Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Social Awareness in Monkeys

Author(s): Robert M. Seyfarth and Dorothy L. Cheney


Source: American Zoologist, Vol. 40, No. 6 (Dec., 2000), pp. 902-909
Published by: Oxford University Press
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/3884338 .
Accessed: 19/10/2014 18:28

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Oxford University Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to American
Zoologist.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 165.123.34.86 on Sun, 19 Oct 2014 18:28:45 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Amer. Zool., 40:902-909 (2000)

Social Awareness in Monkeys1

Robert M. Seyfarth2 and Dorothy L. Cheney


Departments of Psychology and Biology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 19104

Synopsis. Tests of self-awareness in nonhuman primates have to date been con-


cerned almost entirely with the recognition of an animal's reflection in a mirror.
By contrast, we know much less about non-human primates' perception of their
place within a social network, or of their understanding of themselves as individ?
uals with unique sets of social relationships. Here we review evidence that monkeys
who fail the mirror test may nonetheless behave as if they recognize themselves as
distinct individuals, each of whom occupies a unique place in society and has a
specific set of relations with others. A free-ranging vervet monkey, baboon, or
macaque recognizes other members of his group as individuals. He also recognizes
matrilineal kin groups, linear dominance rank orders, and behaves as if he rec?
ognizes his own unique place within them. This sense of "social self" in monkeys,
however, is markedly different from self-awareness in humans. Although monkeys
may behave in ways that accurately place themselves within a social network, they
are unaware of the knowledge that allows them to do so: they do not know what
they know, cannot reflect on what they know, and cannot become the object of
their own attention.

Introduction the existence of consciousness and self-


In our every day lives we take human con? awareness.
sciousness and self-awareness for granted. William James (1892/1984) was perhaps
We assume that each person is actively the first to propose that human self-con-
aware of his own body and thoughts and of sciousness, or self-awareness, has many dif?
his identity as an individual distinct from all ferent components. At the most basic level,
others. We also assume that each individual there is what James calls the "material"
is aware of the thoughts and individual iden? self, or awareness of the physical aspects of
tities of others. Common sense, experience, one's own body. It seems probable that al?
introspection, and our conversations with most all animals have an elementary rec?
others reassure us that these assumptions are ognition of their material self, in the sense
valid for humans. Unfortunately, however, that they react to painful stimuli and distin-
none of these methods is available in studies guish between sensory inputs that come
of animals. As a result, we know very little from their own bodies and sensory inputs
about whether non-human creatures are that come from elsewhere (see also Heyes,
"conscious" in the human sense, whether 1994). With the possible exception of some
they have a sense of their own identity, or of the great apes, however, most animals'
whether they have a sense of the identities sense of their material self seems to remain
of others. Lacking a method for the study of tacit, without the individual being actively
animal consciousness or self-awareness, psy- aware that the body he can see or touch is
chologists and ethologists have directed their a part of his unique self. Tests of mirror
attentions instead to behaviors and cognitive devised by Gal-
self-recognition, originally
abilities that can be studied and that may be
lup (1970) and recently extended by Povi-
correlated with, or linked in some way, to nelli et al. (1993, offer
1994), strong evi?
dence that apes, but not monkeys, recognize
1From the
Symposium Animal Consciousness: His? that the body they see in the mirror is their
torical, Theoretical, and Empirical Perspectives pre? own (see also Heyes, 1994; Tomasello and
sented at the Annual Meeting of the Society for Inte?
Call, 1997). Although it is not clear what
grative and Comparative Biology, 6-10 January 1999,
at Denver, Colorado. aspects of consciousness are reflected by
2 E-mail: tests with mirrors, they do reveal a consis-
seyfarth@psych.upenn.edu

902

This content downloaded from 165.123.34.86 on Sun, 19 Oct 2014 18:28:45 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Social Awareness in Monkeys 903

tent and qualitative difference between ness of the unique social identities of other
monkeys and apes, and they suggest that individuals (Damon and Hart, 1982; Roten-
apes may have some capacity to make bodi- berg, 1982; Gopnik and Astington, 1988;
ly self-recognition at least partially acces- Wellman, 1983; Gopnik and Meltzoff,
sible to active thought. 1994). If we accept the view that knowl?
At higher, more complex levels, James edge about one's own identity and knowl?
describes in humans the "spiritual" self, edge about the identities of others are de?
defined as one's "psychic faculties and dis- velopmentally linked in children, and fur?
positions" (1892, p. 163), and the "social" ther assume that this linkage holds for non?
self, defined as awareness of oneself as a human primates as well (see, for example,
distinct individual, embedded in a group or Gallup, 1982), then we may be able to use
society that includes many other distinct in? a monkey's knowledge of other individuals
dividuals. We define our social selves by as an indirect measure of the monkey's
reference to others; there cannot be an "I" knowledge of him or herself. Here are some
without a "you" or a "they" for compari? examples of how this might be done.
son.
In this paper we consider whether mon? Social Knowledge in Vervet Monkeys
keys have a sense of their social self as and Baboons
James defines it, and we contrast monkeys' Knowledge of other animals' kin relations
social knowledge with self-awareness in
East African vervet monkeys (Cercopi-
humans, defined as the ability to become
thecus aethiops) live in groups of 10-20
the object of one's own attention.
individuals. Each group occupies a territory
For those who propose to study self-
that is surrounded by the territories of other
awareness in animals, James' formulation
vervet groups. A typical group contains 3-
provides a useful starting point because it
4 adult males, together with 5-8 adult fe?
takes a complex issue and breaks it down
males and their offspring. When young
into component parts, some of which, like
males reach adult size at approximately 4-
an elementary sense of one's own body, are
5 yr of age, they leave the group where they
likely to be widespread throughout the an?
were born and join another, neighboring
imal kingdom, while others, like an explicit
awareness of one's own psychological dis- group. Young females remain in the group
will probably be found only in where they were born throughout their lives
positions,
humans. and form close, long-lasting bonds with
James' notion of a social self is particu? their matrilineal relatives. Adult female ver-
vets and their offspring can be arranged in
larly useful to ethologists because it links
an individual's of his own social a linear dominance hierarchy in which off?
knowledge
identity with the individual's knowledge of spring rank immediately below their moth?
the social identities of others. For James, an ers. The stable core of a vervet social
individual's self-awareness (what he thinks group, then, is a hierarchy of matrilineal
about himself) derives, at least in part, his families (Cheney and Seyfarth, 1990).
knowledge of the individual identities of Most affinitive social interactions, such
others. In studies of adult humans such an as grooming, mutual tolerance at feeding
assumption is not controversial. In young sites, and the formation of aggressive alli-
children, however, an awareness of the in? ances, occur within families (Seyfarth,
dividual identities of others and an aware? 1980; Whiten, 1983; reviewed in Cheney
ness one's own identity appear to develop and Seyfarth, 1990). Clearly, individuals
gradually (e.g., Flavell, 1985), and there is distinguish their own close matrilineal rel?
no a priori reason to assume that they de? atives from all others because their behavior
velop together. toward them is so different. For a monkey
Fortunately, however, there is now em? to achieve a complete understanding of her
pirical evidence that a child's awareness of society, however, she must be able to step
her own identity as a unique person devel? outside her own sphere of interactions and
ops at roughly the same time as her aware- recognize the relations that exist among

This content downloaded from 165.123.34.86 on Sun, 19 Oct 2014 18:28:45 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
904 R. M. Seyfarth and D. L. Cheny

others (Cheney and Seyfarth, 1986; Har- heard two control sequences of calls. The
court, 1988). Such knowledge can only be first sequence mimicked a fight involving
obtained by observing interactions in which the dominant subject's relative and an in?
oneself is not involved and making the ap? dividual unrelated to either female; the sec?
propriate deductions (Cheney and Seyfarth, ond mimicked a fight involving two indi?
1990). There is, in fact, growing evidence viduals who were both unrelated to either
that monkeys do possess knowledge of oth? female (for details see Cheney and Sey?
er animals' social relationships and that farth, 1999).
such knowledge affects their behavior. After hearing the test sequence, a signif?
Evidence that vervet monkeys recognize icant number of subjects looked toward the
other animals' social relations first emerged other female, suggesting that they recog?
as part of a relatively simple playback ex? nized not just the calls of unrelated individ?
periment designed to document individual uals, but also those individuals' kin (or
recognition by voice (Cheney and Seyfarth, close associates). Moreover, in the minutes
1980). We began by noting that, when in? following playback, dominant subjects were
fant and juvenile vervets screamed during significantly more likely to supplant sub-
rough play, their mothers often ran to sup? ordinate subjects, suggesting that the dom?
port them. This observation, like many oth? inant female's behavior toward others was
er studies (e.g., Hansen, 1976; Gouzoules influenced by her perception of whether one
et al, 1984) suggested that mothers recog? of her own relatives and another individu-
nized the calls of their offspring. To test this al's relative had recently been involved in
hypothesis, we waited until three vervet fe? a fight. Females' responses following the
males were sitting close enough together to test sequence differed significantly from
be filmed, and then from a concealed speak? their responses following control sequenc?
er played the distress scream of one of the es. Following the first control sequence,
female's two year-old offspring. As expect? when only the dominant subject's relative
ed, mothers consistently looked toward or appeared to be involved in the fight, only
approached the loudspeaker for longer du? the subordinate subject looked at her part?
rations than did control females. Even be? ner. Following the second control sequence,
fore she had responded, however, a signif? when neither of the subjects' relatives was
icant number of control females looked at involved, neither subject looked at the oth?
the mother. They behaved as if they rec? er. Finally, following both control sequenc?
ognized the close social bonds that existed es, the two subjects were significantly more
between particular juveniles and particular likely to approach each other and interact
adult females (Cheney and Seyfarth, 1980, in a friendly manner than following the test
1982). sequence.
In an attempt to replicate these results, Taken together, these experiments argue
we recently carried out a similar set of ex? that baboon and vervet monkeys recognize
periments on free-ranging baboons (Papio the members of their group as distinct in?
cynocephalus ursinus) in the Okavango dividuals and classify these individuals into
Delta of Botswana (for details of the study what we call matrilineal families based on
area and subjects, see Hamilton et al their associations with each other.
(1976). The social organization of baboons Other studies provide additional evidence
is similar to that of vervets. In these exper? of monkeys' abilities to distinguish both
iments, two unrelated females were played their own and other individuals' close as?
a sequence of calls that mimicked a fight sociates. For example, in a playback study
between each of their close relatives. The using the contact calls of rhesus macaques
females' immediate responses to the play? (Macaca mulatta), Rendall et al. (1996)
back were videotaped, and both subjects found that females not only distinguish the
were also followed for 15 min after the identities of different signallers but also cat-
playback to determine whether their behav? egorize signallers according to matrilineal
ior was affected by the calls they had heard. kinship. Similarly, in an experiment per?
In separate trials, the same two subjects also formed on captive long-tailed macaques

This content downloaded from 165.123.34.86 on Sun, 19 Oct 2014 18:28:45 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Social Awareness in Monkeys 905

(Macaca fascicularis), Dasser (1988) was more likely to threaten another individ?
trained a female subject to choose between ual if one of her own close relatives and
slides of one mother-offspring pair and one of her opponent's close relatives had
slides of two unrelated individuals. Having recently been involved in a fight (Fig. 1; see
been trained to respond to one mother-off? Cheney and Seyfarth, 1986, 1989). These
spring pair, the subject was then tested with results support Dasser's (1988) contention
14 novel slides of different mothers and off? that monkeys recognize that certain types of
spring paired with an equal number of nov? social relationships share similar character?
el pairs of unrelated animals. In all tests, istics. When a vervet monkey (A2 in Fig.
she correctly selected the mother-offspring 1) threatens B2 following a fight between
pair. In so doing, she appeared to use an one of her own relatives (Al) and one of
abstract category to classify pairs of indi? her opponent's relatives (Bl), A2 acts as if
viduals that was analogous to our concept she recognizes that the relationship between
of "mother-child affiliation." B2 and Bl is in some way similar to her
In each of these studies, animals that own relationship with Al (Cheney and Sey?
were grouped into familial associations farth, 1990). In a similar manner, when a
nonetheless retained their individual iden? baboon female hears a playback sequence
tities: a mother and her offspring, for ex? mimicking a fight between her own relative
ample, were judged to be alike in belonging and the relative of another female, this tem-
to the same family but still recognized as porarily increases the likelihood that her
distinct individuals. It appears, therefore, subsequent interactions with that female
that monkeys have a sophisticated knowl? will be antagonistic (Cheney and Seyfarth,
edge of the social identities, or social 1999).
selves, of others: they recognize others not In order to selectively target a relative of
only as individuals but also as individuals her own relative's opponent, however, a fe?
that occupy particular places in the social male vervet or baboon must have some rec?
group. If we accept the view that an aware? ognition of the distinction between her own
ness of oneself as an individual is linked to social relationships and the social relation?
an awareness of others as individuals, then ships of others. This would seem to require
it seems possible that baboons, vervets, and that she perceive herself as a distinct social
macaques may also have some understand? being, one who occupies a particular place
ing of their own social identities or social in a society that is defined according to a
selves, in the sense that they recognize their specific set of behavioral relations with cer?
position and status in the social group. tain other individuals.
In many species of monkeys, an individ?
*
ual who has just threatened or been threat- Knowledge of other animals dominance
ened by another animal will often 'redirect ranks
aggression' by threatening a third, previ? A similar phenomenon may underlie
ously uninvolved, individual. Judge (1982) monkeys' knowledge of their relative place
was the first to note that redirected aggres? in a linear dominance hierarchy. As already
sion does not always occur at random; rath? noted, individuals in many species of Old
er than simply threatening any nearby in? World monkeys can be arranged in a linear
dividual, animals will instead specifically dominance hierarchy that accurately pre?
target a close matrilineal relative of their dicts the direction of competitive interac?
recent opponent. Similar kin-biased redi? tions between any two individuals. A linear
rected aggression occurs in Japanese ma? rank order might be indicative of an ability
caques (Macaca fuscata) (Aureli et al, to recognize transitive relations among oth?
1992) and vervets (Cheney and Seyfarth, er group members. It is also possible, how?
1986, 1989). ever, that monkeys simply recognize who is
Kin-biased redirected aggression also ap? dominant or subordinate to themselves. In
pears in more complex forms. In two dif? the latter case, a linear hierarchy could
ferent vervet groups studied over two dif? emerge as the incidental outcome of paired
ferent time periods, we found that a female interactions. The hierarchy would be a

This content downloaded from 165.123.34.86 on Sun, 19 Oct 2014 18:28:45 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
906 R. M. Seyfarth and D. L. Cheny

Fig. 1. Redirected aggression in vervet monkeys. Nonkin were significantly more Likely to act aggressively
toward an opponent after a fight between their own relatives And their oppenent's relatives than during matched
control periods. Data from Cheney and Seyfarth (1989); drawing by John Watanabe from Cheney and Seyfarth
(1990).

product of the human mind, not the minds consistent call sequence in which a lower-
of the monkeys themselves. ranking female apparently grunted to a
There is evidence, however, that mon? higher-ranking female and the higher-rank-
keys do recognize the rank relations that ex? ing female apparently responded with fear
ist among others in their group. For exam? barks. As a control, the same subjects heard
ple, dominant female baboons often grunt the same sequence of grunts and fear barks
to mothers with infants as they approach the made causally consistent by the inclusion of
mothers and attempt to handle or touch additional grunts from a third female who
their infants. The grants seem to function was dominant to both of the others. For ex?
to facilitate social interactions by appeasing ample, if the inconsistent sequence was
anxious mothers, because an approach ac? composed of female 6's grunts followed by
companied by a grunt is significantly more female 2's fear barks, the corresponding
likely to lead to subsequent friendly inter? consistent sequence might begin with fe?
action than is an approach without a grunt male l's grunts, followed by female 6's
(Cheney et al, 1995a). Occasionally, how? grunts and ending with female 2's fear
ever, a mother will utter a submissive call, barks. Subjects responded significantly
or "fear bark," as a dominant female ap? more strongly to the causally inconsistent
proaches. Fear barks are an unambiguous sequences, suggesting that they recognize
indication of subordination; they are never not only the factors that cause one individ?
given to lower-ranking females. To test ual to give submissive vocalizations to an?
whether baboons recognize that only a other, but also the rank relations that exist
more dominant animal can cause another among others in their group (Cheney et al,
individual to give a fear bark, we designed 19951?).
a playback experiment in which adult fe? Further evidence that monkeys recognize
males subjects were played a causally in- other individuals' ranks comes from cases

This content downloaded from 165.123.34.86 on Sun, 19 Oct 2014 18:28:45 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Social Awareness in Monkeys 907

Fig. 2. Competition over access to a grooming partner in vervet monkeys. When a high-ranking female (for
example, female 2) approached two lower-ranking females (for example, females 4 and 5) and supplanted one
individual and groomed the other, of the two females who were approached the lower-ranking was significantly
more likely to be supplanted. Drawing by John Watanabe and data from Cheney and Seyfarth (1990).

in which adult female vervet monkeys com- their size, or the number of objects con?
pete with one another for access to a tained within an array (e.g., Hauser et al,
grooming partner (Seyfarth, 1980). Such 1996; Brannon and Terrace, 1998). What
competition occurs whenever one female distinguishes the social example, however,
approaches two that are grooming, sup- is the fact that, even in the absence of hu?
plants one of them, and then grooms with man training, female monkeys seem able to
the female that remains. In a small propor? construct a rank hierarchy and then place
tion of cases, this competition takes a form themselves at the appropriate location with?
that is especially interesting for our present in it.
purposes. As shown in Figure 2, a high-
Discussion
ranking female (ranked 2, for example) ap?
proaches two groomers who are both sub- Tests of self-awareness in nonhuman pri?
ordinate to herself (say, females ranked 4 mates have to date been concerned almost
and 5). Though 4 and 5 both rank lower entirely with the recognition of an animal's
than 2, they are not equally likely to depart. reflection in a mirror. As Tomasello and
In a significant number of cases, the higher- Call point out, however, "Mirror self-rec-
ranking of the two females remains seated, ognition is . . . about perception of the
while the lower-ranking of the two moves body" (1997, p. 337). By contrast, we
away (Cheney and Seyfarth, 1990). know much less about non-human pri?
In so doing, the higher-ranking of the two mates' perception of their place within a so?
females acts as if she recognizes that, al? cial network, or of their understanding of
though she is lower-ranking than the ap- themselves as individuals with unique sets
proaching female, her grooming partner is of social relationships.
even more subordinate. In order to accom- Although monkeys consistently fail the
plish this ranking, a female must know not mirror test of self-recognition, under natural
only her own status relative to other indi? conditions they seem to recognize their own
viduals but also other individuals' status close associates and the close associates of
relative to each other. In other words, she others. They also seem to recognize their
must recognize a rank hierarchy (Cheney own and other individuals' dominance
and Seyfarth, 1990). ranks. They behave as if they recognize
The ability to rank other group members their own unique place in a network of so?
is perhaps not surprising, given the evi? cial relations. In other words, they behave
dence that captive monkeys and apes can as if they have what William James might
be taught to rank objects according to an have called a sense of their "social selves."
arbitrary sequential order (D'Amato and This kind of knowledge, however, is very
Columbo, 1988), the amount of food con? different from the conscious self-awareness
tained within a container (Gillan, 1981), exhibited by humans. By the time they are

This content downloaded from 165.123.34.86 on Sun, 19 Oct 2014 18:28:45 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
908 R. M. Seyfarth and D. L. Cheny

about four years of age, humans are explic- propriate position in a network of social re?
itly aware of the thoughts, beliefs, and men? lationships. The acquisition of this knowl?
tal states, both of others and of themselves. edge, however, does not require active self-
In contrast, all the evidence gathered to date reflection or awareness. Indeed, it demands
suggests that monkeys cannot attribute only observational learning and a sensitiv?
mental states to others and are unaware of ity to complex behavioral contingencies.
their own knowledge (reviewed by Cheney Although monkeys' knowledge of social re?
and Seyfarth, 1990; Tomasello and Call, lationships may exceed that of many other
1997). The inability to examine their own social mammals, they apparently achieve
knowledge or to recognize the mental states such knowledge without reflecting actively
of others (what Premack and Woodruff upon their behavior, examining their own
(1978) termed the lack of a "theory of knowledge, or conceiving of themselves as
mind") means that, when monkeys interact social agents. They place themselves within
with other group members, their under? a social network without being aware of do-
standing of those individuals' social rela? ing so.
tionships and behavior derives primarily
from what they have observed those indi? ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
viduals do in the past. Monkeys can use this Research on vervet and ba?
monkeys
information to predict another animal's be? boons has been supported by grants from
havior, but they have little understanding of NSF, NIH, The National Geographic Soci?
the motives, belief, or knowledge that ety, and the University of Pennsylvania.
caused it to occur.
Similarly, if knowledge of self and References
knowledge of others develop in parallel,
Anderson, J. R. 1984. The development of self-rec?
then monkeys' apparent ability to place ognition: A review. Develop. Psychobiol. 17:35-
themselves within a social network may 49.
also derive primarily from learned behav? Aureli, F, R. Cozzolino, C. Cordischi, and S. Scucchi.
ioral contingencies. 1992. Kin-oriented redirection among Japanese
Although monkeys
macaques: An expression of a revenge system?
may behave as if they recognize their own Anim. Behav. 44:283-291.
relative ranks and kin relations, they are Brannon, E. M. and H. S. Terrace. 1998. Ordering of
probably not be aware of the knowledge the numerosities 1 to 9 by monkeys. Science 282:
that allows them to do so: they do not know 746-749.
what they know, cannot reflect on what they Cheney, D. L. and R. M. Seyfarth. 1980. Vocal rec?
ognition in free-ranging vervet monkeys. Anim.
know, and therefore cannot become the ob- Behav. 28:362-367.
ject of their own attention. Cheney, D. L. and R. M. Seyfarth. 1982. Recognition
In conclusion, monkeys appear to view of individuals within and between groups of free-
their social groups not just in terms of the ranging vervet monkeys. Amer. Zool. 22:519-
529.
individuals that comprise them but also in
Cheney, D. L. and R. M. Seyfarth. 1986. The recog?
terms of a web of social relationships in nition of social alliances among vervet monkeys.
which certain individuals are linked with Anim. Behav. 34:1722-1731.
particular others. Their behavior is influ? Cheney, D. L. and R. M. Seyfarth. 1989. Reconcilia-
tion and redirected aggression in vervet monkeys,
enced not only by their own interactions
Cercopithecus aethiops. Behaviour 110:477-486.
with others but also by their observation of Cheney, D. L. and R. M. Seyfarth. 1990. How monkeys
interactions in which they are not them? see the world. University of Chicago Press, Chi?
selves involved. As a result, they recognize cago.
not only their own relative ranks and kin- Cheney, D. L. and R. M. Seyfarth. 1999. Recognition
of other individuals' social relationships by ba?
ship relations but also the relative ranks and boons. Anim. Behav. (In press)
kinship relations of others. Perhaps more Cheney, D. L., R. M. Seyfarth, and J. B. Silk. 1995a.
important, monkeys seem able to integrate The role of grunts in reconciling opponents and
information about their own social relations facilitating interactions among adult female ba?
with information boons. Anim. Behav. 50:249-257.
about the social relations
Cheney, D. L., R. M. Seyfarth, and J. B. Silk. 19956.
that exist among others, and through such The responses of female baboons (Papio cynoce-
computations place themselves at the ap- phalus ursinus) to anomalous social interactions:

This content downloaded from 165.123.34.86 on Sun, 19 Oct 2014 18:28:45 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Social Awareness in Monkeys 909

Evidence for causal reasoning? J. Comp. Psychol. humans, pp. 132-152. Oxford University Press,
109:134-141. Oxford.
D'Amato, M. and M. Colombo. 1988. Representation Hauser, M. D., P. MacNeilage, and K. E. Ware. 1996.
of serial order in monkeys (Cebus apella). J. Exp. Numerical representations in primates. Proc. Nat.
Psychol. Anim. Behav. Proc. 14:131-139. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93:1514-1517.
Damon, W. and D. Hart. 1982. The development of Heyes, C. M. 1994. Reflections oneself-recognition in
self-understanding from infancy through adoles- primates. Anim. Behav. 47:909-919.
cence. Child Develop. 53:841-864. James, W. 1982. Psychology: Briefer course. Harvard
Dasser, V. 1988. A social concept in Java monkeys. University Press, Cambridge, MA.
Anim. Behav. 36:225-230. Judge, P. 1982. Redirection of aggression based on kin-
Flavell, J. H. 1985. Cognitive development. 2nd ed. ship in a captive group of pigtail macaques. Int.
J. Primatol. 4:346.
Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.
Povinelli, D. J., A. B. Rulf, K. R. Landau, and D. T.
Gallup, G. 1970. Chimpanzees: Self-recognition. Sci? Bierschwale. 1993. Self-recognition in chimpan?
ence 167:86-87. zees (Pan troglodytes): Distribution, ontogeny,
Gallup, G. 1982. Self-awareness and the emergence of and patterns of emergence. J. Comp. Psychol. 107:
mind in primates. Am. J. Primatol. 2:237-248. 347-372.
Gillan, D. J. 1981. Reasoning in the chimpanzee. II. Povinelli, D. J., A. B. Rulf, and D. T. Bierschwale.
Transitive inference. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Be? 1994. Absence of knowledge attribution and self-
hav. Proc. 7:150-164. recognition in young chimpanzes (Pan troglo?
Gopnik, A. and J. W. Astington. 1988. Children's un? dytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 108:74-80.
derstanding of representational change and its re? Premack, D. and G. Woodruff. 1978. Does the chim?
lation to the understanding of false belief and the panzee have a theory of mind? Behav. Brain Sci.
appearance-reality distinction. Child Develop. 59: 1:515-526.
26-37. Rendall, D., P. S. Rodman, and R. E. Emond. 1996.
Gopnik, A. and A. N. Meltzoff. 1994. Minds, bodies, Vocal recognition of individuals and kin in free-
and persons: Young children's understanding of ranging rhesus monkeys. Anim. Behav. 51:1007-
the self and others as reflected in imitation and 1015.
theory of mind research. In S. T. Parker, R. W. Rotenberg, K. J. 1982. Development of character con?
Mitchell, and M. Boccia (eds.), Self awareness in stancy of self and others. Child Develop. 53:505-
animals and humans, pp. 166-186. Cambridge 515.
University Press, Cambridge. Seyfarth, R. M. 1980. The distribution of grooming
and related behaviors among adult female vervet
Gouzoules, S., H. Gouzoules, and P Marler. 1984.
Rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) screams: Rep? monkeys. Anim. Behav. 28:798-813.
resentational signalling in the recruitment of ag? Tomasello, M. and J. Call. 1997. Primate cognition.
Oxford University Press, Oxford.
onistic aid. Anim. Behav. 32:182-193.
Wellman, H. 1993. Early understanding of mind: The
Hansen, E. W. 1976. Selective responding by recently normal case. In: S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flus-
separated juvenile rhesus monkeys to the calls of berg, and D. J. Cohen (eds.), Understanding other
their mothers. Dev. Psychobiol. 9:83-88. minds: Perspectives from autism, pp. 10-39. Ox?
Harcourt, A. H. 1988. Alliances in contests and social ford University Press, Oxford.
intelligence. In R. W. Byrne and A. Whiten (eds.), Whitten, P. L. 1983. Diet and dominance among fe?
Machiavellian intelligence: Social expertise and male vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops).
the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and Am. J. Primatol. 5:139-159.

This content downloaded from 165.123.34.86 on Sun, 19 Oct 2014 18:28:45 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like