Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

J Am Acad Audiol 8 : 163-172 (1997)

Performance Norms for the VA Compact Disc


Versions of CID W-22 (Hirsh) and
PB-50 (Rush Hughes) Word Lists
Anastasia L. Heckendorf*
Terry L. Wileyt
Richard H . Wilson*

Abstract
The purpose of this study was to establish normative articulation functions for the Hirsh
recordings of the CID W-22 word lists and the Rush Hughes recordings of the PB-50 word
lists as recorded on the VA compact disc. Twenty-four young adults with normal hearing lis-
tened to both sets of materials presented in quiet at 12 levels (0-56 dB HL). Presentation
levels on average needed to be 10 dB higher for the PB-50 word lists to obtain word recog-
nition performance scores equal to those for the W-22s. Maximum word recognition scores
of 95 to 100 percent were obtained for the W-22s at lower presentation levels (>40 dB HL),
compared to 80 to 90 percent for the PB-50s at the maximum presentation level (56 dB HL).
Mean slopes for the W-22 and PB-50 functions were 4.1 percent/dB and 1 .9 percent/dB,
respectively . Test-retest reliability was judged to be clinically appropriate for both sets of
materials. Item analyses revealed a substantially skewed distribution toward easy items for
the W-22 word lists compared to a more balanced distribution of item difficulty for the PB-50
lists. List equivalency was also judged to be clinically appropriate for the PB-50 word lists.

Key Words : Articulation functions, compact disc, speech audiometry, word recognition

Abbreviations : ANSI = American National Standards Institute, CD = compact disc,


CNC = consonant- n ucleus-co nsonant

n 1991, the Department of Veterans Affairs Despite a paucity of data on the character-
produced a compact disc (CD) (Speech Recog- istics of the Rush Hughes recordings of the PB-
Inition and Identification Materials, Disc 1.1) 50 lists, available evidence indicates that the
that contained several sets of word recognition PB-50 materials are more sensitive than the
materials, including (1) a male recording of the W-22 lists to individual differences in word
Maryland consonant-nucleus-consonant (CNC) recognition performance for listeners with nor-
lists, (2) a female recording of the CID W-1 mal hearing and listeners with hearing impair-
spondaic words and the Northwestern Univer- ment (Davis, 1948, 1978 ; Thurlow et al, 1948,
sity Auditory Test No . 6 (NU-6), (3) the Rush 1949 ; Eldert and Davis, 1951 ; Hirsh et al, 1952 ;
Hughes recording of the PB-50 lists, and (4) the Silverman and Hirsh, 1955 ; Goldstein et al,
Hirsh recordings of the CID W-22 monosyllables 1956 ; Goetzinger et al, 1961 ; Carhart, 1965 ;
(Wilson, 1993). Normative data on the CD ver- Lovrinic et al, 1968 ; Goetzinger, 1972). An issue
sion of the W-1 lists (Cambron et al, 1991) and of primary interest in the present study was
the NU-6 lists (Wilson et al, 1990) are available . whether changes associated with the produc-
Normative data on the W-22 and PB-50 mate- tion of the CD recordings altered the perfor-
rials as recorded on the CD, however, are not mance characteristics of either the W-22 or the
available. PB-50 material . Also, although several reports
discuss differences in word recognition perfor-
mance for the W-22 and PB-50 lists (Hirsh et al,
1952 ; Goetzinger and Rousey, 1959 ; Goetzinger
*CESA 5-Audiology, Portage, Wisconsin ; tDepart- et al, 1961 ; Carhart, 1965, 1970 ; Goetzinger,
ment of Communicative Disorders, University of Wisconsin,
Madison, Wisconsin ; tVA Medical Center-Audiology, Moun- 1972 ; Davis, 1978), a direct performance com-
tain Home, Tennessee parison of the two tests on the same group of sub-

163
Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 8, Number 3, June 1997

jects is lacking. Thus, the main purpose of the 11) or the W-22 word lists (Lists 1, 2, 3, 4) were
current investigation was to provide normative presented monaurally in quiet at 12 levels (0, 4,
word recognition data on the W-22 (Hirsh) and 8, 12, 16, 20, 24, 28, 32, 40, 48, 56 dB HL). The
PB-50 (Rush Hughes) word lists as recorded on remaining set of lists was presented under the
the VA CD . Secondarily, test-retest measures same conditions during the second session. Dur-
and item analyses were examined for the two ing the third session, performed within 1 week
sets of materials. Finally, list equivalency was of initial testing, each subject was retested with
evaluated for the PB-50 word lists. three lists from each set of materials at levels
of 8, 20, and 32 dB HL to provide data for
METHOD test-retest reliability. All four PB-50 lists also
were presented during the final third session at
Subjects 56 dB HL for evaluation of list equivalency.
The order of list presentation was random-
The volunteer subject group consisted of 12 ized with restrictions across subjects and levels
men and 12 women (N = 24) 19 to 34 years so that the same word list was never presented
(mean = 26 years) of age . All subjects had (1) a at consecutive levels . An ascending order of lev-
negative history of any recent otic disorder and els was used to minimize any possible learning
normal otoscopic findings (ASHA, 1990), (2) nor- effects. For all word recognition testing, sub-
mal middle ear function defined according to jects wrote their responses. Correctness was
ASHA (1990) guidelines, (3) an ipsilateral based on phonetic or homonym spelling with
acoustic reflex for a 1000-Hz tonal activator omissions of final consonants, plurals, or tense
<95 dB HL, and (4) air-conduction thresholds markers judged as incorrect.
of 0 to 10 dB HL (ANSI, 1989) at octave inter-
vals from 250 to 8000 Hz . For each subject, the RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
ear with the lower mean threshold at 1000,
2000, and 4000 Hz served as the test ear; an Articulation Functions
equal number of right and left ears were
represented. Table 1 lists and Figure 1 illustrates mean
word recognition scores in percent correct
Instrumentation and Calibration obtained for the W-22 and PB-50 materials from
the 24 listeners . The lines through the data
Tympanograms and acoustic reflexes were points are the best-fit, second-degree polyno-
obtained using an acoustic admittance screen- mials. Except for the two lowest presentation
ing instrument (Grason-Stadler, Model GSI 27A) levels, recognition performance on the W-22s
that met ANSI S3 .39-1987 specifications. Pure- exceeded performance on the P13-50s . As
tone air-conduction audiometry was performed expected from the binomial model (Thornton
with a console audiometer (Grason-Stadler,
Model GSI 16) in accordance with ANSI S3 .6-
1989 . Testing was performed with the subject Table 1 Mean Word Recognition Scores and
seated in a sound room (Industrial Acoustics Standard Deviations (Both in Percent Correct)
Company, 1200 Series). The test signals were by Presentation Level (dB HL) for the W-22 and
delivered to an earphone (TDH 50-P) mounted the PB-50 Materials from 24 Listeners with
in a supra-aural cushion (Telephonics P/N Normal Hearing
5100017-1) . Word recognition stimuli from
Speech Recognition and Identification Materials, Level (dB HL) W-22 PB-50
Disc 1 .1 were directed from a CD player (Sony,
0 0 .8 (1 .7) 6 .3 (5 .6)
Model 497) to a single channel of the audiome- 4 12 .4 (12 .5) 13 .4 (7 .3)
ter. The speech channel of the audiometer was 8 25 .1 (14 .3) 24 .1 (9 .6)
calibrated to 19 .7 dB SPL for a 1000-Hz tone . 12 45 .9 (16 .6) 33 .8 (12 .7)
16 61,7 (12 .4) 46 .2 (10 .0)
20 73 .3 (11 .1) 53 .0 (12 .0)
Procedures
24 80 .5 (9.4) 59 .8 (9 .2)
28 86 .7 (7 .3) 64 .8 (10 .5)
Each subject participated in three 60- to 32 91 .8 (4.7) 71 .1 (8 .3)
90-minute sessions, with a 10-minute break in 40 95 .2 (3 .9) 79 .8 (8 .8)
the middle of each session. During the first ses- 48 96 .7 (2 .3) 84 .7 (6 .3)
56 98 .2 (1 .8) 86 .3 (6 .8)
sion, either the PB-50 word lists (Lists 8, 9, 10,

164
Norms for W-22 and PB-50s on CD/Heckendorf et al

(±0.4%/dB). The mean slopes were significantly


100 different (F[1 .23] = 181 .32; p < .05) .
The results of the current investigation sug-
gested that the PB-50 word lists recorded by
80
Rush Hughes (recordings available on CD) con-
tinued to be more sensitive to individual differ-
60 ences in word recognition abilities than the CID
W-22 recordings, at least for listeners with nor-
40 mal hearing. The PB-50 lists did not result in
articulation functions that demonstrated pro-
nounced plateau regions, as observed for the
20
W-22 functions. Rather, the function for the PB-
50s continued to rise gradually even at the high-
0 est presentation levels for the majority of
subjects . The steeper slope of the W-22 function
0 10 20 30 40 50 60
(4 .1%/dB) indicated that small increases in pre-
PRESENTATION LEVEL (dB HL) sentation level resulted in larger changes in
word recognition performance relative to the
Figure 1 Articulation functions for the W-22 (circles)
PB-50s . A single increase in presentation level
and PB-50 (squares) lists recorded on the VA CD . The
mean correct recognition (%) is plotted as a function of of 4 to 5 dB corresponded to a 16 to 20 percent
the presentation level (dB HL). The lines connecting the improvement in W-22 word recognition perfor-
data points are the best-fit, second-degree polynomials mance. In contrast, the shallower slope (1 .9%/dB)
(W-22-y = -2 .2045 + 4.5885x - 0.051125x2, R2 = 0.99;
associated with the PB-50 function indicated
P13-50-y = 3.9302 = 2.9315x - 0.026023x2, R2 = 0.99) .
that considerably smaller changes in performance
occurred with increments in presentation level.
An increase of 4 to 5 dB corresponded to only an
8 to 10 percent improvement in word recognition
and Raffm, 1978), variability was greatest at the scores . The PB-50 1ists more precisely differen-
middle presentation levels (see Table 1) . Ceiling tiated individual word recognition abilities across
effects were observed for the W-22s with many levels than did the W-22 lists.
100 percent scores obtained at the highest lev-
els. No 100 percent scores were obtained on the Original vs Current Functions
PB-50s .
The differences between functions calcu- For the past 40 years, the original articu-
lated from the polynomial equations progressed lation function for the W-22s (Hirsh et al, 1952)
from 0.7 dB at 20 percent, to 5.5 dB at 50 per- has been a benchmark against which practi-
cent, to 15 .8 dB at 80 percent. Data for the PB- cally all word recognition materials have been
50s were at higher levels for each percentage . compared. The slope of the original W-22 func-
The slopes of the mean functions calculated tion was about 4 percent/dB with the 50 percent
from the same equations at the 20 percent and point at 24 dB SPL. Based on this original func-
80 percent correct points were 3 .1 percent/dB for tion, the W-22s are always cited as being easier
the W-22s and 1.7 percent/dB for the PB-50s . In than any other comparable monosyllabic word
addition to the slopes of the mean functions, recognition material . Until the current study,
the mean slopes of the individual functions were however, the original articulation function for the
calculated. For each individual set of data, slopes Hirsh version of the W-22s has not been repli-
of the articulation functions were estimated at cated. Figure 2 illustrates the articulation func-
the presentation levels that most closely approx- tions for the original data (filled circles) and for
imated word recognition scores of 20 percent the current data (open circles) . The original W-
and 80 percent correct. The slopes of the func- 22 data were extracted from Hirsh et al (Fig . 1;
tions for the individual subjects ranged from 1952) by adding 20 dB to sound pressure level
2 .8 percent/dB to 5 .7 percent/dB (W-22s) and to determine the corresponding presentation
from 1.2 percent/dB to 2.8 percent/dB (PB-50s) . level (dB HL). For reference purposes, the
The mean slope of the individual functions for derived data points from Hirsh et al are listed
the W-22s was 4.1 percent/dB (-}0 .9%/dB), in Table 2 . To simplify computational compar-
whereas the mean slope of the individual func- isons, the data were then fit with a second-
tions for the PB-50s was 1 .9 percent/dB degree polynomial . The two W-22 functions are

165
Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/ Volume 8, Number 3, June 1997

Table 2 Levels (dB HL) Corresponding to The reasons for the differences between the
Percent Correct Performance Extracted from functions are not entirely clear. The same record-
Second-Degree Polynomial Equations Applied ings of the materials by the same speaker (Hirsh)
to Data Obtained from the Original W-22 and were evaluated, so there was no speaker differ-
PB-50 Recordings Reported by Hirsh et al ence . Calibration, however, may have been a
(1952) and Davis (1948) and the Current
major contributor to the observed differences
Investigation (CD 95) for the CD Version of
for the two functions. A difference between the
These Same Tests
levels of the calibration tone and W-22 materi-
als on the Technisonic records has been appar-
Percent W- 22 PB- 50
Correct Hirsh (1952) CD 95 Davis (1948) CD 95 ent for many years (Gengel and Kupperman,
1980). The Technisonic records were derived
80 12 .8 24 .8 22 .6 40 .5 directly from the magnetic tapes from which
70 9 .3 20 .4 18 .9 31 .1 the original W-22 function in Figure 2 was gen-
60 6 .5 16 .6 15 .8 24 .4
erated . To verify the reported difference between
50 4 .0 13 .4 13 .0 18 .9
40 1 .8 10 .4 10 .3 14 .0 the levels of the calibration tone and W-22 mate-
30 -0 .2 7 .7 7 .7 9 .7 rials on the Technisonic records, the levels on the
20 -2 .2 5 .1 5 .1 5 .8 1969 records were examined using a computer
Mean slope 4 .0%/dB 3 .1%/dB 3 .4%/dB 1 .7%/dB and vu meter arrangement. The calibration tone
and stimulus materials were reproduced (Pana-
sonic, Model SG-HM09) with a 1-mil stylus
displaced about 10 dB with the original function (Lilly and Franzen, 1968) and digitized with a
requiring less SPLs for comparable recognition 16-bit analog-to-digital converter (Antex, Model
performance. The differences between the W-22 SX-10) at 20,000 samples/sec. Subsequently,
functions averaged 9.5 dB and ranged from 7.3 each carrier phrase and target word set were
dB at 20 percent correct, to 9.4 dB at 50 percent edited into individual files. Routines with a
correct, to 12 .0 dB at 80 percent correct. The waveform editor were then used to block the
slopes derived from the 20 to 80 percent points carrier phrase and repetitively reproduce it with
on the polynomials fit to the mean W-22 func- the output of the digital-to-analog converter
tions were 4 percent/dB (Hirsh et al, 1952) and monitored on a vu meter (Tascam, Model MU-
3.1 percent/dB (current study) . 40). After monitoring the stimulus several times
on the vu meter, the peak vu reading could be
estimated within ± 0.5 dB . Here and through-
out, 1 vu is equal to 1 dB for a sinusoid (IEEE,
100 1992, p. 1475). A similar process was used with
the target words except that the last word of the
80 carrier phrase ("say") was included in the block.
Because the calibration tone was about 8 dB
below the vu level of the carrier phrase peaks,
60
8 dB were added digitally to the level of the dig-
itized calibration tone . In this manner, all read-
40 ings were accomplished around 0 vu (i .e ., the
lower end of the vu scale was not used).
20 The level measurements revealed that the
1000-Hz calibration tones on the Technisonic
discs were on average 10 .1, 8 .9, 8.2, and 10 .0 vu
0
below the peaks of the carrier phrases in Lists
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 1, 2, 3, and 4, respectively. Standard deviations
were approximately 1.0 vu . The target words for
PRESENTATION LEVEL (dB HL)
Lists 1, 2, 3, and 4 were 7.1, 7.9, 7.6, and 8.8 vu,
Figure 2 Articulation functions for the original Hirsh respectively, above the level of the calibration
et al (1952) W-22 data (filled circles) and for the current tone . Similar findings have been reported using
W-22 data (circles) from the same recording with Hirsh a graphic level recorder (Gengel and Kupperman,
as the speaker. The mean correct recognition (%) is plot- 1980). In the Gengel and Kupperman report, the
ted as a function of the presentation level (dB HL). The
carrier phrases on the Technisonic discs were
lines connecting the data points are the best-fit, second-
degree polynomials (Hirsh et al, 1952-y = 31 .281 + about 8 dB above the level of the calibration
5.0414x - .095064x2, R2 = 0.99) . tone, with the W-22 words being 6 to 7 dB above

166
Norms for W-22 and PB-50s on CD/Heckendorf et al

the level of the tone . Further confirmation of the


level difference on the Technisonic records of 100
the W-22 materials comes from the slightly dif-
ferent prospective of Studebaker and Sherbecoe 80
(1991), who reported that the calibration tone
was "2 .3 dB below the long term RMS level of
60
the test words." Thus, data on the Technisonic
Studio recordings of the W-22 materials from
three measurement techniques indicates that the 40
calibration tone is 8 to 10 vu (or dB) below the
peak level of the carrier phrases. 20
In the original report, Hirsh et al (1952, p.
188) reported that "the 1,000 cps calibration 0
tone on the inner face of every record is at the
average level of the carrier phrases." Two rele- 0 10 20 30 40 50 60
vant assumptions can be posited. First, the PRESENTATION LEVEL (dB HL)
"average level" used by Hirsh et al (1952) to
calibrate the discs is not the same as the "peak Figure 4 Articulation functions for the VA CD ver-
level" currently used to calibrate. Second, the sions of the W-22 (circles), PB-50 (squares), and North-
western University Auditory Test No . 6 (triangles) word
same calibration procedures and levels were
recognition lists. The mean correct recognition (%) is
used on both the magnetic tape version and the plotted as a function of the presentation level (dB HL).
record version of the W-22s. Psychometrically, The lines connecting the data points are the best-fit, sec-
this set of circumstances would result in an ond-degree polynomials (W-22s and PB-50s) and the
articulation function that is displaced to lower best-fit, third-degree polynomial (NU-6-y = -1 .3643
- 0.65480x = 0.32045x2 - 0.0065301x3^, R2 = 0.99) .
sound pressure levels in comparison to the artic-
ulation function that is based on the calibration
tone reflecting the peaks of the carrier phrases .
This is consistent with the relation observed in lower than the W-22 function generated in the
Figure 2 in which the W-22 function from Hirsh current investigation.
et al (1952) occurs at presentation levels 10 dB Perhaps an additional contributing vari-
able to the difference between the Hirsh et al
(1952) and the current study was the familiar-
ity that the subjects had with the stimulus
.. 100 materials, the listening task, the listening envi-
ronment, and the speaker. In the original study,
80 the subjects (1) were given a list of the 200
words to read at the beginning of each of eight
2.5-hour test sessions on consecutive days ; (2) lis-
60
tened to six randomizations of each of the four
lists presented at 100 dB SPL, which served to
40 "indoctrinate" the listeners; and (3) listened to
each of the 24 lists (six randomizations of four
20 lists) at five to seven 10-dB intervals on the
articulation functions. Thus, each subject heard
each of the four word lists 36 to 48 times. In the
0
current study, the subjects were not allowed to
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 read or listen to test lists other than when they
were exposed to each list four times during the
PRESENTATION LEVEL (dB HL)
experiment . Furthermore, an ascending level
Figure 3 Articulation functions for the Davis (1948) PB- procedure was used so that the initial presen-
50 data (filled squares) and for the current PB-50 data tations would not influence performance at sub-
(squares) from the same recording with Rush Hughes as sequent higher presentation levels . Based on
the speaker. The mean correct recognition (%) is plotted
the familiarity variable, we conclude, as did
as a function of the presentation level (dB HL). The lines
connecting the data points are the best-fit, second-degree
Hirsh et al (1952), that the data from the orig-
polynomials (Davis, 1948-y = -1 .8857 + 4.6412x inal W-22 study should be considered "experi-
- 0.047892x2, R2 = 0.99) . mental" based on the use of "experienced"
Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 8, Number 3, June 1997

listeners, whereas the data from the current the means for the NU-6 word lists were lower
study are more directly representative of the clin- than those for both the W-22 and PB-50 lists. At
ical situation that involves patients with "naive" 16 dB HL, the means for the NU-6 and PB-50
listening skills . recordings were essentially the same (44.0%
In Figure 3, the original articulation func- [±15 .6%] and 46 .2% [±10 .0%], respectively).
tions for the Rush Hughes recording of the PB- Above 24 dB HL, the mean scores for the NU-6
50s (filled squares) are illustrated along with the word lists closely approximated the mean scores
PB-50s' articulation function from the current for the W-22 word lists. Differences between
study (open squares) . The original PB-50 data mean scores for the W-22 and NU-6 recordings
based on 10 listeners were extracted from Davis were less than 4 percent at presentation levels
(Fig. 1; 1948), again using 20 dB to convert to of 24, 28, and 32 dB HL . Data for the NU-6
hearing level (see Table 2), and were fit with a recordings were not available for the three high-
second-degree polynomial . For equal intelligi- est presentation levels (40, 48, and 56 dB HL)
bility, the PB-50 materials presented from the used in the current investigation.
CD required higher presentation levels than The corresponding slopes of the mean func-
were required in the reports of 40 years ago. tions for each of the three sets of materials were
For the PB-50 materials, the functions in compared . The steepest slope occurred for the
Figure 3 differed by 0.7 dB at 20 percent correct, NU-6 word lists (4 .5%a/dB) ; the slope for the CID
5.9 dB at 50 percent, and 17 .9 dB at 80 percent W-22 word lists (4 .1%/dB) was slightly shal-
correct with an average difference between the lower. Beyond the linear portion of the articu-
20 percent and 80 percent correct points of 7.3 lation function (28 dB HL), however, the plateau
dB . Additionally, the observed slope of the orig- regions of the functions were essentially indis-
inal function (3 .4%/dB) is twice the slope of the tinguishable . In contrast, the mean slope for
function from the current study (1 .7%/dB) . The the PB-50 word lists (1 .9°10/dB) was about half
paucity of details in the Davis report (1948) as steep as the functions for either the W-22 or
about the experimental conditions, subjects, cal- NU-6 recordings . Not only does the linear por-
ibration, etc. makes it difficult to speculate on tion vary, but the function for the PB-50s did not
the differences between the two functions. We result in a characteristic plateau region . Per-
would, however, like to offer two scenarios. First, formance for the PB-50s continued to improve
with the original Davis data, there is the sug- with increased presentation levels, even at the
gestion (Davis et al, 1949) that the 1000-Hz cal- highest levels studied.
ibration tone was 2.5 dB below the peaks of the
carrier phrase, which would account for some of Test-Retest Reliability
the difference between the two functions shown
in Figure 3. Second, time and the copying/record- Retest performance scores were compared
ing process have degraded the PB-50 materials to initial test scores at levels of 8, 20, and 32 dB
with the substantial loss of high-frequency HL . The results of this comparison are listed in
energy. Support for this comes from observa- Table 3 and are illustrated as bivariate plots in
tions of the digital waveforms of the Rush Figures 5 (W-22) and 6 (PB-50). Six individual
Hughes recordings . The amplitudes of the ini- one-way, repeated measures analyses of variance
tial and final consonants, especially the /s/ and (ANOVAs) were completed by level to determine
/sh/, are substantially reduced in comparison if significant differences existed between initial
to initial and final consonants in other speech and retest scores for both the W-22s and PB-50s .
materials that we have observed . This type of A significant difference between initial and
degradation could account for portions of the retest conditions was found only for the CID W-
difference in the slopes of the functions shown 22 word lists at a presentation level of 8 dB HL
in Figure 3. (F[1,23] = 14 .84; p < .05) ; a test-retest difference
of approximately 9 percent was observed for
Comparisons across CD Functions this condition, compared to 3 to 4 percent for
other retest conditions . For all other analyses,
Figure 4 illustrates the functions obtained there were no significant differences (p > .05)
in the present investigation for the CD versions between initial and retest conditions . Mean
of the W-22 (circles) and PB-50 (squares) word retest scores were higher than initial mean
lists and in a study by Wilson et al (1990) for the scores for each condition, indicating the possi-
CD version of the NU-6 (triangles) word lists. At bility of a slight learning effect . The retest
the lower presentation levels (below 12 dB HL), increase of 1 to 4 percent, however, corresponded

168
Norms for W-22 and PB-50s on CD/Heckendorf et al

Table 3 Mean Word Recognition Scores and


Standard Deviations (Both in Percent Correct) N
w
Obtained at Three Levels (8, 20, and 32 dB HL) W 100
for Test and Retest Presentations of the W-22 cc
and PB-50 Recordings 80
OR
0

8 dB HL 20 dB FIL 32 dB HL Z
O 60
H
Test Retest Test Retest Test Retest Z
C7 40
W-22 O
V
Mean 25 .1 33 .7 73 .3 75 .4 91 .8 92 .4 W
M 20
SD 14 .3 13 .7 11 .1 11 .0 4 .7 4 .5
H
PB-50 V
W 0
Mean 24 .1 27 .2 53 .0 56 .6 71 .0 73 .9
SD 9 .6 9 .0 12 .0 11 .0 8 .4 8 .5
0 0 20 40 60 80 100
Subjects were 24 listeners with normal hearing . V CORRECT RECOGNITION (%)--TEST
Figure 5 Bivariate plots of the test (abscissa) and
retest (ordinate) percent correct recognition of the W-22
materials at 8 dB HL (squares), 20 dB HL (circles), and
32 dB HL (triangles). The diagonal line represents equal
to only a one- or two-word difference . The stan- test-retest performance.
dard deviations for each retest comparison were
within 1 percent of the initial standard devia-
tions. Previously reported results obtained from by a few earlier researchers suggested that the
phonograph and magnetic tape versions of the original phonograph versions of the PB-50 word
CID W-22 recordings resulted in similar mean lists were not equivalent ; some lists were report-
difference scores (<4%) and standard deviations edly easier or harder than others (Eldert and
(<1%) between initial and retest scores (Hunt- Davis, 1951 ; Wolfe and O'Connell, 1959). The
ington and Ross, 1962 ; Beattie and Raffm, 1985). four lists available on CD (8, 9, 10, 11) resulted
Larger mean differences (<_8%) were reported by in mean word recognition scores and standard
Beattie and Raffin (1985) at lower presentation deviations of 88 .1 percent (±4.5), 89 .0 percent
levels of 25 and 30 dB SPL (5 and 10 dB HL) for (±4 .3), 84 .0 percent (±4.4), and 91 .7 percent
the Auditec of St . Louis recordings of the CID W- (±4.2), respectively. These results were similar
22 word lists (magnetic tape). The test-retest to previously reported data in that List 11 was
results of the current investigation, then, closely consistently easier than the other three lists
parallel those previously reported for other (Eldert and Davis, 1951 ; Wolfe and O'Connell,
media versions of the CID W-22 recordings . 1959). In contrast to earlier data for phono-
Thus, the transfer of the original recordings to graph versions of the PB-50s (Eldert and Davis,
CD did not alter test-retest reliability. Test-retest 1951), List 10 resulted in the lowest mean score
reliability was judged to be good for both the CID rather than List 8. Mean scores for the current
W-22 and Rush Hughes recordings based on the investigation varied by less than 8 percent with
similarities between mean performance scores, the standard deviations across lists essentially
low standard deviations, and overall agreement equivalent (approximately 4%). A list x sub-
in scores across sessions . jects, repeated measures ANOVArevealed a sig-
nificant main effect for list (F[3,691 = 19 .09; p <
List Equivalency .05) . Post-hoc analyses were completed using
Scheffe's test (Bruning and Kintz, 1977) to fur-
Although Hirsh et al (1952) demonstrated ther evaluate the interactions between lists; the
list equivalency for the W-22 word lists, similar critical difference score associated with a prob-
information was not available for the PB-50 ability of .05 was 3.66 percent correct. Statisti-
lists. Therefore, performance equivalency for cal analyses revealed that only interactions with
the four PB-50 word lists recorded by Rush List 10 were significant; scores for List 10 were
Hughes (8, 9, 10, 11) was investigated to deter- consistently lower than for the other lists. Even
mine if word recognition scores differed across though statistical analyses revealed a significant
lists; presentation was at 56 dB HL in an attempt difference between lists, the amount of difference
to elicit maximum performance. Data presented between means (2-8%) corresponded to one to
Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 8, Number 3, June 1997

H
N F is
W 0.07 -
100
w
0.06 -
80 0.05 10
oc
w 0
a. 0.04 C
60 z z
0 I
F 0 .03
a4 -5
40 0.02
0
aa
0.01 -
20
nIrIiIi~IIIIIIII~III~IIII~IIII~III~iIIIlI1
18 36 54 72
0
Number of Correct Responses

Figure 6 Bivariate plots of the test (abscissa) and


retest (ordinate) percent correct recognition of the PB-50 F lo
materials at 8 dB HL (squares), 20 dB HL (circles), and
32 dB HL (triangles). The diagonal line represents equal -8
es 0.04
test-retest performance.
m
W 0.03 -6
C
z
z 0.02 4 .1
0
Pe.
four items based on a 50- or 25-item test . Only 2
0 .01-
for extreme word recognition scores of 0 percent 0
and 100 percent would differences of these per-
a
a ~~(~mil~ll~llllllll~lllllllll~lllllllllllllll
T
11 1l
72
centages shift an individual's score to a differ- 0 18 36 54

ent confidence interval (Thornton and Raffin, Number of Correct Responses

1978). Therefore, the PB-50 word lists on CD


Figure 7 Density plots of the number ofitems included
recorded by Rush Hughes were judged to be in the W-22 (upper) and PB-50 (lower) word lists that
equivalent because clinical decisions would not resulted in a given number of correct responses of 72 pos-
be altered based on differences of this magnitude. sible occurrences per item. For example, from the W-22
word lists, 12 items were identified correctly 54 times of
72 possible occurrences (upper). From the PB-50 word
Error Analyses
lists, five items were identified correctly 54 times of 72
possible occurrences (lower).
Correct and incorrect responses for each
word contained in the W-22 and PB-50 word
lists were tallied based on 72 possible occur-
rences for each item (24 subjects were presented
each word at three levels). Several items on the half of the presentations across levels . In con-
two tests were either scored correct or incor- trast, items included in the Rush Hughes lists
rect by a large proportion (>80%) of subjects . were more evenly distributed across a wide
There were 18 such words identified correctly for range of difficulty just as Egan (1948) had
the W-22s, compared to 15 for the PB-50 lists. intended when he constructed the PB-50 word
In contrast, there were 11 words in the PB-50 lists. The majority of PB-50 items were near
word lists that were incorrect for more than 80 the center of the distribution, indicating that they
percent of the subjects . None of the W-22 words were almost equally identified as correct and
were incorrect 80 percent of the time . Figure 7 incorrect. Although fewer items were present
provides the number of items within each set of near the ends otthe distribution, all levels of dif-
lists associated with a given number of correct ficulty were represented to some degree for the
responses across presentation levels . Visual PB-50 words.
inspection of the graphs in addition to the item- These error analyses are similar to those
ized error analysis revealed a highly skewed reported by Lovrinic et al (1968) for a single
clustering of easy items for the CID W-22 word list of both the CID W-22 and Rush Hughes
lists. Very few items were missed for more than recordings available on phonograph records.
Norms for W-22 and PB-50s on CD/Heckendorf et al

For both the current investigation and that of The first author would like to thank her coauthors for their
numerous revisions and input leading to the completion
Lovrinic et al (1968), more items were consis-
of this project.
tently scored correct from the W-22 word lists
A paper based on portions of this research was presented
than from the PB-50 word lists. Similarly, the
at the 1994 meeting of the American Speech-Language-
P13-50s resulted in a number of items that were Hearing Association in New Orleans, LA .
perceived incorrectly by a large proportion of sub- This work is based in large part on a master's thesis by
jects compared to 0 and 2, for those same two the first author.
studies, respectively.
The results of the current study, then, indi-
cated that the transfer of the original tape record- REFERENCES
ings to CD did not alter the distribution of item
American National Standards Institute. (1987) . American
difficulty or the pattern of item identification for National Standard Specifications for Instruments to
either test. The item analysis provided addi- Measure Aural Acoustic Impedance and Admittance
tional support for the greater sensitivity of the (Aural Acoustic Immittance) (ANSI S3 .39-1987) . New
York : ANSI .
Rush Hughes recordings of the PAL PB-50 word
lists for differentiating word recognition abilities American National Standards Institute. (1989) . American
National Standard Specification for Audiometers (ANSI
compared to the CID W-22 recordings . Results
S3 .6-1989) . New York : ANSI .
of the current item analysis may provide insight
into the development of more sensitive word American Speech-Language-Hearing Association . (1990) .
Guidelines for screening for hearing impairment and
recognition tests based on reconstruction of the
middle-ear disorders. ASHA 32(Supp12) :17-24 .
Rush Hughes word lists . Specifically, elimination
of items that were extremely easy or difficult Beattie R, Raffin M. (1985). Reliability of threshold, slope,
and PB Max for monosyllabic words. J Speech Hear Disord
would provide a more balanced range of difficulty 50 :166-178 .
and, potentially, a more sensitive test.
Bruning J, Kintz B. (1977) . Computational Handbook of
Statistics . 2nd ed . Glenview, IL : Scott, Foresman and
Summary
Company.

The purpose of this investigation was to Cambron N, Wilson R, Shanks J. (1991) . Spondaic word
detection and recognition functions for female and male
provide normative data for the CID W-22 and speakers . Ear Hear 12 :64-70.
PB-50 word recognition tests available on CD .
Presentation levels on average needed to be 10 Carhart R. (1965) . Problems in the measurement of speech
discrimination. Arch Otolaryngol 82 :253-260 .
dB higher for the PB-50 word lists in order to
obtain word recognition performance scores Carhart R. (1970) . Contemporary American tests and
equal to those for the W-22s . Maximum word procedures . In : Rojskjar C, ed . Speech Audiometry-
Second Danauox Symposium. Odense, Denmark :
recognition scores of 95 to 100 percent were Andelsbogtrykkeriet, 28-36.
obtained for the W-22s at lower presentation
Davis H. (1948) . The articulation area and the Social
levels (?40 dB HL), compared to 80 to 90 percent
Adequacy Index for hearing. Laryngoscope 58 :761-778 .
for the P13-50s at the maximum presentation
level (56 dB HL). The slope for the W-22 artic- Davis H. (1978) . Audiometry: pure-tone and simple speech
tests. In : Davis H, Silverman S, eds. Hearing and
ulation function (4.1%/dB) was over twice that
Deafness . 4th ed. NewYork: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston,
for the PB-50 function (1 .9%/dB). Error analy- 183-221 .
ses demonstrated significant differences in item
Davis H, Morrical K, Harrison C. (1949) . Memorandum
difficulty between W-22 and PB-50 word lists;
on recording characteristics and monitoring of word and
W-22 scores were skewed toward high scores sentence tests distributed by Central Institute for the
and PB-50 scores evidenced centralized distri- Deaf. JAcoust Soc Am 21 :552-553 .
butions. Test-retest reliability was judged to be Egan J. (1948). Articulation testing methods . Laryngoscope
good for both the W-22 (Hirsh) and PB-50 (Rush 58 :955-991 .
Hughes) recordings available on CD . Similarly,
Eldert E, Davis H. (1951) . The articulation function of
the PB-50 word lists on CD were judged to be patients with conductive deafness . Laryngoscope 61 :
clinically equivalent . 891-909.

Gengel R, Kupperman G. (1980) . On the calibration of


Acknowledgments. The CD used for this study, Speech
two commercially recorded versions of CID Auditory Test
Recognition and Identification Materials, Disc 1.1, can be
W-22 . Ear Hear 1:229-231 .
obtained through the Long Beach Research Foundation .
Contact Richard Wilson, Ph .D ., at the Veterans Affairs Goetzinger C. (1972). The Rush Hughes Test in auditory
Medical Center, 126 Audiology, Mountain Home, TN diagnosis. In : Katz J, ed . Handbook of Clinical Audiology.
37684, (423) 926-1171, Ext. 7553 . 1st ed . Baltimore, MD : Williams & Wilkins, 325-333.
Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 8, Number 3, June 1997

Goetzinger C, Proud G, Dirks D, Embrey J . (1961) . A Studebaker G, Sherbecoe R . (1991) . Frequency-impor-


study of hearing in advanced age . Arch Otolaryngol 73 : tance and transfer functions for recorded CID W-22 word
60-72 . lists . J Speech Hear Res 34 :427-438 .

Goetzinger C, Rousey C . (1959) . Hearing problems in Thornton A, Raffin M. (1978) . Speech-discrimination


later life . Medical Times 87 :771-780 . scores modeled as a binomial variable . J Speech Hear
Res 21 :507-518 .
Goldstein R, Goodman A, King R. (1956). Hearing and
speech in infantile hemiplegia before and after left hemi- Thurlow W, Davis H, Silverman S, Walsh T. (1949) .
spherectomy. Neurology 6:869-875 . Further statistical study of auditory tests in relation to
the fenestration operation . Laryngoscope 59 :113-129 .
Hirsh I, Davis H, Silverman S, Reynolds E, Eldert E,
Benson R. (1952). Development of materials for speech Thurlow W, Silverman S, Davis H, Walsh T . (1948) . A
audiometry. J Speech Hear Disord 17 :321-337 . statistical study of auditory tests in relation to the fen-
estration operation. Laryngoscope 58 :43-66 .
Huntington D, Ross M. (1962) . The Reliability and
Equivalency of the CID W-22 Auditory Tests. Brooks AFB, Wilson, R. (1993) . Development and use of auditory
TX : USAF Aerospace Medical Center. compact discs in auditory evaluation. J Rehabil Res 30 :
342-351.
Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers. (1992) .
Electrical and Electronics Terms. York : IEEE .
Wilson R, Preece J, ThorntonA . (1990a) . Clinical use of
the compact disc in speech audiometry. ASHA 32 :47, 51 .
Lilly D, Franzen R . (1968) . Reproducing styli for speech
audiometry. J Speech Hear Res 11 :817-824 .
Wilson R, Zizz C, Shanks J, Causey G . (1990b) . Normative
data in quiet, broadband noise, and competing message
Lovrinic J, Burgi E, Curry E . (1968) . A comparative eval-
for Northwestern University Auditory Test No . 6 by a
uation of five speech discrimination measures . JSpeech
female speaker. J Speech Hear Disord 55 :771-778 .
Hear Res 11 :372-381 .

Silverman S, Hirsh I. (1955). Problems related to the use Wolfe A, O'Connell M. (1959) . Sources of Variability among
of speech in clinical audiometry. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol Articulation Tests for Normal Ears with Pb Word Lists.
64 :1234-1244 . Randolph AFB, TX : USAF School of Aviation Medicine .

You might also like