Lyubomirsky S Sheldon and Schkade 2005 Pursuing Happiness PDF

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 21

Review of General Psychology Copyright 2005 by the Educational Publishing Foundation

2005, Vol. 9, No. 2, 111–131 1089-2680/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1089-2680.9.2.111

Pursuing Happiness: The Architecture of Sustainable Change


Sonja Lyubomirsky Kennon M. Sheldon
University of California, Riverside University of Missouri—Columbia

David Schkade
University of California, San Diego

The pursuit of happiness is an important goal for many people. However, surprisingly
little scientific research has focused on the question of how happiness can be increased
and then sustained, probably because of pessimism engendered by the concepts of
genetic determinism and hedonic adaptation. Nevertheless, emerging sources of opti-
mism exist regarding the possibility of permanent increases in happiness. Drawing on
the past well-being literature, the authors propose that a person’s chronic happiness
level is governed by 3 major factors: a genetically determined set point for happiness,
happiness-relevant circumstantial factors, and happiness-relevant activities and prac-
tices. The authors then consider adaptation and dynamic processes to show why the
activity category offers the best opportunities for sustainably increasing happiness.
Finally, existing research is discussed in support of the model, including 2 preliminary
happiness-increasing interventions.

The pursuit of happiness holds an honored such as “Are you happy?” and “Are you having
position in American society, beginning with fun?” fit nearly every occasion (Markus &
the Declaration of Independence, where it is Kitayama, 1994). Not surprisingly, the majority
promised as a cherished right for all citizens. of U.S. residents rate personal happiness as very
Today, the enduring U.S. obsession with how to important (Diener, Suh, Smith, & Shao, 1995;
be happy can be observed in the row upon row Triandis, Bontempo, Leung, & Hui, 1990) and
of popular psychology and self-help books in report thinking about happiness at least once
any major bookstore and in the millions of every day (Freedman, 1978). Furthermore, the
copies of these books that are sold. Indeed, pursuit of happiness is no longer just a North
many social contexts in the United States have American obsession, but instead it is becoming
the production of happiness and positive feel- ever more global as people seek to fulfill the
ings as their primary purpose, and questions promises of capitalism and political freedom
(Diener et al., 1995; Freedman, 1978; Triandis
et al., 1990). It seems that nearly all people
believe, or would like to believe, that they can
Sonja Lyubomirsky, Department of Psychology, Univer- move in an “upward spiral” (Sheldon & Houser-
sity of California, Riverside; Kennon M. Sheldon, Depart- Marko, 2001) toward ever greater personal
ment of Psychology, University of Missouri—Columbia; well-being.
David Schkade, Rady School of Management, University of
California, San Diego. Is the pursuit of happiness merely a bourgeois
This work was supported in part by grants from the concern, a symptom of Western comfort and
Positive Psychology Network. We are grateful to Linda self-centeredness, a factor that has no real im-
Houser-Marko, Kathleen Jamir, and Chris Tkach for con- pact on psychological adjustment and adapta-
ducting library research and to Shelley Taylor, David Sher- tion? The empirical evidence suggests that this
man, and the other members of Psychology 421 for valuable
comments on a draft. is not the case. Indeed, a number of researchers
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad- and thinkers have argued that the ability to be
dressed to Sonja Lyubomirsky, Department of Psychology, happy and contented with life is a central crite-
University of California, Riverside, CA 92521, or Kennon rion of adaptation and positive mental health
M. Sheldon, Department of Psychological Sciences, 112
McAlester Hall, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO (e.g., Diener, 1984; Jahoda, 1958; Taylor &
65211. E-mail: sonja@citrus.ucr.edu or sheldonk@missouri Brown, 1988). Bolstering this notion, Ly-
.edu ubomirsky and her colleagues recently com-
111
112 LYUBOMIRSKY, SHELDON, AND SCHKADE

piled evidence showing that happiness has nu- ples work? Do some work better than others?
merous positive byproducts that appear to ben- Do the principles work better for some people
efit individuals, families, and communities than for others? Are any positive effects of the
(Lyubomirsky, King, & Diener, 2004; see also principles due, ultimately, to placebo effects? If
Fredrickson, 2001). Furthermore, Lyubomirsky the book actually helps people “get happier,”
et al.’s analysis revealed that happy people gain does the happiness boost last? Although it is
tangible benefits in many different life domains possible that some of the advice given in this
from their positive state of mind, including and other similar books could well be appropri-
larger social rewards (higher odds of marriage ate and effective, the authors provide almost no
and lower odds of divorce, more friends, stron- empirical research in support of their claims.
ger social support, and richer social interac- One receives little more guidance from con-
tions; e.g., Harker & Keltner, 2001; Marks & temporary academic psychology. Of course, re-
Fleming, 1999; Okun, Stock, Haring, & Witter, search psychologists have identified many pre-
1984), superior work outcomes (greater creativ- dictors of people’s happiness or subjective well-
ity, increased productivity, higher quality of being. For example, well-being has been shown
work, and higher income; e.g., Estrada, Isen, & to be associated with a wide variety of factors,
Young, 1994; Staw, Sutton, & Pelled, 1995), including demographic status (e.g., Argyle,
and more activity, energy, and flow (e.g., Csik- 1999; Diener, Suh, Lucas, & Smith, 1999; My-
szentmihalyi & Wong, 1991). ers, 2000), personality traits and attitudes (e.g.,
Further supporting the argument that subjec- Diener & Lucas, 1999), and goal characteristics
tive happiness may be integral to mental and (e.g., McGregor & Little, 1998). However, a
physical health, happy people are more likely to limitation of previous research is that the vast
evidence greater self-control and self-regulatory majority of studies have been cross sectional
and coping abilities (e.g., Aspinwall, 1998; and have reported between-subjects effects
Fredrickson & Joiner, 2002; Keltner & Bon- rather than investigating well-being longitudi-
anno, 1997), to have a bolstered immune system nally and examining within-subject effects. In
(e.g., Dillon, Minchoff, & Baker, 1985; Stone et addition, very few happiness intervention stud-
al., 1994), and even to live a longer life (e.g., ies have been conducted. Thus, researchers still
Danner, Snowdon, & Friesen, 2001; Ostir, know surprisingly little about how to change
Markides, Black, & Goodwin, 2000). Also, well-being, that is, about the possibility of “be-
happy people are not just self-centered or self- coming happier.” Doubtless, part of the reason
ish; the literature suggests that happy individu- for this neglect is the difficulty of conducting
als instead tend to be relatively more coopera- longitudinal and intervention studies. The prob-
tive, prosocial, charitable, and “other-centered” lem is further compounded by the tendency of
(e.g., Isen, 1970; Kasser & Ryan, 1996; Wil- applied mental health researchers to focus on
liams & Shiaw, 1999). pathology rather than on positive mental health
In summary, happy individuals appear more (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000) and by
likely to be flourishing people, both inwardly the thorny issues raised when theorists speculate
and outwardly. Thus, we argue that enhancing on how people “should” live their lives to max-
people’s happiness levels may indeed be a wor- imize their potential for happiness (Schwartz,
thy scientific goal, especially after their basic 2000). However, we believe the principal rea-
physical and security needs are met. Unfortu- son for the neglect of this question is the con-
nately, however, relatively little scientific sup- siderable scientific pessimism over whether it is
port exists for the idea that people’s happiness even possible to effect sustainable increases in
levels can change for the better. For example, happiness.
the happiness-boosting techniques proposed in
the self-help literature generally have limited Historical Sources of Pessimism
grounding in scientific theory and even less
empirical confirmation of their effectiveness Three considerations serve to illustrate the
(Norcross et al., 2000). Consider a representa- depth of this pessimism. First is the idea of a
tive best seller, You Can Be Happy No Matter genetically determined set point (or set range)
What: Five Principles for Keeping Life in Per- for happiness. Lykken and Tellegen (1996)
spective, by Carlson (1997). Do the five princi- have provided evidence, based on twin studies
SPECIAL ISSUE: ARCHITECTURE OF SUSTAINABLE HAPPINESS 113

and adoption studies, that the heritability of (1978) showed that, after 1 year, lottery winners
well-being may be as high as 80% (although a were no happier than controls, and furthermore
more widely accepted figure is 50%; Braungart, recent paralysis victims were not as unhappy as
Plomin, DeFries, & Fulker, 1992; Tellegen et one would expect. Further evidence of hedonic
al., 1988; cf. Diener et al., 1999). Whatever the adaptation comes from findings of remarkably
exact coefficient, its large magnitude suggests small correlations between happiness and
that for each person there is indeed a chronic or wealth (Diener & Lucas, 1999) and Myers’s
characteristic level of happiness. Consistent (2000) observation that while U.S. citizens’ per-
with this idea, Headey and Wearing (1989) sonal income has more than doubled in the
found, in a four-wave panel study, that partici- past 50 years, their happiness levels have re-
pants tended to keep returning to their own mained the same. The notion of an individual
baselines over time (see also Suh, Diener, & fighting against the effects of adaptation brings
Fujita, 1996). Thus, although there may be sub- to mind an image of a pedestrian walking up a
stantial variation around this baseline level in descending escalator. Although the improving
the short term, in the long term people perhaps circumstances of her life may propel her upward
cannot help but return to their set point, or to the toward ever greater happiness, the process of
middle of their set range: “What goes up must adaptation forces her back to her initial state.
come down” (a more detailed description of the Together, these concepts and findings suggest
happiness set point is provided later). that trying to become happier may be as futile as
A second and closely related source of pes- trying to become taller (Lykken & Tellegen,
simism comes from the literature on personality 1996). Indeed, some have argued that pursuing
traits. Traits are cognitive, affective, and behav- happiness may backfire altogether, if the pursuit
ioral complexes that are, by definition, consis- becomes a conscious “extrinsic” goal that dis-
tent across situations and across the life span tracts people from enjoying the moment
and therefore may account for part of the sta- (Schooler, Ariely, & Loewenstein, in press; see
bility of the set point. In support of the latter also Sheldon, 2004). Moreover, striving for
assumption, McCrae and Costa (1990) have happiness may inevitably result in deep disap-
shown impressive long-term stability for the pointment for many people. From this perspec-
“Big Five” traits, including the two traits most tive, rather than seeking an upward spiral,
closely related to well-being: neuroticism and maybe people would be better off simply ac-
extraversion. Specifically, people tend to main- cepting their current personality and happiness
tain the same rank ordering in their levels of levels (McCrae & Costa, 1994). In Zen terms,
worry, rumination, and guilt, as well as in their perhaps one should try to transcend the pursuit
levels of social engagement, enthusiasm, and of happiness rather than trying to maximize it
self-confidence. Because of the close relation (Gaskins, 1999). Indeed, a number of philo-
between psychological well-being and these sophical traditions embrace the notion that hap-
personality characteristics, McCrae and Costa piness should not be increased beyond an ideal
argued that people also tend to maintain the level, one akin to a “Golden Mean” (Aristotle,
same relative level of happiness over time (see 1974) between agony and ecstasy. To be sure,
also Costa, McCrae, & Zonderman, 1987; Die- most people would undoubtedly reject an unre-
ner & Lucas, 1999). strained, ceaseless pursuit of well-being.
A third source of pessimism arises from the
concept of the hedonic treadmill (Brickman & Present Sources of Optimism
Campbell, 1971), which suggests that any gains
in happiness are only temporary, because hu- Is the pursuit of happiness futile? We believe
mans so quickly adapt to change (see also Kah- not. Despite the seemingly compelling reasons
neman, 1999; Tversky & Griffin, 1991). Thus, we have listed for pessimism regarding attempts
although new circumstances may temporarily to elevate levels of well-being, there are also
cause people to become happier or sadder, they compelling reasons for optimism. In the follow-
rapidly adjust, and the effect of these new cir- ing, we briefly describe four sources of opti-
cumstances on happiness then diminishes mism, returning to consider some of them in
quickly or even disappears entirely. For exam- greater detail later. First, some researchers have
ple, Brickman, Coates, and Janoff-Bulman had success, albeit limited and short term, in
114 LYUBOMIRSKY, SHELDON, AND SCHKADE

using interventions to increase happiness (e.g., just by a few people but perhaps by the majority
Fava, 1999; Fordyce, 1977, 1983, Lichter, of people. Indeed, Carstensen’s (1995) socio-
Haye, & Kammann, 1980; Sheldon, Kasser, emotional selectivity theory suggests that older
Smith, & Share, 2002). The potential of happi- people learn to structure their lives and pursue
ness-enhancing interventions is further reflected particular goals that maximize positive emo-
in emerging research in the positive psychology tions, consistent with the proposal that people
tradition demonstrating that practicing certain can learn to sustainably increase their well-
virtues, such as gratitude (Emmons & McCul- being. Further supporting this notion are Shel-
lough, 2003), forgiveness (McCullough, Parga- don and Kasser’s (2001) results, which showed
ment, & Thoresen, 2000), and thoughtful self- that age-related increases in well-being are in
reflection (King, 2001; Lyubomirsky, Sousa, & part mediated by volitional changes, including
Dickerhoof, 2004), can bring about enhanced older people’s ability to select more enjoyable
well-being. Furthermore, research documenting and self-appropriate goals.
the long-term effectiveness of cognitive and Yet another reason why genes are not neces-
behavioral strategies to combat negative affect sarily destiny is that they appear to influence
and depression has encouraging implications happiness indirectly, that is, by influencing the
for the possibility of elevating long-term happi- kinds of experiences and environments one has
ness (e.g., Gloaguen, Cottraux, Cucherat, & or seeks to have. Thus, unwanted effects of
Blackburn, 1998; Jacobson et al., 1996). genes could be minimized by active efforts to
Second, many different motivational and at- steer oneself away from situations that detract
titudinal factors have been linked to well-being, from well-being or by avoiding being enticed
factors that are presumably amenable to some toward maladaptive behaviors (Lykken, 2000;
volitional control. Examples of possible moti- Lyubomirsky, 2001). In addition, it is worth
vational factors include the successful pursuit of noting that heritability coefficients describe co-
life goals that are intrinsic in content (e.g., variations, not mean levels. Furthermore, even a
Kasser & Ryan, 1996); concordant with a per- high heritability coefficient for a particular trait
son’s interests, motives, and values (Brunstein, (such as happiness) does not rule out the possi-
Schultheiss, & Grassman, 1998; Sheldon & El- bility that the mean level of that trait for a
liot, 1999; Sheldon & Kasser, 1995); and inter- specific population can be raised. Under the
nally consistent (e.g., Emmons & King, 1988; right conditions, perhaps anyone can become
Sheldon & Kasser, 1995). Examples of poten- happier, even if her or his rank ordering relative
tially controllable attitudinal factors include the to others remains stable.
tendency to take an optimistic perspective on To summarize, it appears there is a paradox:
one’s life situations (e.g., DeNeve & Cooper, Some theoretical perspectives and empirical
1998; McCrae & Costa, 1986), the inclination data suggest that happiness can be increased,
to avoid social comparisons and contingent self- whereas other theories and data imply that it
evaluations (e.g., Lyubomirsky & Ross, 1997), cannot. How can these conflicting perspectives
and the tendency to feel a sense of optimism or on the possibility of happiness enhancement be
efficacy regarding one’s life (Bandura, 1997; resolved? Also, if enhanced happiness is indeed
Scheier & Carver, 1993; Seligman, 1991; Tay- possible, what kinds of circumstances, activi-
lor & Brown, 1988). ties, or habits of mind are most likely to bring
A third reason for optimism is provided by gains, especially gains that can be maintained?
recent findings that older people tend to be
somewhat happier than younger people Model of Happiness
(Charles, Reynolds, & Gatz, 2001; Diener &
Suh, 1998; Roberts & Chapman, 2000; Sheldon Accordingly, the primary question addressed
& Kasser, 2001). Specifically, both cross-sec- in this article is the following: Through what
tional and longitudinal work has shown that mechanisms, if any, can a chronic happiness
older persons report higher life satisfaction and level higher than the set point be achieved and
lower negative affect. Although these main ef- sustained? To this end, we describe the archi-
fects do not always emerge, they are observed tecture of sustainable happiness. The integra-
frequently enough to suggest that greater hap- tive model of happiness we present accommo-
piness can indeed be achieved over time, not dates the role of both personality/genetic and
SPECIAL ISSUE: ARCHITECTURE OF SUSTAINABLE HAPPINESS 115

circumstantial/demographic factors in happi- It is important to note as well that we use a


ness. However, it also goes beyond these cross- subjectivist definition of happiness, one that
sectional or concurrent factors to incorporate commonly relies on people’s self-reports. We
dynamic, time-sensitive factors. This extension believe this is appropriate and even necessary
allows the question of within-subject change in given our view that happiness must be defined
well-being, and maintained change, to be ad- from the perspective of the person. In other
dressed. Most important, the model incorpo- words, happiness is primarily a subjective phe-
rates the role of motivational and attitudinal nomenon for which the final judge should be
factors, consistent with the assumption that hap- “whoever lives inside a person’s skin” (Myers
piness can be actively pursued. We attempt to & Diener, 1995, p. 11; see also Diener, 1994).
show that certain types of intentional activities However, the fact that the judgment of happi-
indeed offer ways to achieve sustainable ness is necessarily subjective does not mean that
changes in well-being, despite the counteracting influences on that judgment cannot be studied
effects of adaptation. empirically; for example, researchers might in-
In the sections to follow, we first provide a vestigate the effects of factors such as a per-
working definition of chronic happiness. Then son’s recent experiences of positive emotion
we define the three factors that affect it (genetic (Frijda, 1999), the frame in which the question
set point, circumstances, and activities) and ar- is presented (Larsen & Fredrickson, 1999), the
gue that intentional activities offer the best po- meaning that the person ascribes to the question
tential route to higher and sustainable levels of (Schwarz & Strack, 1999), and the person’s
happiness. Subsequently, we consider some sense of making satisfactory progress toward
more complex issues pertaining to the achieve- life goals at the time of the judgment (Carver &
ment of sustainable well-being via intentional Scheier, 1990). We consider some of these fac-
activity, such as the role of person–activity fit, tors in greater detail in a later section. Finally,
optimal timing and variety of activity, and the the fact that self-reported happiness is subjec-
supportive role of sustained effort and posi- tive does not mean that it is unrelated to rela-
tive habits. Then, in the final section of the tively more “objective” variables. For example,
article, we describe several preliminary efforts research has shown significant convergence of
to increase happiness, based on our model, self-reported well-being with peer and spouse
and discuss the nature of effective happiness reports of well-being (e.g., Lyubomirsky &
interventions. Lepper, 1999; Sandvik, Diener, & Seidlitz,
1993), with recall of particular types of events
(e.g., Seidlitz, Wyer, & Diener, 1997), with
Defining Happiness smiling behavior (e.g., Harker & Keltner,
2001), and with physiological responses (e.g.,
Here we define happiness as it is most often Lerner, Taylor, Gonzalez, & Stayn, 2002).
defined in the literature, that is, in terms of
frequent positive affect, high life satisfaction, Chronic Happiness Level
and infrequent negative affect. These three con-
structs are the three primary components of Our primary focus in this article is on a
subjective well-being, according to Diener and person’s characteristic level of happiness during
colleagues (for reviews, see Diener, 1984, 1994; a particular period in his or her life, which we
Diener et al., 1999). Supporting the legitimacy term the chronic happiness level. We define
of considering them as indicators of the same happiness this way because we wish to identify
underlying construct, we find that the measures a quantity that is more enduring than momen-
are highly correlated and typically yield a single tary or daily happiness but that is also somewhat
factor after negative affect has been recoded malleable over time and, thus, amenable to
(Sheldon & Kasser, 1998, 2001; Sheldon & meaningful pursuit. According to this defini-
Lyubomirsky, 2004). To refer to this group of tion, although it is possible to alter one’s
measures, we use the term happiness or subjec- chronic happiness level, it is much more diffi-
tive well-being, although we also discuss mood cult to do so than to alter one’s happiness level
and life satisfaction at times according to the at a particular moment or on a particular day.
specific ideas and data being presented. Operationally, one might define a person’s
116 LYUBOMIRSKY, SHELDON, AND SCHKADE

chronic happiness level in terms of his or her proximate percentage of the variance that each
retrospective summary judgments regarding his of the three factors accounts for in cross-sec-
or her mood and satisfaction during some recent tional well-being, as suggested by past research.
period (such as the past 2, 6, or 12 months) or as As can be seen in the pie chart, existing evi-
the average of momentary judgments of mood dence suggests that genetics account for approx-
and satisfaction made at several times during imately 50% of the population variation (Braun-
the selected period. It is worth adding, however, gart et al., 1992; Lykken & Tellegen, 1996;
that people may vary in their “hedonic profiles,” Tellegen et al., 1988), and circumstances ac-
such that two individuals with similar chronic count for approximately 10% (Argyle, 1999;
happiness levels might differ in their relative Diener et al., 1999). This leaves as much as
levels of contentment with life versus their rel- 40% of the variance for intentional activity,
ative frequency of experiencing positive and supporting our proposal that volitional efforts
negative mood states. offer a promising possible route to longitudinal
increases in happiness. In other words, changing
Determinants of the Chronic Happiness one’s intentional activities may provide a hap-
Level piness-boosting potential that is at least as large
as, and probably much larger than, changing
We focus on three primary types of factors one’s circumstances. In the following, we pro-
that we believe causally affect the chronic hap- vide a definition of each factor, briefly consider
piness level, namely, the set point, life circum- whether and how changing that factor can lead
stances, and intentional activity. We focus on to changes in people’s chronic well-being, and
these three factors because they have histori- discuss whether such changes may be sustain-
cally received the majority of attention in the able over the long term, that is, whether the
well-being literature, providing a substantial re- forces of hedonic adaptation can be counter-
search base. We also focus on this three-factor acted by that factor.
distinction because it allows us to address sev-
eral important issues and paradoxes, such as the Happiness Set Point
question of whether it is even possible to “be-
come happier” given strong genetic influences We assume that an individual’s chronic hap-
on happiness, the question of why past well- piness level is in part determined by her or his
being research has revealed such weak associ- set point, which is defined as the central or
ations between demographic/circumstantial expected value within the person’s set range.
variables and happiness, and the question of The happiness set point is genetically deter-
how a person might appropriately take action to mined and is assumed to be fixed, stable over
“pursue” happiness. time, and immune to influence or control. Con-
Figure 1 provides an illustration of the ap- sistent with this assumption, twin studies
(Lykken & Tellegen, 1996; Tellegen et al.,
1988), long-term panel studies (Headey &
Wearing, 1989), and studies of the effects of life
events on well-being (Brickman et al., 1978) all
indicate substantial long-term stability in hap-
piness. For example, Lykken and Tellegen
(1996) assessed well-being in twins at 20 years
of age and then again at 30 years of age. The
test–retest correlation was a considerable .50.
Even more important, the cross-twin, cross-time
correlation for the happiness of monozygotic
twins was .40 (or 80% of the test–retest corre-
lation), suggesting that the heritability of the
“stable” component of happiness is approxi-
mately .80. In contrast, the cross-twin, cross-
Figure 1. Three primary factors influencing the chronic time correlation for dizygotic twins was close to
happiness level. zero (.07). Other studies, although differing in
SPECIAL ISSUE: ARCHITECTURE OF SUSTAINABLE HAPPINESS 117

their estimates of heritability, have consistently relatively stable facts of an individual’s life.
shown that monozygotic twins exhibit consid- Happiness-relevant circumstances may include
erably more similar patterns of happiness the national, geographical, and cultural region
change than do dizygotic twins, providing con- in which a person resides, as well as demo-
verging support that the variance in adult hap- graphic factors such as age, gender, and ethnic-
piness is in large part determined genetically. ity (see Diener et al., 1999, for a review). Cir-
The set point probably reflects relatively im- cumstantial factors also include the individual’s
mutable intrapersonal, temperamental, and af- personal history, that is, life events that can
fective personality traits, such as extraversion, affect his or her happiness, such as having ex-
arousability, and negative affectivity, that are perienced a childhood trauma, being involved in
rooted in neurobiology (e.g., Ashby, Isen, & an automobile accident, or winning a presti-
Turken, 1999; Davidson, 1999; Depue & Col- gious award. Finally, circumstantial factors in-
lins, 1999; Gray, 1990; Kagan, 2003; Robinson, clude life status variables such as marital status,
Emde, & Corley, 2001), are highly heritable occupational status, job security, income,
(Tellegen et al., 1988), and change little over health, and religious affiliation.
the life span (McCrae & Costa, 1990). For ex- Again, previous cross-sectional research has
ample, Kagan has followed children from 4 linked all of the circumstantial factors just de-
months to 11 years of age and shown that so- scribed to subjective well-being (Diener et al.,
ciability in 11-year olds can be traced to a 1999). For example, empirical evidence shows
particular type of infant temperament (called that people who are paid more are relatively
“low reactive”) that appears to involve a distinct happier (e.g., Diener, Sandvik, Seidlitz, & Die-
neurochemical profile. Other writers, including ner, 1993) and that middle-class individuals are
Gray and Depue, have also compiled persuasive somewhat happier than working-class individu-
evidence for the neurobiological underpinnings als (e.g., Warr & Payne, 1982). Married people
of personality. This rapidly growing body of are happier than those who are single, divorced,
research supports the set point theory of person- or widowed (e.g., Mastekaasa, 1994), even in
ality and affect. cultures as diverse as those of Belarus and Spain
(Diener, Gohm, Suh, & Oishi, 2000). Findings
Implications of the Set Point for also reveal that religiously committed people
Sustainable Increases in Chronic are relatively more likely to rate themselves as
Happiness “very happy” (Gallup, 1984) and that, not sur-
prisingly, healthy people, especially older ones,
The preceding analysis implies that one’s declare themselves to be slightly happier than
chronic happiness during a particular life period sick people (e.g., Okun et al., 1984).
can be increased, but not by changing one’s set However, as suggested earlier, all circum-
point, because by definition it is constant. In stances combined account for only 8% to 15%
other words, although it is possible that future of the variance in happiness levels (Argyle,
scientists will learn how to alter people’s basic 1999; Diener et al., 1999). These relatively
temperaments and dispositions, at present it ap- weak associations have been deemed surprising
pears that focusing on the set point is not a and paradoxical, given well-being researchers’
fruitful avenue for happiness increase. Again, initial expectations that circumstantial factors
however, one can posit that nongenetic factors such as income and physical health would be
also influence a person’s chronic happiness strongly related to happiness (Diener et al.,
level, helping to determine whether the person 1999). We believe that these counterintuitively
falls in the lower or upper portion of his or her small effects can be largely accounted for by
potential range at a particular time. The remain- hedonic adaptation and the fact that people
ing variables in the model are designed to rep- adapt rapidly to new circumstances and life
resent these other factors. events. This appears to be the case because
adaptation—whether it is sensory (e.g., to a foul
Circumstances odor or a heavy weight; Brown, 1953), physio-
logical (e.g., to very hot or cold temperatures;
This category consists of happiness-relevant Dar, Ariely, & Frank, 1995), or hedonic (e.g., to
circumstantial factors, that is, the incidental but a salary raise; Brickman et al., 1978; Parducci,
118 LYUBOMIRSKY, SHELDON, AND SCHKADE

1995)— occurs in response to stimuli that are ducing sustainable changes in chronic happi-
constant or repeated. By definition, constancy is ness. Although this strategy may work in the
a feature of most circumstantial changes. short term, it probably will not work in the long
term. Of course, if people have not achieved
Implications of Circumstances for basic subsistence and security, then it is logical
Sustainable Increases in Chronic for them to attend to these circumstances and
Happiness basic needs first, before focusing on maximiz-
ing their happiness. However, we assume that,
Of the different types of circumstances, life at best, satisfying basic needs can move people
status variables in particular seem to offer some only up to their set point, not beyond.
potential for increasing chronic happiness, in
that individuals often have considerable control Intentional Activity
over them. For example, a college football
player may sign a lucrative NFL contract, a Now we turn to the third and arguably most
middle-aged divorcee may remarry, or a retired promising means of altering one’s happiness
couple may move to Florida to a condominium level: intentional activity. This is a very broad
with a view, all becoming happier as a result. category that includes the wide variety of things
Will such new happiness last, however? Per- that people do and think in their daily lives.
haps not, because, as mentioned earlier, hedonic Obviously, humans are very active creatures,
adaptation tends to shuttle people back to their with innumerable behaviors, projects, and con-
starting point following any positive circum- cerns to which they devote energy. By “inten-
stantial change. For example, Headey and tional,” we mean discrete actions or practices in
Wearing (1989) found in their four-wave panel which people can choose to engage (although
study that positive and negative events (e.g., the choice to initiate the activity may have be-
“made lots of new friends,” “got married,” “ex- come habitual, as discussed in a later section).
perienced serious problems with children,” or We also assume that intentional activities re-
“became unemployed”) influenced life satisfac- quire some degree of effort to enact. That is, the
tion, positive affect, and negative affect as person has to try to do the activity; it does not
would be expected but that people kept return- happen by itself. Indeed, this point touches on
ing to their original baselines. And Schkade and one of the critical distinctions between the cat-
Kahneman (1998) revealed that although “liv- egory of activity and the category of life cir-
ing in California” is a seductive notion for cumstances; that is, circumstances happen to
many, it does not actually make people any people, and activities are ways that people act
happier in the long run. Furthermore, Lucas, on their circumstances.
Clark, Georgellis, and Diener (2003) showed There is good reason to believe that inten-
that, for most people, the life satisfaction ben- tional activity can influence well-being. For ex-
efits derived from getting married tended to fade ample, some types of behavioral activity, such
over the years. Thus, although one may gain a as exercising regularly or trying to be kind to
temporary “boost” by moving to a new region, others, are associated with well-being (e.g.,
increasing one’s income level, or changing Keltner & Bonanno, 1997; Magen & Aharoni,
one’s appearance, such boosts will probably not 1991), as are some types of cognitive activity,
last, because people tend to adapt to constant such as reframing situations in a more positive
circumstances. Other reasons why circumstan- light or pausing to count one’s blessings (Em-
tial changes may prove ineffectual for perma- mons & McCullough, 2003; King, 2001; Selig-
nently increasing happiness include the fact that man, 1991), and some kinds of volitional activ-
circumstantial changes can be costly (e.g., in ity, such as striving for important personal goals
terms of money, resources, and time) and, in (Sheldon & Houser-Marko, 2001) or devoting
many cases, impractical or even impossible. effort to meaningful causes (M. Snyder &
Also, once a realistic “ceiling” of positive cir- Omoto, 2001). Notably, it is impossible to fully
cumstances is reached, it may be difficult to separate behavioral, cognitive, and volitional
improve matters further. activity; still, we believe the distinction is use-
In short, the data suggest that changes in ful, and we continue to use it throughout the
circumstances have limited potential for pro- article.
SPECIAL ISSUE: ARCHITECTURE OF SUSTAINABLE HAPPINESS 119

Implications of Intentional Activity for highlighted one potential mediator from suc-
Sustainable Increases in Chronic cessful volitional activity to enhanced well-be-
Happiness ing, namely, accumulations of positive daily
experiences along the way. The question of
Again, it appears that increasing one’s set what other proximal factors may mediate
point and changing one’s life circumstances are changes in chronic happiness is addressed in
not fruitful avenues for sustainable increases in more detail in a later section.
chronic happiness. What, if anything, can pro- Notably, these studies do not extend beyond a
vide such an avenue? In the following, we argue single span of time. Thus, they do not directly
that intentional behavioral, cognitive, or voli- address the crucial question raised by the cur-
tional activity offers the best potential route. rent article: whether gains in well-being last.
Some work has already investigated the impact Although Headey and Wearing’s important
of adopting new behaviors on longitudinal well- (1989) work suggests that gains in happiness do
being, showing, for example, that faithfully en- not last, notably, their study focused only on life
gaging in a new exercise program positively events (“circumstances,” in our model) and did
boosts people’s mood and vitality and can even not take intentional activity into direct account.
maintain the boosts for as long as 6 months Recently, Sheldon and Houser-Marko (2001)
(e.g., Ransford & Palisi, 1996; Stewart et al., addressed the question of sustainability by ex-
1997). Although little work has directly inves- amining the effects of goal attainment on emo-
tigated the longitudinal effects of changing tional well-being over two consecutive semes-
one’s cognitive attitudes and practices on en- ters. Consistent with earlier studies, they found
hanced well-being, the general success of cog- that students who attained their personal goals
during the first semester of their freshman year
nitive– behavioral therapy in reducing suffering
experienced enhanced adjustment and emo-
(Gloaguen et al., 1998) and recent work indi-
tional well-being at the end of that semester.
cating positive effects of prompting people to
More important, they found that students could
practice positive psychological “virtues” such
maintain their enhanced level of well-being, but
as gratitude (Emmons & McCullough, 2003),
only if they continued to do well at their goals
hope (C. R. Snyder, Ilardi, Michael, & Cheav- during the second semester. In contrast, stu-
ens, 2000), and forgiveness (McCullough et al., dents who did well in the first semester but not
2000) suggest that cognitive activity offers in the second semester tended to regress back to
many excellent possibilities for happiness inter- their original well-being levels. This study of-
ventions (Fordyce, 1983). fers direct support for our assumption that hap-
Turning to the third type of intentional activ- piness can be enhanced and then maintained at
ity, recent longitudinal studies have focused the new level, especially when volitional activ-
specifically on volitional activity as a producer ity is effectively pursued over long periods of
of enhanced well-being (see Sheldon, 2002, for time. Further supporting this conclusion, Shel-
a review). In such studies, students are typically don and Lyubomirsky (2004) recently resur-
asked to pursue self-generated personal goals veyed these participants 3 years after the origi-
over the course of a semester. High levels of nal study and found that initially high-perform-
goal progress or attainment consistently predict ing students had maintained their earlier gains
increased well-being (i.e., higher positive affect in emotional well-being throughout their col-
and life satisfaction and lower negative mood) lege career.
from the beginning to the end of the semester, But what about adaptation? Is it not the case
whereas low levels of progress predict reduced that even the most successful striver adapts to
well-being (Brunstein, 1993; Sheldon, 2002). his or her happy situation eventually? More
Specifically, Sheldon’s longitudinal research in generally, is it not the case that people ulti-
this area (Sheldon & Elliot, 1998, 1999; Shel- mately adapt to the positive effects of any ac-
don & Kasser, 1995, 1998) has shown that tivity in which they engage, whether it be be-
well-being increases are most likely when a havioral, cognitive, or volitional, so that the
person chooses and attains self-concordant activity loses its potency over time?
goals, that is, goals that “fit” the person (as Although hedonic adaptation undoubtedly
described subsequently). This work has also constrains the happiness-inducing effects of in-
120 LYUBOMIRSKY, SHELDON, AND SCHKADE

tentional activities, just as it does for circum- between circumstantial changes and activity
stances, this adaptation effect appears to be changes. Although the boundaries between
weaker in the case of activity, as shown by these categories can be fuzzy, apparently they
recent data. For example, Sheldon and Ly- are clear enough to produce the predicted
ubomirsky (2004) recently conducted several effects.
short-term longitudinal studies in which partic-
ipants’ well-being (positive affect, negative af- Specific Advantages of Intentional Activity
fect, and life satisfaction) was measured at
Time 1, and positive circumstantial and activ- What are the sources of the sustainable hap-
ity-based life changes were measured at Time 2. piness gains afforded by intentional activity?
Well-being was then measured twice more, at We posit that activity-based change, unlike cir-
Times 3 and 4. These investigators found con- cumstance-based change, has several desirable
sistent support for a path model, displayed in features that may help to combat adaptation.
Figure 2, in which both positive circumstantial Intentional activity is episodic. One feature
change and positive activity change predicted of activities is that they are, by definition, epi-
enhanced life satisfaction and positive affect at sodic and transient; after all, people cannot
Time 3, but only positive activity change pre- spend all of their time doing one thing. This in
dicted maintained happiness gains at Time 4, itself suggests that individuals may adapt less
with positive circumstantial change dropping readily to new activities than to new circum-
out of the model. In other words, consistent with stances. The episodic nature of activity also
the present model, only activity-based well-be- suggests that an additional way to maximize the
ing change lasted; circumstance-based happi- impact of an activity is to attend to the timing of
ness change did not. that activity. For example, a person might
In a separate study, Sheldon and Lyubomir- choose to “count her blessings” only after brav-
sky (2004) randomly assigned participants to ing a difficult period, or only when she is espe-
report on either activity-based positive changes cially needful of a boost. Suppose instead that
or circumstantially based positive changes in she counts the same blessings every day, in a
their lives. Relative to those in the circumstan- nonvarying routine. This person may become
tial-change group, those in the activity-change bored with the routine and cease to extract
group reported a weaker sense of “having gotten meaning from it. The length of time before one
used to the change, such that it does not give the reengages in a happiness-boosting activity is an
same boost as before,” and more strongly en- important part of its potency in the next appli-
dorsed the statement “the change is something cation. By being mindful of the “refractory pe-
that varies over time, that is, something that riod” (Kalat, 2001) after which a recently per-
adds variety to my life.” These findings further formed activity regains its full happiness-induc-
support the claim that activity changes are char- ing potential, individuals may maximize the
acterized by less hedonic adaptation than cir- benefits of the activity over time and avoid
cumstantial changes. Parenthetically, Sheldon reducing or eliminating the activity’s effective-
and Lyubomirsky’s (2004) findings, taken as a ness through overuse. Thus, people should
whole, support the validity of our distinction strive to discover the optimal timing for each
activity, that is, a frequency of engagement that
allows that activity to remain fresh, meaningful,
and positive for a particular person.
Intentional activity can be varied. Another
important parameter of behavioral, cognitive,
and volitional activities is that people can con-
tinually vary them, both in their foci and in the
ways they engage in them. This may help to
reduce adaptation to the activity, allowing it to
retain its potency (McAlister, 1982). Indeed, by
Figure 2. Longitudinal path model predicting maintained definition, adaptation does not occur to stimuli
changes in well-being from positive circumstantial changes that are variable or changeable but only to those
and positive activity changes. Asterisks indicate p ⬍ .01. that are constant or repeated (cf. Frederick &
SPECIAL ISSUE: ARCHITECTURE OF SUSTAINABLE HAPPINESS 121

Loewenstein, 1999). For example, a scientist ities (an attitudinal activity), and he can try to
may regularly ask new questions and become remember to bring her flowers (a behavioral
involved in new projects. In the process, she activity). A person who performs these activi-
often feels the joy of making fascinating new ties would probably best counteract adaptation
discoveries and thus may remain particularly to this particular circumstance and derive the
happy (i.e., at the upper end of her potential most benefit from it. In contrast, consider a
range) over a long period of time. If the person husband who is not intentionally engaged in his
counting her blessings varies the domains of life marriage; for him, this demographic circum-
in which she counts them (i.e., in relationships, stance would essentially become a background
in work, in her health, or in her most recently factor, to which adaptation is very likely.
successful domain), then the strategy may re- For all of these reasons, intentional activity
main “fresh” and meaningful and work indefi- appears to offer the best prospects for increasing
nitely. Supporting this notion, past research and sustaining happiness. Of course, following
suggests that people tend to seek variety in their through on new intentions, such as the ubiqui-
behavior (McAlister, 1982; Ratner, Kahn, & tous “New Year’s resolutions,” is not necessar-
Kahneman, 1999), perhaps because change—in ily easy (Sheldon & Elliot, 1998). Indeed, we
both thoughts and actions—is innately pleasur- assume that happiness-increasing strategies can
able and stimulating (Berlyne, 1970; Rolls et be initiated and effectively pursued only with
al., 1981). concerted, consistent commitment and effort.
Intentional activity can directly counteract Still, activity-based factors are, by definition,
adaptation. Yet another advantage of inten- under greater potential control by the individual
tional activity is that it can directly tackle the than are genetic, demographic, and most life
problem presented by adaptation. For example, status factors. In other words, if anything can do
the cognitive practice of pausing to savor the it, intentional activity can.
good things in one’s life can directly counteract
the effects of hedonic adaptation to one’s con-
stant circumstances by drawing attention to the Implementing Happiness-Increasing
features that produced the initial happiness Strategies
boost and helping to keep them from being
In this section, we briefly consider several
taken for granted. As another example, prac-
important issues pertaining to how intentional
ticed meditators frequently report renewed ap-
activity might be used for increasing happi-
preciation of the ordinary as a result of their
ness. In other words, having established that
intentional reencounters with the world.
activity can potentially sustainably elevate
The fact that intentional activity can directly
happiness, how might one put this potential to
counteract adaptation and the hedonic treadmill
work? We discuss these strategic issues in
helps shed further light on the distinction be-
roughly chronological order, proceeding from
tween life circumstances and intentional activi-
the question of how to choose a particular
ties. Obviously, many personal characteristics
happiness-boosting activity to the question of
are both. For example, “being married” and
how such activity may be initiated and the
“being a student” both denote demographic sta-
question of how the activity can be main-
tus, yet they also reflect particular sorts of ac-
tained over time to produce a sustained in-
tivities. From our perspective, the crucial dis-
crease in the chronic level of happiness. In the
tinction with respect to well-being is whether
process, we discuss the issue of person–strat-
one exerts intentional effort with respect to the
egy fit, the meaning and nature of effort, the
circumstantial category, that is, whether one
definition and role of habits, and the impact of
acts upon the circumstance (e.g., using inten-
short-term versus long-term considerations.
tional practices to keep the circumstance
“fresh”). For example, an individual can engage
in a number of intentional activities with respect Choosing an Activity: The Role of
to the circumstantial category “marriage”: A Person–Activity Fit
husband can have the goal of making his mar-
riage work (a volitional activity), he can make Any one particular activity will not help ev-
the effort to appreciate his wife’s positive qual- ery person become happier. People have endur-
122 LYUBOMIRSKY, SHELDON, AND SCHKADE

ing strengths, interests, values, and inclinations activity. This distinction is necessary because it
that undoubtedly predispose them to benefit is clear that many activities have definite posi-
more from some strategies than others. For ex- tive effects if the person can only get started
ample, extraverts may benefit most from activ- doing them. For example, exercising in the
ities that bring them into regular contact with morning, making time to work on at least one
other people, and people high in nurturance important project during the day, or pausing to
motivation may benefit most from activities that count one’s blessings at the end of the day can
afford them opportunities to take care of others. have significant benefits, but only if the person
This general “matching” hypothesis (Harack- can “get over the hurdle” of remembering to do
iewicz & Sansone, 1991) is supported by much them and overcoming any obstacles to initiating
recent work showing that the positive effects of them. Obviously, those who do not implement
goal attainment on well-being are moderated by their activity intentions stand a worse chance of
goal–person fit (Brunstein et al., 1998; Diener & benefiting from them than those who do! We
Fujita, 1995; Sheldon & Elliot, 1999; Sheldon assume that this kind of self-regulatory effort
& Kasser, 1998). It is also supported by past requires considerable self-discipline and will-
well-being intervention research. For example, power. Furthermore, such effort may constitute
in several studies that instructed participants to a limited resource, one that must be marshaled
apply 14 different techniques to increase their carefully; in Muraven and Baumeister’s (2000)
personal happiness, the particular techniques terms, self-regulatory will is like a “muscle”
considered most effective for raising happiness that has limited capacity in a given unit of time
varied greatly from one individual to another and must be used strategically to avoid fatigue.
and appeared to be determined by each partici- If this analogy is accurate, then it seems
pant’s needs and areas of specific weakness logical that some people develop the muscle to
(Fordyce, 1977, 1983). a greater extent than others, thus attaining a
The fit of an activity with a person might be greater ability to “get started” on their intentions
conceptualized in a variety of ways, for exam- and gaining greater happiness potential. Of
ple, with respect to individuals’ motive dispo- course, some activities will appear intrinsically
sitions, basic needs, core values, signature more appealing and will be easier to jumpstart;
strengths, personal resources, hedonic profiles, this is undoubtedly one advantage of selecting
or other individual-difference characteristics. an activity that fits one’s personality. For exam-
There are also a variety of ways that fit might be ple, rather than running on a track, a fitness-
operationalized, such as in terms of self-re- seeking wilderness lover might instead choose
ported fit, in terms of consistency between im- to run on a trail through the woods, thereby
plicit and explicit measures of activity-relevant feeling much less initial resistance to beginning
motives, or in terms of informant-rated person– the activity. As another example, rather than
activity fit. Another approach is to assume that learning classical pieces, a jazz-loving piano
certain kinds of experiences are likely to be student might instead choose to work on jazz
beneficial to anyone, because these experiences standards, enhancing the intrinsic appeal of sit-
reflect universal psychological needs. From this ting down to practice.
point of view, any activity that provides certain Maintaining an activity. This brings us to
experiences, such as those involving belonging- the second type of effort. Obviously, if a par-
ness (Baumeister & Leary, 1995), self-efficacy ticular activity is to yield sustained happiness
(Bandura, 1997), or autonomy (Deci & Ryan, change, the person must keep performing the
2000), might be assumed to “fit” the person, a activity over the long term. For many effective
priori. happiness-enhancing activities, this will not be
difficult, because the task will probably be in-
Role of Effort herently interesting or rewarding and thus will
be “autotelic” in nature (Deci & Ryan, 2000),
Initiating an activity. We assume that en- that is, self-reinforcing and self-sustaining. This
gaging in an activity requires at least two dif- is especially true to the extent that the person
ferent kinds of effort: first, the effort required to continually varies what he or she does. If, for
initiate the activity and, second, the effort re- example, a person shifts attention among sev-
quired to actually carry out and maintain the eral projects at work, explores new trails in the
SPECIAL ISSUE: ARCHITECTURE OF SUSTAINABLE HAPPINESS 123

state park, or seeks out interesting new piano turned a formerly conscious activity into an
pieces, his or her activities should remain intrin- unconscious routine, practiced automatically
sically enjoyable and conducive to many reward- and without variation? If so, is it not the case
ing “flow” experiences (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990). that one is especially likely to experience hedo-
What if the activity is not enjoyable and thus nic adaptation to that activity, such that it loses
difficult to maintain? In this case, stopping the its happiness-boosting potential?
activity may not be problematic, because it We think not. However, to illustrate, we must
probably is not working anyway. By emphasiz- first distinguish between the habit of regularly
ing the importance of enjoying one’s intentional initiating a potentially beneficial activity and
activity, however, we do not mean to imply that the habit of implementing it the same way every
people should seek out only “fun” activities. time (the two types of effort mentioned earlier).
Sometimes choosing to endure boring or even We assume that hedonic adaptation occurs only
aversive experiences in the short term can have with respect to particular experiences, and not
considerable positive effects on chronic happi- with respect to the decisions that give rise to
ness in the long term; for example, studying for those experiences. Thus, making a habit out of
an important exam in a tedious but required deciding to initiate an activity is not problem-
class may well represent an excellent invest- atic but may instead help people to keep getting
ment in one’s future chronic happiness, even “over the hump.” For example, a woman might
though it may detract from one’s momentary make running an automatic part of her daily
happiness. As another example, a naval officer routine, such that she does not even have to
candidate is paying a short-term cost (boot make the decision of whether or not to run each
camp) to receive a longer term benefit (a career day, thus deriving considerable benefit. What is
as an officer). potentially problematic is when people make a
Of note, self-determination theory (Deci & habit out of how they implement the activity.
Ryan, 2000; Sheldon, Joiner, & Williams, When this happens, the flow of experiences
2003) posits that the crucial factor in such cases produced by such a habit is likely to remain
is whether the person has internalized the non- relatively constant, and thus, adaptation is likely
enjoyable activity, that is, whether he or she is to have the most pernicious effects. To over-
able to find meaning and value expression in it, come this, as suggested earlier, people should
even if it is not pleasant to perform. From this mindfully attend to optimal timing and variety
perspective, the naval officer candidate would in the ways they practice an activity. For exam-
pay a smaller short-term cost if he could un- ple, the woman might want to vary the route,
dergo boot camp thinking that “this is important time of day, and speed of her running. This will
and valuable” rather than thinking that “this is help forestall the effects of adaptation.
unnecessary and stupid.” The question of when
and how to sacrifice short-term happiness in Extensions and Further Questions
exchange for longer term happiness is an im-
portant one, as is the question of how to pro- Now that we have presented our basic con-
mote internalization of important happiness-rel- ceptual model of sustainable changes in happi-
evant activities that are not intrinsically enjoy- ness, we briefly consider a variety of additional
able. These questions represent promising issues that extend beyond this basic model.
directions for future research. What are the key ingredients of particular ac-
tivities that lead a person to a higher level of
Role of Habitual Activity well-being? Although this question is somewhat
peripheral to our model, it merits brief discus-
If activities such as “looking on the bright sion. We assume that happiness increases come
side,” “making time for the things that matter,” from at least two sources that are described,
and “working on an important life goal” make a respectively, by bottom-up and top-down theo-
difference for happiness, then it seems it would ries of well-being (Diener, 1994). Bottom-up
be a good idea to make a habit of doing them. theories postulate that people make global well-
However, on the surface, habits appear to being judgments in part with reference to emo-
present a conundrum for our model. Is it not the tions associated with their recent experiences
case that acquiring a habit means that one has (Kahneman, 1999). If they can recall a large
124 LYUBOMIRSKY, SHELDON, AND SCHKADE

number of recent affectively positive experi- of their coursework. All of the strategies fit into
ences, then they report being very happy (see one or more of the three categories of activity
Sheldon & Elliot, 1999, for supporting data). outlined earlier: behavioral (e.g., “spend more
Studies have produced support for this bot- time socializing”), cognitive (e.g., “become
tom-up perspective by showing that accumula- present oriented”), and volitional (e.g., “get bet-
tions of need-satisfying daily experiences over ter organized and plan things out”). Consistent
time (such as competence, relatedness, and au- with our conceptual model, Fordyce found that
tonomy; Deci & Ryan, 2000) lead to enhanced the strategies worked; that is, a significant main
global well-being at the end of that time (Reis, effect of participation was found for the exper-
Sheldon, Ryan, Gable, & Roscoe, 2000; Shel- imental conditions. Again, intentional activity
don, Ryan, & Reis, 1996). Furthermore, Shel- can successfully increase happiness. Also con-
don and Lyubomirsky (2004) found, in their sistent with our model, he found that some
comparison of the sustained effects of circum- strategies worked better than others and, in ad-
stantial changes and activity changes on dition, that person–strategy fit had a moderating
changes in well-being among students, that the effect on strategy effectiveness.
more enduring activity-based effects on happi- More recently, Sheldon and colleagues
ness were mediated by the greater feelings of (2002) conducted an intervention study based
competence and relatedness associated with ac- on participants’ personal goals. Early in the
tivity changes during the semester. semester, they taught experimental participants
But what about when people say they are a set of four strategies for enhancing their ex-
happy despite having had recent negative emo- perience and attainment of their personal goals:
tional experiences? Although bottom-up theo- “own the goal,” “make it fun,” “keep a bal-
ries cannot account for this, top-down theories ance,” and “remember the big picture.” Consis-
can. According to such models, well-being tent with the studies cited earlier, goal attain-
judgments are in part determined by global at- ment predicted increases in well-being at the
titudinal or meaning-based factors. Thus, a per- end of the semester. Interestingly, there was no
son who “suffers for a cause” might still feel main effect of experimental condition on in-
very happy because her suffering demonstrates creased well-being or goal attainment. Instead, a
her commitment to, and also perhaps moves her significant interaction was observed, such that
closer to obtaining, an important life goal. As only participants whose goals “fit” their inter-
another example, a man who has a bad day at ests and values benefited from the intervention.
work might still report being very happy that In other words, those with self-concordant goals
night, because of a short but meaningful visit who received the intervention evidenced the
from his grandchildren that evening that helped greatest goal attainment and, thus, the greatest
him to reframe the day. Again, we believe that increase in well-being. In addition to demon-
intentional activity can lead to new well-being strating that happiness-boosting interventions
by both top-down and bottom-up routes, that is, can work for at least some people, this finding
both via accumulations of small positive expe- provides further support for our proposition that
riences and via a sense of global meaning and the fit of the activity to the person makes a
purpose. difference.
Obviously, much future work remains to be
Preliminary Data: Happiness Interventions done regarding happiness-increasing interven-
tions, particularly research that explicitly ma-
The model of sustainable happiness that we nipulates the various factors in our model. As a
have proposed has clear implications for how to preliminary test, Lyubomirsky, Tkach, and
design interventions for increasing happiness. Sheldon (2004) recently conducted two 6-week
Before describing these, we first discuss some happiness-enhancing interventions based in be-
of the few happiness intervention studies that havioral and cognitive–attitudinal change.
have been conducted, to show their general con- Drawing on promising interventions grounded
sistency with our approach. As noted earlier, in the positive psychology tradition—that is,
Fordyce (1977, 1983) conducted several happi- focused on building positive affect and personal
ness intervention studies in which he taught 14 strengths rather than on reducing or coping with
happiness-relevant strategies to students as part negative affect, pathology, or weakness—they
SPECIAL ISSUE: ARCHITECTURE OF SUSTAINABLE HAPPINESS 125

used two diverse strategies (one cognitive and


one social behavioral) to serve as initial exis-
tence proofs of our conceptual model. To this
end, experimental participants were prompted
to perform kind acts or to pause and “count their
blessings.”
The strategy of committing acts of kindness
was expected, on the basis of previous theory
and research, to boost temporary moods and
long-lasting well-being. For example, individu-
als who report a greater interest in helping oth-
ers, an inclination to act in a prosocial manner,
or intentions to perform altruistic or courteous
behaviors are more likely to rate themselves as
dispositionally happy (see Lyubomirsky, King,
& Diener, 2004, for a review). We assume that
acts of kindness and generosity can boost hap-
piness in a variety of ways. Such acts may foster
a charitable perception of others and one’s com-
munity, an increased sense of cooperation and
interdependence, and an awareness of one’s
good fortune. In addition, people who commit
acts of kindness may begin to view themselves Figure 3. Changes in well-being over the course of two
as altruistic people, as well as to feel more 6-week interventions: performing acts of kindness (top) and
counting one’s blessings (bottom).
confident, efficacious, in control, and optimistic
about their ability to help. Furthermore, acts of
generosity can inspire greater liking by others,
along with appreciation, gratitude, and proso- ticipants experienced a reduction in happiness
cial reciprocity (Trivers, 1971), all of which are over the course of the 6-week period, partici-
valuable in times of stress and need. Finally, pants who committed acts of kindness experi-
kind behaviors may help satisfy a basic human enced a significant increase in well-being, but
need for relatedness (Baumeister & Leary, this increase was evident only among those who
1995), thereby contributing to increased showed their weekly generosity all in a single
happiness. day. Because many of the kind acts that students
Thus, in their first intervention, Lyubomir- performed were small ones, spreading them
sky, Tkach, and Sheldon (2004) asked students over the course of a week might have dimin-
to perform five acts of kindness per week over ished their salience and power or made them
the course of 6 weeks, either all five acts in 1 less distinguishable from participants’ habitual
day or five acts spread over the week. Such acts kind behavior.
were described as behaviors that benefit other The second intervention tested a cognitive
people or make others happy, usually at some happiness-increasing activity. Recently, Em-
cost to oneself (e.g., donating blood, helping a mons and McCullough (2003) found that prac-
friend with a paper, visiting an elderly relative, ticing grateful thinking on a regular basis can
or writing a thank-you note to a former profes- enhance concurrent well-being. Gratitude pro-
sor). A no-treatment control group simply com- motes the savoring of positive life experiences
pleted measures of well-being immediately be- and situations so that maximum satisfaction and
fore the intervention and immediately after. The enjoyment are distilled from one’s circum-
results, displayed in the top panel of Figure 3, stances. As noted earlier, this practice may di-
provided preliminary evidence that a short-term rectly counteract the effects of hedonic adapta-
happiness-enhancing activity can increase well- tion by helping people extract as much appre-
being. Furthermore, supporting our model’s ciation from the good things in their lives as
predictions, Lyubomirsky et al. found that op- possible. In addition, the ability to appreciate
timal timing was critical. Whereas control par- their life circumstances may also be an adaptive
126 LYUBOMIRSKY, SHELDON, AND SCHKADE

coping strategy by which people positively re- to such stable factors. Again, however, one can
interpret stressful or negative life experiences, deter, or at least delay, such adaptation to pos-
bolster coping resources, and strengthen social itive circumstantial changes by engaging in in-
relationships. Finally, the practice of gratitude tentional effort and activity with respect to
appears to be incompatible with negative emo- them. That is, if one can remember to appreciate
tions and thus may reduce feelings of envy, or actively engage with the object or circum-
anger, or greed. stance (i.e., pause to savor the new Mercedes or
Thus, in the second 6-week intervention, stu- take advantage of the California weather), then
dents were instructed to contemplate “the things stable objects and circumstances may not be
for which they are grateful” either once a week stable after all, from a phenomenological per-
or three times a week. Examples of “blessings” spective. Thus, it remains the case that only life
listed by students included “a healthy body,” changes involving intentional activity can be
“my mom,” and “AOL instant messenger.” expected to lead to sustainable changes in
Control participants completed only the happi- well-being.
ness assessments. The results again suggested
that short-term increases in happiness are pos- Conclusion
sible and, furthermore, that optimal timing is
important. In summary, students who regularly If it is meaningful and important to pursue
expressed gratitude showed increases in well- happiness, then it is crucial to find out how this
being over the course of the study relative to can be accomplished. To what extent, and how,
controls, but these increases were observed only can people succeed in making themselves hap-
among students who performed the activity just pier? In this article, we have attempted to inte-
once a week (see Figure 3, bottom panel). Per- grate what is known about happiness change,
haps counting their blessings several times a especially longitudinal variations in well-being,
week led people to become bored with the prac- into a single summary model. The model en-
tice, finding it less fresh and meaningful over compasses a wide variety of findings and sug-
time. gests a number of new directions for research.
Although the results of these two interven- More than two centuries have passed since the
tions are encouraging, they notably did not test “pursuit of happiness” was proclaimed as a di-
the sustainability of the well-being increases for vinely ordained human right. We believe it is
the experimental groups (i.e., “kindness” and finally time for the issue of sustainable well-
“blessings”) and did not examine the impact of being to be given the scientific attention that it
key moderators of activity effects. In the future, deserves.
in addition to assessing the efficacy of different
activities for producing sustainable increases in
well-being, we will investigate the effects of References
such potential moderators as fit, effectiveness,
Argyle, M. (1999). Causes and correlates of happi-
timing, variety, cultural membership, social ness. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz
support, and the habits associated with the (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic
activity. psychology (pp. 353–373). New York: Russell
What are the most general recommendations Sage Foundation.
for increasing happiness suggested by our Aristotle. (1974). The Nichomachean ethics (J. A. K.
model? Simply, happiness seekers might be ad- Thomson, Trans.). New York: Penguin.
vised to find new activities to become engaged Ashby, F. G., Isen, A. M., & Turken, A. U. (1999). A
in, preferably activities that fit their values and neuropsychological theory of positive affect and
interests. They should make a habit out of ini- its influence on cognition. Psychological Review,
tiating the activity while at the same time vary- 106, 529 –550.
Aspinwall, L. G. (1998). Rethinking the role of pos-
ing their focus and timing in terms of the way itive affect in self-regulation. Motivation and Emo-
they implement the activity. People might be tion, 22, 1–32.
advised to avoid basing their happiness on the Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of
acquisition of particular circumstances or ob- control. New York: Freeman.
jects (e.g., buying a luxury car or moving to Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need
California), because they will tend to habituate to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as
SPECIAL ISSUE: ARCHITECTURE OF SUSTAINABLE HAPPINESS 127

a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Danner, D. D., Snowdon, D. A., & Friesen, W. V.
Bulletin, 117, 497–529. (2001). Positive emotions in early life and longev-
Berlyne, D. (1970). Novelty, complexity, and hedo- ity: Findings from the nun study. Journal of Per-
nic value. Perception & Psychophysics, 8, 279 – sonality and Social Psychology, 80, 804 – 813.
286. Dar, R., Ariely, D., & Frank, H. (1995). The effect of
Braungart, J. M., Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & past injury on pain threshold and tolerance.
Fulker, D. W. (1992). Genetic influence on tester- Pain, 60, 189 –193.
rated infant temperament as assessed by Bayley’s Davidson, R. J. (1999). Neuropsychological perspec-
Infant Behavior Record: Nonadoptive and adop- tives on affective styles and their cognitive conse-
tive siblings and twins. Developmental Psychol- quences. In T. Dalgleish & M. J. Power (Eds.),
ogy, 28, 40 – 47. Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 103–
Brickman, P., & Campbell, D. T. (1971). Hedonic 123). Chichester, England: Wiley.
relativism and planning the good society. In M. H. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and
Appley (Ed.), Adaptation-level theory (pp. 287– “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the
302). New York: Academic Press. self-determination of behavior. Psychological In-
Brickman, P., Coates, D., & Janoff-Bulman, R. quiry, 4, 227–268.
(1978). Lottery winners and accident victims: Is DeNeve, K. M., & Cooper, H. (1998). The happy
happiness relative? Journal of Personality and So- personality: A meta-analysis of 137 personality
cial Psychology, 36, 917–927. traits and subjective well-being. Psychological
Brown, D. R. (1953). Stimulus similarity and the Bulletin, 124, 197–229.
anchoring of subjective scales. American Journal Depue, R. A., & Collins, P. F. (1999). Neurobiology
of Psychology, 66, 199 –214. of the structure of personality: Dopamine, facilita-
Brunstein, J. (1993). Personal goals and subjective tion of incentive motivation, and extraversion. Be-
well-being: A longitudinal study. Journal of Per- havioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 491–569.
sonality and Social Psychology, 65, 1061–1070. Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psycholog-
Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Grassman, R. ical Bulletin, 95, 542–575.
(1998). Personal goals and emotional well-being: Diener, E. (1994). Assessing subjective well-being:
The moderating role of motive dispositions. Jour- Progress and opportunities. Social Indicators Re-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, search, 31, 103–157.
494 –508. Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1995). Resources, personal
Carlson, R. (1997). You can be happy no matter strivings, and subjective well-being: A nomothetic
what: Five principles for keeping life in perspec- and idiographic approach. Journal of Personality
tive. Novato, CA: New World Library. and Social Psychology, 68, 926 –935.
Carstensen, L. L. (1995). Evidence for a life-span Diener, E., Gohm, C. L., Suh, E., & Oishi, S. (2000).
theory of socioemotional selectivity. Current Di- Similarity of the relations between marital status
rections in Psychological Science, 4, 151–156. and subjective well-being across cultures. Journal
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 31, 419 – 436.
functions of positive and negative affect: A con- Diener, E., & Lucas, R. E. (1999). Personality and
trol-process view. Psychological Review, 97, 19 – subjective well-being. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener,
35. & N. Schwartz (Eds.), Well-being: The founda-
Charles, S. T., Reynolds, C. A., & Gatz, M. (2001). tions of hedonic psychology (pp. 213–229). New
Age-related differences and change in positive and York: Russell Sage Foundation.
negative affect over 23 years. Journal of Person- Diener, E., Sandvik, E., Seidlitz, L., & Diener, M.
ality and Social Psychology, 80, 136 –151. (1993). The relationship between income and sub-
Costa, P. T., McCrae, R. R., & Zonderman, A. B. jective well-being: Relative or absolute? Social
(1987). Environmental and dispositional influ- Indicators Research, 28, 195–223.
ences on well-being: Longitudinal follow-up of an Diener, E., & Suh, E. M. (1998). Subjective well-
American national sample. British Journal of Psy- being and age: An international analysis. In K. W.
chology, 78, 299 –306. Schaie & M. P. Lawton (Eds.), Annual review of
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology gerontology and geriatrics: Focus on emotion and
of optimal experience. New York: Harper & Row. adult development (Vol. 17, pp. 304 –324). New
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Wong, M. M. (1991). The York: Springer.
situational and personal correlates of happiness: A Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L.
cross-national comparison. In F. Strack, M. Ar- (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of
gyle, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Subjective well-being: progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 276 –302.
An interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 193–212). Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Smith, H., & Shao, L. (1995).
Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press. National differences in reported well-being: Why
128 LYUBOMIRSKY, SHELDON, AND SCHKADE

do they occur? Social Indicators Research, 34, cognitive therapy in depressed patients. Journal of
7–32. Affective Disorders, 49, 59 –72.
Dillon, K. M., Minchoff, B., & Baker, K. H. (1985). Gray, J. A. (1990). Brain systems that mediate both
Positive emotional states and enhancement of the emotion and cognition. Cognition and Emotion, 4,
immune system. International Journal of Psychi- 269 –288.
atry in Medicine, 15, 13–18. Harackiewicz, J. M., & Sansone, C. (1991). Goals
Emmons, R. A., & King, L. A. (1988). Conflict and intrinsic motivation: You can get there from
among personal strivings: Immediate and long- here. In M. L. Maehr & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.),
term implications for psychological and physical Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 7,
well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- pp. 21– 49). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
chology, 54, 1040 –1048. Harker, L., & Keltner, D. (2001). Expressions of
Emmons, R. A., & McCullough, M. E. (2003). positive emotions in women’s college yearbook
Counting blessings versus burdens: An experimen- pictures and their relationship to personality and
tal investigation of gratitude and subjective well- life outcomes across adulthood. Journal of Person-
being in daily life. Journal of Personality and ality and Social Psychology, 80, 112–124.
Social Psychology, 84, 377–389. Headey, B., & Wearing, A. (1989). Personality, life
Estrada, C., Isen, A. M., & Young, M. J. (1994). events, and subjective well-being: Toward a dy-
Positive affect influences creative problem solving namic equilibrium model. Journal of Personality
and reported source of practice satisfaction in phy- and Social Psychology, 57, 731–739.
sicians. Motivation and Emotion, 18, 285–299. Isen, A. M. (1970). Success, failure, attention and
Fava, G. (1999). Well-being therapy: Conceptual and reaction to others: The warm glow of success.
technical issues. Psychotherapy and Psychosomat- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 15,
ics, 68, 171–179. 294 –301.
Fordyce, M. W. (1977). Development of a program Jacobson, N. S., Dobson, K. S., Truax, P. A., Addis,
to increase happiness. Journal of Counseling Psy- M. E., Koerner, K., Gollan, J. K., et al. (1996). A
chology, 24, 511–521. component analysis of cognitive-behavioral treat-
Fordyce, M. W. (1983). A program to increase hap- ment for depression. Journal of Consulting and
piness: Further studies. Journal of Counseling Psy- Clinical Psychology, 64, 295–304.
chology, 30, 483– 498. Jahoda, M. (1958). Current concepts of positive men-
Frederick, S., & Loewenstein, G. (1999). Hedonic tal health. New York: Basic Books.
adaptation. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Kagan, J. (2003). Biology, context and developmen-
Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of tal inquiry. Annual Review of Psychology, 54,
hedonic psychology (pp. 302–329). New York: 1–23.
Russell Sage Foundation. Kahneman, D. (1999). Objective happiness. In D.
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emo- Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-
tions in positive psychology: The broaden-and- being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp.
build theory of positive emotions. American Psy- 3–25). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
chologist, 56, 218 –226. Kalat, J. W. (2001). Biological psychology (7th ed.).
Fredrickson, B. L., & Joiner, T. (2002). Positive Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
emotions trigger upward spirals toward emotional Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining
well-being. Psychological Science, 13, 172–175. the American dream: Differential correlates of in-
Freedman, J. (1978). Happy people: What happiness trinsic and extrinsic goals. Personality and Social
is, who has it, and why. New York: Harcourt Brace Psychology Bulletin, 22, 280 –287.
Jovanovich. Keltner, D., & Bonanno, G. A. (1997). A study of
Frijda, N. H. (1999). Emotions and hedonic experi- laughter and dissociation: Distinct correlates of
ence. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz laughter and smiling during bereavement. Journal
(Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 687–
psychology (pp. 190 –210). New York: Russell 702.
Sage Foundation. King, L. A. (2001). The health benefits of writing
Gallup, G. G., Jr. (1984, March). Commentary on the about life goals. Personality and Social Psychol-
state of religion in the U. S. today. Religion in ogy Bulletin, 27, 798 – 807.
America: The Gallup Report, No. 222. Larsen, R. J., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1999). Measure-
Gaskins, R. W. (1999). “Adding legs to a snake”: A ment issues in emotion research. In D. Kahneman,
reanalysis of motivation and the pursuit of happi- E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The
ness from a Zen Buddhist perspective. Journal of foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 40 – 60).
Educational Psychology, 91, 204 –215. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Gloaguen, V., Cottraux, J., Cucherat, M., & Black- Lerner, J. S., Taylor, S. E., Gonzalez, R. M., & Stayn,
burn, I. (1998). A meta-analysis of the effects of H. B. (2002). Emotion, physiological reactivity,
SPECIAL ISSUE: ARCHITECTURE OF SUSTAINABLE HAPPINESS 129

and visceral self perception. Manuscript submitted Mastekaasa, A. (1994). Marital status, distress, and
for publication. well-being: An international comparison. Journal
Lichter, S., Haye, K., & Kammann, R. (1980). In- of Comparative Family Studies, 25, 183–205.
creasing happiness through cognitive retraining. McAlister, L. (1982). A dynamic attribute satiation
New Zealand Psychologist, 9, 57– 64. model of variety-seeking behavior. Journal of
Lucas, R. E., Clark, A. E., Georgellis, Y., & Diener, Consumer Research, 9, 141–150.
E. (2003). Reexamining adaptation and the set McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1986). Personality,
point model of happiness: Reactions to changes in coping, and coping effectiveness in an adult sam-
marital status. Journal of Personality and Social ple. Journal of Personality, 54, 385– 405.
Psychology, 84, 527–539. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1990). Personality in
Lykken, D. (2000). Happiness: The nature and nur- adulthood. New York: Guilford Press.
ture of joy and contentment. New York: St. Mar- McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1994). The stability of
tin’s Press. personality: Observations and evaluations. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 3, 173–175.
Lykken, D., & Tellegen, A. (1996). Happiness is a
McCullough, M. E., Pargament, K. I., & Thoresen,
stochastic phenomenon. Psychological Science, 7,
C. E. (Eds.). (2000). Forgiveness: Theory, re-
186 –189.
search, and practice. New York: Guilford Press.
Lyubomirsky, S. (2001). Why are some people hap- McGregor, I., & Little, B. R. (1998). Personal
pier than others?: The role of cognitive and moti- projects, happiness, and meaning: On doing well
vational processes in well-being. American Psy- and being yourself. Journal of Personality and
chologist, 56, 239 –249. Social Psychology, 74, 494 –512.
Lyubomirsky, S., King, L. A., & Diener, E. (2004). Is Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-
happiness a strength?: An examination of the ben- regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does
efits and costs of frequent positive affect. Manu- self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological
script submitted for publication. Bulletin, 126, 247–259.
Lyubomirsky, S., & Lepper, H. S. (1999). A measure Myers, D. G. (2000). The funds, friends, and faith of
of subjective happiness: Preliminary reliability and happy people. American Psychologist, 55, 56 – 67.
construct validation. Social Indicators Re- Myers, D. G., & Diener, E. (1995). Who is happy?
search, 46, 137–155. Psychological Science, 6, 10 –19.
Lyubomirsky, S., & Ross, L. (1997). Hedonic con- Norcross, J. C., Santrock, J. W., Campbell, L. F.,
sequences of social comparison: A contrast of Smith, T. P., Sommer, R., & Zuckerman, E. L.
happy and unhappy people. Journal of Personality (2000). Authoritative guide to self-help resources
and Social Psychology, 73, 1141–1157. in mental health. New York: Guilford Press.
Lyubomirsky, S., Sousa, L., & Dickerhoof, R. Okun, M. A., Stock, W. A., Haring, M. J., & Witter,
(2004). The medium is the message: The costs and R. A. (1984). The social activity/subjective well-
benefits of thinking, writing, and talking about being relation: A quantitative synthesis. Research
life’s triumphs and defeats. Manuscript submitted on Aging, 6, 45– 65.
for publication. Ostir, G. V., Markides, K. S., Black, S. A., & Good-
Lyubomirsky, S., Tkach, C., & Sheldon, K. M. win, J. S. (2000). Emotional well-being predicts
(2004). [Pursuing sustained happiness through ran- subsequent functional independence and survival.
dom acts of kindness and counting one’s blessings: Journal of the American Geriatric Society, 48,
473– 478.
Tests of two six-week interventions]. Unpublished
Parducci, A. (1995). Happiness, pleasure, and judg-
raw data.
ment: The contextual theory and its applications.
Magen, Z., & Aharoni, R. (1991). Adolescents’ con-
Hove, England: Erlbaum.
tributing toward others: Relationship to positive Ransford, H. E., & Palisi, B. J. (1996). Aerobic
experiences and transpersonal commitment. Jour- exercise, subjective health and psychological well-
nal of Humanistic Psychology, 31, 126 –143. being within age and gender subgroups. Social
Marks, G. N., & Fleming, N. (1999). Influences and Science and Medicine, 42, 1555–1559.
consequences of well-being among Australian Ratner, R. K., Kahn, B. E., & Kahneman, D. (1999).
young people: 1980 –1995. Social Indicators Re- Choosing less-preferred experiences for the sake
search, 46, 301–323. of variety. Journal of Consumer Research, 26,
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1994). The cultural 1–15.
shaping of emotion: A conceptual framework. In Reis, H. T., Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., Gable,
S. Kitayama & H. R. Markus (Eds.), Emotion and S. L., & Roscoe, J. (2000). Daily well-being: The
culture: Empirical studies of mutual influences role of autonomy, competence, and relatedness.
(pp. 339 –351). Washington, DC: American Psy- Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26,
chological Association. 419 – 443.
130 LYUBOMIRSKY, SHELDON, AND SCHKADE

Roberts, B. W., & Chapman, C. N. (2000). Change in search (pp. 65– 86). Rochester, NY: University of
dispositional well-being and its relation to role Rochester Press.
quality: A 30-year longitudinal study. Journal of Sheldon, K. M. (2004). The benefits of a “sidelong”
Research in Personality, 34, 26 – 41. approach to self-esteem need-satisfaction: Com-
Robinson, J. L., Emde, R. N., & Corley, R. P. (2001). ment on Crocker and Park (2004). Psychological
Dispositional cheerfulness: Early genetic and en- Bulletin, 130, 421– 424.
vironmental influences. In R. N. Emde & J. K. Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1998). Not all per-
Hewitt (Eds.), Infancy to early childhood: Genetic sonal goals are personal: Comparing autonomous
and environmental influences on developmental and controlled reasons for goals as predictors of
change (pp. 163–177). London: Oxford University effort and attainment. Personality and Social Psy-
Press. chology Bulletin, 24, 546 –557.
Rolls, B., Rowe, E., Rolls, E., Kingston, B., Megson, Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1999). Goal striving,
A., & Gunary, R. (1981). Variety in a meal en- need-satisfaction, and longitudinal well-being: The
hances food intake in man. Physiology and Behav- self-concordance model. Journal of Personality
ior, 26, 215–221. and Social Psychology, 76, 482– 497.
Sandvik, E., Diener, E., & Seidlitz, L. (1993). Sub- Sheldon, K. M., & Houser-Marko, L. (2001). Self-
jective well-being: The convergence and stability concordance, goal-attainment, and the pursuit of
of self-report and non-self-report measures. Jour- happiness: Can there be an upward spiral? Journal
nal of Personality, 61, 317–342. of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 152–
Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1993). On the power 165.
of positive thinking: The benefits of being optimis- Sheldon, K. M., Joiner, T., & Williams, G. (2003).
tic. Current Directions in Psychological Sci- Self-determination theory in the clinic: Motivating
ence, 2, 26 –30. physical and mental health. New Haven, CT: Yale
Schkade, D. A., & Kahneman, D. (1998). Does living University Press.
in California make people happy?: A focusing Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (1995). Coherence and
illusion in judgments of life satisfaction. Psycho- congruence: Two aspects of personality integra-
logical Science, 9, 340 –346. tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Schooler, J. W., Ariely, D., & Loewenstein, G. (in ogy, 68, 531–543.
press). The explicit pursuit and assessment of hap- Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (1998). Pursuing per-
piness can be self-defeating. In J. Carrillo & I. sonal goals: Skills enable progress but not all
Brocas (Eds.), Psychology and economics. Oxford, progress is beneficial. Personality and Social Psy-
England: Oxford University Press. chology Bulletin, 24, 1319 –1331.
Schwartz, B. (2000). Pitfalls on the road to a positive Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (2001). Getting older,
psychology of hope. In J. E. Gillham & J. Temple- getting better?: Personal strivings and psycholog-
ton (Eds.), The science of optimism and hope: ical maturity across the life span. Developmental
Research essays in honor of Martin E. P. Seligman Psychology, 37, 491–501.
(pp. 399 – 412). Philadelphia: Templeton Founda- Sheldon, K. M., Kasser, T., Smith, K., & Share, T.
tion Press. (2002). Personal goals and psychological growth:
Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. (1999). Reports of subjec- Testing an intervention to enhance goal-attainment
tive well-being: Judgmental processes and their and personality integration. Journal of Personal-
methodological implications. In D. Kahneman, E. ity, 70, 5–31.
Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The Sheldon, K. M., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2004). Achiev-
foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 61– 84). ing sustainable gains in happiness: Change your
New York: Russell Sage Foundation. actions, not your circumstances. Manuscript sub-
Seidlitz, L., Wyer, R. S., & Diener, E. (1997). Cog- mitted for publication.
nitive correlates of subjective well-being: The pro- Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., & Reis, H. T. (1996).
cessing of valenced life events by happy and un- What makes for a good day?: Competence and
happy persons. Journal of Research in Personal- autonomy in the day and in the person. Personality
ity, 31, 240 –256. and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 1270 –1279.
Seligman, M. E. P. (1991). Learned optimism. New Snyder, C. R., Ilardi, S., Michael, S. T., & Cheavens,
York: Alfred A. Knopf. J. (2000). Hope theory: Updating a common pro-
Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). cess for psychological change. In C. R. Snyder &
Positive psychology: An introduction. American R. E. Ingram (Eds.), Handbook of psychological
Psychologist, 55, 5–14. change: Psychotherapy processes and practices
Sheldon, K. M. (2002). The self-concordance model for the 21st century (pp. 128 –153). New York:
of healthy goal-striving: When personal goals cor- Wiley.
rectly represent the person. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Snyder, M., & Omoto, A. M. (2001). Basic research
Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination re- and practical problems: Volunteerism and the psy-
SPECIAL ISSUE: ARCHITECTURE OF SUSTAINABLE HAPPINESS 131

chology of individual and collective action. In W. Tellegen, A., Lykken, D. T., Bouchard, T. J., Wilcox,
Wosinska, R. B. Cialdini, D. W. Barrett, & J. K. J., Segal, N. L., & Rich, S. (1988). Personality
Reykowski (Eds.), The practice of social influence similarity in twins reared apart and together. Jour-
in multiple cultures (pp. 287–307). Mahwah, NJ: nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,
Erlbaum. 1031–1039.
Staw, B. M., Sutton, R. I., & Pelled, L. H. (1995). Triandis, H. C., Bontempo, R., Leung, K., & Hui,
Employee positive emotion and favorable out- C. H. (1990). A method for determining cultural,
comes at the workplace. Organization Science, 5, demographic, and personal constructs. Journal of
51–71. Cross-Cultural Psychology, 21, 302–318.
Stewart, A. L., Mills, K. M., Sepsis, P. G., King, Trivers, R. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altru-
A. C., McLellan, B. Y., Roitz, K., & Ritter, P. L. ism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35–57.
(1997). Evaluation of CHAMPS, a physical activ- Tversky, A., & Griffin, D. (1991). Endowment and
ity promotion program for older adults. Annals of contrast in judgments of well-being. In F. Strack,
Behavioral Medicine, 19, 353–361. M. Argyle, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Subjective well-
Stone, A. A., Neale, J. M., Cox, D. S., Napoli, A., being: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 101–
Vadlimarsdottir, V., & Kennedy-Moore, E. (1994). 118). Oxford, England: Pergamon Press.
Daily events are associated with a secretory im- Warr, P., & Payne, R. (1982). Experience of strain
mune response to an oral antigen in men. Health and pleasure among British adults. Social Science
Psychology, 13, 440 – 446. and Medicine, 16, 1691–1697.
Suh, E. M., Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1996). Events Williams, S., & Shiaw, W. T. (1999). Mood and
and subjective well-being: Only recent events mat- organizational citizenship behavior: The effects of
ter. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- positive affect on employee organizational citizen-
ogy, 70, 1091–1102. ship behavior intentions. Journal of Psychology,
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and 133, 656 – 668.
well-being: A social psychological perspective on
mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193– Received September 25, 2004
210. Accepted September 28, 2004 䡲

You might also like