Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Aging & Mental Health

ISSN: 1360-7863 (Print) 1364-6915 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/camh20

Attentional avoidant biases as mediators in the


association between experiential avoidance and
blood pressure in dementia family caregivers

María Márquez-González, Isabel Cabrera, Andrés Losada & Bob G. Knight

To cite this article: María Márquez-González, Isabel Cabrera, Andrés Losada & Bob G. Knight
(2017): Attentional avoidant biases as mediators in the association between experiential avoidance
and blood pressure in dementia family caregivers, Aging & Mental Health

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2017.1293003

Published online: 02 Mar 2017.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 21

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=camh20

Download by: [University of Arizona] Date: 04 April 2017, At: 23:55


AGING & MENTAL HEALTH, 2017
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2017.1293003

Attentional avoidant biases as mediators in the association between experiential


avoidance and blood pressure in dementia family caregivers
Marıa Marquez-Gonzalez a
, Isabel Cabrera a
, Andres Losada b
and Bob G. Knightc
a
Department of Biological and Health Psychology, Universidad Autonoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain; bDepartment of Psychology, Universidad Rey
Juan Carlos, Madrid, Spain; cSchool of Psychology and Counselling, University of Southern Queensland, Toowoomba, Australia

ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY


Objectives: Experiential avoidance in caregiving (EAC) has been found to be related with dementia Received 31 May 2016
family caregivers distress and blood pressure (BP). The association between EAC and avoidant Accepted 5 February 2017
attentional biases to emotional stimuli in dementia caregivers, and the potential mediating role of KEYWORDS
these attentional biases in the association between EAC and increased BP are explored. Experiential avoidance;
Method: Seventy nine dementia family caregivers performed a dot-probe task with emotional pictures attentional biases; dementia
(distressing and positive) varying in content (general vs. caregiving-related (CR)) and time of exposure caregivers; blood pressure;
(100 vs. 500 ms). They also completed measures of EAC, anxiety, depression, alexithymia and dot probe task
rumination, and their BP was measured.
Results: EAC was associated with avoidant attentional biases to CR emotional pictures and negative
pictures in general at 100 ms. Experiential Avoidance in Caregiving Questionnaire (EACQ) ‘avoidant
behaviors’ and EACQ ‘intolerance of negativity’ factors were associated with diastolic and systolic BP,
respectively, with attentional avoidance of CR emotional pictures (distressing and positive,
respectively) mediating this association.
Conclusion: Attentional avoidance of CR emotional stimuli may be the link between EAC and
increased BP, as it prevents emotional processing and facilitates the maintenance of physiological
activation. EAC may pose a risk for cardiovascular disease in dementia caregivers.

Introduction Some studies find an anxiety-related pattern of vigilance-


avoidance: attention is initially oriented to threatening stimuli
Dementia caregiving represents a prototypical example of
(e.g. at 100 ms), but subsequently directed away from them
chronic stress that can lead to impairments in caregivers
(e.g. at 500 ms) (Koster, Crombez, Vershuere, Van Damme, &
physical health, due to the numerous physical and psychoso-
Wiersema, 2006; Lee & Knight, 2009), probably reflecting
cial demands they have to face during a long period of time
peoples attempts to regulate their negative emotion via dis-
(3–15 years) (Shahly et al., 2013; Vitaliano, Zhang, & Scanlan,
traction (Cisler & Koster, 2010).
2003). Caregivers distress have been found to be associated
with high blood pressure (BP) (Chattillion et al., 2013;
Mausbach, Patterson, Rabinowitz, Grant, & Schulz, 2007), which
is a risk factor for cardiovascular disease (Roepke et al., 2011). Experiential avoidance in dementia caregiving
EA in caregiving (EAC) occurs when the caregiver is unwilling to
remain in contact with particular negative experiences associ-
Experiential avoidance
ated to care and takes steps to escape from or to avoid them,
Experiential avoidance (EA) is the tendency to ignore or avoid trying to alter the form or frequency of these thoughts and feel-
aversive internal experiences (emotions, thoughts or sensa- ings and the contexts that occasion them (Marquez-Gonzalez,
tions) and the circumstances which trigger them (Hayes, Stro- Romero-Moreno, & Losada, 2010). EA has been identified as a
sahl, & Wilson, 2012). EA has been shown to be associated correlate of dementia caregiversdistress by the only two studies,
with different psychological problems, such as depression to our knowledge, which have analyzed this variable in demen-
and anxiety disorders, with physical health problems, and dys- tia caregivers using self-report methods (Losada, Marquez-
functional psychological variables such as alexithymia and Gonzalez, Romero-Moreno, & Lo pez, 2014; Spira et al., 2007).
rumination (Cribb, Moulds, & Carter, 2006; Tull, Gratz, Salters, Losada et al. (2014) identified three dimensions of EAC, as
& Roemer, 2004; Venta, Hart, & Sharp, 2013). assessed with the Experiential Avoidance in Caregiving Ques-
EA may be associated with attentional avoidance of dis- tionnaire (EACQ): (a) Factor 1: active avoidant behaviors (e.g. ‘I
tressing stimuli as seen in laboratory studies of attention and tend to ignore the negative thoughts that come to me about
emotion using paradigms such as the dot probe task my relative’); (b) Factor 2: intolerance of negative thoughts and
(MacLeod, Mathews, & Tata, 1986). Attentional biases have emotions towards the relative (e.g. ‘One shouldn’t feel rejection
been studied in clinically anxious and non-clinically high anx- or other unpleasant emotions about the person you’re caring
ious people, who have been found to show initial attentional for’); and (c) Factor 3: apprehension concerning negative inter-
bias towards distressing stimuli (Bar-Haim, Lamy, nal experiences related to caregiving (e.g. ‘I’m scared by the
Pargamin, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van Ijzendoorn, 2007). emotions and thoughts I have about my relative’).

CONTACT Marıa Marquez-Gonzalez maria.marquez@uam.es


© 2017 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group
2 
M. MARQUEZ-GONZ 
ALEZ ET AL.

Experiential avoidance in caregiving, blood pressure and tasks (Obisesan, 2009), and there is evidence that hyperten-
attentional biases sion may impair performance in tasks involving executive
functions, such as the dot probe task used in this study
In their study, Losada et al. (2014) found a positive correlation
(Harrington, Saxby, McKeith, Wesnes, & Ford, 2000; Raz,
between EACQ Factor 2 and mean arterial pressure, but
Rodrigue, & Acker, 2003). Five participants were excluded due
potential psychological or physiological mechanisms explain-
to their high error rate in the attentional task (see Results).
ing this association were not addressed. Several studies sup-
The final sample was composed of 79 caregivers (77.22%
port the association between avoidance-related traits and
women) with a mean age of 58.42 years old (SD = 13.56).
strategies such as avoidant and defensive coping style, denial
or suppression, and cardiovascular diseases in the general
population (Campbell-Sills, Barlow, Brown, & Hofmann, 2006; Procedure
Low, Stanton, & Bower, 2008), and in stressful situations such
The variables included in this study were collected as part of
as caregiving (Billings, Folkman, Acree, & Moskowitz, 2000).
an assessment protocol for a wider intervention study (Losada
Although the specific physiological mechanisms linking the
et al., 2015). Given that the measurement of attentional biases
avoidance of negative emotions and thoughts with poorer
was not included in all the stages of the study, the sample
health are not yet fully understood, it has been associated
included in the present study is a subsample of the one
with increased neuroendocrine reactivity, implying increased
included in the Losada et al. (2014) article, which describes
sympathetic nervous system activity (Gross & John, 2003), and
the development and initial validation of the EACQ but does
an increased physiological threat reactivity (Mendes, Reis,
not analyze caregiversattentional biases.
Seery, & Blascovich, 2003). These physiological reactions, in
Assessment interviews took place in the Health and Social
turn, have cumulative adverse effects potentially leading to
Services Centers. First, participants read and signed an
health problems such as increased BP (Matthews et al., 2004).
informed consent form, and then BP was measured. After BP
Self-reported EAC may be associated with attentional
measurement, sociodemographic, health and stress variables,
biases involving the avoidance of the processing of distress-
alexithymia, rumination and EA were assessed. Then, BP was
ing emotional stimuli (Lo pez et al., 2010; Pickett & Kurby,
measured again and, after that, participants performed the
2010), which can be captured with a dot probe attentional
attentional task and anxiety and depression were measured,
task. This EAC-related pattern of attentional avoidance may
followed by the third and last BP measurement.
prevent the emergence of uncomfortable emotions and
All procedures contributing to this work comply with the
thoughts in caregivers and may contribute to explain the con-
ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional
nection between EAC and increased BP which has been found
committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki
in previous studies (Losada et al., 2014).
Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.
The aims of the present study are: (1) to explore the atten-
tional correlates of EAC in a sample of dementia caregivers;
(2) to test the potential mediating role of avoidant attentional Materials
biases in the relationship between EA and BP; and (3) to ana-
Questionnaires
lyze the associations between EAC and other relevant varia-
Contextual, health and stress variables. Caregivers age and
bles in the understanding of caregivers distress: anxiety,
number of daily hours of sleep were assessed in the interview.
depression, alexithymia and rumination.
Disruptive behaviors of the care recipient were assessed with
Drawing upon the stress and coping model adapted to care-
the 8-items Disruptive Behaviors subscale of the Revised
giving (Knight & Sayegh, 2010), and integrating the EA literature
Memory and Behavioural Problems Checklist (RMBPC) (Teri
described above, three hypothesis were formulated according
et al., 1992).
to which: (a) EAC will be associated with attentional biases
Experiential Avoidance in Caregiving Questionnaire (Losada
reflecting avoidance of caregiving-related (CR) distressing emo-
et al., 2014). The EACQ was developed to measure the specific
tional stimuli; specifically, a pattern of vigilance-avoidance will
manifestations of EA most prevalent in caregivers. It consists
be observed, with attentional vigilance of these stimuli at a
of 15 items and presents a three-factor structure: (1) Factor 1:
short exposure time (100 ms) and attentional avoidance of
avoidant behaviors; (2) Factor 2: intolerance of negativity
those stimuli at a longer exposure time (500 ms); (b) avoidant
(negative thoughts and emotions towards the relative); and
attentional biases will mediate the association between EAC
(3) Factor 3: apprehension of negativity (negative internal
and caregiversBP; and (c) EAC will be positively associated with
experiences related to caregiving).
emotion-regulation variables (alexithymia and rumination) and
Depressive symptomatology. The Center for Epidemiological
with caregiversdistress (anxiety and depression).
Studies-Depression Scale (CES-D) (Radloff, 1977) was used. It
consists of 20 items assessing depressive symptoms that care-
Method givers’ might have felt over the last week.
Anxiety. Anxiety symptomatology was measured via the 9-
Participants
item tension-anxiety subscale from the profile of mood states
Participants were 102 dementia family caregivers (74.51% (POMS) (McNair, Lorr, & Droppleman, 1971).
women) recruited through Health and Social Services Centers Alexithymia. This variable was assessed with the factor ‘dif-
(e.g. day-care centers). Caregivers were required to be at least ficulty identifying and describing feelings’ of the Toronto
18 years old, to identify themselves as the main source of Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) (M€ uller, B€
uhner, & Ellgring, 2003),
help for their relative, to report devoting more than one daily which measures difficulty identifying feelings and distinguish-
hour to caregiving duties and that they had been providing ing between feelings and the bodily sensations of emotional
care for more than three consecutive months. Hypertensive arousal.
caregivers (n = 18) were excluded, as the use of medication Rumination. This variable was measured with the Rumina-
for their condition may affect their performance on cognitive tive Responses Scale (RRS-Brief Version) (Jackson & Nolen-
AGING & MENTAL HEALTH 3

Hoeksema, 1998), which assesses the frequency of appear- the study. These pictures were of two types: (a) negative:
ance of ruminative thoughts. scenes of frail, dependent, ill or distressed (e.g. sad, scared,
Blood pressure. Systolic blood pressure (SBP) and diastolic etc.) older adults in a caregiving context (e.g. a sad old
(DBP) blood pressure were measured at three different points woman with dementia sat on a wheelchair); and (b) positive:
during the interview (see Procedure). No significant differen- frail elderly people in harmonious and happy scenes with
ces were found between these three measures (for SBP: F(2,77) their relatives, with positive facial expressions (e.g. a smiling
= 1.26; p = .29; for DBP: F(2,77) = 0.33, p = .72). Global scores for frail old man hugged by his son). An initial pool of 257 pic-
SBP and DBP were calculated by obtaining the mean between tures with CR content was created by the members of the
the three measurements taken during the interview. The BP research team. Nine independent people were asked to make
readings were obtained using a noninvasive monitor (OMRON two judgments for each picture: (a) What is the emotional
M7) validated according to the British Hypertension Society valence? (positive, distressing or neutral); (b) Is the picture
Protocol (Coleman, Steel, Freeman, de Greeff, & Shennan, related to the context of caregiving and older adultsdepend-
2008), following the recommendations for its measurement ency? (yes/no). Only those pictures in which all nine judges
(O’Brien et al., 2003). agreed that its content was caregiving related were selected:
80 pictures which were judged by all judges as ‘distressing’
Attentional task and 80 as ‘positive’.
The dot probe task (MacLeod et al., 1986) was used to mea-
sure attentional biases. This task consists of the simultaneous
presentation of two stimuli (e.g. faces) for a brief duration in a
separate location on the computer screen (e.g. left/right). On
Data analysis
the critical trials, one stimulus is emotionally neutral and the
other is emotional (negative or positive). After the stimuli pair Reaction time data (RT) from the dot probe task were analyzed.
disappears, a neutral probe (e.g. dot) appears in the same Attentional bias scores for each emotional picture content
screen location of one of the stimuli. Participants are then (general/CR) and valence of pictures (distressing/positive) at
asked to identify the probe as quickly as possible by pressing both exposure times (100 and 500 ms) were calculated for
a response key. Responses are most rapid in trials where the each participant, by subtracting the mean RT when probes
probe appears in the spatial location where the participant is appeared in the same location as the emotional picture, from
already attending. the mean RT when probes occurred in the opposite location
(e.g. Mogg, Millar, & Bradley, 2000). The following attentional
Trial types and timing trials. The task consisted of 16 practice, bias scores were obtained: attentional biases towards general
320 experimental trials and 80 filler trials which were pre- distressing pictures at 100 ms (AB-GD-100) and at 500 ms (AB-
sented in a new random order for each participant. There GD-500); towards CR distressing pictures at 100 ms (AB-CRD-
were 160 trials with general emotional pictures (80 distressing 100) and at 500 ms (AB-CRD-500); towards positive general pic-
and 80 positive), 160 trials with CR emotional pictures (80 dis- tures at 100 ms (AB-GP-100) and at 500 ms (AB-GP-500); and
tressing and 80 positive) and 80 trials with neutral pictures towards CR positive pictures at 100 ms (AB-CRP-100) and at
(filler trials). Each trial began with a central fixation cross for 500 ms (AB-CRP-500). Positive values of attentional bias scores
500 ms. All experimental trials consisted of a pair of pictures, indicate vigilance for the emotional picture and negative val-
an emotional and a neutral one. One of the pictures was dis- ues reflect attentional avoidance.
played on the left of the screen and the other on the right Participants were divided into a high or low EAC group on
and remained for 100 or 500 ms. Immediately following the the basis of a median split carried out on their scores on the
display of the picture pair, the probe stimulus was presented EACQ. In order to explore the avoidant attentional correlates
in the location of one of the pictures, and participants were of EAC, a 2 £ 2 £ 2 £ 2 mixed design ANOVA of attentional
required to press one of two keys in a response pad (one bias scores was carried out. The between-subjects variable
located at the left and one at the right) to indicate the type of was the EAC group (low/high in EACQ). The within-subject
probe (: or ..). The probe remained displayed until a response variables were emotional valence of pictures (distressing/posi-
was made. The next trial commenced 1000 ms later. Experi- tive), picture content (general/CR) and exposure time (100/
mental trials were balanced for the position of the emotional 500 ms). Omega squared (v2) effect sizes were calculated.
picture and the target to be detected, for the exposure time In order to test the mediating role of attentional biases in
and for the type of probe. the association between EAC and BP, two path models were
estimated to predict SBP and DBP (through the AMOS 21 soft-
Experimental stimuli. In order to explore the specificity of ware). Following the suggestions by McDonald and Ho (2002),
avoidance effects, emotional stimuli both general and specifi- an analysis of the correlations between the variables included
cally related to dementia caregiving were used. Pictures with in the study was performed prior to the performance of path
general content were selected from images collected in the analysis. Indices of model fit included the chi-square (x2) sta-
International Affective Picture System (IAPS) (Lang, Bradley, & tistic; the chi-square value divided by the degrees of freedom
Cuthbert, 1999). Three types of pictures were selected, (x2/df), with values under or near three indicating good
according to its emotional valence as classified in the IAPS: (a) model fit (Bollen, 1989); the root-mean-square error of
negative (e.g. scenes of persons with expressions of negative approximation (RMSEA), with values under .06 indicating
emotions and/or in emotionally impacting situations); (b) pos- good fit, the comparative fit index (CFI) and the Tucker-Lewis
itive (e.g. people laughing); and (c) neutral (e.g. a car). index (TLI), with values over .95 indicating good fit for the
Pictures with CR content were selected both from the IAPS latter two (Hu & Bentler, 1998). Mediation was analyzed fol-
and other sources in Internet, as the range of pictures with lowing Preacher and Hayes (Preacher & Hayes, 2004) recom-
this type of content in the IAPS fell short for the purposes of mended bootstrapping approach.
4 
M. MARQUEZ-GONZ 
ALEZ ET AL.

Results interaction effect was found, F(1,78) = 7.762, p = .007, v2 =


.041. Participants with high levels of EAC avoided distressing
Participants with more than 25% of trials with errors in the dot
pictures and attended to positive pictures at 100 ms, whereas
probe task (n = 5) were excluded from the analyses. First, RT
those with low levels avoided positive pictures at 100 ms
data from trials with errors were discarded (1.49% of data).
(Figure 1).
Second, we verified that there were not any RTs lower than
The second significant three-way interaction effects was
250 ms. Third, RTs of more than 1.96 SDs (p < .05) below or
EAC x picture content x exposure time, F(1,77) = 6.587, p = .012,
above each participant’s mean in each trial type were
v2 = .017. To break down this interaction data were also
excluded in order to reduce the influence of outliers (Heerena,
examined for 100 and 500 ms separately and a 2 £ 2 mixed
Mogoas, McNallyc, Schmitz, & Philippot, 2015). This resulted in
design ANOVA of attentional bias scores were carried out for
the elimination of 5.01% of the trials.
each exposure time. The between-subjects variable was the
EAC (low/high in EACQ). The within-subject variables were the
picture content (general/CR). There were no significant main
Analysis of the results in the dot probe task: ANOVA
or interaction effects at 500 ms. However, at 100 ms a signifi-
There were no significant main effects, but two significant cant interaction effect was found, F(1,77) = 5.004, p = .028, v2 =
three way-interaction effects were found. First, EAC x picture .024. Participants with high levels of EAC avoided CR pictures
valence x exposure time, F(1,77) = 6.272, p = .014, v2 = .016. To and attended to general pictures at 100 ms, whereas those
break down this interaction, data were examined for 100 with low levels avoided general pictures and attended CR
and 500 ms separately and a 2 £ 2 mixed design ANOVA of information at 100 ms (Figure 2).
attentional bias scores were carried out for each exposure
time. The between-subjects variable was the EAC (low/high in
Path analyses
EACQ). The within-subject variables were the picture valence
(distressing /positive). There were no significant main or inter- Drawing on the stress and coping model adapted to caregiving
action effects at 500 ms. However, at 100 ms a significant (Knight & Sayegh, 2010), and on the basis of the correlation

10
Mean attentional biases (ms) at 100

2
ms

Positive
-2
Negative

-6

-10
Low High
Experiential avoidance in caregiving
Figure 1. Mean attentional biases (ms) at 100 ms for positive and distressing pictures in low and high EAC caregivers.

10
Mean attentional biases (ms) at 100

-2
ms

General

-6 Caregiving

-10

-14
Low High
Experiential avoidance in caregiving
Figure 2. Mean attentional biases (ms) at 100 ms for general and CR pictures in low and high EAC caregivers.
AGING & MENTAL HEALTH 5

Table 1. Correlations between the variables of the study.


EACQ Factor 1: EACQ Factor 2: EACQ Factor 3: EACQ
Avoidant Intolerance of Apprehension of Total
behaviors negativity negativity score SBP DBP Alexithymia Rumination Anxiety Depression
AB-GD-100 ¡.06 .12 ¡.05 .02 ¡.19 ¡.17 .03 .24 ¡.08 ¡.19
AB-GD-500 .10 ¡.04 .01 .04 .08 ¡.01 .02 .14 ¡.01 ¡.10

AB-CRD-100 ¡.26 ¡.08 ¡.11 ¡.24 ¡.21 ¡.26
¡.01 ¡.14 ¡.14 ¡.08
AB-CRD-500 ¡.06 ¡.03 ¡.15 ¡.11 ¡.00 ¡.17 ¡.17 .05 .01 .08
AB-GP-100 .18 .44 .05 .38 .07 .04 .07 .12 ¡.08 ¡.06
AB-GP-500 ¡.24 ¡.06 ¡.22 ¡.25 ¡.05 ¡.09 ¡.16 ¡.20 ¡.03 ¡.01
AB-CRP-100 .11 ¡.16 .37 .11 ¡.28 ¡.13 .14 ¡.14 ¡.01 ¡.02
AB-CRP-500 .01 ¡.23 .09 ¡.10 ¡.30 ¡.24 .01 ¡.20 ¡.29 ¡.27
SBP .08 .22 ¡.13 .12 ¡ .77 ¡.15 .04 .17 .19
DBP .23 .15 ¡.11 .18 .77 ¡ .03 .14 .26 .18
Alexithymia .46 ¡.06 .36 .38 ¡.15 .03 ¡ .38
.33
.23
Rumination .37 ¡.07 .16 .24 .04 .14 .38 ¡ .61 .51
Anxiety .17 .01 .05 .12 .17 .26 .33 .61 ¡ .66
Depression .08 ¡.11 ¡.05 ¡.03 .20 .17 .23 .51 .66 ¡
Disruptive behaviors .02 .06 .17 .04 .03 .01 .08 .26 .31 .23
of care recipient
Sleep hours ¡.12 .06 ¡.08 ¡.07 ¡.16 ¡.14 .24 .17 .41 ¡.21
Caregivers age ¡.15 .05 ¡.15 ¡.12 .38 .13 ¡.14 ¡.12 ¡.11 .14
Caregivers gender ¡.11 .17 .03 .03 .15 ¡.01 ¡.14 ¡.24 ¡.20 ¡.19
Note: AB-GD-100 = attentional bias towards general distressing pictures at 100 ms; AB-GD-500 = attentional bias towards general distressing pictures at 500 ms;
AB-CRD-100 = attentional bias towards CR distressing pictures at 100 m; AB-CRD-500 = attentional bias towards CR distressing pictures at 500 ms; AB-GP-100
= attentional bias towards positive general pictures at 100 ms; AB-GP-500 = attentional bias towards positive gGeneral pictures at 500 ms; AB-CRP-100 =
attentional bias towards CR positive pictures at 100 ms; AB-CRP-500 = attentional bias towards CR positive pictures 500 ms; Gender: 0 = female, 1 = male.

p .05.

p  .01.

p .001.

analysis (Table 1), separate models for SBP and DBP were gen- Initially, all paths reflecting the significant associations
erated. Mean SBP was 11.87 (SD = 1.75; range = 8.60–16.30) showed in the correlation matrix were tested. However, fol-
and mean DBP was 7.70 (SD = 0.99; range = 5.60–10.10). lowing a model generating strategy (Joreskog, 1993), only
Contextual and health variables (caregivers’ age, gender and those associations that remained significant (p  .05) once all
hours of sleep) and caregiving stressors (care recipient’s disrup- the considered variables were included were retained in the
tive behaviors) were included in both models. Besides these final model. Figures 3 and 4 show the final models for SBP
variables, emotion-regulation variables and attentional biases and DBP, respectively.
which had shown to be significantly associated with either EAC The obtained model for SBP (Figure 3) explained 19% of
(EACQ ‘Avoidant Behaviors’ and ‘Intolerance of Negativity’) or the variance in caregivers’ SBP. The obtained fit indices (x2 =
BP (SBP or DBP) were included in the models. 6.54; p = .37; x2/df = 1.09; CFI = .985; TLI = .976 and RMSEA =
The SBP model included the following variables: (a) EACQ- .034) suggest an adequate fit of the model to the data. The
intolerance of negativity factor (Factor 2) and AB-CRP-500, as results of the bootstrap analysis for testing mediation suggest
they correlated significantly with SBP; and (b) AB-GP-100, due that the indirect effect of EACQ-Intolerance of negativity
to its significant correlation with EACQ-intolerance of negativ- (Factor 2) on SBP through the attentional bias AB-CRP-500 is
ity (Factor 2). In the DBP model, the following variables were significant (standardized indirect effect = .06; p = .04; SE = .04;
included: (a) EACQ-avoidant behaviors factor (Factor 1), AB- 95% CI = 0.03–0.16).
CRD-100 and anxiety, as they had shown significant associa- Regarding DBP (Figure 4), the obtained model explained
tions with DBP; (b) AB-GP-500, alexithymia and rumination, 11% of the variance of caregivers DBP. The obtained fit
because they were significantly associated with EACQ-avoi- indexes also suggest a good fit of the model to the data (x2 =
dant behaviors factor (Factor 1). 27.68; p = .43; x2/df = 1.03; CFI = .993; TLI = .991, and RMSEA

Attentional bias towards


General Positive
Pictures at 100 ms
.45
(AB-GP-100)
Experiential avoidance
“Intolerance of negativity”
Attentional bias towards
-.23 Care-Related Positive
Pictures at 500 ms
-.26
(AB-CRP-500)
.19
Caregivers´ age Systolic BP
.36

Figure 3. Model with SBP as the outcome variable.


Note: All paths significant at the p < .05 level. Standardized coefficients are displayed.Errors have been omitted to improve clarity.
6 
M. MARQUEZ-GONZ 
ALEZ ET AL.

Attentional Bias
towards Care-Related
Alexithymia -.24 Distressing pictures at
100 (AB-CRD-100)
.47
Experiential -.26 Attentional Bias
avoidance towards Care-Related
“Avoidant Behaviors” Distressing pictures at -.23
.28 100 (AB-CRD-100) .11

Problem behaviors Rumination Diastolic BP


in CR .26 .57
.57
Hours of sleep -.38
Anxiety

Figure 4. Model with DBP as the outcome variable.


Note: All paths significant at the p < .05 level. Standardized coefficients are displayed.Errors have been omitted to improve clarity.

= .018). The bootstrap analysis for testing mediation suggest 30 ms) (Lee & Knight, 2009; Mogg, Bradley, & Williams, 1995).
that the indirect effect of EACQ-avoidant behaviors (Factor 1) Interestingly, the pattern of very rapid vigilance of threaten-
on DBP through the attentional bias AB-CRD-100 is significant ing cues has been found in repressors (Derakshan, Eysenck, &
(standardized indirect effect = .06; p = .04; SE = .04; 95% CI = Myers, 2007). Future studies using shorter, even subliminal,
0.02–0.16). This model also confirms the significant associa- exposure times in the attentional task are needed to explore
tions between anxiety and DBP, as well as between EACQ- the hypothesis of an EAC-related early vigilance. Also, studies
avoidant behaviors and the emotion regulation variables using eye-tracking methodology may allow the measurement
(alexithymia and rumination). of gaze and focus of visual attention.
In both models, the associations between BP and the
mediating attentional biases are negative, which suggests
that increased BP is associated with either less attentional vig-
Attentional biases as mediators of the association
ilance or more avoidance of the emotional stimuli addressed
between EAC and blood pressure
by the bias. To clarify the direction of this association, inde-
pendent t-test were performed, comparing caregivers with BP The results of this study confirm previous findings regarding
scores in the lower and upper tertiles with regard to the mag- the significant association between EAC and BP (Losada et al.,
nitude of the associated attentional biases (AB-CRP-500 for 2014). In this study, EACQ ‘Avoidant Behaviors’ (Factor 1) and
SBP and AB-CRD-100 for DBP). This analysis revealed that high EACQ ‘Intolerance of Negativity’ (Factor 2) have been found
SBP caregivers displayed an avoidant bias for CR positive pic- to be associated, respectively, with DBP and SBP. Further-
tures (M = ¡9.9; SD = 59.8) which significantly differed (t50 = more, these relationships are mediated by attentional biases.
2.1, p = .043) from the vigilant attentional bias towards these The association between EACQ ‘Avoidant Behaviors’ and DBP
pictures showed by low SBP caregivers (M = 19.9; SD = 42.6). is mediated by the atentional bias towards CR Distressing pic-
In a similar way, high DBP caregivers showed an avoidant bias tures at 100 ms, while the association between EACQ ‘Intoler-
for CR distressing pictures (M = ¡18.7; SD = 42.7), which was ance of Negativity’ and SBP is mediated by the attentional
significantly different (t50 = 2.2, p = .031) from the vigilant bias bias towards CR Positive pictures at 500 ms. In both cases, the
showed by low SBP caregivers (M = 8.7; SD = 47.3). association between attentional biases and BP is inverse and
reflects that increased BP is associated with more avoidance
of caregiving-related emotional pictures (CR distressing pic-
Discussion
tures at 100 ms for DBP, and CR positive pictures at 500 ms
In the following sections, the main findings of the study are for SBP). These results suggest that EAC-attentional avoidance
discussed. is not limited to negatively valenced stimuli, and that the
overgeneralization of attentional avoidance to positive
aspects of caregiving may be associated with physiological
Experiential avoidance and avoidant attentional biases
stress.
The ANOVA reveals that EAC is associated with avoidance of: Attentional avoidance of CR emotional stimuli may prevent
(a) distressing emotional pictures; and (b) CR emotional pic- emotional processing to be completed and conscious emo-
tures. These biases have been detected at 100 ms, which sug- tional experience to arise, while also facilitating the mainte-
gest that they operate very early in the processing of nance of the physiological activation which has been already
emotional information. Assuming that, in order to avoid generated by the preattentive processing of these stimuli
attending a stimulus, it has to be previously processed at (Derakshan et al., 2007). This heightened physiological activa-
some level, these results suggest that EAC may be associated tion could contribute to explain the association found in this
with a heightened sensitivity to detect potential distressing study between EAC and increased BP. Furthermore, EAC-
stimuli, which may operate at a very early stage of informa- related attentional avoidance of CR emotional stimuli may

tion processing or even preattentively (Ohman & Soares, interfere with caregivers appropriate coping with caregiving
1993). Previous studies have detected preattentive biases in problems or situations, hampering the identification of com-
anxious subjects using very short exposure times (e.g. 14 or ponents of problematic situations or care-recipient behaviors
AGING & MENTAL HEALTH 7

which could be essential for their resolution (e.g. contextual volunteered to participate in the study. This characteristic lim-
factors, care recipient’s facial expressions). its the generalizability of the results to dementia caregivers
who are less prone to participate in this type of studies.
Life style factors and health variables known to be associated
Experiential avoidance, emotion regulation and distress
with BP (e.g. Body Mass Index, smoking, etc.) were not assessed
This study supports previous findings showing that the EACQ and, consequently, were not controlled as potential confound-
‘Avoidant Behaviors’ factor (Factor 1) is related to alexithymia ers. However, given the preliminary nature of this study, we
and rumination, emotion regulation variables which have believe that, despite these limitations, it provides interesting
shown to be relevant in the explanation of caregiving stress findings which can guide future research studies which can
and coping (Losada et al., 2014). Alexithymic caregivers diffi- control these important biological variables.
culty to appropriately identify and describe negative emotions
may contribute to make them distressing, and thus may facili-
tate avoiding experiencing them. The association between EAC Concluding remarks
and rumination provide support to EA conceptualizations of
rumination (Giorgio et al., 2010), according to which it may The present study is, to our knowledge, the first one analyzing
serve an avoidant function whereby the focus on recursive cog- the association between EA and avoidant attentional biases
nition facilitates the avoidance of difficult emotions. using the dot probe paradigm. EAC-related avoidant atten-
However, contrary to what was expected, no significant tional biases have a mediating role in the association between
associations have been found between EAC and anxious and EAC and increased BP. This study represents a significant
depressive symptomatology. The association of EAC with alex- advance with regard to the study by Losada et al. (2014) on
ithymia may contribute to explain these findings: the difficulty EAC, as it explores a potential mechanism explaining the asso-
for identifying and describing emotions may interfere with ciation between EAC and BP, providing evidence suggesting
caregiversawareness and expression of emotional distress. In that attentional biases in the processing of emotional infor-
fact, the construct of EAC itself, as measured with the EACQ, mation may be important mediators in this relationship. Spe-
involves subtle forms of avoidance, such as denial of negative cifically, the results of the study suggest that attentional
emotions and lack of awareness of one’s own negative emo- avoidance of CR emotional stimuli might be the link between
tions, potentially close to repression strategies (Myers, 2010). EAC and increased BP, as it inhibits the conscious experience
The differential associations found between EACQ factors and of negative emotion; this inhibition, in turn, may facilitate the
the rest of variables analyzed in this study confirm the multi- maintenance of physiological activation, which can eventually
dimensionality and complexity of EA found in previous stud- lead to a chronic elevation in BP.
ies (Gamez, Chmielewski, Kotov, Ruggero, & Watson, 2011), The relationships identified in this study between EA,
which should be further explored by future research. attentional biases and BP are preliminary, and future studies
are needed to confirm them and to further clarify the poten-
tial links between these variables.
Clinical implications
The negative health implications of EAC and attentional
avoidance of CR emotional stimuli found in this study suggest Acknowledgments
the potential health benefits of acceptance and mindfulness
techniques to enhance the ability of caregivers with high We thank all the caregivers for their participation in the study and also the
following centers for collaborating with us in the project: Fundaci on Cien,
EAC to attend and process their disturbing emotions, which is Fundacion Marıa Wolff, Centros de dıa Vitalia, Centro de Salud General
a requisite for their appropriate regulation, and for taking Ricardos, Centro de Salud Garcıa Noblejas, Centro de Salud Benita de
advantage of their informative value (Oatley, 2010). According 
Avila, Centro de Salud Vicente Muzas, Centro Reina Sofıa de Cruz Roja,
to the guidelines for intervention on cardiovascular problems, Unidad de Memoria de Cantoblanco, Servicio de Neurologıa del Hospital
training caregivers in stress management techniques may be La Paz, Asociacion de Familiares de Alzheimer de Alcorc on, Instituto de
familia de la Universidad CEU San Pablo, Centro de Psicologıa Aplicada de
an important secondary prevention intervention (e.g. Leon la Universidad Autonoma de Madrid, and Servicios Sociales de Getafe.
et al., 2005), as presenting high levels of BP (prehypertension)
is considered to be a risk factor for hypertension (e.g. Julius
et al., 2006). Acceptance and mindfulness techniques are
stress management techniques which represent essential Disclosure statement
tools of acceptance and commitment therapy (Hayes et al., No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.
2012), which has recently been found to be an effective ther-
apy for dementia caregivers (Losada et al., 2015), and mindful-
ness-based interventions such as mindfulness-based stress Funding
reduction programs (MBSR; Kabat-Zinn, 1990) and mindful-
ness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT). Some intervention The Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness [grant number
studies raise empirical evidence supporting the capacity of PSI2015-65152-C2-2-R]; the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation
[grant number PSI2009-08132].
these therapies to reduce BP (Berman, Morton, & Hegel, 2016;
Hughes et al., 2013; Rosenzweig et al., 2009).

ORCID
Limitations
Marıa Marquez-Gonz
alez http://orcid.org/0000-0001-6572-8949
Some limitations of this study should be acknowledged. The Isabel Cabrera http://orcid.org/0000-0001-8362-764X
used sample is composed of dementia caregivers who Andres Losada http://orcid.org/0000-0002-3134-7133
8 
M. MARQUEZ-GONZ 
ALEZ ET AL.

References an angiotensin-receptor blocker. New England Journal of Medicine,


354, 1685–1697. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa060838
Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pargamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your body
Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and and mind to face stress, pain and illness. New York, NY: Delacorte.
nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, Knight, B. G., & Sayegh, P. (2010). Cultural values and caregiving: The
133, 1–24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1 updated sociocultural stress and coping model. Journal of Gerontol-
Berman, M. I., Morton, S. N., & Hegel, M. T. (2016). Uncontrolled pilot study ogy: Psychological Sciences, 65B, 5–13. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp096
of an acceptance and commitment therapy and health at every zize Koster, E. H. W., Crombez, G., Vershuere, B., Van Damme, S., & Wiersema, J.
intervention for obese, depressed women: Accept yourself! Psycho- R. (2006). Components of attentional bias to threat in high trait anxi-
therapy, 53, 462–467. doi: 10.1037/pst0000083 ety: Facilitated engagement, impaired disengagement, and atten-
Billings, D., Folkman, S., Acree, M., & Moskowitz, J. (2000). Coping and tional avoidance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1757–1771. doi:
physical health during caregiving: The roles of positive and negative 10.1016/j.brat.2004.08.004
affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 131–142. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1999). International affective
doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.79.1.131 picture system (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. Gaines-
Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New York, ville: University of Florida, Center for Research in Psychophysiology.
NY: John Wiley & Sons. Lee, L. O., & Knight, B. G. (2009). Attentional bias for threat in older adults:
Campbell-Sills, L., Barlow, D. H., Brown, T. A., & Hofmann, S. G. (2006). Moderation of the positivity bias by trait anxiety and stimulus modal-
Effects of suppression and acceptance on emotional responses on ity. Psychology & Aging, 24, 741–747. doi: 10.1037/a0016409
individuals with anxiety and mood disorders. Behavior Research & Leon, A., Franklin, B., Costa, F., Balady, G., Berra, K., Stewart, K., & Lauer, M.
Therapy, 44, 1251–1263. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.001 (2005). Cardiac rehabilitation and secondary prevention of coronary
Chattillion, E. A., Ceglowski, J., Roepke, S. K., von K€anel, R., Losada, A., Mills, heart disease. Circulation, 111, 369–376. doi: 10.1161/01.
P. J., … Mausbach, B. T. (2013). Pleasant events, activity restriction, CIR.0000151788.08740.5C
and blood pressure in dementia caregivers. Health Psychology, 32, opez, J. C., Ruiz, F. J., Feder, J., Rubio, A. B., Suarez, J. J., Rodrıguez, J. A., &
L
793–801. doi: 10.1037/a0029412 Luciano, C. (2010). The role of experiential avoidance in the perfor-
Cisler, J. M., & Koster, E. H. (2010). Mechanisms of attentional biases mance on a high cognitive demand task. International Journal of Psy-
towards threat in anxiety disorders: An integrative review. Clinical Psy- chology and Psychological Therapy, 10, 475–448.
chology Review, 30, 203–216. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.003 Losada, A., Marquez-Gonzalez, M., Romero-Moreno, R., & L opez, J. (2014).
Coleman, A., Steel, S., Freeman, P., de Greeff, A., & Shennan, A. (2008). Valida- Development and validation of the experiential avoidance in caregiv-
tion of the Omron M7 (HEM-780-E) oscillometric blood pressure monitor- ing questionnaire (EACQ). Aging & Mental Health, 18, 894–904. doi:
ing device according to the British Hypertension Society protocol. Blood 10.1080/13607863.2014.896868
Pressure Monitoring, 13, 49–54. doi: 10.1097/MBP.0b013e3282cb57b6 Losada, A., Marquez-Gonzalez, M., Romero-Moreno, R., Mausbach, B. T.,
Cribb, G., Moulds, M., & Carter, S. (2006). Rumination and experiential Lopez, J., Fernandez-Fernandez, V., & Nogales-Gonzalez, C. (2015). Cog-
avoidance in depression. Behavior Change, 23, 165–176. doi: 10.1375/ nitive behavioral therapy (CBT) versus acceptance and commitment
bech.23.3.165 therapy (ACT) for dementia family caregivers with significant depressive
Derakshan, N., Eysenck, M. W., & Myers, L. B. (2007). Emotional information symptoms: Results of a randomized clinical trial. Journal of Consulting
processing in repressors: The vigilance-avoidance theory. Cognition & and Clinical Psychology, 83, 760–772. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000028
Emotion, 21, 1585–1614. doi: 10.1080/02699930701499857 Low, C. A., Stanton, A. L., & Bower, J. E. (2008). Effects of acceptance-ori-
Gamez, W., Chmielewski, M., Kotov, R., Ruggero, C., & Watson, D. (2011). ented versus evaluative emotional processing on heart rate recovery
Development of a measure of experiential avoidance: The multidi- and habituation. Emotion, 8, 419–424. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.3.419
mensional experiential avoidance questionnaire. Psychological Assess- MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional
ment, 23, 692–713. doi: 10.1037/a0023242 disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 15–20. doi: 10.1037/
Giorgio, J. M., Sanflippo, J., Kleiman, E., Reilly, D., Bender, R. E., Wagner, C. 0021-843X.95.1.15
A., … Alloy, L. B. (2010). An experiential avoidance conceptualization Marquez-Gonzalez, M., Romero-Moreno, R., & Losada, A. (2010). Caregiving
of pathological rumination: Three tests of the model. Behaviour issues in a therapeutic context: New insights from the acceptance and
Research and Therapy, 48, 1021–1031. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.07.004 commitment therapy approach. In N. Pachana, K. Laidlaw, & B. Knight
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion reg- (Eds.), Casebook of clinical geropsychology: International perspectives on
ulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. practice (pp. 33–53). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348–362. doi: 10.1037/ Matthews, K. A., Katholi, C. R., McCreath, H., Whooley, M. A., Williams, D. R.,
0022-3514.85.2.348 Zhu, S., & Markovitz, J. H. (2004). Blood pressure reactivity to psycho-
Harrington, F., Saxby, B. K., McKeith, I. G., Wesnes, K. A., & Ford, G. A. (2000). logical stress predicts hypertension in the CARDIA study. Circulation,
Cognitive performance in hypertensive and normotensive older sub- 110, 74–78. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.0000133415.37578.E4
jects. Hypertension, 36, 1079–1082. doi: 10.1161/01.HYP.36.6.1079 Mausbach, B. T., Patterson, T. L., Rabinowitz, Y. G., Grant, I., & Schulz, R.
Hayes, S.C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (2012). Acceptance and commit- (2007). Depression and distress predict time to cardiovascular disease
ment therapy: The process and practice of mindful change (2nd ed.). in dementia caregivers. Health Psychology, 26, 539–544. doi: 10.1037/
New York, NY: The Guilford Press. 0278-6133.26.5.539
Heerena, A., Mogoas, C., McNallyc, R. J., Schmitz, A., & Philippot, P. (2015). McDonald, R. P., & Ho, M. R. (2002). Principles and practice in reporting
Does attention bias modification improve attentional control? A dou- structural equation analyses. Psychological Methods, 7, 64–82. doi:
ble-blind randomized experiment with individuals with social anxiety 10.1037//1082-989X.7.1.64
disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 29, 35–42. doi: 10.1016/j. McNair, D. M., Lorr, M., & Droppleman, L. F. (1971). Profile of mood states
janxdis.2014.10.007 manual. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Services.
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1998). Fit indices in covariance structure modeling: Mendes, W. B., Reis, H. T., Seery, M. D., & Blascovich, J. (2003). Cardiovascu-
Sensitivity to underparameterized model misspecification. Psychologi- lar correlates of emotional expression and suppression: Do content
cal Methods, 3, 424–453. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.3.4.424 and gender context matter? Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Hughes, J. W., Fresco, D. M., Myerscough, R., van Dulmen, H. M., Carlson, L. ogy, 84, 771–792. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.771
E., & Josephson, R. (2013). Randomized controlled trial of mindfulness- Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., & Williams, R. (1995). Attentional bias in anxiety
based stress reduction for prehypertension. Psychosomatic Medicine, and depression: The role of awareness. British Journal of Clinical Psy-
75, 721–728. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3182a3e4e5 chology, 34, 17–36. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1995.tb01434.x
Jackson, B., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1998). Mediators of the gender differ- Mogg, K., Millar, N., & Bradley, B. P (2000). Biases in eye movements to
ence in rumination. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 25, 37–47. doi: Threatening facial expressions in generalized anxiety disorder and
10.1111/1471-6402.00005 depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 695–704.
Joreskog, K. G. (1993). Testing structural equation models. In K. A. Bollen & doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.109.4.695
J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 294–316). New- M€uller, J., B€
uhner, M., & Ellgring, H. (2003). Is there a reliable factorial struc-
bury Park, CA: Sage. ture in the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale? A comparison of factor
Julius, S., Nesbitt, S. D., Egan, B. M., Weber, M. A., Michelson, E. L., Kaciroti, models in clinical and normal adult samples. Journal of Psychosomatic
N., … Schork, M. A. (2006). Feasibility of treating prehypertension with Research, 55, 561–568. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00033-3
AGING & MENTAL HEALTH 9

Myers, L. B. (2010). The importance of the repressive coping style: Findings allostatic load. Journal of Health Psychology, 16, 58–69. doi: 10.1177/
from 30 years of research. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 23, 3–17. doi: 1359105310369188
10.1080/10615800903366945 Rosenzweig, S., Reibel, D. K., Greeson, J. M., Edman, J. S., Jasser, S. A.,
Oatley, K. (2010). Two movements in emotions: Communication and McMearty, K. D., & Goldstein, B. J. (2007). Mindfulness-based stress
reflection. Emotion Review, 2, 29–35. doi: 10.1177/1754073909345542 reduction is associated with improved glycemic control in type 2
Obisesan, T.O. (2009). Hypertension and cognitive function. Clinics in Geri- diabetes mellitus: A pilot study. Alternative Therapies in Health and
atric Medicine, 25, 259–288. doi: 10.1016/j.cger.2009.03.002 Medicine, 13, 36–38.
O’Brien, E., Asmar, R., Beilin, L., Imai, Y., Mallion, J. M., Mancia, G., … Ver- Shahly, V., Chatterji, S., Gruber, M. J., Al-Hamzawi, A., Alonso, J., Andrade, L.
decchia, P. (2003). European society of hypertension recommenda- H., … Kessler, R. C. (2013). Cross-national differences in the prevalence
tions for conventional, ambulatory and home blood pressure and correlates of burden among older family caregivers in the World
measurement. Journal of Hypertension, 21, 821–848. doi: 10.1097/01. Health Organization World Mental Health (WMH) Surveys. Psychologi-
hjh.0000059016.82022.ca cal Medicine, 43, 865–879. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712001468

Ohman, A., & Soares, J. J. (1993). On the automatic nature of phobic fear: Spira, A. P., Beaudreau, S. A., Jimenez, D., Kierod, K., Cusing, M. M., Gray, H.
Conditioned electrodermal responses to masked fear-relevant stimuli. L., & Gallagher-Thompson, D. (2007). Experiential avoidance, accep-
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 121–132. doi: 10.1037/0021- tance, and depression in dementia family caregivers. Clinical Gerontol-
843X.102.1.121 ogist, 30, 55–64. doi: 10.1300/J018v30n04_04
Pickett, S. M., & Kurby, C. A. (2010). The impact of experiential avoidance Teri, L., Truax, P., Logsdon, R., Uomoto, J., Zarit, S., & Vitaliano, P. P. (1992).
on the inference of characters’ emotions: Evidence for an emotional Assessment of behavioral problems in dementia: The revised memory
processing bias. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 34, 493–500. doi: and behavior problems checklist (RBPC). Psychology and Aging, 7, 622–
10.1007/s10608-009-9262-x 631. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.7.4.622
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating Tull, M. T., Gratz, K. L., Salters, K., & Roemer, L. (2004). The role of experiential
indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, avoidance in posttraumatic stress symptoms and symptoms of depres-
Instruments, and Computers, 36, 717–731. doi: 10.3758/BF03206553 sion, anxiety, and somatization. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease,
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for 192, 754–761. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000144694.30121.89
research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measure- Venta, A., Hart, J., & Sharp, C. (2013). The relation between experiential
ment, 1, 385–401. doi: 10.1177/014662167700100306 avoidance, alexithymia and emotion regulation in inpatient adoles-
Raz, N., Rodrigue, K. M., & Acker, J. D. (2003). Hypertension and the brain: cents. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 18, 398–410. doi:
Of the prefrontal regions and executive functions. Behavioral Neurosci- 10.1177/135910451245581
ence, 117, 1169–1180. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.6.1169 Vitaliano, P. P., Zhang, J., & Scanlan, J. M. (2003). Is caregiving hazardous to
Roepke, S. K., Mausbach, B. T., Patterson, T. L., von K€anel, R., Ancoli-Israel, one’s physical health? A Meta-Analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 946–
S., Harmell, A. L., … Grant, I. (2011). Effects of Alzheimer caregiving on 972. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.6.946

You might also like