Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 16

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/249285919

The Omnivore Thesis Revisited: Voracious Cultural Consumers

Article  in  European Sociological Review · December 2006


DOI: 10.1093/esr/jcl024

CITATIONS READS
141 684

2 authors:

Oriel Sullivan Tally Katz-Gerro


University College London The University of Manchester
54 PUBLICATIONS   2,282 CITATIONS    66 PUBLICATIONS   2,222 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Intergenerational transmission of environmental behaviors View project

Cultural Cosmopolitanism in Comparative Perspective View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Tally Katz-Gerro on 07 July 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


European Sociological Review VOLUME 23 NUMBER 2 2007 123–137 123
DOI:10.1093/esr/jcl024, available online at www.esr.oxfordjournals.org
Online publication 13 December 2006

The Omnivore Thesis Revisited:


Voracious Cultural Consumers
Oriel Sullivan and Tally Katz-Gerro

We augment measures of cultural omnivorousness, based theoretically on the breadth


of cultural tastes, with a new but related dimension of voraciousness. This reflects
a ‘quantitative’ dimension of leisure consumption based upon both the range and the
frequency of leisure participation. Voraciousness is theoretically interpreted in relation to
notions of cultural repertoires, to the changing pace of work and leisure in late modernity,
and to the ‘insatiable’ quality of contemporary consumption. From British time use data,
voraciousness proved to share many relationships found in the analysis of omnivorousness,
for example, with educational qualifications and job’s social status. Moreover, these
relationships persisted over time irrespective of individuals’ time and money resources.
Since voraciousness is associated with high status individuals, and since it is not primarily
about the availability of time or money, we argue that it is a symbolic status marker
associated with notions such as being harried, keeping busy, multitasking, and embracing
a diverse cultural consumption pattern.

Introduction to that of cultural tastes expands the study of


omnivorousness as a phenomenon related not only
In this article we contribute to the literature on to the sociology of culture and consumption but also
cultural omnivorousness by introducing to the discus- to the sociology of work and time, since the money
sion a new but related dimension of leisure consump- and time commitments involved in actual leisure
tion. Since the concept of omnivorousness was first participation (as opposed to reports of cultural tastes
defined by Peterson and Simkus (1992), research on it or preferences) entail considerations of money and
has rested theoretically on the way cultural capital time management. Thus we are able to combine two
involves an appreciation of a wide range of cultural strands of research, the first of which pertains to the
forms, including the fine arts, popular culture, and folk familiar fields of the sociology of culture and
culture. Accordingly, measures of omnivorousness have consumption and highlights concepts such as culture,
been primarily based on the breadth of cultural tastes. tastes, and distinction (Bourdieu, 1984; Peterson,
However, another important dimension of cultural 2005). The second strand, less visible in the omniv-
consumption relates to the frequency of participation orous literature to date, pertains to time and to
in relevant leisure activities. While there is a growing the changing pace of life and leisure in late modernity
literature on omnivorousness within the sociology of (Linder, 1970; Garhammer, 1998; Gershuny, 2000).
culture, there has been no previous research that We develop these arguments and present empir-
attempts to theoretically locate the frequency of leisure ical analyses that identify and classify a measure of
participation in relation to cultural omnivorousness. the range and frequency of leisure participation
The addition of the dimension of cultural participation (‘voraciousness’), in relation to the socio-economic

ß The Author 2006. Published by Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oxfordjournals.org
124 SULLIVAN AND KATZ-GERRO

and cultural indicators that have been commonly probed (Bihagen and Katz-Gerro, 2000; Han, 2003;
used in the analysis of omnivorousness. We show a Katz-Gerro, 2002, 2004, 2006). Specifically, research on
consistent pattern over time in these relationships, and the omnivore type generally finds that younger age,
we situate our findings within the theoretical discus- higher education, higher income, and higher occupa-
sion of cultural repertoires and cultural boundaries. tional status are strongly associated with omnivorous
cultural preferences (Warde et al., 1999; Van Eijck,
2001; López Sintas and Garcia Álvarez, 2002; Holbrook
The Omnivore Thesis et al., 2002; Emmison, 2003).
In a series of influential articles, Peterson and his
colleagues (Peterson, 1992, 1997; Peterson and Simkus, Revisiting Omnivorousness
1992; Peterson and Kern, 1996) reformulated the link
between cultural capital and social boundaries. They The general goal of the research described here was to
argued that members of the upper class in the US, who problematize, revisit, and modify the contours of the
formerly had been defined as cultural snobs in terms omnivore concept, through complementing it with a
of their preference for a limited range of highbrow measure based on both the range and the frequency of
cultural tastes (Levine, 1998), are turning into cultural cultural participation. Our reconsideration of omniv-
omnivores; namely, an upper class that experiences orousness stems from three sources. First, Bourdieu’s
and appreciates a variety of cultural tastes: highbrow, (1984) emphasis on the differences in the way
middlebrow, and lowbrow. The omnivore thesis stands individuals consume rather than on what they
in contrast to Bourdieu’s formulation of homology consume led us to look more closely at the (voracious)
in cultural stratification, that upper class groups pattern of consumption in addition to the (omniv-
show preference for highbrow culture while lower orous) contents of consumption. This emphasis stems
class groups prefer lowbrow culture. The omnivore from a perspective that sees the expression of cultural
thesis also differs from accounts of cultural consump- capital as embodied in consumer actions (Holt, 1997).
tion as an aspect of lifestyles that is fluid, flexible, and Holt asserts that research on the association between
transient, in the sense that consumption patterns are cultural capital and social divisions should rely less
individualized (Giddens, 1991), elective, do not reflect on cultural contents and more on consumption
well-established communities, and involve short-term practices. This is because the content of objectified
commitments (Slater, 1997). cultural capital has become a relatively weak exclu-
The snob versus omnivore thesis caught the atten- sionary mechanism due to increasingly blurred cultural
tion of a number of scholars who demonstrated its hierarchies in contemporary societies.
applicability and its variations in diverse cultural fields Campbell (1987) had earlier linked the idea of the
in different countries (e.g. Bryson, 1997; Van Rees insatiable consumer to the unique dynamic character
et al., 1999; Warde et al., 1999; Holbrook et al., 2002; of modern-day consumption. High levels of individ-
López Sintas and Garcia Álvarez, 2002; Emmison, ual consumption in contemporary developed societies
2003). These works characterized omnivorousness in stem from the insatiability of consumers (the fact that
sundry ways based either on cultural tastes (particu- their wants appear never to be exhaustible). Hedonistic
larly musical genres: Peterson and Simkus, 1992; consumers are unlimited in their appetites for intense
Peterson and Kern, 1996; Bryson, 1997; Emmison, emotional experiences (Bell, 1979) and their focus of
2003, and reading: Van Rees et al., 1999) or on cultural desire is the experience the consumer anticipates of the
behaviour (particularly leisure activities: López Sintas act of consumption (McCracken, 1990). If this desire
and Garcia Álvarez, 2002; Holbrook et al., 2002, were provided not by leisure participation in itself
but also eating habits: Warde et al., 1999). but by anticipation of a novel leisure activity, we
They then looked at the social bases of the would expect modern consumerism to involve a high
omnivorous type. In line with several major studies turnover of goods and experiences, or, in our terms,
that have long demonstrated a strong correlation a voracious pattern of cultural participation. In a
between economic class and patterns of cultural different but related account of cultural behaviour,
consumption and lifestyle (Weber, 1974; Gans, 1974; Schulze (1992) offers a perspective in which cultural
Bourdieu, 1984), several studies in recent years, while repertoires are characterized by the aesthetics of
acknowledging the importance of this relationship, also excitement (‘Spannungschema’). For Schulze, it is self-
argued that the complexity of the connections between realization that drives the cultural preferences of the
class location and cultural tastes should be further affluent middle class, who engage in various highbrow,
THE OMNIVORE THESIS REVISITED: VORACIOUS CULTURAL CONSUMERS 125

popular, and folk cultural genres, providing a mix of or dining out (for examples see Warde et al., 1999;
cultural experiences. Highbrow culture is, according to Van Rees et al., 1999; López Sintas and Garcia Álvarez,
Schulze, related to the experience of transcendence, 2002). However, some of these previous attempts to
popular culture is related to the experience of fun and measure omnivorousness based upon consumption
pleasure, and folk culture is related to the experience practices have tended to elide the original meaning
of incorporation and belonging. The three realms act of omnivorousness (a broad range of cultural tastes)
as organizing principles in the utilization of culture for with the type and frequency of participation in different
this class. activities. In this article we insist upon preserving the
In many ways our depiction of a voracious cultural original meaning of omnivorousness, distinguishing
consumer also resonates with Swidler’s tool kit (1986) it from the measure of voraciousness, which is based
or cultural repertoire (2001) metaphor. Swidler argues on the frequency of participation in specific cultural
that a person’s cultural repertoire works like a tool kit, activities.
playing a dominant role in structuring strategies for Additionally, most empirical studies of omnivorous-
action. It is a repertoire of habits, skills, and styles ness have used standard survey research methods based
from which people construct their understanding on questionnaire measures (Peterson, 2005). In our
of the world and how to conduct themselves in it. study we employed time use data—to the best of
Similarly, a voracious cultural consumer feels comfor- our knowledge not used before in the analysis
table with switching and constantly making choices of omnivorousness—to investigate the relationship of
between activities, which s/he engages in for brief status, time and participation in cultural activities.
periods. In terms of orientation to time, this pattern The final issue relates to time pressure and the need
implies a polychronic (multi-tasking) ‘timestyle’ to take time into account when describing cultural
(Feldman and Hornik, 1981).1 A polychronic orienta- participation. A fast pace of life is a feature of modern
tion to time, in the context of consumer behaviour, and post-modern societies (Toffler, 1970; Shaw, 1998).
means avoidance of immersion in one specific con- Findings in the literature link economic development
sumer activity and a preference for acting on a whim with time congestion and reduced opportunity for
or hopping back and forth from one activity to leisure on the one hand, and a shift of time use from
another (Cotte et al., 2004). production to consumption on the other (Gershuny,
The second issue in revisiting omnivorousness, 2000). An early perspective on the way individuals
which is closely related to the theoretical discussion deal with modern society that emphasized the rhythm
above, is a consideration of measurement. In this of time was offered by Simmel (1950; 1957). Simmel
research we decided to employ time use data on leisure emphasized the tempo and multiplicity of economic,
participation; this was in response to research that has occupational, and social life in the urban setting,
drawn attention to the fact that omnivorousness is in which individuals experience many contacts, short
composed of several different aspects, not all of which contacts, and a quicker tempo. In a setting character-
have received the same expression in measurement ized by fluctuations and discontinuities, individuals
(Warde et al., 2000; Emmison, 2003; Peterson, 2005). assert a personality by adopting habits, styles, and
Van Eijck (2000) outlined three such aspects used for fashion that make them stand out. Rosa (2003) claims
measuring the omnivorous concept: musical tastes; that to understand the nature and character of moder-
attendance at ‘high arts’ events; and types of leisure nity and the logic of its structural and cultural
activity patterns. In their early studies Peterson and developments we should add a temporal perspective
Simkus (1992) and Peterson and Kern (1996) mea- to social analysis. He uses the term social acceleration
sured omnivorousness using tastes in musical genres, to describe a cardinal change in temporal patterns
and this has continued to be an important area whereby acceleration of the pace of life becomes the
of research (see, for example, Bryson, 1996, 1997; overriding principle for individuals in late modernity.
van Eijck, 2001). Subsequently, other scholars too used Within accelerated society, speed becomes an impera-
cultural preferences to operationalize omnivorousness tive, and time compression and the intensification of
(Bryson, 1997; Emmison, 2003). Yet, while several processes are necessary.
authors have emphasized the importance of measuring In his discussion of the ‘harried leisure class’ Staffan
what people report doing rather than relying on Linder was one of the first to identify the change that
self-reported cultural tastes (Van Rees et al., 1999; has occurred over time in the association of work,
López Sintas and Garcia Álvarez, 2002), fewer studies leisure and class (Linder, 1970; Sullivan and Gershuny,
have chosen to look at actual behaviour in relation to 2001). At the turn of the nineteenth century, when
leisure activity participation, such as arts attendance Veblen (1994[1899]) was writing about conspicuous
126 SULLIVAN AND KATZ-GERRO

consumption among the rising professional classes, the higher-class groups. This is designated by a cultural
distribution of leisure time was markedly different change from ‘honour’ attached to large amounts of
from today. As an example, we can compare the leisure time to ‘honour’ attached to ‘busyness’ at work
perhaps apocryphal image of the ‘bankers’ lunch—a (Jacobs and Gerson, 2004; Gershuny, 2005). Could a
long, alcoholic midday indulgence—with the image of fast pace of out-of-home leisure participation also be
the present-day investment banker or stockbroker, conceived of as a new marker of status distinction?
frenetically busy on several telephones for the entire According to this hypothesis, the ‘tasting’ of many
day and much of the night. The current and growing different out-of-home leisure activities with a fast
association between high (earned) income and time turnover would imply a kind of ‘‘multi-cultural
scarcity has already been well documented (e.g. Jacobs capital’’ (Bryson, 1997) of leisure. This argument is
and Gerson, 1998; Gershuny, 2000; Sullivan and consonant with other claims in the literature that some
Gershuny, 2004). Evidence of the working hours of high-status groups increasingly work more but also
better-qualified and high-income earners suggests that consume more (Schor, 1992). This indicates in such
as qualification levels rise, so do hours of work groups a fast tempo of life and a pattern of high
(Robinson and Godbey, 1997; Gershuny, 2000). turnover of cultural experiences, combining a high
The connection between this literature and vora- capacity to spend, little free time, and a consump-
ciousness is that one solution, which has been tion profile that aims at exhibiting a diverse cultural
suggested for the increasing scarcity of time among repertoire.
certain groups, is an increase in intensity of activities, The theoretical considerations discussed here suggest
at work and at leisure alike. This increase in intensity the development of a complementary measure to
involves more activities being done simultaneously, omnivorousness, which combines an assessment of
but it may also involve shorter spells being spent the range of activities (to reflect a diverse cultural
on each activity, so that the sequence becomes more repertoire) with the frequency of participation
crowded, but also more fragmented, leading to ever (to characterize a high turnover of activities). In this
heavier feelings of time pressure (e.g. Shaw, 1998; article we introduce the concept of voraciousness as
Bittman and Wajcman, 2000; Bittman, 2002; Mattingly a theoretical variation of omnivorousness. Time use
and Bianchi, 2003; Southerton, 2003; Katz-Gerro and data are applied to provide a measure based on the
Sullivan, 2004). Individuals feel pressured to reduce reported frequency of participation in different leisure
process time in their public and their private lives, and activities rather than on tastes. This measure reflects
they respond, among other things, by increasingly a ‘quantitative’ dimension of leisure consumption
compressing, fragmenting, and compartmentalizing based not only on the breadth of cultural tastes
time (Southerton, 2003). For example, Hochschild (as in omnivorousness) but also on the frequency of
(1997) has argued that one of the corollaries of the participation in different leisure activities. By combin-
‘time bind’ for dual-career couples is that individuals ing this measure with the literature on time pressure
apply a pattern of workplace temporal organization to and the pace of leisure in modern societies we are
their leisure time by fragmenting and re-sequencing it. able to readdress the concept of omnivorousness with
In a study that reformulates Bourdieu’s theory a theoretical emphasis on how people consume rather
of taste and applies it in an American context, Holt than on what they consume.
(1997, 1998) also acknowledges the important link
among work, leisure and taste, and raises the question
of status. He argues that for individuals with high
Method
cultural capital, the orientation to leisure mirrors their
The Data
approach to work. Individuals with high cultural
capital have careers characterized by an emphasis on Our data comprised the first wave of ‘Home OnLine’,
symbolic analysis (e.g. the necessity to synthesize and a panel study undertaken by the Institute for Social
manipulate information). Since the cultural skills of and Economic Research at the University of Essex,
symbolic analysis are required for occupational success, England, of adult individuals in households in Britain.2
they play a role in class reproduction and put demands The first wave was conducted between October and
on busy individuals to acquire them through intense December (inclusive) of 1998, and was selected accord-
involvement in leisure activities. In fact Linder (1970) ing to a qualified form of randomization, which
suggested that to be ‘harried’ (to have too little time ensures inclusion of geographically clustered areas
and too much to do) might be a new form of status with representation of different social strata similar
distinction because of its growing association with to that of the population. Selection of households was
THE OMNIVORE THESIS REVISITED: VORACIOUS CULTURAL CONSUMERS 127

random within these areas, with an overrepresentation of retrospective recall or respondents’ estimations
of homes with personal computers. Counterweights of their ‘usual’ behaviour (see Juster, 1985; Robinson,
were included in the dataset to reproduce the expected 1985; Kalfs, 1993). A further advantage of the partic-
sample without overrepresentation of households ular diary used in the Home OnLine study is that
with computers. Two methods were used to collect it was kept for a week rather than for a single day
the data. Firstly, interviews were conducted with all (the more common data-collection procedure).
adult members (aged 16 or older) of the household. It therefore permits analysis of a wider and more
In addition, interviewed respondents were provided representative range of activities, encompassing those
with a week-long diary in which they were asked to done less frequently (e.g. once a week instead of
record, from a list of activity categories, what they once a day). This is a valuable feature in the analysis
did every quarter hour on each day of that week. of leisure, particularly leisure activities outside the
The activities recorded in the diary were based but home, since many such activities fall into this category.
were not identical to) the standardized categories used Moreover, the data set contained a larger selection
in the Multi-national Time Use Study (MTUS),3 of socio-economic and cultural variables, for use as
a cross-national archive of time use diary studies independent variables, than that available in other data
held at the University of Essex. Diaries were returned sets such as the standardized data files of the MTUS.
by post; in return, respondents received a gift voucher. From these data we were thus able to construct and
compare different measures of voraciousness, based
Sample Size and Response Rates both on time use diary data and on questionnaire
responses. Two questionnaire measures of voracious-
The original sample comprised 1000 households, ness were derived from a question in the interview
containing 2034 adult individuals. Of these, 1093 schedule asking respondents how frequently they
responded to the interview and completed the diary engaged in particular leisure activities.5 The specific
and 668 responded to the interview only, a total out-of-home leisure activities selected for the measure
response rate of 87 percent. From this data set we were: going to the cinema/concerts/the theatre; leisure
selected a subset of respondents aged between 16 and group participation; eating/drinking out; watching
65 who were identified in the interview as either the sports; and doing sports/keeping fit/walking. Out-
head of household or the partner of the head of of-home leisure activities in particular are chosen
household. This yielded a sample of 1317 individuals because they express active consumer behaviours that
and couples in households.4 in general take both time and money to engage in, and
consequently provide a link to both socio-economic
The Variables and time resources. The measure was constructed
by summing the number of these activities done on
The first advantage of this data set for our purposes ‘most days’, ‘at least once a week’, and ‘at least once
was that it contained information on participation a month’, yielding a scale of 0 (none)–5 (all).
in leisure activities both from questionnaire and The second measure of voraciousness was based on
time use diary sources, making possible a choice time use diary information, and was designed to be
and a comparison of measures of voraciousness. as compatible as possible with the questionnaire
Questionnaire measures of activity participation are measures. In their diaries, respondents recorded their
the most familiar means of data-collection on leisure participation in different leisure activities throughout
participation in the area of cultural consumption, the week in quarter-hour slots. The diary measure
although time use measures are becoming increas- simply counted the number of different out-of-home
ingly applied (e.g. Garhammer, 1998; Robinson and leisure activities done in the diary week, giving a range
Godbey, 1999). Therefore, it was important to be from 0 (none) to 4 (all). The out-of-home leisure
able to compare the new diary source with the more activities selected for the diary measure were: going
standard measures. It has been shown that diary to concerts/the cinema; walking; eating/drinking out;
estimates of time spent in different activities, where and doing sports.6 Like the questionnaire measures
people record their participation in those activities it was therefore a measure both of the range (since it
with at least some degree of contemporaneity (i.e., in involves counting how many different activities are
their diaries), differ from estimates based upon participated in) and of the frequency (since in order
responses to retrospective questions. It can safely be to be recorded during a specific week an activity has
assumed that diary estimates are in fact the more to be done on average at least weekly) of participation
accurate, since they do not involve the same problems in different out-of-home leisure activities.
128 SULLIVAN AND KATZ-GERRO

Independent Variables age with number of children were also used as control
variables in the multivariate analyses.
Indicators of human capital (as measured by highest
educational qualification), of economic capital
(as measured by the social status of the occupational Results
category), and of cultural capital (as measured by the
type of daily newspaper read), constituted our Comparison of Measures
independent variables. These three indicators represent The first step was to compare the time use and
the most common determinants of cultural participa- questionnaire measures of voraciousness. There has
tion as indicated by Bourdieuvian theory and as found been some criticism in the literature of time use diaries
in the empirical literature (Bourdieu, 1984; Katz-Gerro, as data collection instruments in the area of free time
2002; Chan and Goldthorpe, 2006). Education is and leisure activities (see the Round Table Discussion
regarded as representing cognitive competence, eco- on non-response bias in time use surveys in Leisure &
nomic capital represents financial ability, and cultural Society, 1998). One of the most common arguments
capital represents cultural disposition. While the three is that, while time use diaries may be able to accurately
are typically closely related and positively correlated, record activities which are done frequently and regu-
each one operates through a different mechanism in larly, they may be unreliable when it comes to less
its influence on consumption patterns (Van Eijck frequently performed activities. The use of a weeklong
and Bargeman, 2004). We included the type of daily diary, however, seems to go a long way in meeting
newspaper read as an independent variable, because as these objections. In Table 1, the first questionnaire
a measure of cultural capital we expected a correlation measure of voraciousness is based on the number of
between it and the extent of cultural participation. activities reported from the survey as being done at
Reading is considered part of cultured consumption least once a week. With a weekly diary one would
and is important for the pursuit of status (Zavisca, expect on the average to record all these activities. The
2005), and newspaper reading is a low-cost readily second questionnaire measure also contains those
available form of reading. Since for most people read- activities that are reported on the survey question
ing a newspaper is a daily activity, and since the choice as being done ‘‘at least once a month’’. On average
of newspaper is quite one-dimensional in terms of one-quarter of these activities will appear in a weekly
a cultural hierarchy, we considered it a reasonably diary. Assuming that the diary gives an accurate record
unequivocal indicator of cultural capital (Chan and of activity participation we would therefore expect
Goldthorpe, 2006). Sex and a family structure variable the diary estimate to have a higher distribution and
based on a ’life-cycle’ set of categories combining mean than the first questionnaire measure, but a lower

Table 1 Distributions and means of the time use diary and questionnaire measures of voraciousness

Number of different Questionnaire Weekly diary Questionnaire


out-of-home leisure measure 1 N (%) measure N (%) measure 2 N (%)
activities

None 135 (19.7) 103 (15.0) 57 (8.3)


One 304 (44.2) 232 (33.7) 179 (26.0)
Two 186 (27.1) 227 (32.9) 247 (35.8)
Three 60 (8.7) 102 (14.8) 150 (21.8)
Four 2 (0.3) 24 (3.5) 49 (7.1)
Five 1 (0.1) – 7 (1.0)
N = 100% 688y 688y 688y
Mean 1.26 1.58 1.96
Correlation coefficient
(Spearman’s rho) with
weekly diary measure 0.363 0.350
y
The analyses are based on a sample of 1317 respondents. However, the total N in this and all subsequent analyses in the article are a result of
the sample weighting which corrects for both the original over-sampling of households with a personal computer and for patterns of non-response.
Source: Home OnLine, Britain, 1998 (first wave).
THE OMNIVORE THESIS REVISITED: VORACIOUS CULTURAL CONSUMERS 129

distribution and mean than the second questionnaire service class (López Sintas and Garcia Álvarez, 2002);
measure. Since both the distribution and mean of highly educated, higher income, younger, white collar
the diary measure sit squarely between those of the occupation (Warde et al., 1999); highly educated
two questionnaire measures, we can conclude that time (Holbrook et al., 2002); highly educated, higher
use diaries are indeed effective in measuring leisure occupational status, younger (Van Eijck, 2001).
participation, even for activities done monthly. Table 2 shows mean values for our measure of
In assessing the two measures we decided that voraciousness according to our various measures of
the time-use diary instrument (recorded over a week of capital: educational level, social status of job, and type
activities) may be the more accurate in recording the of newspaper read.
true range of different weekly activities, since in the In keeping with the above findings on omnivorous-
diary activities were recorded simultaneously (or nearly ness, statistically strong relationships proved to exist
simultaneously) with their actual performance, while in between these measures of human, economic, and
the questionnaire respondents were asked to assess the cultural capital and the number of different out-
frequency with which they participated in particular of-home leisure activities participated in per week.
activities. Comparison of these measures suggested The means in the table’s columns show that for all
under-reporting by women to the survey question three of our independent variables those who occupied
about the number of different leisure activities, and the ‘higher’ categories participated in a greater average
indeed, general under-reporting of activities in surveys number of different activities. All the relationships
by comparison to time-use diary data has been are positive and monotonic in character (such that
commented on before in the methodological literature the higher the level of capital the higher the number
on time use diaries: see Dow and Juster 1985). of different activities participated in), and they are
We therefore decided to present findings from the all statistically significant.8
diary measure only in the remaining analyses.7 Having demonstrated the relationships between
voraciousness and the independent variables reflecting
Who are the ‘‘Voracious’’? different aspects of capital, we performed a multiple
analysis of variance on each of these relationships,
The next stage of the analysis was to see if the which held constant the effects of sex and of family
relationship between our new measure of voracious- structure.9 These analyses were necessary to show that
ness and our measures of human, economic, and the strong linear associations seen in Table 2 were not
cultural capital corresponded to the relationships attributable to the effects of these other important
known from the literature on omnivorousness. For structural variables. The results (not shown here,
example, in different studies the following groups were available from the authors) demonstrate that all the
found to exhibit omnivorous tastes: younger people independent variables remained highly significant at
(Peterson and Kern, 1996); younger, highly educated, the P < 0.005 level or higher.10 The strong positive

Table 2 Number of different out-of-home leisure activities/week by various measures of capital

Social status of job Mean (N) Highest qualification Mean (N) Newspaper Mean (N)
read

High management 1.79 (50) Degree, nursing 1.89 (141) Quality 2.00 (81)
Low management 1.83 (136) A-levely, higher vocational 1.63 (122) Medium 1.66 (161)
Intermediate 1.77 (80) GCSEy, lower vocational 1.59 (241) Tabloid 1.38 (176)
Small employer 1.56 (43) Failed GCSEy, youth training 1.47 (26)
Low supervisor 1.49 (52)
Semi-routine 1.39 (56)
Routine 1.35 (54)
(Undefined) (1.58) (20) (None) 1.30 (167) (None) (1.55) (278)
Total 1.64 (491) Total 1.58 (696) Total 1.58 (696)

One-way analysis of variance statistically significant at P < 0.05.

One-way analysis of variance statistically significant at P < 0.001.
y
Key: A-level ¼ School matriculation exam.
GCSE ¼ General school certificate exam.
Source: Home OnLine, Britain, 1998 (first wave).
130 SULLIVAN AND KATZ-GERRO

association found in the one-way relationships between (that people with higher levels of human and cultural
the number of different out-of-home leisure activities capital have more money) is almost certainly true; the
and our measures of the level of human and economic second (that they have more time) is more doubtful,
capital (highest qualification level, social status of job) considering the increasingly reported association
and of cultural capital (type of newspaper read) between high levels of employment income and long
persisted even when each analysis controlled for the hours of work (e.g. Sullivan and Gershuny, 2004).
effects of family structure and of sex. However, both propositions could be tested through
In addition, although the raw means show that men these data, to see whether the observed associations
participated in more different out-of-home leisure between high levels of human and cultural capital and
activities than women, the effect of sex in the above voraciousness can be attributed to time or money.
multiple analyses of variance was not statistically We examined the relationship of our independent
significant. With regard to the family structure vari- measures with income (measured as usual net monthly
able, adults living alone participate in the greatest pay for the employed/self-employed) and with time
number of different out-of-home leisure activities. (measured as usual hours of work per week, for the
Among those in couples, young couples (aged employed, and as total leisure time). In keeping with
under 36) without children participated in the highest previous findings (see for example Jacobs and Gerson,
number of different out-of-home leisure activities 1998; Robinson and Godbey, 1999; Gershuny, 2000;
(i.e. were the most voracious) and those with children Sullivan and Gershuny, 2004), these analyses support
aged under 12 in the household participated in the the generally positive relationships expected between
lowest number. These results accord with findings on higher levels of pay and higher levels of human,
leisure participation more generally, and also with the economic, and cultural capital, and the negative
findings from the omnivorous literature, in which it relationship expected between available time for leisure
is younger people without families who in general and higher levels of capital (shown in Appendix 1).
display the widest range of cultural tastes. On the whole people with higher levels of human,
The findings set out so far confirmed that we had a economic, and cultural capital had more money but
measure of voraciousness that bore a strong similarity less total leisure time than others.
to some of the behaviourally based measures of But how were these facts about income and time
omnivorousness used previously in the literature availability related to our measure of voraciousness?
(e.g. López Sintas and Garcia Álvarez, 2002) To answer this question we needed to examine whether
(as opposed to those focusing on expressed preferences the one-way associations between our measure of
or tastes). However, this measure, we maintain, should voraciousness and the independent variables represent-
be conceptually distinguished from the meaning of ing different aspects of capital remained significant in
omnivorousness as it was originally conceived, namely a multiple analysis of covariance controlling not just
as a measure of the breadth of cultural tastes of for sex and family structure (as above) but also for
highbrow consumers. Nevertheless, the association we covariates representing available time and income.
found between high levels of status with voraciousness Table 3 shows the results.
accords with the same association found in the liter- The relationship of all three of our independent
ature for measures of omnivorousness, including those variables (social status of job, level of highest qualifi-
based on tastes. cation, and type of newspaper read) with voraciousness
To strengthen this connection, however, we need to is seen to have remained highly statistically significant
consider more closely what our new measure actually even when controlling for the effects of time and
means. There is a difference, after all, between enjoying income as covariates in the analysis. The only cases
a wide selection of different music genres (the basis for where this was not so were for the models in which
some measures of omnivorousness) and having time net monthly pay was the covariate, and highest
to spend on several different out-of-home leisure qualification level and type of newspaper read were
activities during the week. It could be hypothesized, the independent variables. In both these models
for example, that the extent of participation in out- the relationship with voraciousness is in the expected
of-home leisure activities was primarily a function of direction (i.e. higher levels of human, economic, and
time or of money. The observed association between cultural capital were associated with a higher mean
high levels of human and cultural capital and number of different out-of-home leisure activities
voraciousness may simply have been because such in a clear monotonic sequence), but does not reach
people tend to have more money to spend on leisure, the accepted boundary of statistical significance.
or more time for it. The first of these propositions However, in identical analyses that we ran with the
THE OMNIVORE THESIS REVISITED: VORACIOUS CULTURAL CONSUMERS 131

Table 3 Multiple analysis of variance models showing effect of various variables on the number of different
out-of-home leisure activities per week

Covariate ¼ net Covariate ¼ hours


monthly income worked per week Covariate ¼ total
Independent variables (employees only) (employees only) leisure time

Social Status of Job P ¼ 0.004, beta¼ 0.22 P ¼ 0.000, beta¼ 0.20 P ¼ 0.000, beta¼0.20
Predicted means (adjusted):
Management 1.78 1.81 1.83
Intermediate 1.86 1.77 1.74
Small employer/low supervision 1.62 1.57 1.55
Semi/unskilled routine 1.28 1.30 1.33
Family Structure P ¼ 0.001, beta¼ 0.26 P ¼ 0.002, beta¼0.20 ns
Sex ns ns ns
Covariate ns ns P ¼ 0.000
Model R2 0.11 0.08 0.12
Highest Qualification Level ns (beta ¼ 0.13) P ¼ 0.033, beta¼0.14 P ¼ 0.000, beta¼0.20
Predicted means (adjusted):
Degree, nursing qualification 1.82 1.85 1.89
A-level, higher vocational 1.64 1.64 1.65
GCSE, lower vocational 1.55 1.58 1.59
None 1.48 1.44 1.30
Family Structure P ¼ 0.008, beta¼0.23 P ¼ 0.02, beta¼0.17 ns
Sex ns ns ns
Covariate ns ns P ¼ 0.000
Model R2 0.07 0.06 0.09
c. Type of Newspaper Read NS (beta ¼ 0.12) P ¼ 0.000, beta¼0.20 P ¼ 0.000, beta¼0.17
Predicted means (adjusted):
Quality 1.82 2.06 1.98
Medium 1.73 1.70 1.68
Tabloid 1.48 1.39 1.38
None 1.60 1.60 1.54
Family Structure P ¼ 0.004, beta¼0.24 P ¼ 0.004, beta¼0.19 P ¼ 0.001, beta¼0.17
Sex ns ns ns
Covariate ns ns P ¼ 0.000
Model R2 0.07 0.08 0.09
Source: Home OnLine, Britain, 1998 (first wave).

questionnaire-derived dependent variable measure, which the total amount of time spent on the same four
the direction of the predicted means was the same, out-of-home leisure activities as were used to calculate
but, because of the larger sample numbers responding the dependent variable was entered as a covariate into
to the questionnaire, these relationships were found the same multiple analysis of variance. In this analysis,
to be statistically significant, at P ¼ 0.015 and 0.000 which assessed the number of different out-of-home
respectively. leisure activities performed while holding constant the
To summarize, our overall conclusion is that total amount of time spent in these four activities, the
individuals with high levels of human, economic, and effects of social status of job, highest qualification level,
cultural capital have less time for leisure but still and type of newspaper read remained statistically
engage in a greater range of out-of-home leisure significant. The implication is that the higher levels of
activities. We can assume that what characterizes these diversity of participation in out-of-home leisure for
groups in general is shorter periods of leisure, which those with higher levels of human capital and cultural
are also more diverse in terms of the range of different taste are independent of the total amount of time
out-of-home activities participated in. This conclusion spent on these activities. A summary of these results
is supported by a further refinement of the analysis in is shown in Table 4.
132 SULLIVAN AND KATZ-GERRO

Table 4 Statistical significance levels for different zero to three. We found a moderately low, statistically
independent variables on the number of out- significant correlation between this measure of omniv-
of-home activities per week when holding constant orousness and that of voraciousness (Spearman’s
total time spent in those activities r ¼ 0.200, statistically significant at P < 0.01). That
Levels of statistical there was a correlation was to be expected, since the
significance construction of the omnivorousness measure was based
from multiple partly on activity participation rates; but the overlap
was not extensive and there proved to be consider-
analyses of variance
able independence between voraciousness and this
Total time spent Statistically significant measure of omnivorousness. Further, direct compar-
in four out-of-home at 0.000 in all ison between these measures must for the meantime
leisure activities three analyses wait for more suitable data to become available.
(covariate)
Social Status of Job 0.014 Cross-Time Comparisons
Highest Qualification Level 0.001
Type of Newspaper Read 0.004 Two national-level British data-sets from the MTUS
Note: Analyses also include Family Structure and Sex as independent
archive: the 1975 BBC and the 1987 SCELI surveys11
variables. (both of which included a week-long diary element)
Source: Home OnLine, Britain, 1998 (first wave). permitted us to conduct a cross-time comparison to
see if our measure of voraciousness displayed the same
relationship with measures of human and economic
In addition to stating the theoretical difference capital over time. As in any cross-time comparison
between voraciousness and omnivorousness, and there were some questions of data compatibility to be
establishing that our new measure of voraciousness overcome. First, since the activity categories in the
shares many of the same relationships with aspects harmonized MTUS data-sets are slightly different
of human, economic, and cultural capital reported from those used in the Home OnLine survey, the
from the literature on omnivorousness, we would, in voraciousness variable (number of different out-
principle, also need to show how omnivorousness and of-home leisure activities participated in during the
voraciousness are themselves related. Unfortunately, week) was calculated slightly differently, on the basis
the Home OnLine data are not ideal for constructing of a more detailed list of such activities. Secondly,
a measure of omnivorousness, as they contained no one of the problems of the MTUS archive is known
direct information on cultural tastes. Indeed, large- to be the relative lack of variables reflecting aspects
scale data including a combination of time-use data of human capital and social status, which confined us
with details about cultural tastes, or highly specific to level of education, and a 3-category income variable
information about cultural activities, are extremely (for 1987 only). However, with these limitations in
rare, if non-existent. However, in keeping with other mind, it was nevertheless possible to construct effec-
measures of omnivorousness found in the literature, tively the same dependent variable, and to test
we were able to devise a measure of omnivorousness its relationship with these aspects of human and
according to a ranking of leisure activities as represent- economic capital in multiple analyses of variance in
ing highbrow, middlebrow, and lowbrow cultural the same way as for the Home OnLine data.
tastes. This ranking was based both on the cultural To summarize the findings, we do see from the
content of the activity as it is commonly contextualized earlier data some of the same effects as in the Home
in the literature (as high, middle, or low culture) and OnLine data regarding the direction of the overall
on information on rates of participation in particular relationship between voraciousness and measures of
activities. The second criteria is increasingly used in human and economic capital.12 And in an analysis
the literature, and is predicated on the tendency of covariance equivalent to that shown in Table 3
for highbrow leisure activities to be practiced less (i.e. with education level, sex, and family status as
frequently in the population as a whole, and for the independent variables, and time in paid work and
lowbrow leisure activities to be more popular, thus time in leisure as covariates in two separate models)
avoiding researcher-defined classifications of cultural the only difference between the results from the Home
content (e.g. Peterson and Simkus, 1992; López Sintas OnLine data and those from the earlier surveys
and Garcia Álvarez, 2005; López Sintas and Katz- was the clear inverted J-shape of the relationship in
Gerro, 2005). Values on this measure ranged from the earlier surveys between educational level
THE OMNIVORE THESIS REVISITED: VORACIOUS CULTURAL CONSUMERS 133

and voraciousness. Those with only secondary school an insatiable consumer behaviour (Campbell, 1987),
education appeared to participate in a greater number and a plentiful cultural tool kit (Swidler, 1986) among
of different activities per week than those with higher status groups.
education above secondary school level. This is in Third, we provided a theoretical link between these
contrast to the 1998 results, where there was an dimensions of cultural omnivorousness and configura-
unambiguous positive relationship between level of tions of late modern cultural consumption, in which
education and voraciousness. While this may merely be status is continuously being reproduced. While we
the outcome of data inconsistencies, it might also were not able, from this data, to directly compare
represent a real change over time in the relationship voraciousness with omnivorousness as it was originally
between voraciousness and human capital, reflecting, measured according to cultural tastes (this would
for example, changes in the distribution of income, require the rare combination of time-use data with
leisure time, and leisure preferences across different details about cultural tastes), it was clear from our
groups. analyses that both omnivorousness and voraciousness
are associated with high status, in terms of higher
levels of education, job status, and cultural capital.
Discussion The activity patterns of high status individuals attested
that they participated in the greatest number of
Our revisiting of the omnivore thesis in this article different cultural activities, and we showed that this
took three directions. First, we introduced a method- could probably not be attributed simply to having
ological innovation into the omnivorousness literature more time or money than others. We postulated a
by exploiting time use data to measure actual behav- pattern of high status individuals with an ‘insatiable’
iour. We believe that it is more accurate to measure appetite for multiple leisure activities notwithstanding
respondents’ behaviour directly rather than to rely on the time squeeze. Since voraciousness seems neither
responses to questionnaire items on ‘usual’ behaviour to be about monetary access to leisure goods and
or preferences. services nor about the overall availability of leisure
Second, by looking at activity patterns we empha- time, we argue that its significant feature is its role as
sized the way individuals consume, rather than what a status marker, a cultural boundary, and a sign of
they consume. The aim here was to address the social exclusion. Further, by adding time pressure and
temporal aspect of cultural omnivorousness, which, pace of life to the discussion of cultural distinction,
we argued, is manifested both in a wide band of we argue that voraciousness, like omnivorousness,
cultural tastes and in a voracious frequency of may combine an element of the status accruing to
cultural participation. While omnivorousness is usually time pressuredness with a connection to multi-cultural
regarded as relating to the range of cultural tastes, capital (as identified by Bryson, 1996) through the
voraciousness, as we define it, relates both to the range knowledge and performance of a range of time and
and frequency of cultural practices. We found money-intensive (out-of-home) leisure activities.
voraciousness to share many of the relationships We interpret these findings as suggesting a depiction
commonly found in the analysis of omnivorousness of status distinction associated with high status
(for example, to educational qualifications, to measures individuals who tend to be harried or busy, to work
of job social status and to age, as well as to aspects more and consume more, and to have a fast life
of cultural taste such as choice of daily newspaper), tempo while also embracing a diverse cultural
and we argued that voraciousness and omnivorousness repertoire. The delineation of cultural boundaries is
represent two separate but related dimensions of modified here to depict not only an omnivorous
cultural consumption. We showed that the relationship profile, but also a voracious pattern that may
between voraciousness and measures of human and characterize the dynamics of cultural exclusion.
economic capital, family structure, time in paid work, As Veblen (1994 [1899]) pointed out, it is possible
and time in leisure generally persist over time that the lines of demarcation between social classes are
when we compared data from 1975, 1987, and 1998. in general becoming more vague and temporary,
These findings and their interpretation confirmed and that voraciousness is therefore actually related to
the validity of the conceptual stimuli that initially a globalized widening of shared tastes among
led us to look for the division of omnivorousness into high status groups. Nevertheless, our theoretical
different dimensions. We interpret the findings as expectation would be that social emulation that
depicting a culturally active leisure-style, a tendency to trickles down the social structure, resulting in
seek diverse experiences (Holt, 1997; Schulze, 1992), more blurred cultural boundaries between classes,
134 SULLIVAN AND KATZ-GERRO

always antecedes specialization and innovation adopted several times a year; once a year or less; never/
by the upper classes, thereby reinforcing cultural almost never.
boundaries (see McCracken, 1990). 6. The diary’s leisure categories were not precisely
The conclusions presented above should be qualified comparable with the questionnaire’s leisure
and further explored in future research on several categories.
grounds. First, cross sectional data does not allow for 7. The unavoidable disadvantage of this decision was
causal ordering, so the direction of the relationship slightly to reduce the effective sample size, since
between human, economic, and cultural capital and not all the respondents returned a completed time
consumption cannot be assumed from these data use diary.
alone. Second, it is possible that there are anteceding 8. When job status categories were combined into
factors, not included in these analyses, which influence a smaller number of categories to enlarge the cell
both levels of achieved capital and types and levels of sizes the relationship became statistically signifi-
cultural consumption. Third, future research could cant at the P<0.005 level.
develop more sensitive measures of active leisure-styles 9. The measure of family structure was based on
and link them more systematically to measures both life-cycle stages, including aspects of age and the
of insatiability in the sphere of consumption and to presence and age of dependent children in the
the acceleration of life tempo both over time and household. The categories are: living alone, aged
across different countries. under 36; living with spouse, aged under 36 with
Our analysis fleshes out a dimension of cultural no dependent children; living with spouse, over
consumption that is not necessarily about participating age 36 with no dependent children; living with
seriously in large numbers of activities for long periods spouse, dependent children aged under 12 in the
of time but about cultural tasting and switching house; living with spouse, dependent children
among, or differently combining, those activities. aged 12 or more in the house; other.
Voraciousness may therefore not be about commit- 10. For the purposes of this analysis the categories of
ment to many activities but about commitment to not job status were recoded into a 4-category variable:
leaving many activities untouched or unpractised.13 management; Intermediate; small employers and
The voracious activity pattern may perhaps be one way supervisors; semi-skilled and unskilled routine.
to reconcile the contradiction between the ‘increasingly 11. Further details about each may be obtained from
overworked’ contention and the ‘increase in leisure the MTUS website <http://iser.essex.ac.uk/mtus>.
time’ contention, if we maintain that discretionary 12. Details of these analyses may be obtained from
time can be devoted to work and to leisure alike by the authors.
means of voracious cultural participation. 13. See Han (2003) for a discussion of the link
between social position and cultural disposition
in terms of tastes in music. Han argues that
Notes omnivorousness is not only about a large number
of genres liked; it is more about switching
1. Feldman and Hornik (1981) coined the term
evaluation-distinction criteria.
timestyle to suggest that the choice of daily
activities constitutes the locus of the meaning of
time to individuals. References
2. The original data collection was funded by British
Telecommunications plc. Bell, D. (1979). The Cultural Contradictions of
3. For further details of the MTUS see Gershuny Capitalism. London: Heinemann.
et al., 2000. Bihagen, E. and Katz-Gerro, T. (2000). Culture
4. We also derived a randomly selected sample of one Consumption in Sweden: The Stability of Gender
adult individual per household, and all analyses Differences. Poetics, 27, 327–349.
were performed for both these samples. The results Bittman, M. (2002). Social Participation and Family
for the individual sample were identical to those Welfare: The Money and Time Costs in Leisure in
for the sample of individual and couple house- Australia. Social Policy and Administration, 36,
holds; given the greater numbers in the latter, we 408–425.
present here only those results. Bittman, M. and Wajcman, J. (2000). The Rush Hour:
5. The response categories were: most days; at The Character of Leisure Time and Gender Equity.
least once a week; at least once a month; Social Forces, 79(1), 165–189.
THE OMNIVORE THESIS REVISITED: VORACIOUS CULTURAL CONSUMERS 135

Bourdieu, P. (1984). Distinction: A Social Critique on Hochschild, A. (1997). The Time Bind. New York:
the Judgment of Taste. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Metropolitan Books.
University Press. Holbrook, M. B., Weiss, M. J. and Habich, J. (2002).
Bryson, B. (1996). ‘Anything but Heavy Metal’: Disentangling Effacement, Omnivore, and
Symbolic Exclusion and Musical Dislikes. Distinction Effects on the Consumption of
American Sociological Review, 61, 884–899. Cultural Activities: An Illustration. Marketing
Bryson, B. (1997). What about the Universe? Musical Letters, 13(4), 345.
Dislikes and Group-Based Identity Construction Holt, D. (1997). Distinction in America? Recovering
among Americans with Low Levels of Education. Bourdieu. Poetics, 25(2–3), 93–120.
Poetics, 25(2–3), 141–156. Holt, D. (1998). Does Cultural Capital Structure
Campbell, C. (1987). The Romantic Ethic and the Spirit American Consumption? Journal of Consumer
of Modern Consumerism. Oxford: Blackwell. Research, 25(1), 1–25.
Chan, T. W. and Goldthorpe, J. (2006). Social Status Feldman, L. P. and Hornik, J. (1981). The Use of
and Newspaper Readership. American Journal of Time: An Integrated Conceptual Model. Journal of
Sociology, Forthcoming. Consumer Research, 7, 407–419.
Cotte, J., Ratneshwar, S. and Mick, D.G. (2004). Jacobs, J. A. and Gerson, K. (1998). Who Are the
The Times of Their Lives: Phenomenological Overworked Americans?. Review of Social Economy,
and Metaphorical Characteristics of Consumer 1(4), 442–459.
Timestyles. Journal of Consumer Research, 31, Jacobs, J. A. and Gerson, K. (2004). The Time Divide:
333–345. Work, Family, and Gender Inequality. Cambridge,
Dow, G. K. and Juster, F. T. (1985). Goods, Time, and MA: Harvard University Press.
Well-being: The Joint Dependence Problem. In Juster, F. T. (1985). The Validity and Quality of Time
Juster, F. T and Stafford, P. (Eds), Time, Goods, Use Estimates Obtained from Recall Diaries. In
and Well-Being. Michigan: University of Michigan Juster, F. T. and Stafford, F. P. (Eds), Time, Goods,
Survey Research Centre, Chapter 16. and Well-Being. Michigan: Survey Research Centre:
Emmison, M. (2003). Social Class and Cultural University of MichiganChapter 4.
Mobility: Reconfiguring the Cultural Omnivore Kalfs, N. (1993). Hour by Hour: Effects of the Data
Thesis. Journal of Sociology, 39(3), 211–230.
Collection Mode in Time Use Research. Amsterdam:
Erickson, B. H. (1996). Culture, Class, and Connections.
Nederlands Instituut voor Maatschappij en Markt-
American Journal of Sociology, 102, 217–251.
Onderzoek.
Gans, H. J. (1974). Popular Culture and High Culture:
Katz-Gerro, T. (2002). Highbrow Cultural
An Analysis and Evaluation of Taste. New York:
Consumption and Class Distinction in Italy,
Basic Books.
Israel, West Germany, Sweden, and the United
Garhammer, M. (1998). Time Pressure in Modern
States. Social Forces, 81(1), 207–229.
Germany. Loisir et Société, 21(2), 327–352.
Katz-Gerro, T. (2004). Cultural Consumption
Gershuny, J. (2000). Changing Times: Work and Leisure
Research: Review of Methodology, Theory, and
in Postindustrial Society. Oxford: Oxford University
Press. Consequence. International Review of Sociology,
Gershuny, J. (2005). Busyness as the Badge of Honour 14(1), 11–29.
for the New Superordinate Working Class. Social Katz-Gerro, T. (2006). Comparative Evidence of
Research, 72(2), 287–314. Inequality in Cultural Preferences: Gender, Class,
Gershuny, J., Fisher, K., Gauthier, A., Jones, S. and and Family Status. Sociological Spectrum, 26, 63–83.
Baert, P. (2000). A Longitudinal, Multinational Katz-Gerro, T. and Sullivan, O. (2004). Leisure, Tastes
Collection of Time—Use Data—the MTUS. In and Gender in Britain: Changes from the 1960s
Gershuny, J. (Ed.), Changing Times: Work and to the 1990s. Sociologie et Sociétés, XXXVI(1),
Leisure in Postindustrial Society. Oxford: Oxford 165–186.
University Press. Appendix 2. Levine, L. W. (1998). Highbrow/Lowbrow: The
Giddens, A. (1991). Modernity and Self Identity: Self Emergence of Cultural Hierarchy in America.
and Society in the Late Modern Age. Cambridge: Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Polity Press. Linder, S. (1970). The Harried Leisure Class. New York:
Han, S.-K. (2003). Unraveling the Brow: What and Columbia University Press.
How of Choice in Musical Preference. Sociological López Sintas, J. and Garcia Álvarez, E. (2002).
Perspectives, 46(4), 435–459. Omnivores Show up Again: The Segmentation of
136 SULLIVAN AND KATZ-GERRO

Cultural Consumers in Spanish Social Space. Shaw, J. (1998). ‘Feeling a List Coming On’: Gender
European Sociological Review, 18(3), 353–368. and the Pace of Life. Time and Society, 7(2),
López Sintas, J. and Garcia Álvarez, E. (2005). Four 383–396.
Characters on the Stage Playing Three Games: Simmel, G. (1950). The Metropolis and Mental Life.
Performing Arts Consumption in Spain. Journal of In Wolff, K. (Ed.), The Sociology of George Simmel.
Business Research, 58(10), 1446–1455. Glencoe, Illinois: Free Press, pp. 409–424.
López Sintas, J. and Katz-Gerro, T. (2005). From Simmel, G. (1957). Fashion. American Journal of
Exclusive to Inclusive Elitists and Further: Twinty Sociology, 62(6), 541–558.
Years of Omnivorousness and Cultural Diversity Slater, D. (1997). Consumer Culture and Modernity.
in Arts Participation in the USA. Poetics, 33, Cambridge: Polity Press.
299–319. Southerton, D. (2003). Squeezing Time: Allocating
McCracken, G. (1990). Culture and Consumption. Practices, Coordinating Networks and Scheduling
Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press. Society. Time and Society, 12(1), 5–25.
Mattingly, M. and Bianchi, S. (2003). Gender Sullivan, O. and Gershuny, J. (2001). Cross-National
Differences in the Quantity and Quality of Free Changes in Time Use: Some Sociological (Hi)
Time: The US Experience. Social Forces, 81(3), Stories Re-Examined. British Journal of Sociology,
999–1030. 52(4), 331–347.
Peterson, R. A. (1992). Understanding Audience Sullivan, O. and Gershuny, J. (2004). Inconspicuous
Segmentation: From Elite and Mass to Omnivore Consumption: Work-Rich, Time-Poor in the
and Univore. Poetics, 21, 243–258. Liberal Market Economy. Journal of Consumer
Peterson, R. A. (1997). The Rise and Fall of Culture, 4(1), 79–100.
Highbrow Snobbery as a Status Marker. Poetics, Swidler, A. (1986). Culture in Action: Symbols
25, 75–92. and Strategies. American Sociological Review, 51,
Peterson, R. A. (2005). Problems in Comparative 273–286.
Research: The Example of Omnivorousness. Swidler, A. (2001). Talk of Love: How Culture Matters,
Poetics, 33, 257–282. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Peterson, R. A. and Simkus, A. (1992). How Musical Toffler, A. (1970). Future Shock. London: The Bodley.
Tastes Mark Occupational Status Groups. In Van Eijck, K. (2000). Richard A. Peterson and the
Lamont, M. and Fournier, M. (Eds), Cultivating Culture of Consumption. Poetics, 28, 207–224.
Differences: Symbolic Boundaries and the Making of Van Eijck, K. (2001). Social Differentiation in
Inequality. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Musical Taste Patterns. Social Forces, 79,
Press, pp. 152–186. 1163–1184.
Peterson, R. A. and Kern, R.M. (1996). Changing Van Eijck, K. and Bargeman, B. (2004). The Changing
Highbrow Taste: From Snob to Omnivore. Impact of Social Background on Lifestyle:
American Sociological Review, 61, 900–907. ‘‘Culturalization’’ instead of Individualization?
Robinson, J. P. (1985). The Validity and Reliability of Poetics, 32(6), 439–461.
Diaries Versus Alternative Time Use Measures. In Van Rees, K., Vermunt, J. and Verboord, M. (1999).
Juster, F. T. and Stafford, F. P. (Eds), Time, Goods, Cultural Classifications under Discussion: Latent
and Well-Being. Michigan: Survey Research Centre: Class Analysis of Highbrow and Lowbrow Reading.
University of Michigan. Poetics, 26, 349–365.
Robinson, J. P. and Godbey, G. (1999). Time for Life: the Veblen, T. (1994[1899]). The Theory of the Leisure
Surprising Ways Americans Use Their Time. Class. New York: Penguin.
Philadelphia: Penn State University Press (2nd edition). Warde, A., Martens, L. and Olsen, W. (1999).
Rosa, H. (2003). Social Acceleration: Ethnical and Consumption and the Problem of Variety:
Political Consequences of a Desynchronized High- Culture Omnivorousness, Social Distinction and
Speed Society. Constellations, 10(1), 3–33. Dining Out. Sociology, 33, 105–127.
Round Table Discussion. (1998). Non-Response Bias in Weber, M. (1974). The Theory of Social and Economic
Time Use Surveys. Leisure and Society, 21(2), Organization. In Parsons, T. and Henderson, A. M.
548–582. (Eds and trans). New York: Oxford University
Schor, J. (1992). The Overworked American. New York: Press.
Basic Books. Zavisca, J. (2005). The Status of Cultural Omnivorism:
Schulze, G. (1992). Die Erlebnisgesellschaft: A Case Study of Reading in Russia. Social Forces,
Kultursoziologie der Gegenwart. Frankfurt: Campus. 84(2), 1233–1255.
THE OMNIVORE THESIS REVISITED: VORACIOUS CULTURAL CONSUMERS 137

Tally Katz-Gerro, Department of Sociology and


Authors’ Addresses Anthropology, University of Haifa, Haifa 31905,
Oriel Sullivan (to whom correspondence should be Israel. Email: tkatz@soc.haifa.ac.il
addressed), Department of Behavioural Sciences,
Ben-Gurion University, Beer Sheva 84105, Israel.
Email: sullivan@bgu.ac.il Manuscript received: January 2006

Appendix 1 Net pay, hours of work and leisure time by measures of human, economic, and cultural capital

Net usual monthly Usual work Total leisure


pay (pounds) (N) hours/week (N) time/week (N)

Educational level:
Degree, nursing qualification 1,314 (81) 40.9 (114) 33.2 (139)
A-level, higher vocational 1,241 (60) 39.9 (93) 33.7 (119)
GCSE, lower vocational 842 (104) 36.4 (175) 35.0 (239)
None 842 (57) 34.9 (91) 41.2 (190)
N 302yy 472y 688
Statistical significance from one-way 0.000 0.014 0.000
analysis of variance
Social status of job:
Management 1,327 (115) 41.7 (181) 31.5 (184)
Intermediate 817 (54) 32.4 (79) 33.7 (79)
Self-employed, low supervisors 1,189 (50) 43.1 (93) 33.0 (95)
Semi-skilled, unskilled routine 666 (72) 30.9 (102) 34.2 (109)
N 291 454 467
Statistical significance from one-way 0.000 0.000 0.312
analysis of variance
Type of newspaper read:
Quality 1,591 (41) 44.4 (61) 38.7 (79)
Medium 841 (65) 36.1 (103) 38.7 (158)
Tabloid 900 (83) 36.7 (114) 36.6 (174)
None 1,079 (113) 37.4 (193) 33.6 (275)
N 302 472 687
Statistical significance from one-way 0.000 0.005 0.002
analysis of variance
Source: Home OnLine, Britain, 1998 (first wave).
y
Employees only.
yy
Employees only (lower N due to non-response on income questions).

View publication stats

You might also like