Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2021, 670–682

doi: 10.1093/scan/nsab032
Advance Access Publication Date: 26 March 2021
Original Manuscript

Social value orientation modulates fairness processing


during social decision-making: evidence from

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/scan/article/16/7/670/6189093 by guest on 20 March 2023


behavior and brain potentials
Xinmu Hu and Xiaoqin Mai
Department of Psychology, Renmin University of China, Beijing 100872, China
Correspondence should be addressed to Xiaoqin Mai, Department of Psychology, Renmin University of China, 59 Zhongguancun Street, Beijing 100872,
China. E-mail: maixq@ruc.edu.cn.

Abstract
Social value orientation (SVO) characterizes stable individual differences by an inherent sense of fairness in outcome alloca-
tions. Using the event-related potential (ERP), this study investigated differences in fairness decision-making behavior and
neural bases between individuals with prosocial and proself orientations using the Ultimatum Game (UG). Behavioral results
indicated that prosocials were more prone to rejecting unfair offers with stronger negative emotional reactions compared
with proselfs. ERP results revealed that prosocials showed a larger P2 when receiving fair offers than unfair ones in a very
early processing stage, whereas such effect was absent in proselfs. In later processing stages, although both groups were
sensitive to fairness as reflected by an enhanced medial frontal negativity (MFN) for unfair offers and a larger P3 for fair
offers, prosocials exhibited a stronger fairness effect on these ERP components relative to proselfs. Furthermore, the fairness
effect on the MFN mediated the SVO effect on rejecting unfair offers. Findings regarding emotional experiences, behavioral
patterns and ERPs provide compelling evidence that SVO modulates fairness processing in social decision-making, whereas
differences in neural responses to unfair vs fair offers as evidenced by the MFN appear to play important roles in the SVO
effect on behavioral responses to unfairness.

Key words: social value orientation (SVO); fairness decision-making; P2; medial frontal negativity (MFN); P3

Introduction responders are prone to rejecting unfair offers, especially those


below 20% of the total (Camerer, 2003; Sanfey et al., 2003; Güth
In highly complex social environments, people must consider
and Kocher, 2014). Rejection of unfair offers contradicts clas-
the outcomes of their own behaviors along with others’ during
social interactions; such considerations influence the decision- sical game theory, in which the responder should presumably
making process (Rilling and Sanfey, 2011). As per the adage accept any offer because ‘something is better than nothing’
‘inequality rather than want is the cause of trouble’, fair- (Morgenstern and Von Neumann, 1953; Rubinstein, 1982). These
ness norm plays crucial roles in social decision-making. The studies have suggested that people are concerned with their own
Ultimatum Game (UG) has been widely employed to examine interests and prefer fairness. When treated unfairly, many peo-
individuals’ responses to fairness in resource allocation (Güth ple are willing to sacrifice their own interests to punish unfair
et al., 1982). Studies using the UG paradigm have revealed that behaviors in others (Fehr et al., 2002; Decety and Yoder, 2017).

Received: 16 June 2020; Revised: 5 March 2021; Accepted: 26 March 2021

© The Author(s) 2021. Published by Oxford University Press.


This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/),
which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

670
X. Hu and X. Mai | 671

Theoretical explanations of fairness preferences others (Liebrand, 1984; Van Lange and Kuhlman, 1994; Bogaert
et al., 2008). SVO has been regarded as a dispositional per-
Why do humans attach great importance to fairness vis-à-vis
sonality construct due to its temporal stability and situational
resource allocation? Several theoretical models have been pro-
consistency (Van Lange et al., 1997; Balliet et al., 2009; Murphy
posed to explain individuals’ fairness preferences. Inequity
et al., 2011), and it is expressed automatically (Kuss et al., 2015).
aversion theory holds that people prefer equitable outcomes
SVO is also closely related to the honesty–humility trait, which
and are willing to forgo some material payoff in favor of more
describes one’s tendency to be fair and honest and can effec-
equitable outcomes (Fehr and Schmidt, 1999; Fehr et al., 2002).
tively predict prosocial behavior in social dilemmas (Ashton and
Studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)
Lee, 2007; Hilbig et al., 2014).
have demonstrated that brain regions associated with rewards,
The integrative model of SVO suggests that prosocials pursue
such as the ventral striatum and ventromedial prefrontal cortex
equality in outcomes and aim to maximize joint outcomes in
(vmPFC), are more active under fair UG offers than unfair offers
interdependent situations, whereas proselfs seek to maximize
(Kable and Glimcher, 2007; Tabibnia et al., 2008; Tricomi et al.,
personal outcomes and are less concerned with equality (Van
2010; Yu et al., 2014).
Lange, 1999; De Cremer and Van Lange, 2001). Recent evidence
From an evolutionary perspective, the strong reciprocity
has substantiated the claim that equality in outcomes is proso-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/scan/article/16/7/670/6189093 by guest on 20 March 2023


model claims that negative reciprocity (i.e. rejecting unfair
cials’ primary concern (Eek and Gärling, 2006; Van Lange et al.,
offers) reflects prosociality because individuals who reject an
2013). Further, behavioral and neuroimaging studies have pro-
offer sacrifice their own resources to punish unfair behavior,
vided evidence for the integrative model. In the dictator game
which may enforce a fair social norm and promote cooperation.
(a simplified version of the UG wherein the responder becomes
This pattern has important evolutionary significance (Bowles
a passive receiver of the proposer’s offer and therefore can-
and Gintis, 2004; Kaltwasser et al., 2016). Non-invasive brain
not reject it), prosocials allocate more resources to the receiver
stimulation studies have reported that disruption of the right
compared with proselfs, and this prosociality is expressed auto-
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (rDLPFC) using repetitive transcra-
matically (Cornelissen et al., 2011). Moreover, prosocials are less
nial magnetic stimulation or cathodal transcranial direct current
likely to accept unfair offers than proselfs in the UG (Haruno
stimulation is associated with decreased rejection rates of unfair
et al., 2014; Bieleke et al., 2017). When asked to evaluate the desir-
offers in the UG, indicating that the rDLPFC is involved in exert-
ability of reward pairs for the self and another in a reward-pair
ing cognitive effort to override individuals’ prepotent selfish
evaluation task, prosocials disliked large absolute differences
impulses and thus drives people to consider fairness norms
in outcome allocations, whereas proselfs were unaffected by
more carefully (Miller and Cohen, 2001; van’t Wout, Kahn et al.,
such differences and preferred larger personal rewards. Notably,
2005; Knoch et al., 2006, 2008; Feng et al., 2015). Moreover,
the magnitude of differences in allocations was positively cor-
the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) has demonstrated stronger
related with amygdala activation in prosocials but not in pros-
activation under unfair offers, implying that individuals expe-
elfs (Haruno and Frith, 2010). In a follow-up study, researchers
rience greater perceived violations of fairness norms (Fehr and
found stronger amygdala activation for prosocials than pros-
Camerer, 2007; Harlé et al., 2012).
elfs in response to unfair offers in the UG (Haruno et al., 2014).
The emotion model posits that negative emotions provoked
These findings suggest that prosocials possess stronger inequal-
by unfair offers lead to rejections in the UG (Pillutla and
ity aversion than proselfs in social decision-making and are
Murnighan, 1996). fMRI studies have shown that the anterior
more willing to punish unfair behavior at the expense of their
insula and amygdala are more active for unfair offers than fair
own interests.
offers, suggesting that individuals might experience negative
emotions such as aversion and anger when receiving unfair
offers (Sanfey et al., 2003; Gospic et al., 2011; Harlé et al., 2012). Event-related potential components
The inequality aversion theory and strong reciprocity model Although studies involving fMRI have contributed greatly to
explain individuals’ obedience to fairness norms from a moti- identifying brain regions related to individuals’ responses to
vational point of view, whereas the emotion model explores fairness (Haruno and Frith, 2010; Haruno et al., 2014), little is
responses to fairness through an emotional lens. These three known about the temporal characteristics of fairness process-
approaches have been used to explore the rationale behind ing among individuals with different SVOs in social interac-
individuals’ fairness preferences from different perspectives, tions. The event-related potential (ERP) could reveal important
reflecting the complexity of human social behavior. insights about the precise timing of processes underlying cog-
nitive functions given the measure’s excellent time resolution.
Therefore, using the ERP in the present study, we aimed to
Social value orientation examine the time characteristics of SVO in modulating fairness
Individual differences in fairness preferences represent an processing.
other-focused preference in social decision-making. Social value In ERP studies, several ERP components have often been
orientation (SVO) can explain a portion of this variance (Rilling used as biomarkers for outcome evaluation processing: the P2,
and Sanfey, 2011). SVO is defined as the dispositional weights the medial frontal negativity (MFN), and the P3. The P2 is a
that individuals assign to outcomes for themselves and others medial frontal positivity that peaks around ∼200–300 ms post-
in interdependent situations (McClintock, 1972; Murphy and stimulus. It is associated with attentional capture (Potts et al.,
Ackermann, 2014; Pletzer et al., 2018). Two main types of SVO, 2004) and integration of motivational information, specifically
prosocial and proself, are frequently distinguished in this pop- reward information from the mesencephalic dopamine system,
ulation. Individuals with a prosocial orientation are willing to from an ongoing event (Potts et al., 2006; Boudreau et al., 2009);
sacrifice their own interests to establish equal allocations and/or hence, the P2 is commonly considered indicative of automatic
maximize joint outcomes, whereas individuals with a proself processing of a current event (Horat et al., 2016). Prior study using
orientation focus on their personal interests in an effort to DG has found that the P2 is greater for fair offers than for unfair
maximize their own outcomes while largely ignoring those of offers (Wu et al., 2011).
672 | Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2021, Vol. 16, No. 7

The MFN [also known as the feedback-related negativity direct evidence of individual variation in social values contribut-
(FRN)] is a fronto-central negativity that peaks at ∼200–350 ms ing to differences in time-dynamic characteristics underlying
post-outcome onset, presumably generated in the ACC, and fairness processing.
represents a rapid processing of the current outcome in a ‘like–
dislike’ dimension (Miltner et al., 1997; Gehring and Willoughby,
2002; Yeung and Sanfey, 2004; Sambrook and Goslin, 2014;
Methods
Pfabigan et al., 2018). The MFN is typically more pronounced fol- Participants
lowing negative outcomes than positive outcomes (Hajcak et al.,
2006; Paul and Pourtois, 2017; Sidarus et al., 2017) and is sensitive Sixty young, healthy participants (32 women and 28 men)
to the degree to which an outcome deviates from expectations between 18 and 24 years of age (M = 19.55, s.d. = 1.92) were
(Oliveira et al., 2007; Bellebaum et al., 2010; Alexander and Brown, recruited from the university population. All participants were
2011; Ferdinand et al., 2012; Marciano et al., 2018). During the UG, right-handed and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision.
the MFN amplitude is more pronounced in response to unfair They had no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders,
offers than to fair offers (Boksem and De Cremer, 2010; Hewig and none were taking psychoactive medication at the time
et al., 2011; Massi and Luhmann, 2015; Peterburs et al., 2017). of the experiment. Data from one female participant were

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/scan/article/16/7/670/6189093 by guest on 20 March 2023


Following the MFN, the P3 is a centro-parietal positivity excluded due to an insufficient number of trials after arti-
appearing in the 300–600 ms time window (Holroyd and Coles, facts were removed (<20 trials, the minimal number of tri-
2002; Yeung and Sanfey, 2004; San Martín, 2012). The P3 encodes als required for a stable ERP component; Marco-Pallares et al.,
the valence and magnitude of an outcome, with a greater ampli- 2011). The final sample for analysis consisted of 59 participants
tude to an affectively positive than negative outcome and to a (31 women and 28 men) with an average age of 19.51 years
larger than smaller reward (Yeung and Sanfey, 2004; Sato et al., (s.d. = 1.91 years). An a priori sample size estimation was con-
2005; Yeung et al., 2005; Bellebaum and Daum, 2008; Glazer et al., ducted using G*Power v.3.1 (Faul et al., 2009). According to the
2018). The P3 reflects allocation of attentional resources and analysis (d = 0.25, α = 0.05, β = 0.9, analysis of variance (ANOVA):
the subjective importance of a desired outcome (Nieuwenhuis repeated measures, within-between interaction), a total sam-
et al., 2005; Polich, 2007; Mussel et al., 2018). ERP studies in social ple size of 46 participants was required to detect a reliable
decision-making have found that the P3 is also sensitive to the effect. Written informed consent was obtained from all partic-
fairness of outcome allocation, exhibiting a larger amplitude for ipants. The experiment was performed in accordance with the
fair offers than for unfair offers (Wu et al., 2011; Wang et al., Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Ethics Com-
2017a). mittee of the Department of Psychology, Renmin University of
Recent ERP studies have investigated the SVO effect on out- China.
come processing in various social contexts. Hu et al. (2017) found
that individuals with distinct SVOs processed outcomes involv- Procedure
ing others differently; that is, prosocials were more concerned
about others’ outcomes in interdependent situations than pros-
Session 1: measurement of SVO. In the first session, partici-
pants’ SVOs were measured using the Slider Measure (Murphy
elfs as revealed by the FRN, P3 and late positive component. The
et al., 2011), an efficient and simple measure of SVO with
SVO effect was also observed in the Chicken Game. Compared
documented high reliability and excellent convergent validity
with proselfs, prosocials considered non-reciprocated coopera-
(Anderl et al., 2015) and significant predictive validity on social
tion as the least desirable outcome as evidenced by an enhanced
decision-making tasks (Bogaert et al., 2008; Bieleke et al., 2017).
FRN (Wang et al., 2017a). When compared with other social
On this six-item measure, participants were asked to choose
agents, prosocials were sensitive to others’ outcomes in self-
between several self-other payoff combinations. Based on their
gain and self-loss contexts, whereas proselfs were only inter-
decisions, an SVO angle in a two-dimensional space consisting
ested in others’ outcomes in the self-gain context as indicated
of one’s own payoff and others’ payoff was computed (Murphy
by distinct FRN patterns (Qi et al., 2018). These findings sug-
et al., 2011; Pletzer et al., 2018). This measurement allowed for
gest that SVO influences the processing of outcomes involv-
calculation of a continuous SVO score; then participants could,
ing others, further supporting our supposition that SVO could
in principle, be grouped into corresponding categories based on
modulate fairness processing in social decision-making through
their scores on prosocial or proself social preferences (Fiedler
outcome-related ERP indicators.
et al., 2013; Pletzer et al., 2018). The boundary between the
In the present study, a multi-round, one-shot UG task was
two categories was 22.45◦ ; angles below 22.45◦ indicated pro-
conducted with electroencephalography (EEG) recording. A typ-
selfs and greater angles indicated a more prosocial orientation
ical UG involves two players: a proposer and a responder. First,
(Murphy et al., 2011; Murphy and Ackermann, 2014).
the proposer suggests a division of a sum of money. The respon-
der can then either accept the proposal, in which case the sum is
allocated as suggested, or reject it, in which case neither player Session 2: the UG. After measuring SVOs, participants received
receives any money. The P2, MFN and P3 (representing different general instructions about the UG. Each participant played the
stages of outcome processing) were selected for analysis. Due to UG repeatedly in a series of one-shot trials as a proposer and
the characteristics of these ERP components, we hypothesized a responder. In the proposer role, each participant could divide
that the three components would be modulated by SVO for fair 10 Chinese yuan into two shares (one for her-/himself and the
and unfair offers. In particular, we predicted that the fairness other for the unknown responder) for 30 rounds. They were told
factor would be processed automatically in the prosocial group that these offers would be stored in the database and used for
rather than the proself group as reflected by distinct P2 pat- future participants.
terns. Further, we anticipated that the effect of fairness would Afterward, participants played the UG in the responder role,
be more prominent in the prosocial group than the proself group while EEGs were recorded. In this crucial session, participants
as indicated by the MFN and P3. These findings would provide were informed they would receive 300 monetary offers proposed
X. Hu and X. Mai | 673

by volunteers and previous participants from the database, but EEG recording and preprocessing
they would receive only one offer from a particular proposer. In
EEG data were recorded using 64 cap-mounted tin electrodes
addition, participants were informed that the proposers would
arranged according to the 10–20 international placement sys-
never know whether the offer was rejected or accepted. This
tem (Neuroscan Inc., Herndon, VA, USA), with an online ref-
explanation was fabricated to avoid learning and reputation
erence to the left mastoid and an offline re-reference to the
effects, and the proposer session was conducted to render the
averaged mastoid electrodes. A horizontal electro-oculogram
task context more plausible. During the formal experiment, par-
(EOG) was recorded from electrodes placed 1.5 cm laterally to
ticipants sat comfortably in a room shielded from sound and
the outer canthi of both eyes. A vertical EOG was recorded
electrical fields, seated ∼80 cm from a 22-inch computer mon-
from electrodes placed above and below the left eye. All inter-
itor. The entire task consisted of 300 trials in which all stimuli
electrode impedance was kept below 5 kΩ during recording.
were displayed in the center of the computer screen. Figure 1
Signals were amplified using a 0.01–100 Hz band-pass filter and
illustrates the time course of stimulus presentation in this UG
continuously sampled at 500 Hz/channel. Offline analysis of EEG
task. Each trial began with a fixation cross appearing for 500 ms
data was performed using Neuroscan software (Scan 4.5, Neu-
followed by a picture of a 10-yuan bill (2.6◦ × 1.3◦ ) for 500 ms
roScan, Inc., Herndon, VA, USA). Ocular artifacts were removed
against a black background. A fixation cross was then presented

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/scan/article/16/7/670/6189093 by guest on 20 March 2023


from EEGs using a regression procedure implemented in Scan
again for 800 to 1200 ms. Later, the offer screen was presented
4.5 software (Semlitsch et al., 1986). All EEG data were digi-
for 1000 ms, depicting a distributive outcome of 10 yuan between
tally low-pass filtered at 30 Hz (24 dB/oct) and segmented into
the respective proposer and the participant (responder). Par-
epochs time-locked to the onset of offer presentation, beginning
ticipants decided to accept or reject the offer at this point by
200 ms before offer onset and continuing for 1000 ms. Data were
pressing either the F or J key with their left or right index fin-
then baseline-corrected according to the 200 ms pre-offer period.
ger. After an interval of 800–1200 ms, feedback on the offer was
Epochs containing artifacts exceeding ± 70 µV were excluded
presented for 1000 ms. Trials were separated by an inter-trial
from subsequent analyses. Then, epochs were averaged sepa-
interval of 500 ms. In addition, participants were asked to eval-
rately for each condition of each participant.
uate their affective reaction to the currently received offer on
a 9-point scale (1 = extremely negative and 9 = extremely posi-
tive) (Bradley et al., 2001; Hewig et al., 2011; Yang et al., 2015). Statistical analyses
Emotion evaluation occurred three times randomly for each type
Behavioral data. Participants’ SVO scores were analyzed using
of offer.
an independent-samples t-test. Acceptance rates (ARs) and
The entire task was divided into four blocks of 75 trials each
emotion ratings were subjected to mixed two-way repeated-
with a brief break between blocks. Each block consisted of 30
measures ANOVA with SVO type (proself vs prosocial) as a
fair trials (15 offers of 5–5 and 15 offers of 4–6), 30 unfair trials
between-subjects factor and fairness (fair vs unfair) as a within-
(15 offers of 1–9 and 15 offers of 2–8) and 15 moderately unfair
subject factor. Trials with a moderate unfair offer (3–7) in this
offers (3–7). Unbeknownst to the participants, all offers were
task functioned as fillers and thus excluded from analyses; pre-
generated by the computer program rather than actual people.
vious studies reported that responders held diverse opinions
Therefore, when a participant acted as the responder, each type
about whether this offer could be considered fair (Halko et al.,
of offer was presented in a random sequence. Before the for-
2009; Hewig et al., 2011; Luo et al., 2014), resulting in difficulty
mal task, participants completed 10 practice trials to familiarize
classifying this type of offer.
themselves with the UG task. The entire task lasted for ∼30 min.
Before the EEG task, all participants were informed they would
be paid 30 Chinese yuan for their participation plus the cumu- ERP data. Analyzed ERP components included the P2, MFN
lative outcome based on their decisions during the task. Upon and P3. For statistical analyses, based on previous studies and
finishing the experiment, each participant was paid roughly 70 inspection of the grand-averaged waveforms, amplitudes of
Chinese yuan regardless of their decisions in the UG task. Partic- the P2, MFN and P3 were quantified as mean values within
ipants were also asked about the plausibility of the cover story, the time windows of 200–270 ms (Potts et al., 2006; Luo et al.,
and no participants expressed suspicion about it. Stimulus pre- 2015), 290–370 ms (Polezzi et al., 2008; Wu et al., 2011; Massi
sentation and behavioral data acquisition were conducted using and Luhmann, 2015) and 400–600 ms (Luo et al., 2014; Peterburs
E-Prime 2.0 software (PST, Inc., Pittsburgh, PA, USA). et al., 2017; Wang et al., 2017a) following the onset of the offer

Fig. 1. Illustration of a single trial of the multi-round one-shot UG task. Each trial began with presentation of a fixation cross for 500 ms followed by a picture of the
10-yuan bill for 500 ms. Then, a fixation cross was presented again for 800–1200 ms. Later, the offer screen appeared for 1000 ms, depicting a distributive outcome of 10
yuan between the respective proposer and the participant (responder). Participants decided to accept or reject the offer by pressing either the F or J key with their left
or right index finger. After an interval of 800–1200 ms, outcome feedback was presented for 1000 ms. Participants were then asked to evaluate their emotional reaction
to the offer on a 9-point scale. Emotion evaluation occurred randomly three times for each type of offer.
674 | Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2021, Vol. 16, No. 7

small effect, 0.1 representing a medium effect and 0.2 repre-


senting a large effect (Cohen, 1973). Behavioral and ERP data
were analyzed statistically using SPSS software (version 25.0;
IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA).

Results
Behavioral results
Fig. 2. Topographical voltage distributions of the P2 (200–270 ms) and P3 (400–
600 ms) for fair and unfair conditions and topographies of voltage differences
SVO scores and test-retest reliability. The SVO scores of 59
between unfair and fair offers in the MFN time interval (290–370 ms) for SVO participants in this study ranged from −7.80◦ to 37.85◦ , of
groups, respectively. which 30 were proselfs (score < 22.45◦ ) and 29 were proso-
cials (score > 22.45◦ ). Mean (±s.d.) SVO scores of prosocials
were significantly higher than those of proselfs (31.09 ± 7.14◦
stimulus, respectively. Compared to peak measures, mean mea- vs 13.29 ± 4.51◦ ; t(1, 57) = 11.41, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 3.02). To

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/scan/article/16/7/670/6189093 by guest on 20 March 2023


sures are less sensitive to high-frequency noise and not biased test the reliability of SVO, 1 year after the first measurement,
by the noise level, and thus, could make the statistical results all participates were contacted by e-mail and asked to complete
more reliable (Luck, 2014). Based on prior studies (Gehring and the SVO Slider Measure again. Fifty-one participants (25 pros-
Willoughby, 2002; Yeung and Sanfey, 2004; Luo et al., 2015) and elfs and 26 prosocials) completed the SVO Slider Measure again.
scalp topographies of each ERP component (Figure 2), the P2 and Forty-six of them were divided into the same SVO category into
MFN were calculated at the FCz site, and the P3 was calculated two times of the Slider Measure, yielding a consistency of 90%.
at the Pz electrode site where they reached maximum ampli- Further the correlation between the resulting angles from the
tudes. The ERP amplitudes were subjected to mixed two-way test–retest SVO Slider Measure was r = 0.885. The results of test–
repeated-measures ANOVA with SVO type (proself vs prosocial) retest reliability in this study are consistent with the finding of
as a between-subjects factor and fairness (fair vs unfair) as a the previous study (Murphy et al., 2011).
within-subject factor.

ARs. Figure 3A illustrates the ARs of offers. A significant main


Neural-behavioral correlation. To evaluate the relationships effect of fairness was confirmed, F(1,57) = 213.32, P < 0.001,
between behavioral responses and neural activities, Pearson cor- η p 2 = 0.79, with lower ARs for unfair offers than fair offers
relation coefficients were calculated between fairness enforce- (0.34 ± 0.37 vs 0.96 ± 0.11). The main effect of SVO type was
ment performance and brain activities. also significant, F(1, 57) = 12.98, P < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.19, with lower
ARs in the prosocial group than the proself group (0.56 ± 0.44 vs
0.74 ± 0.36). Notably, the interaction effect of SVO type × fairness
Mediation analysis. Mediation analysis was conducted using was significant, F(1, 57) = 15.91, P < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.22. Simple-
the SPSS version of the INDIRECT macro (Preacher and Hayes,
effects analysis of the interaction effect demonstrated that
2008), taking the SVO score as the independent variable,
prosocials were more likely than proselfs to reject unfair offers
d-MFN (MFN for unfair offers minus MFN for fair offers)
(0.17 ± 0.26 vs 0.51 ± 0.38), F(1, 57) = 15.98, P < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.22.
as the mediator and ARs for unfair offers as the outcome
Conversely, no significant difference was found for fair offers
variable.
(0.95 ± 0.11 vs 0.96 ± 0.12), F(1, 57) = 0.07, P = 0.79.
The significance level was set at 0.05 for all analyses. The
Greenhouse–Geisser correction was conducted to account for
sphericity violations whenever appropriate. Post-hoc testing of Emotion ratings. Figure 3B illustrates participants’ emotion
significant main effects was applied with Bonferroni adjust- ratings of offers. For emotional ratings, the main effect of
ments. Partial eta-squared (η p 2 ) values were calculated to indi- fairness was significant, F(1, 57) = 253.90, P < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.82,
cate the effect size in ANOVA models, with 0.05 representing a with more negative emotional reactions to unfair offers than

Fig. 3. Histogram of interaction effect between SVO type and fairness on ARs (A) and emotion ratings (range from 1–9) (B). Error bars indicate standard error of the
mean (SEM). *P < 0.05, ***P < 0.001.
X. Hu and X. Mai | 675

to fair offers (3.91 ± 1.04 vs 5.88 ± 0.86). The interaction effect with fair offers (1.19 µV vs 2.23 µV). Moreover, a significant
of SVO type × fairness was also significant, F(1, 57) = 38.32, interaction effect was observed between SVO type and fair-
P < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.40. Simple-effects analysis of the interaction ness, F(1, 57) = 8.47, P = 0.005, η p 2 = 0.13. Consequently, a
effect revealed that prosocials were more prone than proselfs simple-effects analysis was conducted to investigate the sig-
to experiencing more positive reactions to fair offers (6.11 ± 0.91 nificant interaction. Findings indicated that although the MFN
vs 5.66 ± 0.77; F(1, 57) = 4.30, P = 0.043, η p 2 = 0.07) and to expe- was more negative-going in response to unfair offers than fair
riencing more negative reactions than proselfs to unfair offers offers in both SVO groups, the fairness effect on the MFN was
(3.37 ± 0.69 vs 4.45 ± 1.06), F(1, 57) = 21.48, P < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.27. more prominent in the prosocial group (1.70 µV vs 3.14 µV),
F(1, 57) = 55.10, P < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.49 than the proself group
(0.68 µV vs 1.33 µV), F(1, 57) = 11.07, P = 0.002, η p 2 = 0.16. The SVO
ERP results difference in this fairness effect is shown in a topographic map
of the MFN (Figure 2B).
P2. Figure 4A depicts grand-average ERP waveforms at the
FCz electrode site. The P2 and MFN were measured at the
FCz site. For the P2 amplitude, the main effect of fairness

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/scan/article/16/7/670/6189093 by guest on 20 March 2023


was significant, F(1, 57) = 17.41, P < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.23, indicat- P3. Figure 4B presents grand-average ERP waveforms at the Pz
ing that fair offers elicited a larger P2 compared with unfair electrode site. The main effect of fairness on the P3 amplitude
offers (3.85 µV vs 3.30 µV). Importantly, a significant interaction was significant, F(1, 57) = 42.55, P < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.43, indicating
effect emerged between SVO type and fairness, F(1, 57) = 9.13, that fair offers elicited a larger P3 than unfair offers (4.63 µV
P = 0.004, η p 2 = 0.14. A simple-effects analysis was conducted vs 3.64 µV). A significant interaction effect emerged between
to investigate the two-way interaction; results showed that the SVO type and fairness, F(1, 57) = 6.38, P = 0.014, η p 2 = 0.10. Thus,
fairness effect was only significant for prosocials, indicating a a simple-effects analysis was conducted to explore the sig-
larger P2 in response to fair offers than unfair offers (4.08 µV nificant interaction effect. Results revealed that the P3 was
vs 3.14 µV), F(1, 57) = 25.45, P < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.31. In contrast, larger in response to fair offers than unfair offers in both SVO
the fairness effect on the P2 was not significant for proselfs groups, whereas the fairness effect on the P3 (fair minus unfair)
(3.62 µV vs 3.47 µV), F(1, 57) = 0.67, P = 0.415. The SVO difference appeared more prominent in the prosocial group (5.04 µV vs
of this fairness effect is displayed in a topographic map of the P2 3.67 µV), F(1, 57) = 40.27, P < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.41 than the proself
(Figure 2A). group (4.21 µV vs 3.61 µV), F(1, 57) = 8.12, P = 0.006, η p 2 = 0.13.
This analysis further revealed that the P3 amplitudes elicited by
fair offers were significantly larger in the prosocial group than
MFN. As to the MFN amplitude, the main effect of fairness the proself group (5.04 µV vs 4.21 µV), F(1, 57) = 4.28, P = 0.43,
was significant, F(1, 57) = 58.91, P < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.51, indicating η p 2 = 0.07, whereas the P3 amplitudes elicited by unfair offers
that unfair offers elicited a more negative-going MFN compared were comparable between SVO groups (3.67 µV vs. 3.61 µV),

Fig. 4. Grand-average ERP waveforms from FCz and Pz electrode sites for the SVO groups, respectively. (A) Light gray and darker gray areas denote time windows of
200–270 ms and 290–370 ms in which peak amplitudes of the P2 and MFN were measured, respectively. (B) Light blue areas denote the time window (400–600 ms) in
which the mean amplitude of the P3 was measured.
676 | Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2021, Vol. 16, No. 7

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/scan/article/16/7/670/6189093 by guest on 20 March 2023


Fig. 5. Mediation effect: the effect of SVO scores on ARs for unfair offers was significantly reduced when the d-MFN was included in the regression model. Estimates
are standardized values. ***P < 0.001.

F(1, 57) = 0.02, P = 0.894. The SVO difference of the fairness effect Discussion
is displayed in a topographic map of the P3 (Figure 2C).
The present study investigated the modulating effect of SVO
on individuals’ fairness processing in the UG task and explored
Results of neural-behavioral correlation the temporal characteristics of neural activities underlying this
issue through the ERP technique. As expected, the results
Next, we examined whether increase in individuals’ SVO scores demonstrated that individuals with distinct SVOs responded
was associated with a corresponding decrease in ARs for unfair differently to fairness in the UG and revealed the neural under-
offers. A Pearson correlation analysis was conducted between pinnings of SVO modulation of fairness processing. Prosocials
SVO scores and ARs for unfair offers. The results revealed appeared more satisfied with fair offers and more upset about
that SVO scores were negatively correlated with ARs for unfair being treated unfairly than proselfs, in which case prosocials
offers, r = −0.50, P < 0.01. To address the potential contribu- were less likely than proselfs to accept unfair offers. Elec-
tion of neural activities to decreased ARs for unfair offers, trophysiologically, three ERP indicators (the P2, MFN and P3),
we examined whether the P2, MFN or P3 effects correlated revealed group differences in fairness processing. Compared
with ARs for unfair offers. Results showed only the MFN with proselfs, prosocials exhibited automatic processing to the
effect (d-MFN) was positively correlated with ARs for unfair fairness factor as evidenced by a significant difference in the
offers, r = 0.59, p < 0.01; however, no other correlations survived, P2 for fair and unfair offers. Furthermore, prosocials exhibited
ps > 0.102. deeper processing to fairness in later time windows, reflected
in the MFN and P3. Moreover, the fairness effect on the MFN
(d-MFN) mediated the relationship between individuals’ SVOs
Results of mediation analysis
and behavioral responses to unfairness in the UG. We discuss
In this study, given an SVO effect on both the d-MFN and ARs the implications of these findings in the subsequent sections.
for unfair offers, we conducted a mediation analysis to explore
whether the SVO effect on participants’ responses to unfair-
SVO modulates individuals’ behavioral responses to
ness was mediated by the d-MFN. The findings confirmed that
fairness in social decision-making
the effect of SVO on ARs for unfair offers was mediated by
the d-MFN, Sobel test: t = −2.92, P = 0.003. A stepwise regres- In the present study, unfair offers induced negative affective
sion excluding the factor of SVO was no longer significant when responses from responders, concomitant with findings from
d-MFN was introduced, β = −0.23, p = 0.099, as compared with prior research (Damasio et al., 2000; van’t, Kahn, Sanfey, and
initial coefficient, β = −0.50, P < 0.001, suggesting that the effect Aleman, 2006; Xiao and Houser, 2005; Hewig et al., 2011; Gilam
of SVO on d-MFN acted as a full mediator of the effect of SVO et al., 2015). Also, the behavioral results replicated a well-
on ARs for unfair offers (Figure 5). Bootstrapping results indi- established behavioral pattern wherein people are likely to reject
cated that this mediation effect was different from zero with 95% unfair offers in the UG (Güth et al., 1982; Nowak et al., 2000;
confidence (number of bootstrap resamples = 5000; β = −0.83, Holper et al., 2018).
confidence intervals = −1.55 to −0.21). Confidence intervals for The abovementioned affective and behavioral responses
indirect effect were bias-corrected (Preacher and Hayes, 2008). were modulated by SVO. Studies have found that compared
X. Hu and X. Mai | 677

with proselfs, prosocials tend to prefer equal outcomes in eco- and Zhou, 2010; Hu et al., 2017; Kimura et al., 2018;). Consistent
nomic decision-making, suggesting that fairness is more impor- with prior research, unfair offers elicited a more negative-going
tant for prosocials than for proselfs (Van Lange, 1999; Eek and MFN than fair offers in this study, implying that individuals
Gärling, 2006); therefore, fair offers induced more positive emo- considered unfair offers an unfavorable outcome that violated
tional experiences for prosocials. However, unfair offers vio- social norms (Polezzi et al., 2008; Boksem and De Cremer, 2010;
lated their inner norms of pursuing equality in outcomes and Hewig et al., 2011; Van Der Veen and Sahibdin, 2011; Luo et al.,
thus induced more negative reactions for prosocials than for 2014; Mothes et al., 2016). We found the fairness effect on the
proselfs, as indicated in fMRI studies wherein amygdala activa- MFN to be modulated by SVO; specifically, the fairness effect
tion was significantly stronger for prosocials than for proselfs was more prominent in the prosocial group than in the proself
when encountering unequal outcomes (Haruno and Frith, 2010; group, indicating that prosocials processed fairness in outcomes
Haruno et al., 2014). The trend of prosocials rejecting unfair more deeply than proselfs. Interestingly, the fairness effect on
offers more frequently than proselfs was also consistent with the MFN (d-MFN) mediated the relationship between SVO and
previous research, suggesting that prosocials are more willing behavioral responses to unfair offers in the UG. That is, a more
to sacrifice personal benefits to retaliate against unfair behav- prosocial SVO led individuals to exhibit a stronger fairness effect
iors and enforce fairness norms (Haruno et al., 2014; Bieleke et al., on the MFN, which made them less likely to accept unfair offers.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/scan/article/16/7/670/6189093 by guest on 20 March 2023


2017). Unsurprisingly, as the correlation result implied, individ- Studies using fMRI have shown that receiving unfair offers
uals’ SVO could predict the likelihood of rejecting unfair offers is associated with increased activation in the ACC, a brain
in the UG, with a more prosocial SVO indicating a higher chance region involved in the processing of outcomes that deviate neg-
of rejection. atively from expectations (Sanfey et al., 2003; Amiez et al., 2005;
Matsumoto et al., 2007; Boksem and De Cremer, 2010; Harlé
et al., 2012). According to the reinforcement learning (RL) the-
SVO modulates the early stage of fairness processing
ory of the MFN, the key function of the ACC is the adaptive
The first stage of fairness processing is represented by the P2, control of behavior, and the ACC learns how to perform its
which reflects an initial, automatic and low-level processing of function better from discrepancies between actual and expected
outcomes (McPartland et al., 2011; Van der Molen et al., 2014; Luo positive outcomes (Holroyd and Coles, 2002; Holroyd et al.,
et al., 2015). Research has found that the P2 is sensitive to motiva- 2008). Therefore, the RL theory postulates that when events
tional information of stimuli, with larger amplitudes associated are worse than expected, the decrease of dopamine activity
with outcomes that meet motivational goals (Potts et al., 2006; trains the ACC to adjust the control of the motor system with
Boudreau et al., 2009). In this study, the fairness effect on the P2 an enhanced MFN, which indicates an unexpected and unde-
was modulated by SVO: the fairness effect was only significant sirable outcome (Holroyd and Coles, 2002; San Martín, 2012). In
for prosocials, who demonstrated a larger P2 in response to fair the UG task, prosocials regarded fair offers as favorable ones
offers than to unfair offers. Considerable evidence has suggested owing to the higher earnings and fairness, which elicited a gen-
that SVO is expressed automatically, and prosocials and pros- tler MFN. However, unfair offers were regarded as unfavorable
elfs seem to possess different intrinsic motivations (Cornelissen outcomes because of their lower benefit and equality, which
et al., 2011; Kuss et al., 2015). Specifically, prosocials tend to were much worse than their expected ones and thus elicited a
internalize fairness norms as an intrinsic principle (Cornelissen larger MFN. In contrast, proselfs mainly focused on their per-
et al., 2011). An equal outcome is therefore optimal in prosocials’ sonal interests in the UG and put less value on the fairness
decision-making pursuits, and they process fairness in outcome embedded in the offers. Therefore, the fairness effect of the
allocations automatically as an intuitive response (Van Lange MFN in the proself group was weaker than that in the prosocial
et al., 2013). Proselfs prioritize maximizing self-interest as their group.
decision-making objective, which leads them to process their On the basis of the RL theory, the predicted response–
own benefits preferentially (Balliet et al., 2009). outcome (PRO) model proposed by Alexander and Brown (2010,
Compared with proselfs, prosocials were more sensitive to 2011) suggests that the general role of the ACC is to predict
fairness in outcomes given a stronger fairness principle; proso- the likely outcomes of actions and to signal unexpected non-
cials also demonstrated automatic processing of fairness at the occurrences of events. Therefore, the MFN is larger when an
early stage. Fair offers in the UG met prosocials’ goal of pursuing actual outcome does not match the expected one (Ferdinand
fairness and held stronger motivational salience, thus eliciting et al., 2012). According to the PRO model, prosocials in the
a larger P2 than unfair offers. Comparatively, due to their pri- present study had strong internalized fairness norms and held
mary processing of self-benefits, proselfs were not sensitive to higher expectation for fair offers than proselfs. Therefore,
outcome fairness in the early processing stage; therefore, the fair offers were expected and elicited a gentler MFN, whereas
P2 patterns for fair and unfair offers did not reveal a differ- unfair offers deviated far from their expectations and elicited
ence in the proself group. These results support behavioral and an enhanced MFN. Consequently, the prosocial group demon-
fMRI findings from previous studies indicating that SVO plays strated a stronger fairness effect on the MFN than proself group
an important role in automatic intuitive processing in fairness- did.
related decision-making (Haruno et al., 2010; Cornelissen et al., On the other hand, people tend to form expectations of
2011; Haruno et al., 2014; Bieleke et al., 2017). partners’ behavior in social interactions, especially in situa-
tions in which information about other social agents is lacking
(Kelley and Stahelski, 1970). The Structural Assumed Similar-
SVO modulates the middle stage of fairness processing
ity Bias model proposes that individuals with different SVOs
The middle stage of fairness processing is represented by the are prone to projecting their own dispositions onto others and
MFN, a critical ERP component related to outcome processing; thus expect others to be similar to themselves (Kuhlman and
it reflects a rapid, coarse and semi-automatic coding of ongo- Wimberley, 1976; Ross et al., 1977; Declerck and Bogaert, 2008;
ing events (Yeung and Sanfey, 2004; Hajcak et al., 2006; Leng Pletzer et al., 2018). Accordingly, prosocials expected more fair
678 | Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2021, Vol. 16, No. 7

behavior from their partners in the UG, and this false consensus this fairness preference is so strong that prosocials even pre-
effect elicited a stronger fairness effect on the MFN. Conversely, fer to make personal sacrifices to attain equal outcomes (Eek
proselfs assumed their partners were similar to themselves and Gärling, 2006). As such, in the current study, fair offers in
and thus had lower expectations for fair behavior, generat- the UG carried different meanings for prosocials and proselfs.
ing a weaker fairness effect on the MFN relative to prosocials. For proselfs, a fair offer represented a more profitable outcome
Subsequently, according to the goal-expectation hypothesis, that met these individuals’ motivation to maximize their self-
expectations about others’ behavior can influence individuals’ interests, thus evoking more attentional resources as reflected
decision-making (Pruitt and Kimmel, 1977; Komorita et al., 1992). by enhanced P3 responses. For prosocials, a fair offer indicated
Therefore, in this study, a more prosocial SVO caused individ- greater benefit along with adherence to fairness norms, fulfill-
uals to possess higher expectations for fair outcomes. When ing their primary goal of pursuing equality in outcomes. Prior
confronted with outcomes violating this expectancy, partici- fMRI studies revealed that individuals reacted to fairness with a
pants were more likely to reject unfair offers. This mediating positive hedonic response, and fairness preferences were asso-
effect has been partially supported by previous studies in which ciated with reward-based regions of the brain including the
the anticipated cooperation mediated the relationship between ventral striatum, vmPFC and orbitofrontal cortex (Tabibnia and
SVO and cooperation in social dilemmas (Balliet et al., 2009; Lieberman, 2007; Tabibnia et al., 2008; Tricomi et al., 2010). There-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/scan/article/16/7/670/6189093 by guest on 20 March 2023


Renkewitz et al., 2011; Balliet and Van Lange, 2013; Pletzer et al., fore, compared with proselfs, a fair offer appeared more reward-
2018). ing and desirable for prosocials, who placed greater subjective
In addition to evaluating whether outcomes deviated from importance on such outcomes. Accordingly, the P3 response for
expectations, the ACC is recruited by affectively aversive pro- fair offers was stronger in the prosocial group, lending support
cessing of social pain (Eisenberger, 2012; Rotge et al., 2015; to inequality aversion theory (Chen and Vazsonyi, 2011; Harlé
Dalgleish et al., 2017; Schmälzle et al., 2017; Chester et al., 2018). et al., 2012).
The motivational/affective theory of the MFN holds that the MFN In summary, this study unveiled a modulating effect of
reflects a motivational/affective evaluation of an outcome event, SVO on individuals’ fairness processing during social decision-
and this evaluation is particularly sensitive to a negative out- making using an ERP technique. Behaviorally, prosocials were
come (Gehring and Willoughby, 2002; Pailing and Segalowitz, more willing to reject unfair offers and had stronger negative
2004). That is, the MFN may represent evaluation of the subjec- emotional experiences than proselfs; prosocials were more sat-
tive value of an outcome (Luo et al., 2014). Thus, for prosocials isfied with fair offers. Electrophysiologically, relative to proselfs,
who value fairness, unfair offers that violated fairness norms prosocials exhibited automatic intuitive processing of fairness
elicited stronger negative feelings, reflected in more pronounced at an early stage represented by the P2, suggesting that SVO
MFN responses compared with proselfs (who had less regard for reflects stable individual differences in an inherent sense of
such fairness norms). This result was in line with a previous ERP outcome fairness. In the middle processing stage, unfairness
study in which the fairness effect on the MFN was stronger in greatly violated prosocials’ expectations and triggered stronger
participants with high moral identity (Boksem and De Cremer, negative emotional reactions than in proselfs as depicted by
2010). Moreover, on the basis of the emotion model of fairness the MFN. Importantly, participants’ SVO affected their rejec-
processing, negative emotions induced by perceived unfairness tion of unfair offers via the d-MFN evoked by unfair and fair
have been found to be predictive of rejection behaviors in the UG offers, which might reveal neural mechanisms underlying the
(Pillutla and Murnighan, 1996; Sanfey et al., 2003). Accordingly, SVO effect on individuals’ behavioral reactions to unfairness.
individuals with higher SVO scores (i.e. who are more prosocial) Correspondingly, compared with proselfs, fair outcomes met
may experience stronger negative reactions to unfair offers and prosocials’ decision-making motivation and attracted more of
be more prone to reject such offers to punish unfair behaviors. their attention as indicated by the P3. These findings shed light
on differences in the behavioral patterns and temporal char-
acteristics of neural activities among individuals with distinct
SVO modulates the late stage of fairness processing
SVOs in fairness processing. Such results help extend the lit-
The late stage of fairness processing is represented by the P3, erature on behavioral responses and neuroimaging, offering
another important ERP component reflecting elaborative and compelling evidence for classic models related to fairness norms
sustained processing of a current outcome (Yeung and Sanfey, relevant to numerous perspectives and processing stages.
2004; Philiastides et al., 2010; Hu et al., 2017). Research has indi-
cated that the P3 is related to processes of attentional allocation
Limitations and outlook for subsequent research
and high-level motivational or affective evaluation, namely in
signaling the subjective awareness and importance of a desired There are several limitations in our study. First, we only con-
outcome (Schupp et al., 2004; Nieuwenhuis et al., 2005; Wu and sidered disadvantageous inequality (DI) as unfairness in the
Zhou, 2009; Mussel et al., 2018). Similar to earlier research, fair present study. However, advantageous inequality (AI) also plays
offers evoked a larger P3 than unfair offers in this study; thus, an important role in fairness-related decision-making. Although
compared with unfair offers, fair offers were considered desir- human beings show aversion to both DI and AI, previous stud-
able outcomes consistent with individuals’ fairness preference ies have found that individuals strongly oppose DI, while their
and allocated more attentional resources from them (Wu et al., responses to AI are relatively modest (Falk and Fischbacher,
2011; Qu et al., 2013; Wang et al., 2017a). Remarkably, the fair- 2006; Yu et al., 2014). In addition, studies have shown that eval-
ness effect on the P3 was modulated by SVO: fair offers evoked uating AI requires more cognitive resources, such as reputation
stronger P3 responses among prosocials than proselfs. management and sustainability of cooperation, than evaluating
As proposed in the integrative model of SVO, relative to pro- DI (van den Bos et al., 2006; Fliessbach et al., 2012; Gao et al.,
selfs, prosocials generally assign greater weight to equality in 2018). Since individuals’ SVO has an impact on the trade-off
outcomes and outcomes for others (Van Lange, 1999; Van Lange between economic interests and fairness norm enforcement, it
et al., 2013). Scholars have also found that prosocials prefer equal is meaningful to explore the SVO effect of individual processing
outcomes compared with proselfs when making decisions, and AI offers in the UG.
X. Hu and X. Mai | 679

Second, we only focused on highly unfair offers and fair Bieleke, M., Gollwitzer, P.M., Oettingen, G., Fischbacher, U.
offers and excluded the moderately unfair offer (3–7 offer) in this (2017). Social value orientation moderates the effects of
study. The moderately unfair condition is also worth exploring intuition versus reflection on responses to unfair ulti-
due to its fuzziness and uncertainty (Polezzi et al., 2008; Duek matum offers. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 30(2),
et al., 2014; Wang et al., 2017a). Are prosocials and proselfs’ eval- 569–81.
uation criteria for moderately unfair schemes different? Could Bogaert, S., Boone, C., Declerck, C. (2008). Social value ori-
this diversity affect individuals’ response patterns? These issues entation and cooperation in social dilemmas: a review and
are worthy of further exploration in the future. conceptual model. British Journal of Social Psychology, 47(3),
Finally, other personal characteristics, such as gender dif- 453–80.
ferences, should also be taken into account in future work. Boksem, M.A., De Cremer, D. (2010). Fairness concerns predict
Previous studies have shown that gender has an impact on medial frontal negativity amplitude in ultimatum bargaining.
the level of trust in individuals such that women trust oth- Social Neuroscience, 5(1), 118–28.
ers more than men in economic transactions (Chaudhuri et al., Bowles, S., Gintis, H. (2004). The evolution of strong reciprocity:
2013; Haselhuhn et al., 2015). In addition, signals of trustworthi- cooperation in heterogeneous populations. Theoretical Popula-
ness influence the relationship between SVO and cooperation: tion Biology, 65(1), 17–28.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/scan/article/16/7/670/6189093 by guest on 20 March 2023


compared to proselfs, prosocials are more sensitive to informa- Boudreau, C., Mccubbins, M.D., Coulson, S. (2009). Knowing
tion signaling trustworthiness of others (Bogaert et al., 2008). when to trust others: an ERP study of decision making after
Therefore, it would be interesting to investigate how gender and receiving information from unknown people. Social Cognitive
SVO interactively affect individuals’ fairness processing in social and Affective Neuroscience, 4(1), 23–34.
decision making. Bradley, M.M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B.N., Lang, P.J. (2001).
Emotion and motivation I: defensive and appetitive reactions
in picture processing. Emotion, 1(3), 276–98.
Funding Camerer, C.F. (2003). Behavioral Game Theory: Experiments in Strate-
This work was supported by grants from the National Natural gic Interaction. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Science Foundation of China (31970986, 31771206), the Major Chaudhuri, A., Paichayontvijit, T., Shen, L. (2013). Gender dif-
Project of National Social Science Foundation (19ZDA363) and ferences in trust and trustworthiness: individuals, single
the Outstanding Innovative Talents Cultivation Funded Pro- sex and mixed sex groups. Journal of Economic Psychology, 34,
grams 2019 of Renmin University of China. 181–94.
Chester, D.S., Lynam, D.R., Milich, R., DeWall, C.N. (2018). Neural
mechanisms of the rejection-aggression link. Social Cognitive
Conflict of interest
and Affective Neuroscience, 13(5), 501–12.
The authors declare no conflict of interest. Cornelissen, G., Dewitte, S., Warlop, L. (2011). Are social value
orientations expressed automatically? Decision making in the
dictator game. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(8),
References 1080–90.
Alexander, W.H., Brown, J.W. (2010). Computational models of Dalgleish, T., Walsh, N.D., Mobbs, D., et al. (2017). Social pain
performance monitoring and cognitive control. Topics in Cog- and social gain in the adolescent brain: a common neural cir-
nitive Science, 2(4), 658–77. cuitry underlying both positive and negative social evaluation.
Alexander, W.H., Brown, J.W. (2011). Medial prefrontal cortex Scientific Reports, 7, 42010.
as an action-outcome predictor. Nature Neuroscience, 14(10), Damasio, A.R., Grabowski, T.J., Bechara, A., et al. (2000).
1338–44. Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feel-
Amiez, C., Joseph, J.P., Procyk, E. (2005). Anterior cingulate error- ing of self-generated emotions. Nature Neuroscience, 3(10),
related activity is modulated by predicted reward. European 1049–56.
Journal of Neuroscience, 21(12), 3447–52. De Cremer, D., Van Lange, P.A. (2001). Why prosocials exhibit
Anderl, C., Hahn, T., Notebaert, K., Klotz, C., Rutter, B., greater cooperation than proselfs: the roles of social respon-
Windmann, S. (2015). Cooperative preferences fluctuate sibility and reciprocity. European Journal of Personality, 15(S1),
across the menstrual cycle. Judgment and Decision Making, 5–18.
10(5), 400–6. Decety, J., Yoder, K.J. (2017). The emerging social neuroscience of
Ashton, M.C., Lee, K. (2007). Empirical, theoretical, and practi- justice motivation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(1), 6–14.
cal advantages of the HEXACO model of personality structure. Declerck, C.H., Bogaert, S. (2008). Social value orientation:
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11(2), 150–66. related to empathy and the ability to read the mind in the eyes.
Balliet, D., Parks, C., Joireman, J. (2009). Social value orientation The Journal of Social Psychology, 148(6), 711–26.
and cooperation in social dilemmas: a meta-analysis. Group Duek, O., Osher, Y., Belmaker, R.H., Bersudsky, Y., Kofman, O.
Processes and Intergroup Relations, 12(4), 533–47. (2014). Reward sensitivity and anger in euthymic bipolar dis-
Balliet, D., Van Lange, P.A.M. (2013). Trust, conflict, and cooper- order. Psychiatry Research, 215(1), 95–100.
ation: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 139(5), 1090–112. Eek, D., Gärling, T. (2006). Prosocials prefer equal outcomes to
Bellebaum, C., Polezzi, D., Daum, I. (2010). It is less than you maximizing joint outcomes. British Journal of Social Psychology,
expected: the feedback-related negativity reflects violations 45(2), 321–37.
of reward magnitude expectations. Neuropsychologia, 48(11), Eisenberger, N.I. (2012). The pain of social disconnection: exam-
3343–50. ining the shared neural underpinnings of physical and social
Bellebaum, C., Daum, I. (2008). Learning-related changes in pain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13(6), 421–34.
reward expectancy are reflected in the feedback-related nega- Falk, A., Fischbacher, U. (2006). A theory of reciprocity. Games and
tivity. European Journal of Neuroscience, 27(7), 1823–35. Economic Behavior, 54(2), 293–315.
680 | Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2021, Vol. 16, No. 7

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., Lang, A.G. (2009). Statistical in prosocial and individualistic economic choices. Journal of
power analyses using G*power 3.1: tests for correlation and Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(8), 1861–70.
regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149–60. Haselhuhn, M.P., Kennedy, J.A., Kray, L.J., Van Zant, A.B.,
Fehr, E., Fischbacher, U., Gächter, S. (2002). Strong reciprocity, Schweitzer, M.E. (2015). Gender differences in trust dynam-
human cooperation, and the enforcement of social norms. ics: women trust more than men following a trust violation.
Human Nature, 13(1), 1–25. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 56, 104–9.
Fehr, E., Schmidt, K. M. (1999). A theory of fairness, competition, Hewig, J., Kretschmer, N., Trippe, R.H., et al. (2011). Why humans
and cooperation. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114(3), deviate from rational choice. Psychophysiology, 48(4), 507–14.
817–868. Hilbig, B.E., Glöckner, A., Zettler, I. (2014). Personality and proso-
Feng, C., Luo, Y.J., Krueger, F. (2015). Neural signatures of cial behavior: linking basic traits and social value orientations.
fairness-related normative decision making in the ultima- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(3), 529–39.
tum game: a coordinate-based meta-analysis. Human Brain Holper, L., Burke, C.J., Fausch, C., Seifritz, E., Tobler, P.N. (2018).
Mapping, 36(2), 591–602. Inequality signals in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex inform
Ferdinand, N.K., Mecklinger, A., Kray, J., Gehring, W.J. (2012). The social preference models. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuro-
processing of unexpected positive response outcomes in the science, 13(5), 513–24.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/scan/article/16/7/670/6189093 by guest on 20 March 2023


mediofrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(35), 12087–92. Holroyd, C.B., Pakzad-Vaezi, K.L., Krigolson, O.E. (2008). The
Fiedler, S., Glöckner, A., Nicklisch, A., Dickert, S. (2013). Social feedback correct-related positivity: sensitivity of the event-
value orientation and information search in social dilemmas: related brain potential to unexpected positive feedback. Psy-
an eye-tracking analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human chophysiology, 45(5), 688–97.
Decision Processes, 120(2), 272–84. Holroyd, C.B., Coles, M.G. (2002). The neural basis of human error
Fliessbach, K., Phillipps, C.B., Trautner, P., et al. (2012). Neu- processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-
ral responses to advantageous and disadvantageous inequity. related negativity. Psychological Review, 109(4), 679–709.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 165. Horat, S.K., Herrmann, F.R., Favre, G., et al. (2016). Assessment of
Gao, X., Yu, H., Sáez, I., et al. (2018). Distinguishing neural mental workload: a new electrophysiological method based on
correlates of context-dependent advantageous-and disadvan intra-block averaging of ERP amplitudes. Neuropsychologia, 82,
0tageous-inequity aversion. Proceedings of the National Academy 11–7.
of Sciences of the United States of America, 115(33), E7680–89. Hu, X., Xu, Z., Mai, X. (2017). Social value orientation modulates
Gehring, W.J., Willoughby, A.R. (2002). The medial frontal cortex the processing of outcome evaluation involving others. Social
and the rapid processing of monetary gains and losses. Science, Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(11), 1730–9.
295(5563), 2279–82. Kable, J.W., Glimcher, P.W. (2007). The neural correlates of sub-
Gilam, G., Lin, T., Raz, G., et al. (2015). Neural substrates under- jective value during intertemporal choice. Nature Neuroscience,
lying the tendency to accept anger-infused ultimatum offers 10(12), 1625–33.
during dynamic social interactions. NeuroImage, 120, 400–11. Kaltwasser, L., Hildebrandt, A., Wilhelm, O., Sommer, W. (2016).
Glazer, J.E., Kelley, N.J., Pornpattananangkul, N., Mittal, Behavioral and neuronal determinants of negative reciprocity
V.A., Nusslock, R. (2018). Beyond the FRN: broadening the in the ultimatum game. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuro-
time-course of EEG and ERP components implicated in science, 11(10), 1608–17.
reward processing. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 132, Kelley, H.H., Stahelski, A.J. (1970). Social interaction basis of
184–202. cooperators’ and competitors’ beliefs about others. Journal of
Gospic, K., Mohlin, E., Fransson, P., Petrovic, P., Johannesson, M., Personality and Social Psychology, 16(1), 66.
Ingvar, M. (2011). Limbic justice—amygdala involvement in Kimura, K., Sawada, H., Katayama, J.I. (2018). Outcome
immediate rejection in the ultimatum game. PLoS Biology, 9(5), evaluations in group decision-making using authority
e1001054. rule: an electrophysiological study. Neuropsychologia, 119,
Güth, W., Schmittberger, R., Schwarze, B. (1982). An experi- 271–9.
mental analysis of ultimatum bargaining. Journal of Economic Knoch, D., Pascual-Leone, A., Meyer, K., Treyer, V., Fehr, E.
Behavior and Organization, 3(4), 367–88. (2006). Diminishing reciprocal fairness by disrupting the right
Güth, W., Kocher, M.G. (2014). More than thirty years of ulti- prefrontal cortex. Science, 314(5800), 829–32.
matum bargaining experiments: motives, variations, and a Knoch, D., Nitsche, M.A., Fischbacher, U., Eisenegger, C.,
survey of the recent literature. Journal of Economic Behavior and Pascual-Leone, A., Fehr, E. (2008). Studying the neurobi-
Organization, 108, 396–409. ology of social interaction with transcranial direct current
Hajcak, G., Moser, J.S., Holroyd, C.B., Simons, R.F. (2006). The stimulation—the example of punishing unfairness. Cerebral
feedback-related negativity reflects the binary evaluation of Cortex, 18(9), 1987–90.
good versus bad outcomes. Biological Psychology, 71(2), 148–54. Komorita, S.S., Parks, C.D., Hulbert, L.G. (1992). Reciprocity and
Halko, M.L., Hlushchuk, Y., Hari, R., Schürmann, M. (2009). Com- the induction of cooperation in social dilemmas. Journal of
peting with peers: mentalizing-related brain activity reflects Personality and Social Psychology, 62(4), 607–17.
what is at stake. NeuroImage, 46(2), 542–8. Kuhlman, D.M., Wimberley, D.L. (1976). Expectations of choice
Harlé, K.M., Chang, L.J., van’t Wout, M., Sanfey, A.G. (2012). behavior held by cooperators, competitors, and individualists
The neural mechanisms of affect infusion in social economic across four classes of experimental games. Journal of Personal-
decision-making: a mediating role of the anterior insula. Neu- ity and Social Psychology, 34(1), 69–81.
roImage, 61(1), 32–40. Kuss, K., Falk, A., Trautner, P., Montag, C., Weber, B.,
Haruno, M., Frith, C. D. (2010). Activity in the amygdala elicited Fliessbach, K. (2015). Neuronal correlates of social decision
by unfair divisions predicts social value orientation. Nature making are influenced by social value orientation—an fMRI
Neuroscience, 13(2), 160–161. study. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 40.
Haruno, M., Kimura, M., Frith, C.D. (2014). Activity in the nucleus Liebrand, W.B. (1984). The effect of social motives, communica-
accumbens and amygdala underlies individual differences tion and group size on behaviour in an N-person multi-stage
X. Hu and X. Mai | 681

mixed-motive game. European Journal of Social Psychology, 14(3), Pailing, P. E., Segalowitz, S. J. (2004). The error–related negativity
239–64. as a state and trait measure: Motivation, personality, and ERPs
Leng, Y., Zhou, X. (2010). Modulation of the brain activity in out- in response to errors. Psychophysiology, 41(1), 84–95.
come evaluation by interpersonal relationship: an ERP study. Paul, K., Pourtois, G. (2017). Mood congruent tuning of reward
Neuropsychologia, 48(2), 448–455. expectation in positive mood: evidence from FRN and theta
Luck, S.J. (2014). An Introduction to the Event-related Potential Tech- modulations. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(5),
nique, Cambrigde: MIT press. 765–74.
Luo, Y., Wu, T., Broster, L.S., et al. (2014). The temporal course Peterburs, J., Voegler, R., Liepelt, R., et al. (2017). Processing of
of the influence of anxiety on fairness considerations. Psy- fair and unfair offers in the ultimatum game under social
chophysiology, 51(9), 834–42. observation. Scientific Reports, 7, 44062.
Luo, Y., Feng, C., Wu, T., et al. (2015). Social comparison manifests Pfabigan, D.M., Wucherer, A.M., Wang, X., Pan, X., Lamm, C.,
in event-related potentials. Scientific Reports, 5, 12127. Han, S. (2018). Cultural influences on the processing of social
Marciano, D., Bentin, S., Deouell, L.Y. (2018). Alternative out- comparison feedback signals—an ERP study. Social Cognitive
comes create biased expectations regarding the received and Affective Neuroscience, 13(12), 1317–26.
outcome: evidence from event-related potentials. Neuropsy- Philiastides, M.G., Biele, G., Vavatzanidis, N., Kazzer, P.,

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/scan/article/16/7/670/6189093 by guest on 20 March 2023


chologia, 113, 126–39. Heekeren, H.R. (2010). Temporal dynamics of prediction error
Marco-Pallares, J., Cucurell, D., Münte, T.F., Strien, N., Rodriguez- processing during reward-based decision making. NeuroImage,
Fornells, A. (2011). On the number of trials needed for a stable 53(1), 221–32.
feedback-related negativity. Psychophysiology, 48(6), 852–60. Pillutla, M.M., Murnighan, J.K. (1996). Unfairness, anger, and
Massi, B., Luhmann, C.C. (2015). Fairness influences early signa- spite: emotional rejections of ultimatum offers. Organizational
tures of reward-related neural processing. Cognitive, Affective, Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 68(3), 208–24.
and Behavioral Neuroscience, 15(4), 768–75. Pletzer, J.L., Balliet, D., Joireman, J., et al. (2018). Social value ori-
Matsumoto, M., Matsumoto, K., Abe, H., Tanaka, K. (2007). entation, expectations, and cooperation in social dilemmas: a
Medial prefrontal cell activity signaling prediction errors of meta-analysis. European Journal of Personality, 32(1), 62–83.
action values. Nature Neuroscience, 10(5), 647–56. Polezzi, D., Daum, I., Rubaltelli, E., et al. (2008). Mentalizing in
McClintock, C. G. (1972). Social motivation–A set of propositions. economic decision-making. Behavioural Brain Research, 190(2),
Behavioral Science, 17(5), 438–454. 218–23.
McPartland, J.C., Crowley, M.J., Perszyk, D.R., et al. (2011). Tempo- Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and
ral dynamics reveal atypical brain response to social exclusion P3b. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118(10), 2128–48.
in autism. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(3), 271–9. Potts, G.F., Patel, S.H., Azzam, P.N. (2004). Impact of instructed
Miller, E.K., Cohen, J.D. (2001). An integrative theory of pre- relevance on the visual ERP. International Journal of Psychophys-
frontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24(1), iology, 52(2), 197–209.
167–202. Potts, G.F., Martin, L.E., Burton, P., Montague, P.R. (2006). When
Miltner, W.H., Braun, C.H., Coles, M.G. (1997). Event-related brain things are better or worse than expected: the medial frontal
potentials following incorrect feedback in a time-estimation cortex and the allocation of processing resources. Journal of
task: evidence for a “generic” neural system for error detec- Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(7), 1112–9.
tion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(6), 788–98. Preacher, K.J., Hayes, A.F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling
Morgenstern, O., Von Neumann, J. (1953). Theory of Games and strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in mul-
Economic Behavior, Princeton: Princeton University Press. tiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40(3), 879–91.
Mothes, H., Enge, S., Strobel, A. (2016). The interplay between Pruitt, D.G., Kimmel, M.J. (1977). Twenty years of experimental
feedback-related negativity and individual differences in gaming: critique, synthesis, and suggestions for the future.
altruistic punishment: an EEG study. Cognitive, Affective, and Annual Review of Psychology, 28(1), 363–92.
Behavioral Neuroscience, 16(2), 276–88. Qi, Y., Wu, H., Raiha, S., Liu, X. (2018). Social value orienta-
Murphy, R.O., Ackermann, K.A., Handgraaf, M.J.J. (2011). Measur- tion modulates context-based social comparison preference
ing social value orientation. Judgment and Decision Making, 6, in the outcome evaluation: an ERP study. Neuropsychologia,
771–81. 112, 135–44.
Murphy, R.O., Ackermann, K.A. (2014). Social value orientation: Qu, C., Wang, Y., Huang, Y. (2013). Social exclusion modulates
theoretical and measurement issues in the study of social fairness consideration in the ultimatum game: an ERP study.
preferences. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 18(1), Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 505.
13–41. Renkewitz, F., Fuchs, H.M., Fiedler, S. (2011). Is there evidence
Mussel, P., Hewig, J., Weiß, M. (2018). The reward-like nature of publication biases in JDM research? Judgment and Decision
of social cues that indicate successful altruistic punishment. Making, 6, 870–81.
Psychophysiology, 55(9), e13093. Rilling, J.K., Sanfey, A.G. (2011). The neuroscience of social
Nieuwenhuis, S., Aston-Jones, G., Cohen, J.D. (2005). Decision decision-making. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 23–48.
making, the P3, and the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine sys- Ross, L., Greene, D., House, P. (1977). The “false consensus
tem. Psychological Bulletin, 131(4), 510–32. effect”: an egocentric bias in social perception and attribu-
Nowak, M.A., Page, K.M., Sigmund, K. (2000). Fairness versus tion processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13(3),
reason in the ultimatum game. Science, 289(5485), 1773–5. 279–301.
Oliveira, F.T., McDonald, J.J., Goodman, D. (2007). Perfor- Rotge, J.Y., Lemogne, C., Hinfray, S., et al. (2015). A meta-analysis
mance monitoring in the anterior cingulate is not all error of the anterior cingulate contribution to social pain. Social
related: expectancy deviation and the representation of Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(1), 19–27.
action-outcome associations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, Rubinstein, A. (1982). Perfect equilibrium in a bargaining model.
19(12), 1994–2004. Econometrica, 50, 97–109.
682 | Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2021, Vol. 16, No. 7

Sambrook, T.D., Goslin, J. (2014). Mediofrontal event-related Van der Molen, M.J., Poppelaars, E.S., Van Hartingsveldt, C.T.,
potentials in response to positive, negative and unsigned Harrewijn, A., Gunther Moor, B., Westenberg, P.M. (2014). Fear
prediction errors. Neuropsychologia, 61, 1–10. of negative evaluation modulates electrocortical and behav-
San Martín, R. (2012). Event-related potential studies of outcome ioral responses when anticipating social evaluative feedback.
processing and feedback-guided learning. Frontiers in Human Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 936.
Neuroscience, 6, 304. Van der Veen, F.M., Sahibdin, P.P. (2011). Dissociation between
Sanfey, A.G., Rilling, J.K., Aronson, J.A., Nystrom, L.E., Cohen, J.D. medial frontal negativity and cardiac responses in the ultima-
(2003). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the tum game: effects of offer size and fairness. Cognitive, Affective,
ultimatum game. Science, 300(5626), 1755–8. and Behavioral Neuroscience, 11(4), 516–25.
Sato, A., Yasuda, A., Ohira, H., et al. (2005). Effects of value and Van Lange, P.A., De Bruin, E., Otten, W., Joireman, J.A. (1997).
reward magnitude on feedback negativity and P300. Neurore- Development of prosocial, individualistic, and competitive
port, 16(4), 407–11. orientations: theory and preliminary evidence. Journal of Per-
Schmälzle, R., O’Donnell, M.B., Garcia, J.O., et al. (2017). Brain sonality and Social Psychology, 73(4), 733–46.
connectivity dynamics during social interaction reflect social Van Lange, P.A. (1999). The pursuit of joint outcomes and equal-
network structure. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sci- ity in outcomes: an integrative model of social value orienta-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/scan/article/16/7/670/6189093 by guest on 20 March 2023


ences of the United States of America, 114(20), 5153–8. tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(2), 337–49.
Schupp, H., Cuthbert, B., Bradley, M., Hillman, C., Hamm, A., Van Lange, P.A., Joireman, J., Parks, C.D., Van Dijk, E. (2013).
Lang, P. (2004). Brain processes in emotional percep- The psychology of social dilemmas: a review. Organizational
tion: motivated attention. Cognition and Emotion, 18(5), Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 120(2), 125–41.
593–611. Van Lange, P.A., Kuhlman, D.M. (1994). Social value orientations
Semlitsch, H.V., Anderer, P., Schuster, P., Presslich, O. (1986). and impressions of partner’s honesty and intelligence: a test
A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, of the might versus morality effect. Journal of Personality and
applied to the P300 ERP. Psychophysiology, 23(6), 695–703. Social Psychology, 67(1), 126–41.
Sidarus, N., Vuorre, M., Haggard, P. (2017). How action selection Wang, Y., Kuhlman, D.M., Roberts, K., et al. (2017a). Social value
influences the sense of agency: an ERP study. NeuroImage, 150, orientation modulates the FRN and P300 in the chicken game.
1–13. Biological Psychology, 127, 89–98.
Tabibnia, G., Satpute, A.B., Lieberman, M.D. (2008). The sunny Wu, Y., Leliveld, M.C., Zhou, X. (2011). Social distance modulates
side of fairness: preference for fairness activates reward cir- recipient’s fairness consideration in the dictator game: an ERP
cuitry (and disregarding unfairness activates self-control cir- study. Biological Psychology, 88(2–3), 253–62.
cuitry). Psychological Science, 19(4), 339–47. Wu, Y., Zhou, X. (2009). The P300 and reward valence, magnitude,
Tabibnia, G., Lieberman, M.D. (2007). Fairness and cooperation and expectancy in outcome evaluation. Brain Research, 1286,
are rewarding. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 114–22.
1118(1), 90–101. Xiao, E., Houser, D. (2005). Emotion expression in human punish-
Tricomi, E., Rangel, A., Camerer, C.F., O’Doherty, J.P. (2010). Neu- ment behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of
ral evidence for inequality-averse social preferences. Nature, the United States of America, 102(20), 7398–401.
463(7284), 1089. Yang, Q., Tang, P., Gu, R., Luo, W., Luo, Y.J. (2015). Implicit emo-
van’T Wout, M., Kahn, R.S., Sanfey, A.G., Aleman, A. tion regulation affects outcome evaluation. Social Cognitive and
(2005). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over the Affective Neuroscience, 10(6), 824–31.
right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex affects strategic decision- Yeung, N., Holroyd, C.B., Cohen, J.D. (2005). ERP correlates of
making. Neuroreport, 16(16), 1849–52. feedback and reward processing in the presence and absence
van’T Wout, M., Kahn, R.S., Sanfey, A.G., Aleman, A. (2006). of response choice. Cerebral Cortex, 15(5), 535–44.
Affective state and decision-making in the ultimatum game. Yeung, N., Sanfey, A.G. (2004). Independent coding of reward
Experimental Brain Research, 169(4), 564–8. magnitude and valence in the human brain. Journal of Neuro-
van den Bos, K., Peters, S.L., Bobocel, D.R., Ybema, J.F. (2006). science, 24(28), 6258–64.
On preferences and doing the right thing: satisfaction with Yu, R., Calder, A.J., Mobbs, D. (2014). Overlapping and dis-
advantageous inequity when cognitive processing is limited. tinct representations of advantageous and disadvantageous
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(3), 273–89. inequality. Human Brain Mapping, 35(7), 3290–301.

You might also like