homesickness. A simple experimental design was used
homesickness and phone calls home reflects a causal in which participants were randomly allocated to an
direction where the act of speaking on the phone experimental group where need to belong was primed or
increases homesickness. If this is the case, phone calls to a control group where need to belong was not primed.
made and phone calls received should both positively The need to belong manipulation consisted of completing
predict homesickness. We tested this possibility by a “publication survey” in which students evaluated a
repeating the earlier multiple regression on homesick- number of potential publications. In the experimental
ness, including phone calls made and phone calls condition, some titles evoked the need to belong and in
received (log transformed) as additional predictors. the control condition they did not (a pilot study tested
Results showed phone calls made significantly predicted the efficacy of the manipulation). Participants completed
homesickness (sr = .22, p = .002), but phone calls the homesickness measure and other scales immediately
received did not (sr = .006, p = .94). after the need to belong manipulation.

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016


Watt, Badger / SOCIAL BELONGING AND HOMESICKNESS   523

Method “Person Alone”—This article discusses loneliness and pos-


sible coping mechanisms for loneliness.
Participants
In this research, 144 (50 male and 94 female) first-year A full list of the items in the publication survey is
students at UNE participated. There were 71 participants presented in the appendix. A pilot study evaluated the
in the control condition and 73 in the experimental condi- efficacy of the publication survey in priming need to
tion. Participants were aged between 18 and 28 years belong. A sample of 70 community members aged
(M = 18.60, SD = 1.28). The majority were single (97.2%) between 18 and 61 years of age (M = 36.2, SD = 10.2)
and originated from Australia or New Zealand (93.1%). completed either the control version or the experimental
English was the first language of 93.8% of participants, version of the publication survey and then completed
and religious affiliation included Protestant (37.5%), the Need to Belong Scale. The t test results showed that
Catholic/Orthodox (19.4%), Muslim/Hindu (2.1%), respondents in the control group scored significantly
Buddhist/other (1.4%), and no affiliation (39.6%). lower on the Need to Belong Scale than respondents
in the experimental group, t(68) = –2.13, p = .037,
MControl = 2.87, SD = .55, MExperimental = 3.16, SD = .58,
Measures
indicating that the manipulation influenced need to
Demographics. Participants provided demographic belong. Further analyses showed that age, gender, mari-
information, including gender, age, marital status, coun- tal status, country of birth, and whether English is spo-
try of birth, religion, and whether or not English is their ken as a first language did not contribute significantly
first language. to the scores on the Need to Belong Scale.

Need to belong manipulation. Need to belong was Homesickness. Homesickness was measured with the
manipulated by completing a publication survey that pre- Utrecht Homesickness Scale. Reliability analysis revealed
sented titles and descriptions of nine potential publica- good interitem reliability for this sample (α = .92).
tions. Participants were asked to indicate whether or not
they would be interested in reading each article and how Contact with home. The same questions were used as
valuable they thought it would be. These measures were in Study 1. Once again, analysis focused on number of
intended to ensure participants would read the informa- phone calls home.
tion and to provide face validity for the task. The control
condition presented descriptions of articles on neutral Social network in the new location. Participants were
topics that would not evoke feelings of homesickness or asked how many close friends they have in Armidale.
lack of belonging. For example: They were also asked how many acquaintances, whether
members of their immediate family live in Armidale,
“Comparison of Composition”—This article discusses the and whether they have a boyfriend or girlfriend in
various eras of art history and offers a unique compari- Australia.
son of traditional and contemporary techniques.
“The ‘Natural’”—This article raises the nature/nurture Social activity. Social activity was assessed by asking
debate in relation to musicians and people with unique the number of times during the past 4 weeks partici-
talents—are they “naturals,” or is their ability the direct
result of learning and practice?
pants had gone out socially.

Acceptance at UNE. The measure from Study 1 was


Participants in the experimental condition completed
adapted for this study. Participants rated “I feel accepted
the same publication survey as in the control condition,
by people at UNE” on a 5-point scale from 1 = strongly
but five titles and their accompanying descriptions were
disagree to 5 = strongly agree.
replaced with items designed to provoke the need to
belong. These were drawn from items on the Need to
Belong Scale (Leary, Kelly, Cottrell, & Schreindorfer, as Procedure
cited in Leary et al., 2001). For example: A total of 600 questionnaires inviting participation
in a study of “the adjustment of first-year students to
“I Need to Belong”—This article talks about belongingness—a
university life” were distributed to first-year students
human motivational tendency to form certain relation-
ships or belong to various groups. It discusses positive living in residential colleges and studying at UNE (New
aspects, such as comfort and support, as well as the South Wales, Australia) 6 weeks into their first semester
negatives associated with a lack of belongingness, such as of study. The invitation to participate in the research
anxiety and loneliness. contained a packet of all study materials. Participants

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016


524   PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

were distributed the control condition questionnaire or at UNE (explaining 8.4% of variance), such that those
the experimental questionnaire at random, and response who felt more accepted also felt less homesick, and gen-
rates were almost identical across the two conditions. A der (explaining 2.6% of variance) in a positive direc-
response rate of 24% was obtained. This is a typical tion, indicating that females felt more homesick. Neither
response rate among student samples in Australia and social activity nor number of friends in the new location
did not arouse our concern. predicted homesickness. The interaction of priming
need to belong and acceptance also did not have a sig-
Results and Discussion nificant effect on homesickness, nor did the interaction
of priming need to belong and number of close friends
Homesickness in the new location. Results of the regression analysis at
Step 3 are shown in Table 4.
Homesickness in the control condition showed an
The regression analysis was repeated for each home-
average elevation of 2.12 (SD = .77), which most closely
sickness subscale (adjustment difficulties and rumina-
corresponds with “weak” on the 5-point scale. This is a
tions were first submitted to log 10 transformation to
little lower than the average elevation of 2.43 (SD = .92)
correct positive skew). Priming need to belong increased
among international students that we obtained in Study
homesickness on each subscale. Feeling accepted in the
1. It is comparable with the results obtained by Stroebe
new location was also associated with decreased home-
et al. (2002) of 2.26 (SD = .73) for their U.K. sample and
sickness on each subscale except missing family. Gender
1.93 (SD = .71) for their Dutch sample. Homesickness
predicted scores on just two subscales—loneliness and
was significantly higher in the experimental condition,
missing friends. The effect in both cases was in a posi-
t(142) = –4.62, p < .001, with an average elevation of
tive direction, indicating that females were more lonely
2.72 (SD = .78), which most closely corresponds with
and missed friends more than males.
“moderate” on the 5-point scale. The means and stand-
These results support Hypothesis 1, that need to
ard deviations of the predictor variables and the correla-
belong has a causal effect on homesickness, and are
tions among them are shown in Table 3.
consistent with those of Study 1, especially in the finding
A hierarchical multiple regression analysis was con-
that feeling accepted in the new location predicted less
ducted to examine the predictors of homesickness. The
homesickness on all homesickness subscales except for
predictors entered into Step 1 of the regression model were
missing family (supports Hypothesis 3). Once again,
gender (coded as 1 = male, 2 = female), acceptance in the
there was no evidence to support Hypothesis 2, that
new location, social activity, and number of friends in the
making new social contacts will reduce homesickness.
new location. Step 2 then entered the need to belong
That is, homesickness was not predicted by number of
manipulation (coded as 0 = control condition and 1 =
friends in the new location.
experimental condition). Step 3 entered the interactions of
social acceptance and number of close friends in the new
Contact Initiated With Home
location with the need to belong manipulation. Social activ-
ity and number of friends were submitted to log 10 trans- The variables relating to contact home were substan-
formation prior to analysis to correct positive skew, and the tially skewed. Most participants had initiated a moderate
data were screened for multivariate outliers at p < .001. amount of contact with home in the previous 4 weeks,
Homesickness scores were normally distributed, and unlike with a median of 6.5 phone calls made, 12 mobile phone
Study 1 did not require transformation. Before including text messages sent, and 4 e-mail messages sent. However,
acceptance in the regression model, we checked whether some showed extremely high frequency of contact, with
the experimental manipulation had any effect on this vari- a maximum of 100 phone calls made, 500 mobile phone
able. No significant effect was found, so we proceeded with text messages sent, and 200 e-mails sent. As in Study 1,
including acceptance as a predictor in the regression there was also a significant portion of participants who
model. did not make any phone calls (5.6%), send any mobile
The regression model was statistically significant at phone text messages (16.7%), or send any e-mails
Step 1 (R2 = .17, p < .001). However, it was significantly (22.2%). We again used number of phone calls as the
improved by the addition of the need to belong manipu- dependent variable in the analysis as more people would
lation at Step 2 (R2 change = .09, p < .001). Thus, con- have access to this technology; this provided results that
sistent with Hypothesis 1 there was a significant effect are directly comparable with Study 1.
of need to belong on homesickness. Including the mod- A hierarchical multiple regression analysis was con-
eration terms at Step 3 did not significantly improve the ducted on participants in the control group only to
model (R2 change = .001, p = .94). The other significant examine predictors of phone calls home. The predictors
predictors of homesickness were feelings of acceptance entered on Step 1 of the regression model were gender,

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016


Watt, Badger / SOCIAL BELONGING AND HOMESICKNESS   525

TABLE 3:   Summary Statistics and Correlations Between Variables in Study 2

Correlations

Median M SD 1 2 3 4 5

Gender (1), coded 1 = male, 2 = female — — — 1


Acceptance at University of New England (2) 4.00 4.15 0.78 –.05 1
Social activity (3)  8.00 9.60 7.08 –.06 .15*** 1
Number of friends in new location (4) 4.00 4.60 4.00 .10 .15 .28*** 1
Homesickness (5) 2.40 2.42 0.83 .19* –.37*** –.14 –.05 1
Phone calls made (6) 6.50 12.80 18.20 .25** –.07 .13 .18* .42***

*p ≤ .05. **p ≤ .01. ***p ≤ .001.

acceptance in the new location, social activity, and TABLE 4:  Predictors of Homesickness and Phone Calls Home in
number of friends in the new location. Step 2 then Study 2
entered homesickness and the interaction of homesick-
ness and acceptance. Phone calls home was first submit- Homesickness Phone Calls
ted to log 10 transformation to correct positive skew. As at Step 3 Home at
in Study 1, homesickness was entered on a second step of the Step 2 of
Analysisa the Analysisb
in case it masked any other effects. This was not the
case, so only the results at Step 2 of the analysis are β sr β sr
reported.
Priming need .31*** .31*** — —
The regression model explained 35.9% of the vari-
to belong
ance in phone calls home. The significant predictors Gender .17* .16* .15 .14
were social activity and homesickness (see Table 4). Acceptance at –.31*** –.29*** .05 .04
Homesickness was the strongest predictor, explaining    University
16.4% of the variance. The more homesick, the more    of New England
Social activity –.09 –.09 .25* .24*
phone calls home. Social activity was the only other
Number of friends in .04 .04 .18 .17
significant predictor of phone calls home, explaining    new location
5.8% of the variance in a positive direction. Gender, Priming Need to –.02 –.02 — —
acceptance at UNE, and number of friends in the new    Belong × Acceptance
location did not predict phone calls home, and none of Priming Need to .02 .01 — —
   Belong × Friends
the interaction terms showed a significant effect.
Homesickness — — .44*** .41***
As a final check, we repeated the test conducted in Homesickness × — — .20 .17
Study 1 to examine the relationship between phone    Acceptance at UNE
calls made and received as predictors of homesickness.
a. R = .52, R2 = .27, adjusted R2 = .23, F(7, 135) = 6.99, p < .001.
The results were almost identical to those of Study 1. b. R = .60, R2 = .36, adjusted R2 = .30, F(6, 63) = 5.87, p < .001.
Phone calls made significantly predicted homesickness *p ≤ .05. ***p ≤ .001.
(sr = .20, p = .003), but phone calls received did not
(sr = .05, p = .41). This result again suggests the act of Australia for varying lengths of time, and were confront-
speaking on the phone does not relate to increased or ing cultural differences of more or less extremity. The
decreased homesickness. second sample was comparatively homogenous.
Participants were young Australians 6 weeks into their
university experience. They were not confronting issues
GENERAL DISCUSSION of adjustment to a new culture or discrimination within
the new community, and yet the same effects of need to
The purpose of this research was to investigate belong were present. The combination of correlational
whether homesickness arises in belongingness needs. and experimental results across these two samples adds
The results indicate a causal role of need to belong in to our confidence that need to belong has a causal role in
homesickness. Study 1 found that need to belong cor- homesickness.
related with homesickness, and Study 2 found that This finding offers a valuable new perspective on
priming need to belong influenced feelings of homesick- homesickness. Previous theoretical work has described
ness. These results were obtained in quite different sam- homesickness in frameworks of grief and loss and
ples. The first included participants of many different attachment. These both relate to belongingness needs;
ages, from 42 different countries, who had been in grief arises in the loss of important social connections,

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016


526   PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

and attachment concerns security in one’s bonds with Making friends also did not predict homesickness.
other people. The belongingness hypothesis can there- Previous research has shown that people expect that
fore be seen as a metatheory that encompasses these making new friends will predict positive adjustment
more specific effects of need to belong. and reduce homesickness. Ryan and Twibell (2000)
Need to belong predicted all subscales of the Utrecht reported that a primary concern in international stu-
Homesickness Scale, both loss-oriented (missing family, dents is fitting in socially, and Archer et al. (1998) and
missing friends, loneliness, ruminations about home) Kane (as cited in Archer et al., 1998) found that dis-
and restoration-oriented (adjustment difficulties). In satisfaction with both friends and social life at univer-
other words, it related to homesickness arising in sepa- sity predicted homesickness. Paul and Brier’s (2001)
ration from the old location as well as homesickness study of “friendsickness” found that many students
arising in entry into the new location. This is consistent included only precollege friends in their social net-
with previous findings (Archer et al., 1998; Fisher, work at college, and Van Tilburg et al. (1999) con-
1989; Stroebe et al., 2002) and is also consistent with cluded from open-ended responses that the majority
predictions of the belongingness hypothesis; people are of students attributed recovery from homesickness to
expected to react with distress and protest at separa- making friends in the new location. However, this
tion from existing bonds and also to feel distressed if expectation (that making friends is important to home-
belongingness needs are not fulfilled in the new loca- sickness) may not be correct. Van Tilburg et al. (1997a)
tion or show little potential to be fulfilled. found the majority (52%) of homesickness sufferers
Although need to belong predicted homesickness, there who were surveyed while experiencing homesickness
was little evidence to support the substitution hypothesis. (in contrast with retrospective studies) did not attribute
The belongingness hypothesis proposes that individuals their homesickness to insufficient friends in the new
need a certain amount of relatedness and that social rela- location. The majority attributed it to missing persons
tionships should to some extent be interchangeable (82.7%), missing the environment (81.4%), and miss-
(Baumeister & Leary, 1995). If this is the case, people ing the atmosphere of the old environment (87.7%).
who have formed close relationships in the new location A possible explanation lies in the finding that home-
should be less prone to distress (homesickness) on reloca- sick people are less socially skilled than nonhomesick
tion as their belongingness needs are being met. people. Eurelings-Bontekoe et al. (1994) found that
In Study 1, length of residence in Australia and homesick military conscripts were more likely than con-
number of close friends in Australia did not predict trols to seek social support as a coping strategy. However,
how homesick participants felt. The same effect was these people were also less socially skilled and were
found in Study 2; number of close friends in the new therefore unable to obtain the support they desired.
location did not predict homesickness. One would Consistent with this, Van Tilburg et al. (1999) found
intuitively expect length of residence and number of that seeking social support does not predict adjust-
close friends to predict reduced homesickness, but the ment and suggested that this may be due to poorer
data from previous studies are mixed. For example, social skills in homesick people. In this case, our data
the longer one stays in a new location, the more indicate that even though homesick people report
opportunity to establish meaningful new relationships. similar numbers of friends to less homesick people,
However, effects of length of stay have been very small they may be less able to obtain social activity and
or nonsignificant in other studies. Stroebe et al. (2002) support from those friends. This could lead to the dis-
reported slight but significant effects of duration of satisfaction with friends and social life that predicted
stay among new intake students (Dutch sample path continued homesickness in the studies by Archer et al.
coefficient = –.10; U.K. sample path coefficient = (1998) and Kane (as cited in Archer et al., 1998). The
–.13), and Hojat and Hermann (1985) reported small relation between social activity and homesickness in
but significant correlations between length of stay and Study 1 may also reflect the operation of social skill
a single-item measure of homesickness in Iranian and as an underlying variable. Individuals with more
Filipino physicians in the United States, but unfortu- social skill may be more likely to be included in social
nately did not provide details of these results. In a outings and also less likely to feel homesick because of
more recent study, Tartakovsky (2007) reported in the dynamic previously described. However, an alter-
adolescents who migrated to Israel without their par- native explanation is that social activity may generate
ents a very slight decline in homesickness after 2½ positive affect, which counteracts feelings of home-
years (homesickness declined from average of 2.18 on sickness. Future research could further address the
a 5-point scale 6 months after immigration to 2.08 2½ relation between social skills and homesickness and
years after immigration). However, all these effects are could also benefit from investigating the effects of
small and were not present in our data. social activity in reducing homesickness.

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016


Watt, Badger / SOCIAL BELONGING AND HOMESICKNESS   527

Feeling accepted in the new location was an important proportional to opportunities to develop new friendships.
predictor of homesickness in both our studies. This finding From this, we developed the hypothesis that those who feel
has a number of implications that we explore in the fol- less accepted in the new location will make more calls
lowing. Baumeister and Leary (1995) suggested that home because the new location offers less friendship
opportunities to develop new friendships on relocation potential. The two studies provided very similar results.
will be an important variable, and we extended this to sug- That is, neither study found a relationship between per-
gest that people may monitor a new environment for the ceived acceptance and phone calls home, but both found
potential to make new friends. In line with this, our results positive relations between social activity and phone calls
showed that people who feel less accepted by those in the home and between homesickness and phone calls home.
new community feel more homesick. This was found for This correlation does not tell us about causal direction, but
all subscales of the Utrecht Homesickness Scale in Study 1 there is a further hint in the data. If the causal direction is
and all but missing family in Study 2. Thus, like need to one where the act of speaking on the phone increases
belong, acceptance is related to distress at leaving the old homesickness, we would expect phone calls made and
location and at entering the new location. phone calls received both to relate to homesickness.
The relation between acceptance and homesickness However, both studies found no relation between phone
connects homesickness with community attitudes. This calls received and homesickness. This suggests that phone
is important because a great deal of research has calls do not perpetuate homesickness but are simply made
addressed community attitudes toward immigration, in response to feeling homesick. However, given that talk-
but little research has investigated the impact of these ing on the phone does not relate to reduced homesickness,
attitudes on immigrants. One exception is a longitudi- it is unlikely that making calls home is effective in relieving
nal study conducted by Tartakovsky (2007), which homesickness. They may simply act as a comfort when
found that perceived discrimination in the host country homesickness is strong.
was the strongest single predictor of acculturative stress People who were more socially active also tended to
and homesickness. make more phone calls home. This was originally
Newcomers in communities that are rejecting of immi- included as a control variable because we thought
gration, either domestic or international, can be expected engaging in pleasurable activities might generate posi-
to perceive poor community acceptance. Our results tive affect that would counteract homesickness. However,
show that this is independent of having a network of this cannot explain the relation between social activity
close and supportive friends as well as entry reference and phone calls home; the result may simply reflect the
group size—even newcomers who associate only with a operation of an underlying variable related to sociability
few welcoming friends are more likely to feel homesick or social support seeking.
and unhappy if they live in a rejecting community. These
results therefore add to the evidence that immigration Implications for the
attitudes have very real effects on immigrants. Belongingness Hypothesis
Of interest, social identity theory (Tajfel & Turner,
1986) emphasizes the importance of acceptance by the Need to belong was used in this research to generate
ingroup, but our data suggest that it is acceptance by a theoretical framework to offer a new perspective on
the community at large (“I feel accepted by Australians” homesickness. We have shown that homesickness
or “I feel accepted by people at UNE”) that predicts partly arises in this fundamental motivation. However,
homesickness; developing an ingroup (as indicated by these results also add to our understanding of the need
number of friends) did not predict homesickness in our to belong. Baumeister and Leary (1995) proposed that
studies. Future research on this topic would benefit people experience distress when their belongingness
from examining how acceptance is judged. A related needs are threatened. The current research adds to the
literature has found many biases in the perception of evidence for this basic component of belongingness
discrimination. For example, people commonly believe theory by showing that need to belong predicts the
that discrimination affects members of their group but distress (homesickness) experienced on relocation. Our
not themselves (Ruggiero & Taylor, 1995). We expect tests of the substitution hypothesis further explored the
that judging community acceptance will be an equally belongingness hypothesis, which proposes that the
biased process (Carvallo & Pelham, 2007). motivation to satiate the need to belong should be
reduced when there are sufficient social bonds. As
Contact With Home discussed previously, both studies failed to provide
support for this.
Baumeister and Leary (1995) suggested that efforts to One possibility that we have already considered is
sustain friendships across long distances should be inversely homesick people may have less social skill and are

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016


528   PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

therefore less able to elicit from new friends the close- However, these findings are preliminary. The questions
ness and support that they desire. We discuss here regarding number of friends, for example, did not ask
another possibility. Baumeister and Leary (1995) how close the friends were and how they compared
acknowledged that perhaps some kinds of relationships with friends in the old location. Future research should
cannot effectively be replaced with other kinds of rela- focus on which social connections are important in
tionships, and perhaps people need at least one particu- homesickness. This would allow counselors to advise
larly strong close attachment. Their particular example migrants and sojourners where they could focus their
was romantic relationships. However, Stroebe et al. efforts to relieve homesickness. Our results do suggest
(2002) found that the factor explaining by far the that homesickness should be reduced when the need to
most variance (62%) in the Utrecht Homesickness belong is satisfied in the new location. However, satisfy-
Scale was Missing Family. Family bonds are particu- ing the need to belong may rely more on making one or
larly important relationships. They often are geneti- two very close connections rather than making a large
cally linked, are very long lasting, involve a deep number of new friends.
commitment to the others’ well-being, and are not In summary, this research provides testimony to
easily replaced. It is possible that the relation between belongingness needs. Homesickness arises partly in the
homesickness and need to belong in both our studies need to belong and further relates to perceived accept-
arose mainly in separation from family and that mak- ance in the new location, indicating that happiness in a
ing new friends could not replace those special bonds. new location is partly determined by the potential to fit
We note that friends in the new location also did not in socially. The research implies that experiencing dis-
predict less homesickness on the Missing Friends sub- tress on leaving places where we already have a strong
scale of the Utrecht Homesickness Scale. Like family sense of belongingness is normal and the sense of loss
relations, perhaps long-established close friendships does not dissipate easily. It is simply a product of our
cannot easily be replaced. Future research should construction as social beings.
address which bonds are most necessary to fulfill the
need to belong and which can be substituted for one
another. This might vary with age. The sample of APPENDIX
international students in Study 1 was less homogenous ITEMS IN THE “PUBLICATION SURVEY”
than the sample in Study 2, but both samples were of
relatively young people for whom separation from CONTROL CONDITION
family may have been particularly painful.
“Comparison of Composition”—This article discusses
the various eras of art history and offers a unique
Summary and Directions for comparison of traditional and contemporary tech-
Future Research niques.
“Photography for Beginners”—This article discusses the
This research provides evidence that homesickness relationship between color and emotion.
arises partly in the need to belong. Belongingness needs “The ‘Natural’”—This article raises the nature/nurture
influenced the distress participants experienced on sepa- debate in relation to musicians and people with unique
rating from the old location and on entering the new talents–are they “naturals,” or is their ability the direct
result of learning and practice?
location, and this distress was greater when participants
“Vitamins”—This article provides information on a wide
perceived less acceptance in the new location. The rela- range of vitamins and discusses the comparative value
tionship between feeling accepted in the new location of multivitamins.
and homesickness forms an important bridge between “Experimental Music”—This article discusses an aspect of
research into immigration attitudes and migrant adjust- contemporary music in which everyday sounds and
ment. We are currently planning studies to explore noises are used in the creation of unique composition.
whether acceptance has a causal role in homesickness “Glasses or Contact Lenses?”—This article offers a com-
parison between glasses and contact lenses, including
and adjustment. Future research should also examine
the advantages and disadvantages of each. It also offers
how acceptance is perceived and whose acceptance is advice for choosing which is right for you.
most important “Study Techniques”—This article provides a comparison
As well as showing an effect of need to belong on of popular study techniques and helps you determine
homesickness, this research provided information perti- which method is right for you.
nent to belongingness theory. The results did not sup- “What to Wear”—This article offers advice for choosing
port the substitution hypothesis. Homesickness was not clothing to complement your body type—including sug-
gestions for color, pattern, and design.
reduced when participants had more friends in the new
location or when they had been there for longer. (continued)

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016


Watt, Badger / SOCIAL BELONGING AND HOMESICKNESS   529

APPENDIX (continued) De Cremer, D., & Leonardelli, G. J. (2003). Cooperation in social


dilemmas and the need to belong: The moderating effect of group
size. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research and Practice, 7, 168-174.
“Preschooling”—This article discusses the ramifications of Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does
early learning on achievement in later life. rejection hurt? An FMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302,
290-292.
Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and society (2nd ed.). New York:
Norton.
EXPERIMENTAL CONDITION Eurelings-Bontekoe, E. H. M., Vingerhoets, A., & Fontijn, T. (1994).
Personality and behavioral antecedents of homesickness.
Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 229-235.
“Where to Turn?”—This article highlights the importance of Fisher, S. (1989). Homesickness, cognition, and health. Hillsdale, NJ:
having someone to turn to, for support or advice. Lawrence Erlbaum.
“Photography for Beginners”—This article discusses the Fisher, S., & Hood, B. (1987). The stress of the transition to univer-
relationship between color and emotion. sity: A longitudinal study of psychological disturbance, absent-
“I Need to Belong”—This article talks about belongingness—a mindedness and vulnerability to homesickness. British Journal of
human motivational tendency to form certain relation- Psychology, 78, 425-441.
Fisher, S., & Hood, B. (1988). Vulnerability factors in the transition to
ships or belong to various groups. It discusses positive university: Self-reported mobility history and sex differences as fac-
aspects, such as comfort and support, as well as the negatives tors in psychological disturbance. British Journal of Psychology,
associated with a lack of belongingness, such as anxiety 79, 309-320.
and loneliness. Freud, S. (1946). Civilisation and its discontents (J. Riviere, Trans.).
“Vitamins”—This article provides information on a wide London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1930)
range of vitamins and discusses the comparative value Hojat, M., & Herman, M. W. (1985). Adjustment and psychosocial
problems of Iranian and Filipino physicians in the U.S. Journal of
of multivitamins. Clinical Psychology, 41, 130-136.
“Person Alone”—This article discusses loneliness and pos- Leary, M. R., Cottrell, C. A., & Phillips, M. (2001). Deconfounding
sible coping mechanisms for loneliness. the effects of dominance and social acceptance on self-esteem.
“Glasses or Contact Lenses?”—This article offers a com- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 898-909.
parison between glasses and contact lenses, including MacDonald, G., & Leary, M. R. (2005). Why does social exclusion
the advantages and disadvantages of each. It also offers hurt? The relationship between social and physical pain.
Psychological Bulletin, 131, 202-223.
advice for choosing which is right for you. Maslow, A. (1968). Motivation and personality. New York: Harper
“The Importance of Acceptance”—This article talks about & Row.
the psychological ramifications of feeling accepted by Millard, B. (2004). “Homesick.” Lyrics. Perf. MercyMe. Undone.
others. Inotof, 2004
“What to Wear”—This article offers advice for choosing Nesdale, D., & Mak, A. S. (2000). Immigrant acculturation attitudes
clothing to complement your body type—including sug- and host country identification. Journal of Community and
Applied Social Psychology, 10, 483-495.
gestions for color, pattern, and design. Paul, E. L., & Brier, S. (2001). Friendsickness in the transition to col-
“Moving on”—This article discusses the impact on peo- lege: Precollege predictors and college adjustment correlates.
ple’s lives of friends moving away. It offers advice for Journal of Counseling and Development, 79, 77-89.
coping with associated difficulties. Pickett, C. L., Gardner, W. L., & Knowles, M. L. (2004). Getting a
cue: The need to belong and enhanced sensitivity to social cues.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1095-1107.
Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. New York: Basic Books.
REFERENCES Ruggiero, K. M., & Taylor, D. M. (1995). Coping with discrimina-
tion: How disadvantaged group members perceive the discrimina-
Ainsworth, M. D. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological tion that confronts them. Journal of Personality and Social
study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Psychology, 68, 826-838.
Archer, J., Ireland, J., Amos, S.-L., Broad, H., & Currid, L. (1998). Ryan, M. E., & Twibell, R. S. (2000). Concerns, values, stress, cop-
Derivation of a homesickness scale. British Journal of Psychology, ing, health and educational outcomes of college students who
89, 205-221. studied abroad. International Journal of Intercultural Relations,
Baier, M., & Welch, M. (1992). An analysis of the concept of home- 24, 409-435.
sickness. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing, 6, 54-60. Stroebe, M., & Schut, M. (1987). The dual process model of coping
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire with bereavement: Rationale and description. Death Studies, 23,
for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motiva- 197-224.
tion. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497-529. Stroebe, M., van Vliet, T., Hewstone, M., & Willis, H. (2002).
Billig, M., & Tajfel, H. (1973). Social categorization and similarity in Homesickness among students in two cultures: Antecedents and
intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology, 3, consequences. British Journal of Psychology, 93, 147-168.
27-52. Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of inter-
Bowlby. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol 1. Attachment. New York: group behavior. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology
Basic Books. of intergroup relations (pp. 7-24). Chicago: Nelson.
Bowlby. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol 2. Separation anxiety and Tartakovsky, E. (2007). A longitudinal study of acculturative stress
anger. New York: Basic Books. and homesickness: High-school adolescents immigrating from
Burt, C. D. B. (1993). Concentration and academic ability following Russia and Ukraine to Israel without parents. Social Psychiatry
transition to university: An investigation of the effects of home- and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 42, 485-494.
sickness. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 13, 333-342. Thurber, C. A. (1999). The phenomenology of homesickness in boys.
Carvallo, M., & Pelham, B., W. (2007). When fiends become friends: Journal of Abnormal Child Psychiatry, 27, 125-139.
The need to belong and perceptions of personal and group dis- Van Tilburg, M. A. L. (2005). The psychological context of homesick-
crimination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, ness. In M. A. L. Van Tilburg & A. J. J. M. Vingerhoets (Eds.),
94-98. Psychological aspects of geographical moves: Homesickness and

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016


530   PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

acculturation stress (2nd ed., pp. 37-49). Tilburg, the Netherlands: and personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 27,
Tilburg University Press. 531-539.
Van Tilburg, M. A. L., Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M., & Van Heck, G. L. Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M. (2005). The homesickness concept: Questions
(1997a). Coping with homesickness: The construction of the adult and doubts. In M. A. L. Van Tilburg & A. J. J. M. Vingerhoets (Eds.),
homesickness coping questionnaire. Personality and Individual Psychological aspects of geographical moves: Homesickness and
Differences, 22, 901-907. acculturation stress (2nd ed., pp. 1-16). Tilburg, the Netherlands:
Van Tilburg, M. A. L., Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M., & Van Heck, G. L. Tilburg University Press.
(1997b). Homesickness: A review of the literature. Psychological
Medicine, 26, 899-912.
Van Tilburg, M. A. L., Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M., & Van Heck, G. L. Received October 8, 2007
(1999). Determinants of homesickness chronicity: Coping Revision accepted October 28, 2008

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016

You might also like