Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

T

Theory of Planned Behavior of the construct of perceived behavioral control


(i.e., one’s view of their degree of control over a
Matthew P.H. Kan and Leandre R. Fabrigar behavior) to the TRA. Because the primary dis-
Queen’s University, Kingston, ON, Canada tinction between the TPB and the TRA is the
addition of the perceived behavioral control com-
ponent, most of the key assumptions underlying
Definition the TRA also apply to the TPB. Thus, a review of
the TRA provides a useful foundation for the
The theory of planned behavior is a theory used to discussion of the TPB.
understand and predict behaviors, which posits that
behaviors are immediately determined by behav-
ioral intentions and under certain circumstances, The Theory of Reasoned Action
perceived behavioral control. Behavioral intentions
are determined by a combination of three factors: The creators of the TRA, Martin Fishbein and Icek
attitudes toward the behavior, subjective norms, Ajzen (1975; Ajzen and Fishbein 1980), designed
and perceived behavioral control. the TRA to predict and understand people’s overt
behaviors that are under their volitional control.
The TRA is based on the assumption that humans
Introduction are generally rational beings who make systematic
use of the information available to them. Thus, the
An extension of the theory of reasoned action theory presumes that people’s behaviors are not
(TRA; Fishbein and Ajzen 1975; Ajzen and Fishbein thoughtless, as they consider the implications of
1980), the theory of planned behavior (TPB) was their actions before they decide to engage or not
developed by Icek Ajzen (1985, 1991) as a gen- engage in a given behavior. When explaining the
eral model to predict and explain behavior across TRA, it is useful to begin at the final outcome (i.e.,
a wide range of different types of behaviors. A key behavior) and work backwards along the causal
assumption in the TRA is that behaviors are under chain (see Fig. 1 for the TRA pathway).
one’s volitional control. However, this assump-
tion is likely to be unrealistic in some contexts, Behavior
as one can imagine that the volitional control of In the context of the TRA, behavior is defined as
behaviors may vary across different situations. Rec- an overt action or set of actions that is performed by
ognizing that not all behaviors are under volitional an individual. Behavior can be construed broadly
control, Ajzen (1985, 1991) proposed the addition or narrowly and is a function of the researcher’s
# Springer International Publishing AG 2017
V. Zeigler-Hill, T.K. Shackelford (eds.), Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences,
DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-28099-8_1191-1
2 Theory of Planned Behavior

Theory of Planned
Behavioral Beliefs
Behavior, Fig. 1. The
theory of reasoned action
Beliefs that the
Behavior Leads to
Certain Outcomes Attitude toward
the Behavior

Evaluation of
Outcomes
Relative
Importance of
Intention Behavior
Attitude and
Normative Beliefs Subjective Norms

Beliefs of
Important
Referents
Subjective Norms

Motivation to
Comply with
Referents

theoretical and/or applied objectives (i.e., a re- For instance, exercising is a behavior and weight
searcher chooses the behavior he or she wishes loss is a possible outcome of exercising. However,
to examine). However, according to the theory, the outcome of weight loss could also be influ-
any behavior can be conceptualized in terms of enced by other factors such as dieting. Thus, en-
four components: action (i.e., the specific act per- suring that the behavior is measured, and not the
formed by an individual), target (i.e., who or what outcome, increases the accuracy of the prediction
the behavior is directed toward), context (i.e., in of behavior. Behaviors can be measured using
what situation does the behavior occur), and time behavioral indices generated via observations or
(i.e., when the behavior occurs). An example of a self-reports of behaviors, which can be advanta-
broad behavior would be eating (action only), geous in terms of ease and resources if researchers
whereas a more specific behavior would be eating are confident in the reports’ accuracy (see Ajzen
(action) chips (target) in the lunchroom (context) and Fishbein 1980).
at noon (time). Each of the four components of
behavior can range substantially, as actions can
Behavioral Intention
span from a single action to behavioral categories
According to the TRA, the most proximal deter-
comprised of multiple actions, targets from a sin-
minant of behavior is behavioral intention, which
gle target to multiple targets, context from a single
is an individual’s perceived likelihood that he
context to multiple contexts, and time from a
or she will perform a behavior. This specification
single point in time to a broader period of time.
of intention as the most immediate determinant of
Advocates of the TRA argue that any measure-
behavior has several implications. First, it suggests
ment of behavior must specify the four compo-
that intention is the best predictor of behavior.
nents of behavior if behavior is to be accurately
Second, it posits that any other potential predictors
predicted and understood. Indeed, TRA theorists
of behavior (e.g., attitudes) do not directly influ-
have noted that researchers sometimes mistakenly
ence behavior. Instead, the relationships of these
measure outcomes believing that they are measur-
other factors to behavior must be mediated by
ing behaviors, which may lead to inaccurate re-
intention. Third, it proposes that all behaviors are
sults. Thus, it is important to distinguish behaviors
deliberate, as intentions predicate all behaviors.
from outcomes, as outcomes are the possible re-
The accuracy of intentions in predicting behav-
sult of behaviors – not the behaviors themselves.
iors is a function of two factors. First, prediction
Theory of Planned Behavior 3

accuracy can be increased by ensuring that the one’s beliefs about the behavior. Attitudes toward
four components of behavior (i.e., action, target, a behavior are an aggregate of the beliefs that the
context, and time) correspond with the four com- behavior leads to certain outcomes and the evalu-
ponents of intention. For instance, if the behavior ations of those outcomes. Thus, an attitude toward
is to eat chips in the lunchroom at noon, then a behavior is a function of the extent to which each
asking an individual’s intention to eat chips in outcome is viewed as positive or negative and the
the lunchroom at noon will maximize prediction likelihood that each outcome is seen as a result of
accuracy. Second, prediction accuracy is a func- the behavior. Extremely positive attitudes occur
tion of the stability of intentions. In general, the when each outcome is viewed as extremely posi-
shorter the duration between the measurement of tive and extremely likely. Likewise, negative atti-
an intention and the measurement of its corre- tudes result from sets of beliefs regarding outcomes
sponding behavior, the more accurate the prediction that are seen as extremely negative and extremely
of behavior from intention will be. Thus, intentions likely. Although one can have many behavioral
should be measured as close as possible to behav- beliefs, only the salient behavioral beliefs will be
iors in order to maximize prediction accuracy. the immediate determinants of one’s attitude to-
ward a behavior. Furthermore, like the other direct
Attitude Toward the Behavior relations in the TRA, behavioral beliefs and atti-
According to the TRA, there are two immediate tudes toward a behavior should be measured on the
determinants of behavioral intention. The first de- same four behavioral elements in order to maxi-
terminant is attitude toward the behavior, which mize the beliefs-attitudes correlation.
is an individual’s evaluation (i.e., favorability or
unfavorability) of performing the behavior in Subjective Norms
question as defined by the behavior’s four com- The second determinant of behavioral intention is
ponents. There are two key points to note here. subjective norms, which is a person’s perception
First, the TRA’s definition of attitudes is directed that most people who are important to him (or her)
toward the behavior – not the object associated think he (or she) should or should not perform the
with behavior. For example, researchers should behavior in question. However, this perception
measure an individual’s evaluation of him or her may or may not reflect what the important others
eating chips, rather than the individual’s evalua- actually think. Subjective norms are determined
tion of the chips themselves. As such, this con- by normative beliefs and the motivation to comply
ceptualization of attitudes is different from the with specific referents. Normative beliefs are an
definition of attitudes as it has often been concep- individual’s beliefs that a referent thinks he (or
tualized in the attitude-behavior consistency liter- she) should (or should not) perform the behavior.
ature, which is the positive or negative evaluation Not all referents are equally important; thus, indi-
of the object itself. Second, this definition of atti- viduals are more likely to comply with more salient
tudes refers specifically to an individual’s own referents than less salient referents. Subjective
performance of the behavior, rather than the per- norms can be predicted using an index that multi-
formance of the behavior in general. plies an individual’s normative beliefs regarding a
When measuring one’s attitude toward a be- specific referent by the motivations to comply with
havior, the attitude measure must be framed at the the same referent, and subsequently, sums the prod-
specificity of the behavior in order to maximize ucts for all referents. Finally, once again, the four
the prediction accuracy of the attitude on inten- behavioral components of normative beliefs must
tion. Attitudes toward a behavior can be measured be matched with those of subjective norms.
using traditional attitude scales (e.g., Osgood et al.
1957), as long as they are framed at the appropri- Relative Importance of Attitude toward the
ate specificity of the behavior. However, attitudes Behavior and Subjective Norms
toward a behavior are often assessed by measuring Although the TRA assumes that attitudes toward
one’s behavioral beliefs, as the TRA postulates that the behavior and subjective norms regarding the
one’s attitude toward a behavior is a function of behavior both influence intention, the theory does
4 Theory of Planned Behavior

not assume that these constructs will always be in demographic variables (e.g., age, sex, socioeco-
agreement or that the magnitude of their individ- nomic status, etc.), attitudes toward targets (e.g.,
ual contributions to intention will be invariant people, institutions, etc.), and personality traits (e.g.,
across different behaviors. When these two deter- extraversion, neuroticism, agreeableness, etc.).
minants are in disagreement, the determinant of
intention with the greater weight, as determined
by action, target, context, time, and/or individual
The Theory of Planned Behavior
differences, will be more influential on intention.
Thus, when attitudes toward a behavior are view-
While there has been strong evidence in support of
ed as more important than subjective norms for a
the TRA, many of the studies previously reviewed
certain behavior, then attitudes toward the behavior
assumed high levels of volitional control (i.e., that
will be more influential on intention than subjective
a person can willingly decide to perform or not
norms. Conversely, when subjective norms are per-
perform the behavior; see Sheppard et al. 1988;
ceived as more important than attitudes, then sub-
Albarracín et al. 2001; Hagger et al. 2002). In
jective norms will be more influential than attitudes
recognition of the fact that volitional control can
toward the behavior.
differ across different behaviors, Ajzen (1985,
1991) proposed the TPB. The TPB retains all the
External Variables key constructs comprising the TRA, but proposes
As previously noted, the TRA’s core assumption the addition of a third determinant of intentions:
is that behavior can only be influenced by inten- perceived behavioral control. The performance of
tions, which in turn are influenced by attitudes a behavior depends at least to some degree on
toward the behavior and subjective norms. Thus, one’s actual behavioral control (i.e., the ability
one somewhat controversial assumption of the to perform a behavior). As the required opportu-
TRA is that external variables can only influence nities and resources to perform a behavior (e.g.,
behavior indirectly through the influence on be- time, money, skills, cooperation of others, etc.)
havioral beliefs (which influence attitudes toward increase for an individual, the chances of the
the behavior), normative beliefs (which influence individual performing the behavior increases
subjective norms), and the relative importance of accordingly. Perceived behavioral control refers
attitudes toward the behavior and subjective norms. to people’s perception of the ease or difficulty of
There is no consistent pattern of influence for ex- performing the behavior of interest.
ternal variables on behavior, as their influence on Perceived behavioral control can influence ac-
the determinants of behavioral intention varies ac- tual behavior in two ways (see Fig. 2 for the full
ross different behaviors. External variables include TPB pathway). First, perceived behavioral control

Theory of Planned
Behavior, Fig. 2. The Attitude
Behavioral
theory of planned behavior toward the
Beliefs
Behavior

Normative Subjective
Intention Behavior
Beliefs Norms

Perceived
Control
Behavioral
Beliefs
Control
Theory of Planned Behavior 5

may influence intentions. Individuals who believe Evidence for the Theory of Planned Behavior
they have neither the resources nor opportunities A number of meta-analyses have provided strong
to perform a certain behavior are unlikely to have support for the TPB. These reviews examined the
high intentions to perform the behavior, even if TPB across various domains of behavior (Armitage
they hold a favorable attitude towards the behav- and Conner 2001), including specific areas such as
ior or if there are favorable subjective norms for health-related behaviors (Godin and Kok 1996),
the behavior. Therefore, perceived behavioral con- physical activity (Hagger et al. 2002), and condom
trol should influence intention independent of atti- use (Albarracín et al. 2001). These meta-analyses
tude toward the behavior and subjective norms. found that perceived behavioral control predicted
Second, perceived behavioral control can poten- intention above and beyond attitude toward the
tially influence behavior directly. Under situations behavior and subjective norms. Additionally,
where perceived behavioral control is an accurate some of these meta-analyses indicated a direct
indication of actual behavioral control, perceived influence of perceived behavioral control on be-
behavioral control can serve as a proxy of actual havior (e.g., Armitage and Conner 2001; Hagger
behavioral control. However, in situations where et al. 2002; McEachan et al. 2011). Furthermore,
one’s perceived behavioral control is inaccurate in control beliefs have been found to be predictive
predicting actual behavior control, then perceived of perceived behavioral control (e.g. Ajzen and
behavioral control would be unlikely to predict Madden 1986; Armitage and Conner 1999).
actual behavior directly. Perceived behavioral
control becomes an inaccurate prediction of actual Criticisms of the Theory of Planned Behavior
behavioral control when the individual has little Although the TPB has received substantial empir-
information about the behavior, when require- ical support, the TPB and the TRA have also been
ments or available resources have changed, or criticized. Two criticisms in particular have at-
when new and unfamiliar elements have entered tracted significant attention in the literature (see
into the situation. This relationship between per- Eagly and Chaiken 1993, for a review). The first
ceived behavioral control and actual behavior is criticism was one regarding the TRA, which ap-
indicated by the broken arrow in Fig. 2, as this plies to the TPB as well. Some researchers have
relationship is only expected to emerge when challenged the TRA’s assumption that the two
there is high correspondence between perceived determinants of intention in the TRA (i.e., attitude
behavioral control and actual behavioral control. toward the behavior and subjective norms) are
Similar to attitudes and subjective norms, per- sufficient to fully account for intention. Though
ceived behavioral control is also determined by its Ajzen (1985, 1991) recognized that perceived
beliefs. Control beliefs are beliefs about the pres- behavioral control was another determinant of in-
ence or absence of factors that facilitate or hinder tention, other researchers have proposed a number
performance of a behavior. These beliefs may be of other determinants of intention. Among these
predicated on past experiences with the behavior proposed determinants are past behaviors/habits
or by observation of others engaging in the behav- (Triandis 1977; Bentler and Speckart 1979;
ior. Generally, if an individual believes that he or Ouellette and Wood 1998), perceived moral obli-
she has the required resources and opportunities gation (Schwartz and Tessler 1972), self-identity
that facilitate, as well as the lack of obstacles that (Biddle et al. 1987), and affective beliefs (Triandis
impede the performance of the behavior, he or she 1977). Although many of these other determi-
should have greater levels of perceived behavior- nants have received support (see Conner and
al control. Additionally, like other types of beliefs Armitage 1998), they have not gained much ac-
in the model, external variables can sometimes ceptance with the exception of past behaviors/
influence control beliefs depending on the type habits. Strong past behaviors/habits have been
of behavior. found to sometimes outperform intentions in pre-
dicting behavior (see Ouellette and Wood 1998;
Webb and Sheeran 2006). Additionally, while it
6 Theory of Planned Behavior

may be true that the TPB’s three determinants of have indicated that automatically activated atti-
intention are strong predictors of intentions for tudes are better predictors of behavioral outcomes
behaviors in general, other determinants may be than self-reported attitudes.
just as strong or even stronger predictors for cer- Other theorists have also argued for more spon-
tain behavioral domains. taneous and less deliberative models of behavior.
The second criticism of the TPB is that it only For example, Wood and colleagues (see Wood
applies to comparatively deliberate behaviors be- and Neal 2007; Wood and Neal 2009; Wood
cause it assumes that behaviors are predominantly et al. 2014) have argued that behavioral processes
a result of rational thought (see Eagly and Chaiken are often strongly influenced by habits. In their
1993). Thus, the TPB and the TRA disregard model, contextual cues (e.g., physical locations,
spontaneous attitude-to-behavior processes. Rec- other people, internal states, preceding actions in
ognizing this deficiency in the TPB and TRA, a sequence, etc.) activate an individual’s mental
Fazio (1990; Fazio and Towles-Schwen 1999) representation of habitual response, which, in turn,
proposed a model that incorporates both delib- influences the habitual response (i.e., behavior).
erate and spontaneous attitude-to-behavior pro- Habits develop slowly through context–response
cesses: MODE (Motivation and Opportunity as learning, which can be strengthened through fre-
DEterminants) model. According to this model, quent context–response pairings, as these frequent
deliberate and spontaneous attitude-to-behavior pairings lead to a stronger associative link between
processes are determined by motivation and op- the memory representation of the contextual cues
portunity (i.e., time and resources). When an indi- and that of the response. Frequent pairings of a
vidual has both the motivation and opportunity to context with a particular response may also weaken
engage in effortful analysis of a given behavior, associations between the same context and other
he or she will likely undergo a deliberate attitude- responses.
to-behavior process. These deliberate processes Although habitual response is an automatic
are presumed to be similar to those described in process and does not depend on goals for perfor-
the TRA and the TPB (see Fazio and Roskos- mance (other than when individuals are learning
Ewoldsen 1994; Fazio and Olson 2014). context–response associations; Neal et al. 2006),
Alternatively, when an individual lacks either it can be influenced by controlled processes (see
the motivation or the opportunity to engage in ef- Quinn et al. 2010). When an individual has an
fortful analysis of an attitude, they will likely un- intention that is incompatible with a habit, he or
dergo a spontaneous attitude-to-behavior process. she could inhibit the habitual response via vigilant
A model that maps onto this spontaneous process is monitoring, which involves heightening attentional
Fazio’s (1986) attitude-to-behavior process model. focus on a response to ensure that it is not per-
There is no behavioral intention in this model; formed. Furthermore, an individual could inhibit
instead, the activated attitudes (toward the object) the habitual response through stimulus control (i.e.,
and subjective norms guide behavior through removing oneself from the situation) and/or dis-
the definition of the event. It is this definition of traction (i.e., thinking about something else).
the event that determines the direction and nature More commonly, an individual has an inten-
of the behavior, as approach behaviors typically tion that is in agreement with a habit, because
follow a positive definition of the event and avoid- many habits originate from past goal pursuits. In
ance behaviors typically follow a negative defini- these situations, habits may thoughtlessly perform
tion of the event (Fazio 1990). Additionally, the the intended behavior that initially prompted the
MODE model postulates that these two processes individual to respond repetitively. Ouellette and
can operate simultaneously. The MODE model Wood (1998) supported this notion, as they found
has been supported in the literature by a number that the frequency of past performance (i.e., habit
of studies in domains such as prejudice, relation- strength) positively correlated with behavioral
ship outcomes, and self-control behaviors (see intentions for many behaviors (r = 0.43). More-
Fazio and Olson 2014). Some of these studies over, habits have the potential to guide intention,
Theory of Planned Behavior 7

as individuals who lack access to the motivations and planned behavior as models of condom use:
and cognitions guiding behavior draw inferences A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 127(1),
142–161. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.127.1.142.
about their actions from past behavior (Wood and Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. A. (1999). Distinguishing
Neal 2007; Neal et al. 2012). For example, people perceptions of control from self-efficacy: Predicting
may reason, “I do this often, so I must like to do it.” consumption of a low-fat diet using the theory of planned
behavior 1. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 29(1),
72–90. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1999.tb01375.x.
Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. A. (2001). Efficacy of the
Conclusion theory of planned behavior: A meta-analytic review.
British Journal of Social Psychology, 40(4), 471–499.
doi:10.1348/014466601164939.
In sum, the theory of planned behavior is a theory Bentler, P. M., & Speckart, G. (1979). Models of attitude–-
used to predict and understand behaviors. It posits behavior relations. Psychological Review, 86(5),
that behaviors are immediately determined by be- 452–464. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.86.5.452.
havioral intentions, which in turn are determined Biddle, B., Bank, B, & Slavings, R. (1987). Norms, pref-
erences, identities and retention decisions. Social Psy-
by a combination of three factors: attitude toward chology Quarterly, 50(4), 322–337.
the behavior, subjective norms, and perceived be- Conner, M. A., & Armitage, C. J. (1998). Extending
havioral control. Moreover, under some circum- the theory of planned behavior: A review and avenues
stances, perceived behavioral control can directly for further research. Journal of Applied Social Psy-
chology, 28(15), 1429–1464. doi:10.1111/j.1559-181
predict behaviors. Though proponents of the the- 6.1998.tb01685.x.
ory of planned behavior have proposed that the Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of
theory predicts all behaviors, opponents of the attitudes. Orlando: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich College
theory have argued that the theory only predicts Publishers.
Fazio, R. H. (1986). How do attitudes guide behavior?
deliberate – not automatic – behaviors. Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations
of social behavior (pp. 204–243). New York, NY, US:
Guilford Press.
Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes
Cross-References
guide behavior: The MODE model as an integra-
tive framework. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in
▶ Attitudes experimental social psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 75–109).
▶ Conscious Versus Unconscious Determinants San Diego: Academic Press.
Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2014). The MODE model:
of Behavior
Attitude-behavior processes as a function of motivation
▶ Habits and opportunity. In J. W. Sherman, B. Gawronski, &
Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual process theories of the social
mind (pp. 155–171). New York: Guilford Press.
Fazio, R. H., & Roskos-Ewoldsen, D. R. (1994). Acting
References as we feel: When and how attitudes guide behavior.
In S. Shavitt & T. C. Brock (Eds.), Persuasion: Psy-
Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory chological insights and perspectives (pp. 71–93).
of planned behavior. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckman (Eds.), Needham Heights: Allyn Bacon.
Action-control: From cognition to behavior Fazio, R. H., & Towles-Schwen, T. (1999). The MODE
(pp. 11–39). Heidelberg: Springer. model of attitude-behavior processes. In S. Chaiken &
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organi- Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual process theories in social psy-
zational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, chology (pp. 97–116). New York: Guilford Press.
50(2), 179–211. doi:10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T. Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention,
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and behavior: An introduction to theory and research.
and predicting social behavior. Englewood-Cliffs: Reading: Addison-Wesley.
Prentice-Hall. Godin, G., & Kok, G. (1996). The theory of planned
Ajzen, I., & Madden, T. J. (1986). Prediction of goal- behavior: A review of its applications to health-related
directed behavior: Attitudes, intentions, and perceived behaviors. American Journal of Health Promotion,
behavioral control. Journal of Experimental Social 11(2), 87–98. doi:10.4278/0890-1171-11.2.87.
Psychology, 22(5), 453–474. doi:10.1016/0022-1031 Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Biddle, S. J. H.
(86)90045-4. (2002). A meta-analytic review of the theories of rea-
Albarracín, D., Johnson, B. T., Fishbein, M., & soned action and planned behavior in physical activity:
Muellerleile, P. A. (2001). Theories of reasoned action
8 Theory of Planned Behavior

Predictive validity and the contribution of additional Schwartz, S. H., & Tessler, R. C. (1972). A test of a model
variables. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, for reducing measured attitude-behavior discrepancies.
24(1), 3–32. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24(2),
McEachan, R. R. C., Conner, M., Taylor, N. J., & Lawton, 225–236. doi:10.1037/h0033365.
R. J. (2011). Prospective prediction of health-related Sheppard, B., Hartwick, J., & Warshaw, P. (1988). The
behaviours with the theory of planned behaviour: A meta- theory of reasoned action: A meta-analysis of past
analysis. Health Psychology Review, 5(2), 97–144. research with recommendations for modifications and
doi:10.1080/17437199.2010.521684. future research. Journal of Consumer Research, 15(3),
Neal, D. T., Wood, W., & Quinn, J. M. (2006). Habits – A 325–343.
repeat performance. Current Directions in Psychologi- Triandis, H. C. (1977). Interpersonal behavior. Monterey:
cal Science, 15(4), 198–202. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721. Brooks-Cole.
2006.00435.x. Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Does changing behav-
Neal, D. T., Wood, W., Labrecque, J. S., & Lally, P. (2012). ioral intentions engender behavior change? A meta-
How do habits guide behavior? Perceived and actual analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychological
triggers of habits in daily life. Journal of Experimental Bulletin, 132(2), 249–268. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.
Social Psychology, 48(2), 492–498. doi:10.1016/j.jesp. 132.2.249.
2011.10.011. Wood, W., & Neal, D. T. (2007). A new look at habits and
Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). the habit-goal interface. Psychological Review, 114(4),
The measurement of meaning. Urbana: University of 843–863. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.114.4.843.
Illinois Press. Wood, W., & Neal, D. T. (2009). The habitual consumer.
Ouellette, J. A., & Wood, W. (1998). Habit and intention Journal of Consumer Psychology, 19(4), 579–592.
in everyday life: The multiple processes by which doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2009.08.003.
past behavior predicts future behavior. Psychologi- Wood, W., Labrecque, J. S., Lin, P. Y., & Rünger, D.
cal Bulletin, 124(1), 54–74. doi:10.1037/0033-2909. (2014). Habits in dual process models. In J. W. Sher-
124.1.54. man, B. Gawronski, & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual process
Quinn, J. M., Pascoe, A., Wood, W., & Neal, D. T. (2010). theories of the social mind (pp. 371–385). New York:
Can’t control yourself? Monitor those bad habits. Guilford Press.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(4),
499–511. doi:10.1177/0146167209360665.

You might also like