Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Diversity of Deaf Identities

Yael Bat-Chava

American Annals of the Deaf, Volume 145, Number 5, December 2000, pp.
420-428 (Article)

Published by Gallaudet University Press


DOI: https://doi.org/10.1353/aad.2012.0176

For additional information about this article


https://muse.jhu.edu/article/384063/summary

Access provided by Australian National University (29 Aug 2018 16:52 GMT)
Volume 14 5, N ο. 5, 2000

Diversity of Deaf Identities

ocial Identity Theory (Tajfel, 1981) posits that members of


minority groups achieve positive social identity by (a) attempting
to gain access to the mainstream through individual mobility or
(b) working with other group members to bring about social
change. Some people may use a combination of both strategies.
Through the use of cluster analysis, the existence of three
identities associated with these strategies was discerned in a
sample of 267 deaf adults: culturally hearing identity, culturally
deaf identity, and bicultural identity, each comprising about a
third of the sample. A subset of 56 people were interviewed in
depth; excerpts are presented to illustrate the identity types.
Qualified support was found for the prediction that people with
culturally deaf and bicultural identities would have higher self-
esteem.

Yael Bat-Chava According to Social Identity Theory cial change. Such social change may in-
(Tajfel, 1981), an individual will remain a clude devaluing attributes and values of
member of a group if it contributes to the majority out-group (including disre-
positive aspects of his or her social iden- garding the stigma associated with the in-
Bat-Chava is director
tity (e.g., self-esteem). If group member- group), and valuing attributes and values
of research, League ship does not make such a positive contri- of the in-group.
for the Hard of bution—as in the case of groups whose These two strategies—individual mobil-
Hearing, New York, members are stigmatized by the major- ity and social change—correspond to two
NY.
ity—the individual will attempt to leave identities deaf people may assume.
the group physically or psychologically. Through the route of individual mobility,
One may effect psychological departure deaf people may assume a culturally hear-
by denying one's membership in the ing identity, assimilating as much as pos-
group, attempting to achieve success in sible into the hearing world by using their
the mainstream (the route of individual residual hearing (either through amplifica-
mobility), or by claiming that the negative tion or cochlear implants) and speech-
characteristics associated with the group reading. These individuals will achieve a
do not apply to oneself. Leaving the positive social identity through academic
group, then, will result in higher self-es- and professional attainment. Through the
teem. Conversely, other members may route of social change, other deaf people
enhance their self-esteem by identifying may assume a culturally deaf identity,
with their group and working toward so- coming together with other deaf individu-

Volume 145, No. 5, 2000 American Annals of the Deaf


als by using American Sign Language deafness as a disability. Such parents' culture within a deaf school facilitates
(ASL) and participating in social, civic, deaf children will likely adhere to this the construction of deafness as a cul-
and political organizations and net- view as well.) In contrast, deaf chil- ture (Johnston, 1997). A person who
works. dren whose parents or siblings (or has attended hearing schools, by con-
Although Social Identity Theory both) are members of the Deaf com- trast, is immersed in a hearing culture,
posits the individual mobility and so- munity are more likely to be exposed and absorbs the view of deafness as a
cial change strategies as the two poles to the cultural model of deafness disability.
of a continuum, other points on this through interaction with family and The mode of communication used
continuum may combine both strate- the Deaf community. in the school is also important. Some
gies. For example, some individuals A related feature of the family envi- schools use oral English for instruc-
who are successful professionally in ronment is the communication method tion, while others use fingerspelling,
the hearing world may identify with used in the home. In the past, most English-based sign systems, or ASL.
the Deaf community and fight for so- hearing parents were advised not to Attending schools where sign lan-
cial change. Such individuals could be use signs or gestures with their deaf guage is used may foster the view of
considered bicultural. Indeed, a scale child, but rather to talk to him or her deafness as a culture; attending
that measures deaf identities includes (Jacobs, 1980). Heeding professionals' schools where sign language is not
these three, labeling them "hearing advice, these parents tried to educate used may foster the view of deafness
identity" (perceiving deafness as a dis- their children by means of the oral as a disability.
ability), "immersion" (perceiving deaf- method (Swisher, 1989)· These prac- A person's identity may remain
ness as a culture), and "bicultural" tices, which are less prevalent today, static throughout his or her life, but
(Glickman & Carey, 1993; Leigh, affected the participants in the present more often it changes as the person
Marcus, Dobosh, & Allen, 1998). A study. Many hearing parents would develops. Emerton (1996) suggests
fourth, "marginal" identity is proposed, resist learning sign language even that most young deaf people today are
of people who are not sure about their when their children asked them to bicultural, as a consequence of expo-
feelings toward their deafness. (Foster, 1989). Culturally deaf parents, sure to both the Deaf and hearing
by contrast, typically use ASL with communities. For deaf people who
their deaf children, although in a study grow up culturally hearing, frustration
of deaf students in local public high with oral communication and a desire
Deaf Identities and Family schools, only 4% were reported to use for a richer social life may prompt the
and School Histories ASL at home (Kluwin & Stinson, 1993). learning of sign language and involve-
Deaf people with different identities It is my hypothesis that children ment in the Deaf community. (Alter-
are hypothesized to have different whose parents are hearing or who natively, some people may seek other
family and school histories. The de- grew up in homes where spoken lan- oral deaf people for social purposes
gree to which a deaf child is exposed guage was the primary mode of com- without learning to sign.) Deaf people
to other deaf children and adults in the munication will be likely to adopt the with a culturally deaf background may
family and school is likely to have an view of deafness as a disability, and seek greater involvement in the hear-
effect on the identity he or she will develop a culturally hearing identity. ing world if they become frustrated
develop as an adult. Those who grew up with deaf parents with their more limited access to infor-
or in homes where sign language was mation and to mainstream culture.
used will be likely to regard deafness
as a culture, and will identify them-
Deafness and the Family selves as culturally deaf.
Most deaf children (90%) are born to Deaf Identities and SeIf-
hearing parents (Moores, 2001), who Esteem
previously never thought much about Social Identity Theory predicts no dif-
deafness. Therefore, health profes- Deafness and School ferences in self-esteem between
sionals and educators, who hold the Another crucial factor in the develop- people with different identities. An-
view that deafness is a disability, ment of deaf children is the schools other theory, however, proposes that
shape parents' initial views of deaf- they attend. Deaf children are placed members of stigmatized groups who
ness. These hearing parents then in different educational environments have stronger group identity will have
transmit this view to their deaf chil- that can be ordered along a continuum higher self-esteem than those with
dren. (A minority of deaf parents may of deafness to hearing, from residential weaker group identity (Crocker & Ma-
themselves adhere to this view of schools to full mainstreaming. The jor, 1989). This is achieved through

Volume 145, No. 5, 2000 American Annals of the Deaf


Deaf Identities

various mechanisms, such as devalu- men, 150 women) who participated in ing the view that deafness is a disabil-
ing majority attributes (e.g., speech) the study ranged in age from 16 to 87 ity. Based on Phinney's (1990) list of
and beliefs (e.g., stigmatization of deaf years, with a mean age of 42.9 years. ethnic identity components, two other
people), and valuing minority at- The parents of 237 respondents characteristics of cultural identification
tributes (e.g., signing) and values. (88.8%) were hearing; 28 respondents can be discerned: (a) association with
Research (Bat-Chava, 1993; 1994) has (10.5%) had deaf parents; 2 (0.7%) re- other deaf individuals and involve-
shown that stronger deaf identities are spondents had one deaf parent and ment with deaf institutions and organi-
associated with higher self-esteem. I one hearing parent. These percent- zations, and (b) positive attitudes to-
therefore hypothesize that people with ages are similar to those reported in a ward deafness and deaf people.
culturally deaf and bicultural identities national survey of deaf adults (Schein The importance of sign language
will have higher self-esteem than & DeIk, 1974). Although respondents and the importance of speech were
people with hearing identities. were recruited from the New York City each measured by a single question,
In summary, the present study area, many had grown up in other re- answered on a 4-point scale ranging
sought to discern the presence of three gions of the United States, and thus from "not important at all" (1) to "very
types of deaf identities (culturally deaf, constitute a representative sample of important" (4):
culturally hearing, and bicultural) in a deaf adults. (Additional participant
sample of deaf adults, and to identify characteristics are provided in Bat- Importance of sign: There are some
differences in family and school histo- Chava, 1994). things that deaf people can do and
ries and in self-esteem in adults with hearing people cannot. For ex-
each of the identities. Quantitative ample, most deaf people can sign
data on these groups are reported; one better than hearing people. How
person from each group is presented Measures important do you think signing
to illustrate the types. By means of a short questionnaire, skills are? (M = 3.40; SD = .80)
several variables were assessed. Four
variables (described below) were used Importance of speech: There are
to group research participants into some things that hearing people
Method identity clusters. In addition, the ecol- can do and deaf people cannot.
Data were gathered in twro related ogy of family and school histories, as For example, most hearing people
studies. First, a short questionnaire wrell as self-esteem—the outcome vari- have better spoken language skills
was administered to deaf adults of all able—were measured. Membership in than deaf people. How important
ages, recruited from various sources. identity clusters wras hypothesized to do you think spoken language skills
In the second study, a subset of the be associated both with the ecological are' (M = 3.23; SD= .87)
original sample was interviewed in variables and the outcome variable.
depth. Data from both studies are pre- Questions were pilot-tested and re-
sented. vised when necessary. The construct group identification
was assessed by means of two items:
(a) percentage of deaf friends and (b)
Cluster Variables. Language is con- level of involvement with the Deaf
sidered a main feature of culture in
Questionnaire Study community. (The question "Do you
general (Sherzer, 1987) and of Deaf feel that you are a part of the Deaf
Sample culture in particular: "Within the deaf community?" was scored on a 3-point
Respondents were identified through community, communication prefer- scale from "not at all" to "very much.")
various sources: social/political ence and skill, the relative emphasis The two items were highly correlated,
groups, service agencies, and organi- that members give to signing and r = .71, p < .001. To achieve a com-
zations that advocate either sign lan- speaking, is an important basis on mon scale, percentage of deaf friends
guage or oral communication. Partici- which relationships are organized" was categorized into three groups (0-
pants wrere informed of the purpose of (Higgins, 1980, p. 49). A person who 33%; 34-67%>; and 68-100%), and the
the study in sign language when ques- regards signing as important, there- two items were averaged to a common
tionnaires were distributed in meetings fore, could be classified as adhering to scale from 1 to 3 (M = 2.38; SD = .75).
or classes, or in a cover letter when the model of deafness as a culture. (Group identification measured identi-
questionnaires were sent through the Those who believe that signing is not fication with deaf people. No attempt
mail. Of 646 questionnaires, 41% were important but that speech is important was made to separately measure the
returned. The 267 deaf adults (117 can, conversely, be perceived as hold- level of identification with hearing

Volume 145, No. 5, 2000 American Annals of the Deaf


people.) weighted by number of years in atten- Scores on the four criterion variables
To assess attitudes toward deaf dance at each type of school and were first standardized so that scales
people, three items from the revised summed to a scale from O to 4 (M = with larger variance would not domi-
Attitudes Toward Disabled Persons 1.65; SD = 1.36). nate the analysis (Rapkin & Luke,
scale, form O (ATDP-O), were used Items also assessed participants' 1993)· Based on examination of
(Yuker, Block, & Campbell, I960; re- age, gender, age at onset of deafness, dendograms (tree diagrams of cluster
vised by Furnham & Lane, 1984). and ability to hear with and without a solutions), several solutions were
Higher scores indicate more positive hearing aid. These last two items were closely evaluated for interpretability,
attitudes. The items were: "Deaf scored O (no) or 1 (yes). and a four-cluster solution was se-
people are just as smart as hearing lected. Figure 1 portrays the profiles
people," "Deaf people are more easily Outcome Variable. Self-esteem was of the four clusters. (The zero refer-
upset than hearing people" (reversed), assessed with three items from the ence line is the mean, and scores are
and "Deaf people are as happy as Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale expressed in standard deviations
hearing people." Questions were an- (Rosenberg, 1979): (a) "I take a posi- above and below the mean.)
swered on a 4-point scale ranging tive attitude toward myself"; (b) "On
from "strongly disagree" (1) to the whole, I am satisfied with myself";
"strongly agree" (4); M = 2.94; SD = and (c) "Sometimes I think I am no
.55; alpha = .47. (Although this is a good at all" (reversed). Answers were Description of Clusters
relatively low reliability coefficient, indicated on a 4-point scale ranging People with culturally deaf identities
further analysis indicates that remov- from "strongly disagree" (1) to (n = 81; 33%o of the sample) believe
ing any one of the three items would "strongly agree" (4); M = 3-18, SD = that signing is very important, and that
not result in a higher reliability coeffi- .55, alpha = .63. clear speech is not. They are very
cient. Moreover, correlations were much a part of the Deaf community,
calculated between the three items of and their attitudes toward other deaf
this scale and the other scales de- people are neither positive nor nega-
scribed in the method section of the Interview Study tive. Those with culturally hearing
present study. All three items correlate Semistructured interviews were con- identities (n = 58, 24% of the sample)
with the other scales in the same direc- ducted with 56 respondents, ages 16 to believe that signing well is not impor-
tion and the same magnitude. Thus, it 42 years, who had completed the tant, and that clear speech is very im-
was decided to treat these three items questionnaire. The interview ques- portant. They do not feel at all that
as a scale.) tions asked about family and school they are a part of the Deaf community,
histories, current involvement with and their attitudes toward deaf people
Ecological Variables. Family deaf- deaf individuals and organizations, are negative. People with bicultural
ness was assessed with an index of and attitudes toward the self, deafness, identities (n = 82, 34% of the sample)
two items: (a) family hearing status, and other deaf people. Excerpts from feel that both sign language and
scored on a 3-point scale ranging from the interviews are provided below speech are important, their identifica-
O (no other deaf family members) to 2 under the heading "Examples of Clus- tion with other deaf people is of aver-
(both parents were deaf); and (b) fam- ter Members" to illustrate the identity age strength, and they have the most
ily mode of communication, scored on types derived from the quantitative positive attitudes toward deaf people
a 3-point scale ranging from O (oral data. of all of the clusters. Finally, a small
communication) to 2 (sign as primary group of people have negative identi-
mode of communication). Scores ties (n = 22; 9% of the sample), feeling
were summed to form a single scale that both sign language and speech
from O to 4 (M = .79; SD = 1.23). Results are extremely unimportant, and hav-
School deafness was assessed with an A ¿-means cluster algorithm (Hartigan, ing below-average levels of both
index of three items: (a) types of 1975) was used to discern identities group identity and positive attitude
schools attended, scored on a 3-point based on the four criterion variables: toward deaf people. (Because of miss-
scale ranging from O (hearing school) importance of signing, importance of ing data, only 243 of the 267 partici-
to 2 (residential school); and (b) mode speech, group identity, and attitudes pants in the present study were classi-
of communication used in class and toward deaf people. The ¿-means pro- fied into clusters.)
(c) out of class, each scored on a 2- cedure minimizes within-cluster vari- To examine whether any of the de-
point scale ranging from O (oral) to 1 ance on criterion variables and maxi- mographic, ecological, and outcome
(sign). The three scores were mizes differences between clusters. variables differentiated among the

Volume 145, No. 5, 2000 American Annals of the Deaf


Deaf Identities

clusters, a series of one-way analyses hearing aids, Kd, 231) = 3.15, p < .03. culturally hearing; M = 0.68, bicultural
of variance were conducted, with clus- The culturally deaf group was signifi- identity; M = 0.76, negative identity).
ter membership used as the indepen- cantly older then the bicultural group Cluster membership was only margin-
dent variable. When appropriate, post (Ms = 46.33 and 37.91, respectively), ally related to self-esteem, F(3, 237) =
hoc Scheffé tests were conducted. As culturally hearing people became deaf 2.36, p < .07. Culturally deaf and bi-
predicted, cluster membership was re- later (M = 3.21) than culturally deaf cultural individuals had higher self-es-
lated to school deafness, F(3, 239) = people (M = .41), and bicultural indi- teem scores (Ms = 3-27 and 3.24, re-
8.28, p < .001. People with culturally viduals could hear better with hearing spectively) than culturally hearing
deaf and negative identities attended aids (M = .68) than culturally deaf people and those with negative identi-
schools that were higher in deafness people (M = .44). Cluster membership ties (Ms = 3-05 and 3-06, respectively).
orientation (Ms = 2.16 and 2.31, re- was not related to gender, or ability to To enable a clearer understanding
spectively), compared to people with hear without hearing aids. of these clusters, excerpts from inter-
culturally hearing and bicultural iden- Contrary to prediction, cluster views are presented in the following
tities (Ms = 1.37 and 1.36, respec- membership was not related to family section, illustrating three of the four
tively). deafness, although examination of the identity clusters. (No participants from
Cluster membership was also re- means shows that culturally deaf indi- the negative identity cluster volun-
lated to age, F(3, 239) = 3.25, Ï• < .02; viduals came from homes with higher teered to be interviewed.) Interview
age at onset of deafness, F(5, 221) = levels of deafness orientation (M = excerpts are also used to introduce the
4.11, p < .007; and ability to hear with 1.09) than all other groups (M = 0.62, dynamic nature of identities.

Volume 145, No. 5, 2000 American Annals of the Deaf


Examples of Cluster Members that's it. I'm happy with who I am." it?" he responded, "Of course! That's a
It is worth noting that Mark's iden- ridiculous question, of course I would
Culturally Deaf Identity: Mark tity as a culturally deaf person did not do it!"
Angus shift between childhood and adult- Like Mark's, Oliver's hearing iden-
Mark Angus (all names in this and the hood. He was raised as a culturally tity is static. Oliver was raised in the
following case studies are fictional) deaf person and remains one later in hearing world and remains in that
exemplifies the culturally deaf cluster. life. world in adulthood.
He is in his late thirties, and his par-
ents and sister are deaf. Growing up,
he attended an oral residential school.
He then attended college at Gallaudet Culturally Hearing Identity: Oliver Bicultural Identity: Amanda Scott
University and graduate school at a Garrison Amanda Scott comes from a hearing
hearing university. He now teaches Oliver Garrison exemplifies culturally family. She attended an oral deaf
ASL at an interpreter training program. hearing people. Although he was pro- school and hearing schools; for col-
During the interview he and I used foundly deaf from birth, his hearing lege she attended the National Techni-
ASL to communicate. family strongly stressed oralism. He cal Institute for the Deaf. She has two
Mark believes that skill in sign lan- attended hearing schools all his life, master's degrees and works in a hu-
guage is crucial for deaf people. He and he currently works as an engineer. man services agency with deaf clients.
reports wishing that he had been al- He used speech and lipreading to We used a mix of sign and speech
lowed to use sign in elementary and communicate during the interview. during the interview.
high school, and said that he chose to Regarding the importance of sign, Amanda recommends that ASL be
attend Gallaudet because "finally I Oliver said: "Sign language, I don't used in teaching deaf children, and
could sign in the classroom!" He also believe in it.... I'm as uncomfortable that deaf students have "as many deaf
believes that English should be taught with sign language as someone else. I teachers as possible and hearing
to deaf children as a second language don't understand it and I don't think people skilled in ASL." Amanda re-
through ASL. Discussing the impor- it's necessary for myself, for my situa- ports that her speech and lipreading
tance of speech, Mark said: "Having to tion." Oliver thinks that being able to skills are good. She said: "Do I think
read lips in school was really frustrat- speak well is very important, and he that it is important to speak clearly and
ing; it was a traumatic experience.... rates his speech as "very good" and his to read lips? Maybe. Sometimes yes
Not until I finished school did I under- lipreading as "excellent." He is proud and sometimes no.... It's easier to use
stand that oralism was good for com- of his ability to read people's lips from speech in the hearing world; on the
municating with the hearing." He afar, saying, "Lipreading is great, it re- other hand, there are more important
rates his speech as "poor" and his lip- ally helps me." things to be concerned about than
reading as "fair," but he believes that Oliver refers to himself as a "hear- talking; speech is important, but it
lipreading is important "for ing deaf man": "That means that I'm should not become the raison d'être of
survival...so people can help you." deaf, but I consider myself a hearing deaf education."
About his group identity, Mark person. Being deaf does not hold me Amanda did not associate with deaf
commented: "Growing up, I used to back from being part of the hearing people during early adolescence, but
go to deaf church with my family, pic- world." He has very few friends, and then decided to attend a college that
nics, and other events. Most of the they are all hearing. He is a member had many deaf students. "I started to
time we'd socialize with other deaf of the A. G. Bell Association for the socialize with deaf people and I was
people." As an adult, he said, "I have Deaf, an organization that advocates really in the deaf experience, in the
more deaf friends than hearing, maybe oralism. Oliver expresses very nega- deaf world." Her preference for deaf
75% and 25%>." His choice of occupa- tive feelings toward the deaf world: people extends to her employment.
tion, too, indicates a strong connection "Being part of the deaf world scares She is employed in a deaf program
to the Deaf community. He is a mem- me. I don't agree with the philosophy within a hearing agency, preferring to
ber of many deaf organizations, and of institutions like Gallaudet because I work with deaf clients because of the
he subscribes to deaf newspapers and don't agree with a total deaf environ- ease of communication: "It feels more
magazines. Asked about his attitudes ment." He believes that two deaf normal for me, more natural."
toward deafness and deaf people, he people should not marry lest they Amanda believes that deaf people are
said: "Deafness was given to me by transmit deafness to their offspring. just as intelligent and happy as hearing
God; why fight it? Why object to it? When asked, "If you could take a pill people: "There is nothing negative
Why deny it? It's my life; I'm deaf, and become hearing, would you do about being deaf in and of itself.

Volume 145, No. 5, 2000 American Annals of the Deaf


Deaf Identities

What's negative is other people's treat- membership and other constructs fur- would have higher self-esteem than
ment of deaf people. People should ther validate the meaning of the clus- those with culturally hearing identities
focus on abilities, on health and ters. For example, the finding that received qualified support. Although
strengths, rather than on inabilities." people with culturally hearing identi- Social Identity Theory does not predict
Amanda's identity underwent a shift ties were deafened later than those differences in self-esteem, Crocker and
from childhood to adulthood. Al- with culturally deaf identities can be Major (1989) posit that members of
though she had a culturally hearing explained in two ways. First, cultur- stigmatized groups with stronger
identity earlier in life, in young adult- ally deaf people come from families group identity would employ several
hood she became involved in the Deaf that are more "deaf" than all other psychological mechanisms to protect
community and has remained in- groups (although this difference was their self-esteem from the majority's
volved in it into her adulthood, with- not statistically significant). Deafness, negative attitudes. As hypothesized,
out abandoning some of the culturally then, is more likely to be genetic and the present study finds that deaf
hearing values she grew up with. congenital, rather than acquired. Sec- people employ these mechanisms:
ond, hearing parents of children who valuing deaf attributes and devaluing
have learned spoken language before hearing attributes. In addition, those
becoming deaf are likely to try to raise with stronger deaf identities (culturally
Discussion their children in an oral environment, deaf and bicultural individuals) have
Three types of deaf identities were dis- in contrast to parents whose children somewhat higher self-esteem than
cerned in the present study. About a were born deaf or became deaf before those with weaker deaf identities (cul-
third of the sample identified as cultur- acquiring spoken language. turally hearing and negative identi-
ally deaf, a quarter had culturally hear- Further validating the meaning of ties). As research with ethnic and ra-
ing identities, and another third had the clusters is the finding that school cial groups and with other
bicultural identities. In addition, a deafness is related to cluster member- marginalized groups has demonstrated
minority of deaf adults had negative ship: People with culturally deaf iden- (e.g., Michaelieu, 1997; Walters &
identities. These findings support So- tities attended schools with a stronger Simoni, 1993), identification with one's
cial Identity Theory, which asserts that deafness orientation than the schools group is an asset to one's psychologi-
members of stigmatized groups vari- attended by those people with hearing cal well-being. Further research will
ously use the individual mobility route and bicultural identities. (This differ- need to show whether various deaf
(in this case, culturally hearing iden- ence was marginally significant.) It is identities have an effect on other posi-
tity) and social change route (in this likely that in deaf schools, students tive outcomes, such as academic and
case, culturally deaf identity) in con- interacted with deaf peers and learned occupational achievement, as well as
fronting stigmatization by the majority. early that there were others like them. satisfaction with various aspects of life.
In addition, some people use a combi- Even in schools that used oral commu- The shift in deaf education over the
nation of both routes (in this case, bi- nication in the classroom, sign lan- past two decades (Bowe, 1993) has
cultural identity). Although no previ- guage was used outside of classes with meant that more deaf children and
ous studies used cluster analysis to peers and sometimes with school per- adolescents attend hearing schools
classify deaf identities, similar identi- sonnel (Janesick & Moores, 1992). than in the past. In this educational
ties were found in samples of deaf People who attended deaf schools environment, deaf students have few
adolescents (Weinberg & Steritt, 1986) were easily integrated into the Deaf opportunities to interact with deaf
and adults (Glickman & Carey, 1993). community upon graduation. In con- peers. Their self-esteem and their op-
The advantage of the present study trast, fully mainstreamed deaf students portunities for an active social life and
over previous ones is the use of only a did not know other deaf students or participation in leadership roles are
few questions to categorize people's adults, and did not become part of the therefore more limited. Introducing
identities. In addition, whereas inves- Deaf community in adulthood. Be- adolescents who attend hearing
tigators in previous studies viewed cause of recent shifts in deaf educa- schools and who have hearing identi-
identity as a unidimensional construct, tion, with more children being edu- ties to deaf peers and role models may
the classification in the present study cated in the mainstream than ever lead to a more bicultural identity,
considers attitudes about language before (Bowe, 1993), members of the which would enhance these adoles-
and communication modalities, and culturally deaf cluster wrere signifi- cents' psychological well-being. Fu-
toward deaf people, as well as in- cantly older than those from the bicul- ture research may enable us to identify
volvement with deaf people and insti- tural group. adolescents who could benefit from
tutions. The prediction that people with such a shift in identity, as well as aid
The relationships between cluster culturally deaf and bicultural identities professionals in the field of deafness in

Volume 145, No. 5, 2000 American Annals of the Deaf


designing environments that could incorporate deafness into bicultural point in time would have shown dif-
trigger this shift. identities from those who do not? The ferent proportions of the sample ex-
majority of people in the present hibiting the various identities. Simi-
sample who reported such a shift were larly, with the advent of more
women (Bat-Chava & Lindermayer, sophisticated technological solutions
Static and Dynamic 1995). Why would women be more to hearing loss, children are now using
Identities: Directions for likely to experience an identity shift better hearing aids and some use co-
Future Study than men? This finding needs to be chlear implants. It is possible that
The identities described in the present replicated with other samples. It is these technologies make oral commu-
article are static "snapshots" in time. also not known whether other demo- nication less frustrating, and that
Personal experience as well as theo- graphic variables are associated with today's deaf children educated in the
retical (Deaux, 1993) and empirical lit- such a change in identity. A psycho- mainstream will be less likely to turn
erature (Ethier & Deaux, 1994; logical construct that has been hypoth- to the Deaf culture as a result of frus-
Hormuth, 1990), however, indicate esized to facilitate identity change in tration with oral communication. Fu-
that identities shift and change. Most adulthood is the personality trait of ture research will need to examine
of the people in the present study with openness to experience (Whitbourne, demographic, psychological, and con-
culturally deaf or hearing identities 1986). Another impetus for intentional textual factors that affect deaf identities
reported no major shifts in identity self-change can be provided by a as well as change in identity.
from childhood to adulthood. Indi- stressor (Kiecolt, 1994)—that is, indi-
viduals with bicultural identities, con- viduals who experience more stress
versely, showed a shift in their identi- related to their compromised ability to
ties. Most of them grew up in hearing communicate orally and to feelings of Note
homes and schools and initially had loneliness may be more motivated to This article is based on my doctoral
culturally hearing identities. In late learn sign language and to seek deaf dissertation at the City University of
adolescence or early adulthood, how- peers. Levels of loneliness and frustra- New York, under the supervision of
ever, they discovered sign language or tion with oral communication could be Kay Deaux. I thank my colleagues at
encountered deaf role models. This assessed in future studies to determine the League for the Hard of Hearing for
"encounter," not unlike that described how these conditions relate to change their comments on early drafts of this
by Cross (1971, 1991) among Black in identity. paper. Correspondence regarding this
Americans, prompted these individu- Contextual factors may also have an article should be addressed to Yael
als with hearing identities to learn sign effect on identity change. Several in- Bat-Chava, Ph.D., Director of Re-
language and become involved in the terviews with participants in the search, League for the Hard of Hear-
Deaf community. They did not, how- present study suggest that an identity ing, 71 W. 23rd Street, New York, NY
ever, abandon some of the values change occurred with normative life 10010 (e-mail ybat- chava@lhh.org).-
learned during childhood (such as the transitions. For example, a fully The Author.
importance of speech), and hence can mainstreamed student who started at-
be regarded as bicultural. A small tending a new high school discovered
number of oral deaf people inter- deaf peers and sign language in a pro-
References
viewed for the present study became gram for deaf students in her school. Bat-Chava, Y. (1993). Antecedents of self-esteem
involved with other oral deaf individu- Another person recalled meeting other in deaf people: A meta-analytic review. Re-
als and organizations. They maintain deaf students for the first time when habilitation Psychology, 38(4), 221-234.
Bat-Chava, Y. (1994). Group identification and
their culturally hearing identities and she started attending college. Re- self-esteem of deaf adults. Personality and
foster social relationships with other search with other samples (e.g., Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 494-502.
oral deaf individuals. Because of their Bat-Chava, Y., & Lindermayer, K. (1995, July).
Hormuth, 1990) shows that identity Diversity and change in deaf identities. Paper
small number in the sample, it is im- changes as context shifts. presented at the 18th International Congress
on Education of the Deaf, Tel Aviv, Israel.
possible to determine if their level of A larger contextual factor is the so- Bowe, F. G. (1993). Getting there: Update on
self-esteem is different from that of cial, political, and historical environ- recommendations by the COED. American
Annals of the Deaf, 138, 304-308.'
those who became involved in the ment in which deaf people live. The Crocker, J., & Major, B. (1989). Social stigma and
signing Deaf community. present study was conducted several self-esteem: The self-protective properties of
stigma. Psychological Review, 96, 608-630.
One major question about this years after the 1988 Deaf President Cross, W. (1971). Toward a psychology of Black
change in identity remains open: What Now protest at Gallaudet University. It liberation: The Negro-to-Black conversion
experience. Black World, 21, 13- 27.
are the factors that differentiate people is possible that a similar study con- Cross, W. (1991). Shades of Black: Diversity in
with culturally hearing identities who ducted before the protest or at a future African American identity. Philadelphia:
Temple University Press.

Volume 145, No. 5, 2000 American Annals of the Deaf


Deaf Identities

Deaux, K. (1993). Reconstructing social identity. Washington. DC: Gallaudet College Press. ogy and practice. American Journal of Com-
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Janesick, V. J., & Moores, D. F. (1992). Ethnic munity Psychology, 21, 247-277.
19, 4-12. and cultural considerations. In T. N. Kluwin, Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. New
Emerton, R. G. (1996). Marginality, biculturalism, D. F. Moores, & M. G. Gaustacl (Eds.), To- York: Basic Books.
and social identity of deaf people. In I. ward effective public school programs for Schein, J., & DeIk, M. (1974). '[he deaf popula-
Parasnis (Ed.), Cultural and language diver- deaf students (pp. 49-65). New York: Teach- tion of the United States. Silver Spring, MD:
sity and the deaf experience (pp. 136-145). ers College Press. National Association of the Deaf.
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Johnston, E. (1997). Residential schools offer Sherzer, J. (1987). A discourse-centered approach
Press. students Deaf culture. Perspectives in Educa- to language and culture. American Anthro-
Ethier, K., & Deaux, K. (1994). Negotiating social tion and Deafness, 76(2), 4-5, 24. pologist, 89, 295-309.
identity when contexts change. Journal of Kiecolt, J. K. (1994). Stress and the decision to Swisher, M. V. (1989). The language-learning
Personality and Social Psychology. 67. 243- change oneself: A theoretical model. Social situation of deaf students. Teachers of English
251. Psychology: Quarterly. 57, 49-63. to Speakers of Other Languages Quarterly, 23,
Foster, S. (1989). Social alienation and peer iden- Kluwin, T. N., & Stinson, M. S. (Eds.). (1993). 239-257.
tification: A study of the social construction Deaf students in local public high schools. Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social cat-
of deafness. Human Organization. 48(Y). Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. egories. Cambridge, England: Cambridge Uni-
226-235. Leigh, I. W., Marcus, A. L., Dobosh. P. K., & versity Press.
Furnham, A., & Lane, S. (1984). Actual and per- Allen, T. E. (1998). Deaf/hearing cultural Walters, K. L., & Simoni, J. M. (1993). Lesbian and
ceived attitudes toward deafness. Psychologi- identity paradigms: Modification of the Deaf gay male group identity attitudes and self-es-
cal Medicine, 14, 417-423. Identity Development Scale. Journal of Deaf teem: Implications for counseling. Journal of
Glickman, N. S., & Carey, J. C. (1993). Measur- Studies and Deaf Education, 5(4), 329-338. Counseling Psychology, 40, 94-99.
ing deaf cultural identities: A preliminary in- Michaelieu, Q. (1997). Female identity, Weinberg, N.."& Steritt, M. (1986). Disability and
vestigation. Rehabilitation Psychology, 18, gendered parenting and adolescent women's identity: A study of identity patterns in ado-
275-283. self-esteem. Feminism and Psychology, 7. lescents with hearing impairments. Rehabili-
Hartigan. J. (1975). Clustering algorithms. New 328-333. tation Psychology, 31, 95-102.
York: Wiley. Moores, D. F. (2001). Educating the Deaf Psy- Whitbourne, S. K. (1986). Openness to experi-
Higgins, P. C. (1980). Outsiders in a hearing chology, principles and practices (5th ed.). ence, identity flexibility, and life changes in
world: A sociology of deafness. Beverly Hills, Boston: Houghton Mifflin. adults. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
CA: Sage. Phinney, J. S. (1990). Ethnic identity in adoles- chology. 50. 163-168.
Hormuth, S. E. (1990). The ecology of the self: cents and adults: Review of research. Psycho- Yuker, H. E., Block, J. R.. & Campbell, W. J.
Relocation and self-concept change. Cam- logical Bulletin. 108. 499-514. (I960). A scale to measure attitudes toward
bridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Rapkin, B. D., & Luke, D. A. (1993)- Cluster disabled persons: Human resource study
Jacobs, L. M. (1980). A deaf adult speaks oui. analysis in community research: Epistemol- number 5. Albertson, NY: Human Resources
Center.

Volume 145, No. 5, 2000 American Annals of the Deaf

You might also like