Language Teaching

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Language 

Teaching
http://journals.cambridge.org/LTA

Additional services for Language Teaching:

Email alerts: Click here
Subscriptions: Click here
Commercial reprints: Click here
Terms of use : Click here

A student's contributions to second­language learning. Part II: 
Affective variables
R. C. Gardner and P. D. MacIntyre

Language Teaching / Volume 26 / Issue 01 / January 1993, pp 1 ­ 11
DOI: 10.1017/S0261444800000045, Published online: 23 December 2008

Link to this article: http://journals.cambridge.org/abstract_S0261444800000045

How to cite this article:
R. C. Gardner and P. D. MacIntyre (1993). A student's contributions to second­language learning. Part II: Affective 
variables. Language Teaching, 26, pp 1­11 doi:10.1017/S0261444800000045

Request Permissions : Click here

Downloaded from http://journals.cambridge.org/LTA, IP address: 130.194.20.173 on 25 Jun 2013
State of the art article
A student's contributions to second-language
learning. Part II: Affective variables
R. C. Gardner and P. D. Maclntyre The University of Western Ontario

This is the second part of a two-part article dealing between integrative and instrumental orientations
with individual difference correlates of second- that was an integral part of some of the earlier
language learning. In this part, attention is directed research in this area.
to a consideration of the role of affective variables in The first investigation of the relationship of
second-language learning. By affective variables, we attitudes and motivation to achievement in a second
mean those emotionally relevant characteristics of language was published by Gardner and Lambert
the individual that influence how she/he will (1959), even though such relations had been
respond to any situation. In the language learning hypothesised earlier. Arsenian (1945), for example,
situation, many such factors have been identified, proposed that attitudes could play a role in second-
but this article will focus on two broad classifications language acquisition, and in an inaugural article in
of affective factors, namely, Language attitudes and Language Learning, Marckwardt (1948) argued that
motivation, and Language anxiety and self-con- there were five basic motives for learning a second
fidence. language. These were described as ' provision of a
cultural background, the influence of foreign speech
Attitudes and motivation islands, the necessity for political and cultural
unification, purposes of colonisation and commerce,
Considerable research has been conducted on the and the necessity of reading scientific and technical
role of attitudes and motivation in second- or works' (pp. 10-11). Note that the integrative and
foreign-language learning. This research began with instrumental orientations are well represented in
speculations made by Lambert (1955) that an interest these basic motives.
in learning another language often developed In their initial study, Gardner and Lambert (1959)
because of emotional involvement with the other demonstrated that two independent factors, Lan-
language community or because of a direct interest guage aptitude and Social motivation, were both
in the language itself. In his studies of bilingual related to achievement in French among Canadian
development, Lambert (1955) described two English-speaking high-school students. Subsequent
American university students who had developed research also showed relationships between both
particularly high levels of French/English bilingual language aptitude on the one hand and attitudes and
skill. One had developed an intense identification motivation on the other with achievement in the
with France, while the other had devoted much of second language, though often more than two
her career to teaching French. Lambert speculated factors were obtained. This would be expected as
that their excessively high competence in French the number and complexity of the variables grow
(their second language) was quite likely motivated and as the socio-cultural make-up of the com-
by their somewhat different involvements with the munities and level of training and competence in the
language. We can see here the seeds of the distinction second language varies. The bulk of these studies
were conducted in Montreal, Canada as well as in
Robert C. Gardner is professor of psychology at the cities in Maine, Louisiana, and Connecticut, and the
University of Western Ontario, London, Canada. He Philippines (see Gardner & Lambert, 1972, for a
has written books and articles on the role of attitudes review of much of the earlier research).
and motivation on second language learning, co-edited Gardner and Smythe and their colleagues were
a book on ethnic relations in Canada, and published concerned with formalising the composition and
articles on ethnic stereotypes, psycholinguistics, and measurement of attitudes and motivation as they
statistics. relate to second-language acquisition. They engaged
Peter D. Maclntyre has recently completed his doctoral in a programme of research to develop measures
studies at the University of Western Ontario. He with high reliability and applicability to different
graduated from the University College of Cape Breton age levels, levels of second-language training, and
with an undergraduate degree in psychology and social contexts. Gardner and Smythe (1981) present
speech communication. He has published articles in the summary data on the initial development of the
areas of anxiety and language learning, program Attitude/Motivation Test Battery while Gardner,
evaluation and statistical analysis. Smythe, Clement and Gliksman (1976) present
summary data of the relationships of the various
Lang. Teach. 26, 1-11. Copyright © 1993 Cambridge University Press 1
State of the art: Second language learning. Part 2
measures with achievement in French drawn from targets, largely because they are more generalisable
various grade levels in seven different cities across across different studies. These are (a) Evaluation of
Canada. Gliksman (1981) investigated similar var- the language teacher, and (b) Evaluation of the
iables with students at the university level. language course.
The Attitude/Motivation Test Battery (AMTB) (4) Language anxiety. This refers to students'
was developed to measure a number of attributes anxiety reactions to situations in which they might
associated with second-language learning. These make use of the target language. Depending on the
can be grouped into five categories, four of which language-learning context, it could be possible to
reflect concepts that are often used in this research. identify many possible situations; however, in the
They are as follows: AMTB, two general measures are used, (a) French
(1) Motivation. Three different measures are class anxiety, and (b) French use anxiety. The
needed to assess the various components of mo- former scale refers to anxiety aroused specifically in
tivation. As proposed by Gardner (1985), the the language class, while the latter refers to feelings
motivated individual is one who wants to achieve of anxiety that individuals experience in any context
a particular goal, devotes considerable effort to where they are called upon to speak the target
achieve this goal, and experiences satisfaction in the language.
activities associated with achieving this goal. That is, (5) Other attributes. In many studies, some attri-
motivation is defined by three components, desire butes are included in the AMTB that do not fit
to achieve a goal, effort extended in this direction, into any of the above categories. In decreasing order
and satisfaction with the task. One would expect of use, these are (a) Instrumental orientation, (b)
that these three attributes would be correlated with Parental encouragement, and (c) Orientation index.
one another, but it is possible that they might not In some studies, scores on the various scales are
be, in some circumstances. Consider, for example, a aggregated to produce scores on these general
classroom situation with a stern and severe teacher. concepts, Motivation, Integrativeness, Attitudes
It is conceivable that even individuals who are not toward the learning situation, and Language anxiety.
truly motivated to learn the material may be shown In other studies, higher order aggregates are
to display considerable effort in class. If motivation obtained. One such measure is the Integrative
were defined only in terms of effort, such individuals motivation score which is the sum of Motivation,
may be considered to be motivated, even though Integrativeness and Attitudes toward the learning
they do not have any desire to learn, and even find situation. Another aggregate score that is used in
the experience distasteful. Other examples could some studies is the AMI score which is the aggregate
also be given to show how one of the other elements of the Integrative motivation score plus Instrumental
of motivation may be elevated due to situational as orientation minus language anxiety. Lalonde and
opposed to motivational considerations. Given these Gardner (1985) compared the relative predictive
considerations, motivation is assessed in the AMTB capacity of the three basic aggregates, Motivation,
by three measures, (a) Desire to learn the language, Integrativeness and Attitudes toward the learning
(b) Motivational intensity, and (c) Attitudes toward situation, and found that Motivation correlated
learning the language. more highly with French grades, the behavioural
(2) Integrativeness. Indices of integrativeness are intention to study French the next year, and
comprised of attributes that reflect a positive outlook objective measures of French achievement than did
toward the other language group or out-groups in either the measures of Integrativeness or Attitudes
general. Since the learning of a second language toward the learning situation.
involves acquiring skills associated with another Other research has used the AMTB to attempt to
cultural group, it is proposed that the motivation to understand the causal relationships among attitudes,
learn the language could involve attitudes toward motivation and language achievement, and has
that community or more general attitudes toward sometimes included language anxiety and language
other groups. In earlier research (see, for example, aptitude. Some researchers (Burstall, Jamieson,
Gardner & Lambert, 1959, 1972), attention was also Cohen & Hargreaves, 1974; Backman, 1976;
directed toward a number of measures including Strong, 1984) have argued that achievement
ethnocentrism and authoritarianism, but in the causes attitudes and motivation, while the socio-
AMTB, three measures are used; (a) Attitudes educational model (Gardner, 1985) explicitly pro-
toward the target language group, (b) Interest in poses reciprocal causation. That is, it argues that
foreign languages, and (c) Integrative orientation. motivation influences language achievement, and
(3) Attitudes toward the learning situation. This that language achievement as well as experiences in
concept refers to affective reactions toward the formal and informal language contexts influence
language-learning situation. As such, it could attitudes and motivation (which are viewed as some
involve attitudes toward the instructor, the class, of the many possible non-linguistic outcomes).
the textbooks, the language laboratory, etc. In the Some studies have been concerned with testing
AMTB, attention is directed toward only two ' causal models' of these relationships using linear
State of the art: Second language learning. Part 2
structural relations modelling (LISREL) (Joreskog & found that language aptitude was the best single
Sorbom, 1984). Many of these studies are discussed indicator of English achievement among school
by Gardner (1985, chapter 8). Although the models students. An index of attitudes toward the learning
change somewhat from study to study because of situation was the next best correlate. Muchnick
changes in the socio-cultural context, variables and Wolfe (1982) investigated American students
investigated, and the like, all of them indicate that a studying Spanish as a second language, and found
prime mediator in the language-learning process is that attitudes toward the Spanish course and Spanish
motivation. class anxiety correlated significantly with grades in
Still other research has made use of laboratory Spanish. Sison (1991) made use of causal modelling
techniques to investigate the causal nature of with a group of American students studying Spanish
attitudes, motivation and language achievement. as a second language. She tested a model that had
Such research has the advantage that it controls language attitudes 'causing' both motivation and
extraneous factors that otherwise might cloud causal language anxiety, motivation and language anxiety
interpretations. It has the disadvantage, however, 'causing' language achievement, and motivation
that it is much more artificial than even a classroom ' causing' (negatively) language anxiety. All of the
language-learning situation (that may be artificial paths, with the exception of that between language
in itself) and is concerned only with the acquisition attitudes and language anxiety, were significant, and
of limited vocabulary elements. Nonetheless, such the fit of the model was acceptable.
research has demonstrated that the rate of learning In a most interesting study of the role of attitudes
French/English vocabulary pairs is faster for those and motivation in second-language study, Kraemer
with high as opposed to low levels of AMI (Gardner, (1990) investigated Israeli Jewish students studying
Lalonde & Moorcroft, 1985) as well as integrative either Arabic or French as a foreign language. She
motivation (Gardner & Maclntyre, 1991). Gardner too made use of causal modelling to link language
and Maclntyre also showed that individuals who attitudes, motivation, and indices of proficiency in
were studying the vocabulary items because of the other language. She also included other variables,
instrumental motivation (aroused by a monetary such as social/political attitudes, political optimism,
incentive) learned faster than those without this national security orientation, etc., that were necess-
motivation. Thus, in a controlled laboratory setting ary to reflect the socio-cultural setting there. Similar
at least, both integrative and instrumental mo- causal models were obtained for both students of
tivation were shown to influence second-language Arabic and French. Motivation was found to be a
acquisition. central mediator in the prediction of language
Many studies have used versions of the AMTB in achievement, but as might be expected in this
contexts other than those involving English Can- context, integrative attitudes were not significant
adian students learning French as a second language. contributors to motivation. This study is particularly
Generally speaking, all of these studies found informative because it shows how it is necessary to
evidence that motivation or some aspect of language consider carefully the factors that can contribute to
attitudes correlated significantly with achievement the motivation to learn another language in different
in the second language. However, examination of socio-cultural contexts.
the studies reveals many different forms of this The influence of motivational variables on
relationship. There may be many reasons for this. second-language achievement has also been dem-
The social contexts change, the measures are slightly onstrated in a military context. Lett and O'Mara
different, the nature of the analyses vary, etc. (1990) investigated the predictive power of a number
Nonetheless, in many of them, there is evidence that of variables such as general intellectual ability,
affective variables are associated with achievement language aptitude, demographic variables such as
in the second language. In a series of studies, sex, etc., attitudes, motivation, learning strategies,
Clement and his colleagues investigated French personality variables, etc., on the learning of Korean,
Canadian students learning English as a second Russian, German, and Spanish by American military
language (Clement, Gardner & Smythe, 1977,1980; personnel in intensive language-learning program-
Clement, Major, Gardner & Smythe, 1977). In all of mes. As might be expected, aptitude tended to be
these studies, the highest relationships with achieve- among the most important predictors of proficiency
ment in the second language involved the indices of (especially for Korean and Russian). Attitudes and
self-confidence (discussed below in the section on motivation assessed during the programme con-
Anxiety), but self-confidence had clear relationships tributed significantly to two of the three predictions
with the motivational indices. computed for each language (the criteria were
Many studies have been conducted outside Listening, Reading, and Speaking proficiency at the
Canada. Laine (1977) investigated Finnish students end of the programme). Other variables contributed
learning English and found that indices of self- also, but the fact remains that even in a military
confidence and motivation were associated with context, attitudes and motivation were clearly
English achievement. In Belize, Gordon (1980) related to proficiency in other languages.
State of the art: Second language learning. Part 2
Although most attention has been directed to the a series of attitude and motivation measures with
relation of attitudes and motivation to indices of their participation on 22 student-centred and 17
second-language achievement, some research has teacher-centred classroom behaviours. Many of the
focused attention on other criteria. One such correlations were not significant, but those that
criterion is persistence in language study. Bartley were provided results comparable to those obtained
(1969) investigated the relation of language aptitude by Gliksman (1976). In yet another variant on this
and foreign language attitudes to the tendency of approach, Roger, Bull and Fletcher (1981) compared
grade 8 students in Spanish, French, German or students identified by their teachers as highly
Latin to enrol in their language course in the attentive and enthusiastic with students not so
subsequent term. She found that those who dropped described. The former group had significantly more
out of language study had significantly lower favourable attitudes toward learning foreign lan-
aptitude scores and less positive attitudes than guages than the latter group. Making the assumption
students who continued with their studies. In a that the teacher's characterisations reflected be-
follow-up study, Bartley (1970) assessed student haviour in the classroom, such results again indicate
attitudes at the beginning of the school year and an association between attitudes and classroom
again at the end. She found that those who dropped participation.
out of language study had significantly less favour- This review indicates that attitudes and moti-
able attitudes on both occasions than students who vation play a role in second-language acquisition.
did not drop out and also showed a significant These attributes are related to measures of pro-
decrease in their attitudes from the beginning to the ficiency in the second language, rates of learning
end of the year. In a similar study, Ramage (1990) vocabulary, persistence in language study and
investigated high school students of French and behaviour in the language classroom. The re-
Spanish and found that continuing students were lationships are complex, but taken as a unit, they
more motivated and had more favourable attitudes suggest that the operative variable in the language-
toward the language class than students who were learning process is motivation. Motivation itself,
not continuing with their language study. Clement, however, is also a complex of factors - proposed
Smythe and Gardner (1978) compared high school here to encompass desire to achieve a goal, effort
students who continued French study with those expended in this direction, and reinforcement
who dropped out the following year, and found associated with the act of learning.
that the drop-outs had significantly lower levels of Crookes and Schmidt (1991) provide a similar
motivation, and generally less positive attitudes than characterisation of motivation as it relates to second-
those who continued language study. They also language learning based on their consideration of
tended to have lower levels of language aptitude theories of motivation proposed in various fields
and lower levels of French achievement. By and of psychology. The important point is that moti-
large, the index of motivation (comparable to the vation itself is dynamic. The old characterisation of
aggregate measure of motivation described above) motivation in terms of integrative vs. instrumental
was the single best predictor of who would continue orientations is too static and restricted. Many studies
as opposed to drop out. (see, for example, Dornyei, 1990; Lukmani, 1972;
Another variable that is influenced by attitudes Gardner & Maclntyre, 1991), have shown that
and motivation is that of behaviour in the lan- achievement in a second language is facilitated by
guage classroom. Gliksman (1976) investigated this instrumental orientations (or motivation) as well as
relation in two studies. In the first, he contrasted integrative orientations and attitudes (see, for
individuals characterised as integratively motivated example, Gardner, 1985; Gardner & Maclntyre, in
with those less integratively motivated (as defined press), or other motivational attributes (Clement,
by a median split on an aggregate score based on 1986; Kraemer, 1990). Whereas motivation appears
measures of Integrativeness and Motivation as to play a primary role in second-language learning,
described above). He found, over a series of class various other attributes, including integrativeness
sessions, that those classified as integratively moti- and attitudes toward the learning situation would
vated volunteered more frequently (particularly seem to play a role in supporting levels of moti-
among males), gave more correct answers, and vation.
received more positive reinforcement from the
teacher than students not so motivated. Comparable
results were obtained in a second study (also see Language anxiety
Gliksman, Gardner & Smythe, 1982). Naiman, A topic of rising importance in the study of
Frohlich, Stern and Todesco (1978) obtained similar language learning is the role of language anxiety.
findings though they used a somewhat different This is a relatively new development largely because
tactic. They observed students who had been selected the role of anxiety in language learning was not
by their teachers as being either among the most or recognised in early research. Studies conducted in
the least proficient in class, and correlated scores on the 1970s were difficult to interpret because of
State of the art: Second language learning. Part 2
contradictory results (Chastain, 1975; Kleinmann, cabulary learning and production, while General
1977; Scovel, 1978), probably resulting from using anxiety was not associated with performance in the
general measures of anxiety. Recent studies, how- second language. A follow-up study (Maclntyre &
ever, have focused on a type of anxiety related Gardner, 1991) that employed 22 anxiety measures
specifically to language situations, termed language identified a separate dimension of Language anxiety.
anxiety. A recent volume devoted to the subject Scales of communication apprehension, social eva-
(Horwitz & Young, 1991) demonstrates its emerging luation, and test anxiety were not associated with
significance. this dimension, supporting the suggestion that
Language anxiety can be defined as the ap- language anxiety is a specific, relatively unique type
prehension experienced when a situation requires of apprehension. Finally, Young (1990) has shown
the use of a second language with which the that the most anxiety-provoking tasks in language
individual is not fully proficient. It is, therefore, seen classrooms involve public communication and/or
as a stable personality trait referring to the propensity evaluation, comprising the three sources of anxiety
for an individual to react in a nervous manner when identified by Horwitz et al. (1986).
speaking, listening, reading, or writing in the second The negative effects of language anxiety on
language. As with other forms of anxiety, it is second-language acquisition have been well es-
characterised by derogatory self-related cognitions tablished. Several studies have shown a negative
(e.g. 'I can't do this'), feelings of apprehension, and correlation with grades in language courses
physiological responses such as increased heart rate (Gardner, Smythe, Cle'ment & Gliksman, 1976;
(Endler & Okada, 1975; Eysenck, 1979; Schwarzer, Horwitz, 1986; Trylong, 1987; Gardner &
1986). Maclntyre, in press). Course grades are one of the
Horwitz, Horwitz and Cope (1986) describe most salient measures of language performance;
anxiety in the language classroom as a complex however, correlations with such a global measure
experience related, in part, to communication tell us little about the manner in which anxiety
apprehension, social evaluation, and test anxiety. operates.
Communication apprehension is a response to the There have been several studies that have used
real or anticipated act of speaking, in the same more specific indices of performance. Generally, the
manner as one may become anxious when speaking effects of language anxiety have been shown to be
the native language (Daly, 1991; McCroskey, 1978). pervasive over several types of tasks (Maclntyre &
Social evaluation apprehension emerges from the Gardner, 1992). Young (1986) reports correlations
social nature of language use, because second- between scores on an oral proficiency interview and
language communication involves self-presentation language anxiety. Tucker, Hamayan and Genesee
in a language with which only limited competency (1976) found that being more adventuresome, less
has been attained. The test anxiety component refers anxious, and more willing to use the foreign
to the academic nature of many language-learning language was associated with achievement in a
environments and would be relevant to those group of late immersion students. Gardner, Lalonde,
situations involving formal instruction. Horwitz et Moorcroft and Evers (1987) found that French class
al. (1986) further state that language anxiety should anxiety was significantly correlated with each of
be seen as more than the sum of these parts. They four proficiency measures (word production, theme
view language anxiety '...as a distinct complex of test, listening comprehension, and self-ratings of
self-perceptions, beliefs, feelings, and behaviours proficiency) in grade 12 and grade 13 students.
related to classroom language learning arising from Trylong (1987) found significant correlations of
the uniqueness of the language learning process' anxiety with achievement on written tests, oral
(p. 128). quizzes, and final grades in a first-year university
Evidence in favour of Horwitz et a/.'s theory has French course.
been accumulating rapidly. Horwitz (1986) found Experimental investigations have tried to induce
that other measures of communication apprehen- anxiety in a controlled environment in order to
sion, social evaluation anxiety and test anxiety each study its effects. A study by Steinberg and Horwitz
correlated significantly with anxiety in the language (1986) examined two groups. In one, anxiety was
classroom. Further, language anxiety was sig- aroused by impersonal treatment and videotaping
nificantly, negatively correlated with expected students while they described ambiguous pictures.
and obtained grades in the language course. Along A second group was treated in a friendly manner
similar lines, Maclntyre and Gardner (1989) used and was not videotaped. The group in which
factor analysis to identify two anxiety dimensions, anxiety was induced was found to be significantly
General anxiety and Communicative anxiety. Two less interpretative than the more relaxed group
of the four scales defining Communicative anxiety when describing the scenes. A later study by
referred specifically to second-language situations. Gardner, Day and Maclntyre (1992) found that
Further analyses revealed that Communicative simply introducing a video camera is not sufficient
anxiety negatively influenced second-language vo- to produce anxiety. They suggest that the social
State of the art: Second language learning. Part 2
interaction in Steinberg & Horwitz's study was a as experience and proficiency increase, anxiety
key element in producing differences in the two declines in a fairly consistent manner.
groups. Finally, Maclntyre and Gardner (1991) The theoretical models and related research
asked beginning adult language students to write a discussed thus far have considered the negative,
brief essay on either an anxiety-provoking ex- sometimes debilitating, effects of language anxiety.
perience or a confidence-building one. The groups The suggestion has been made, however, that some
who wrote about anxiety tended to perceive anxiety may be helpful in second-language learning
themselves as less proficient than the group who (Scovel, 1978). Kleinmann (1977) reports positive
described a confidence building experience, and the correlations between this 'facilitating anxiety' and
most anxiety-provoking experience reported was use of difficult linguistic structures. On balance,
almost always related to speaking. however, this hypothesis has not been well sup-
One theory of the development of language ported. In almost all studies that employ a measure
anxiety has been offered by Maclntyre and Gardner specifically related to language anxiety (rather than
(1989). They propose that it develops as the result of a more general anxiety measure) negative cor-
repeated, negative experiences with the second relations with achievement have been obtained.
language. Language anxiety is seen as a learned Moreover, the similarities between facilitating
emotional response. At the earliest stages, the anxiety and motivation are striking, both in how
language learner may experience a form of state they are measured and their conceptual definitions.
anxiety, a transient apprehensive experience. After In fact, it has been suggested that the facilitating
repeated occurrences of state anxiety, the student effects of anxiety could be attributable to motivation
will come to reliably associate anxiety with per- (Gardner et al., 1992).
formance in the second language. Once it has The results of these studies of language anxiety
developed, language anxiety can have a pervasive suggest that anxious students will have lower levels
impact on the performance of language students, of verbal production, will have difficulty in basic
even affecting future learning. The negative effects learning and production, will be less likely to
of language anxiety would be expected to diminish volunteer answers in class, and will be reluctant to
over time as proficiency increases and more positive
express personally relevant information in a second-
experiences accumulate.
language conversation. Further, it would appear
This model has two different components. At the that language anxiety arises from early negative
earliest stages of language learning, language anxiety experiences, particularly with speaking. While
is not very meaningful because negative experiences language anxiety may be high initially, it would be
have not produced the negative affect or expec- expected to decline as the student gains proficiency,
tations of failure that typify anxiety. After several provided that the student continues to study or use
negative experiences, however, the relation between the second language.
anxiety and performance should be observed. A In some ways, the antithesis of the anxious
study by Chapelle and Roberts (1986) found that student is the self-confident one (Maclntyre &
the correlation between English class anxiety and Gardner, 1991). In multicultural settings, however,
TOEFL scores at the beginning of a semester was self confidence may mean more than a lack of
not significant; however, by the end of the semester, anxiety. Cle'ment (1980, 1986) considers self con-
the correlation had increased to a significant level. fidence to be a superordinate construct, encom-
The suggestion that anxiety levels are highest passing both a lack of anxiety and positive self-
early on in language learning and then decline as ratings of proficiency in interactions with members
proficiency increases also has been supported by of the target language community. In a series of
research. Gardner, Smythe and Brunet (1977) factor analytic studies of francophones learning
compared beginning, intermediate, and advanced English in bilingual communities, Clement and his
students of French on subtests of the AMTB before colleagues found evidence for a Self-confidence
and after an intensive summer-school language factor. Clement, Major, Gardner and Smythe (1977)
course. The highest levels of anxiety were shown by found a self-confidence factor that was defined by
the beginners and the least by the advanced students. positive teacher ratings, positive course evaluation,
All three groups showed less French class anxiety at use of the second language outside the classroom
the end of the course than they did at the beginning. and a lack of language anxiety. Clement, Gardner
Desrochers and Gardner (1981) found that anxiety and Smythe (1977, 1980) found that self-confidence
levels declined significantly among English-speaking could be defined by a lack of language anxiety and
grade-eight students after a four-day excursion to a positive self-rated proficiency in the second lan-
French-speaking community. Among adult stu- guage. In all three studies, self-confidence was found
dents, Gardner, Smythe and Clement (1979) found to be related to objective measures of proficiency, as
that proficiency increased while anxiety decreased well as indices of motivation.
after an intensive French summer-school pro- Gardner, Smythe and Lalonde (1984) investigated
gramme. The results of these studies indicate that the factor structure of similar scales in their study of
State of the art: Second language learning. Part 2
English Canadian students of French across different and attitudinal/motivational components. Another
regions of Canada and various grade levels. The is that the underlying construct is the same in both
factors were found to replicate fairly consistently, multicultural and unicultural contexts, but that the
and language anxiety was most often associated focus of the different researchers, and the measures
with a factor related to the self-perception of French used, emphasise slightly different aspects. Clearly,
competence and, less often, a French achievement more research is needed to clarify which possibility
factor. These authors conclude that when the is most appropriate. In any event, indices of lan-
opportunity to use the foreign language is present guage anxiety are generally associated with self-
in the community, language anxiety is negatively perceptions of competency and objective measures
correlated with second language proficiency. of proficiency and often show low but negative
Clement's model has emerged from a com- correlations with motivation. In both research
bination of these studies with the work of social traditions, too, language anxiety is shown to be
psychologists on the social consequences of language associated with experiences with the second lan-
acquisition and has received empirical support guage.
(Clement & Kruidenier, 1985). Clement's theory Such considerations lead to the generalisation that
(1980, 1986) postulates two motivational processes language anxiety has a negative effect on second-
present in bilingual/muticultural communities. The language acquisition. Its relation to motivational
primary motivational process is based on the characteristics is less clear cut, and it may well be
antagonistic interplay between integrativeness and that this is mediated by socio-cultural factors.
the desire to affiliate with the other group, on the Intuitively, it seems reasonable to propose that
one hand, and fear of assimilation and worry over high levels of anxiety might serve to lessen one's
losing one's cultural identity, on the other. Clement motivation to learn the language, because the
(1980) argues that the result of this process will experience is found to be painful, and that high
determine the extent to which individuals make levels of motivation result in low levels of anxiety
contact with members of the other group. The because the student perceives the experience posi-
secondary motivational process is based on the tively and tends to be successful - both of which
frequency and quality of contact between the student decrease anxiety. Nonetheless, further research is
and members of the target language community. required to determine the precise nature of the
This contact, if positive, can improve self con- relationship, if any, between anxiety and mo-
fidence with the new language, thus contributing to tivation.
the student's motivation. Within monolingual
communities, self confidence would be less influ-
ential because of fewer instances of intergroup A theoretical integration
contact. In Part I of this review (Gardner & Maclntyre, 1992)
Labrie and Clement (1986) found support for this attention was directed toward the relation to second-
model in that less frequent, but positive contact was language learning of three classes of cognitive
associated with improved self confidence. Also, self individual difference variables, namely, Intelligence,
confidence was associated with motivation to use Language aptitude, and Language-learning stra-
the second language. In a similar manner, Pak, Dion tegies. In this part, attention focused on attitudes,
and Dion (1985) investigated Chinese immigrants motivation, language anxiety and self-confidence.
learning English in a large city and found that self This final section is devoted to providing a
confidence with English (as defined by self-rated theoretical overview of how such factors can
proficiency only) was associated with linguistic influence, and in some cases be influenced by, the
assimilation into the English speaking community. language-learning process. This overview is pre-
Thus self-confidence, in a multicultural context, sented in the form of a revised version of the socio-
appears to have motivational as well as anxiety- educational model of second-language learning that
related components. was presented in Part I (Gardner & Maclntyre,
Research on language anxiety and self-confidence 1992). This revised model is presented in Figure 1.
indicates that these variables play an important role In the figure, the socio-cultural milieu is shown as
in second language learning. Language anxiety is over-riding all aspects of the model. That is, when
negatively related to proficiency whereas self- considering the process of second-language ac-
confidence is positively related. The processes quisition, it is recommended that close attention be
underlying the two related concepts appear similar directed to the social context in which the learning
in that they both develop as a result of experiences is taking place. Although Au (1988) argues that such
associated with learning and using the second a concept 'may serve only to render the theory
language. One possibility is that a multicultural immune to disconfirming evidence, thereby gran-
social milieu enhances correlates of language anxiety ting infallibility to the theory...' (p. 85), omission
and produces a more complex construct combining of such a concept would make the theory much too
language anxiety, self-perceptions of proficiency simplistic. Rather, what is required is research that
State of the art: Second language learning. Part 2
Socio-cultural milieu

Antecedent Individual Language Outcomes


factors difference acquisition
variables contexs

Intelligence
••

Language
1—•
aptitude

. Formal

—•
' > Strategies - - - •
Linguistic
Biological
\ /

Experiential
Language
— • attitudes

/ >- Non-
linguistic
1—•
— • Motivation Informal
A
1

Language ,—h
anxiety
* ••

Figure 1. Schematic representation of the Socio-education model of second-language acquisition.

delineates the significant features of the social milieu biological and experiential in the model, with no
that influences the role of individual differences in exemplars or roles shown. Examples of such factors,
language acquisition. Some research based on a however, would include gender, age, prior language
different theoretical model is already under way in training, etc. Rather than attempt to describe all the
this regard (see, for example, Clement, 1980, 1986; possible roles that such factors might play, it seems
Labrie & Clement, 1986), while at least one study best to propose that, at a minimum, they should
(Clement & Kruidenier, 1983) demonstrated that be considered by an investigator when planning a
even the reasons for studying a second language study. It seems reasonable to propose, for example,
tended to group together to form different orien- that there is a biological foundation for intelligence,
tations as a function of the socio-cultural milieu. that age might influence what language-learning
Moreover, Kraemer (1990) demonstrated that the strategies are used, or that prior experience with a
socio-educational model could be modified suc- language could influence levels of language attitudes,
cessfully to take into consideration social forces motivation and/or language anxiety. Clearly, any
that might operate in a specific language-learning study of individual differences in second-language
situation. It is simplistic to assume that a language- acquisition would have to ensure that subjects were
learning model should not make explicit recognition relatively homogeneous with respect to prior
of the very dominant role played by the social training in that language in order to rule out
context. Would one really expect, for example, that confounding effects of prior achievement in the
attitudes toward the other community would be current language-learning process.
related positively to the motivation to learn the Six major individual difference variables are
language of that community by military personnel shown in Fig. 1. Intelligence, language aptitude, and
who may consider the community a potential language-learning strategies are considered as cog-
enemy ? nitive individual difference variables. In the model
The model posits that there are a number of they are shown as being relatively independent of
Antecedent factors that must also be considered one another (they are not linked by any directional
when attempting to study the role of individual arrows), even though it is recognised that they
difference variables in the process of learning a might correlate significantly in any study. The
second language. These are described simply as measures might well share some variance in
State of the art: Second language learning. Part 2
common, but it seems reasonable to assume that a will simply not take part in the context, while those
factor analytic study with sufficient marker variables who are motivated will. Once an individual enters
would show that the effective variances are in fact an informal context, however, the other individual
independent. This has been shown to be the case, at difference variables will be implicated and could
least, for intelligence and language aptitude (Gardner influence how well material is learned. As above,
& Lambert, 1965). the role played will be influenced by the relevance
Language attitudes, motivation and language of the task to that particular variable. The indirect
anxiety are considered as affective individual dif- effects of the other variables are shown by the
ference variables. The concept of language attitudes broken arrows linking them to the informal
refers to any attitudinal variables that might be contexts. No link is shown between language
implicated in the language-learning context. Earlier, attitudes and the informal language learning context.
two major classes, Integrativeness and Attitudes This is because it is assumed that motivation medi-
toward the learning situation were discussed, but ates any relation between language attitudes and
others have been investigated (see, for example language achievement.
Gliksman, 1981; Clement & Kruidenier, 1985; Both formal and informal language acquisition
Kraemer, 1990). In the model, language attitudes contexts are assumed to have direct effects on both
are shown as having a causal influence on motivation linguistic and non-linguistic outcomes (the solid
(as indicated by the directional arrow). The point is, arrows). Thus, the model formally recognises the
that motivation needs an affective basis to be importance of what takes place in both contexts.
maintained, and it seems reasonable to argue that Teachers, instructional aids, curricula, and the like
attitudes serve this function. In the figure, moti- clearly have an effect on what is learned and how
vation is shown as having a causal influence and as students react to the experience. Moreover, the
being causally influenced by language anxiety (the model postulates a causal link from the linguistic
two arrows). These two variables tend to be outcomes to the non-linguistic outcomes, a recog-
negatively correlated, and it seems meaningful to nition that individuals' reactions to the learning
argue, given our current state of knowledge, that experience will depend to some extent on their
not only might high levels of motivation tend to relative degree of success. Finally, it is proposed that
depress language anxiety but also that high levels of both linguistic and non-linguistic outcomes will
anxiety might decrease motivation. The model also have an influence on individual difference variables.
shows causal links from language anxiety and Based on the research to date (Oxford & Crookall,
motivation to language-learning strategies. The 1989; Oxford & Nyikos, 1989) it seems clear that
research by Oxford and Nyikos (1989) and by Rost achievement in the language will influence the use
and Ross (1991) points to the motivational founda- of language-learning strategies. For this reason,
tion for the use of language-learning strategies while linguistic outcomes are shown as having an effect on
anxiety reduction techniques could be considered as language-learning strategy use. Non-linguistic out-
indirect learning strategies (Oxford, 1990a). comes are expected in turn to have direct effects
Obviously, more research is required, but at the on language attitudes, motivation and language
present time, it seems meaningful to postulate such anxiety.
causal connections. Other variables might be added to this model
When attention is directed to the Language with no loss in generality, but the intent of this two-
acquisition contexts, all of the individual difference part article was simply to review the major ones.
variables, with the exception of language attitudes, Thus, for example, some research has demonstrated
are shown as having a direct effect on learning in the links between personality variables and language
formal language-learning environment (solid di- achievement (see, for example, Lalonde, 1982;
rectional arrows). This is meant to indicate that in Ehrman, 1990; Oxford, 1990 b) and between lan-
any learning situation where material or skill is guage achievement and learning style variables such
being transmitted to a learner in some way, as field dependence/independence (Oxford, 19906;
individual differences in intelligence, language Chapelle & Green, 1992). These variables were not
aptitude, the use of language-learning strategies, considered in this article, in part because of space
motivation and language anxiety will influence how limitations and in part because of our belief that
successful that individual will be in acquiring that their functional roles in the language-learning
material or skill. Each will play a role in its own process are still not clear. Further research could
way, some of which were discussed or hypothesised well profit from a consideration of their relationship
in earlier sections of this article. to variables discussed in this two-part article.
Only motivation is shown to have a direct role Cook's review of the literature in 1978 dem-
in the informal context (the dotted arrow from onstrated the breadth of variables that can influence
motivation to informal contexts). Because of the language acquisition. Over the past 15 years, the
voluntary nature of the informal context, it is boundaries have been greatly expanded and our
expected that individuals who are not motivated knowledge base deepened. The sheer volume of
State of the art: Second language learning. Part 2
research currently available points to the complexity linguistic perspective. Language Teaching and Linguistics:
of the language-learning process and the need for Abstracts, II, 73-89.
further research to consider the many remaining CROOKES, G. & SCHMIDT, R. W. (1991). Motivation: reopening
the research agenda. Language Learning, 41, 469-512.
questions. As this review attests, there is still much DALY,J. (1991). Understanding communication apprehension:
to be done. Hopefully, the theoretical formulation an introduction for language educators. In E. K. Horwitz, &
proposed here will help in this regard. D. J. Young (eds.), Language anxiety: from theory and research
We would like to thank the Social Sciences and Humanities to classroom implications. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Research Council of Canada for their support by grant no. 410- DESROCHERS, A. & GARDNER, R. C. (1981). Second language
90-0195 to the first author and Doctoral Fellowship no. 453-91- acquisition: an investigation of a bicultural excursion experience.
1277 to the second author. Quebec: International Centre for Research on Bilingualism.
DORNYEI, Z. (1990). Conceptualising motivation in foreign
language learning. Language Learning, 40, 45-78.
ERHMAN, M. (1990). The role of personality type in adult
References language learning: an ongoing investigation. In T. S. Parry,
& C. W. Stansfield (eds.), Language aptitude reconsidered,
ARSENIAN, S. (1945). Bilingualism in the post-war world. pp. 126-78. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall Regents.
Psychological Bulletin, 42, 65-86. ENDLER, N. S. & OKADA, M. (1975). A multidimensional
Au, S. Y. (1988). A critical appraisal of Gardner's social- measure of trait anxiety: the S-R Inventory of general trait
psychological theory of second-language (L2) learning. anxiousness. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43,
Language Learning, 38, 75-100. 319-29.
BACKMAN, N. (1976). Two measures of affective factors as they EYSENCK, M. W. (1979). Anxiety, learning and memory: a
relate to progress in adult second-language learning. Working reconceptualisation. Journal of Research in Personality, 13,
Papers on Bilingualism, 10, 100-22. 363-85.
BARTLEY, D. E. (1969). A pilot study of aptitude and attitude GARDNER, R. C. (1985). Social psychology and second language
factors in language dropout. California Journal of Educational learning: the role of attitudes and motivation. London: Edward
Research, 20, 48-55. Arnold.
BARTLEY, D. E. (1970). The importance of the attitude factor in GARDNER, R. C , DAY, B. & MACINTYRE, P. D. (1992).
language dropout: a preliminary investigation of group and Integrative motivation, induced anxiety, and language
sex differences. Foreign Language Annals, 3, 383-93. learning in a controlled environment. Studies in Second
BURSTALL, C , JAMIESON, M , COHEN, S. & HARGREAVES, M. Language Acquisition, 14, 197-214.
(1974). Primary French in the balance. Windsor, Berks.: NFER.GARDNER, R. C., LALONDE, R. N. & MOORCROFT, R. (1985).
CHAPELLE, C. & GREEN, P. (1992). Field independence/ The role of attitudes and motivation in second language
dependence in second-language acquisition research. Language learning: correlational and experimental considerations.
Learning, 42, 47-83. Language Learning, 35, 207-27.
CHAPELLE, C. & ROBERTS, C. (1986). Ambiguity tolerance and GARDNER, R. C , LALONDE, R. N., MOORCROFT, R. & EVERS, F.
field independence as predictors of proficiency in English as T. (1987). Second language attrition: the role of motivation
a second language. Language Learning, 36, 27-45. and use. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 6, 29-47.
CHASTAIN, K. (1975). Affective and ability factors in second- GARDNER, R. C. & LAMBERT, W. E. (1959). Motivational
language acquisition. Language Learning, 25, 153-61. variables in second language acquisition. Canadian Journal of
CLEMENT, R. (1980). Ethnicity, contact and communicative Psychology, 13, 266-72.
competence in a second language. In H. Giles, W. P. GARDNER, R. C. & LAMBERT, W. E. (1965). Language aptitude,
Robinson & P. M. Smith (eds.), Language: social psychological intelligence, and second-language achievement. Journal of
perspectives. Oxford: Pergamon. Educational Psychology, 56, 191-9.
CLEMENT, R. (1986). Second language proficiency and accul- GARDNER, R. C. & LAMBERT, W. E. (1972). Attitudes and
turation : an investigation of the effects of language status motivation in second language learning. Rowley, Mass.: New-
and individual characteristics. Journal of Language and Social bury House.
Psychology, 5, 271-90. GARDNER, R. C. & MACINTYRE, P. D. (1991). An instrumental
CLEMENT, R., GARDNER, R. C. & SMYTHE, P. C. (1977). motivation in language study: who says it isn't effective?
Motivational variables in second language acquisition: a Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 13, 57-72.
study of francophones learning English. Canadian Journal of GARDNER, R. C. & MACINTYRE, P. D. (1992). A student's
Behavioural Science, 9, 123-33. contributions to second language learning: Part I. Cognitive
CLE'MENT, R., GARDNER, R. C. & SMYTHE, P. C. (1980). Social variables. Language Teaching, 25, 4, 211-20.
and individual factors in second language acquisition. GARDNER, R. C. & MACINTYRE, P. D. (in press). On the
Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 12, 293-302. measurement of affective variables in second language
CLEMENT, R. & KRUIDENIER, B. G. (1983). Orientations in learning. Language Learning.
second language acquisition. I. The effects of ethnicity, GARDNER, R. C. & SMYTHE, P. C. (1981). On the development
milieu, and target language on their emergence. Language of the Attitude/Motivation Test Battery. Canadian Modern
Learning, 33, 273-91. Language Review, 37, 510-25.
CLEMENT, R. & KRUIDENIER, B. G. (1985). Aptitude, attitude GARDNER, R. C , SMYTHE, P. C. & BRUNET, G. R. (1977).
and motivation in second language proficiency: a test of Intensive second language study: effects on attitudes,
Clement's model. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 4, motivation and French achievement. Language Learning, 27,
21-37. 243-61.
CLEMENT, R., MAJOR, L. J., GARDNER, R. C. & SMYTHE, P. C. GARDNER, R. C , SMYTHE, P. C. & CLE'MENT, R. (1979). Intensive
(1977). Attitudes and motivation in second language ac- second language study in a bicultural milieu: an investigation
quisition: an investigation of Ontario francophones. Working of attitudes, motivation and language proficiency. Language
Papers on Bilingualism, 12, 1-20. Learning, 29, 305-20.
CLEMENT, R., SMYTHE, P. C. & GARDNER, R. C. (1978). GARDNER, R. C , SMYTHE, P. C , CLEMENT, R. & GLIKSMAN, L.
Persistence in second language study: motivational con- (1976). Second language acquisition: a social psychological
siderations. Canadian Modern Language Review, 34, 688-94. perspective. Canadian Modem Language Review, 32, 198-
COOK, V.J. (1978). Second-language learning: a psycho- 213.
10
State of the art: Second language learning. Part 2
GARDNER, R. C. & SMYTHE, P. C. & LALONDE, R. N. (1984). effects of language anxiety on second language performance.
The nature and replicability of factors in second language Canadian Psychology, 33, 276 (Abstract C#ll).
acquisition. Research Bulletin No. 605, Department of MARCKWARDT, A. H. (1948). Motives for the study of modern
Psychology, The University of Western Ontario, London, languages. Language Learning, 1, 3-99. (Reprinted in Language
Canada. Learning (1988), 38, 161-9.)
GLIKSMAN, L. (1976). Second language acquisition: the effects of MCCROSKEY, J. C. (1978). Validity of the PRCA as an index of
student attitudes on classroom behaviour. Unpublished master's oral communication apprehension. Communication Mono-
thesis, University of Western Ontario, London, Canada. graphs, 45, 192-203.
GLIKSMAN, L. (1981). Improving the prediction of behaviours MUCHNICK, A. G. & WOLFE, D. E. (1982). Attitudes and
associated with second language acquisition. Unpublished motivations of American students of Spanish. The Canadian
Doctoral dissertation, University of Western Ontario, Modern Language Review, 38, 262-81.
London, Canada. NAIMAN, N., FROHLICH, M., STERN, H. H. & TODESCO, A.
GLIKSMAN, L., GARDNER, R. C. & SMYTHE, P. C. (1982). The (1978). The good language learner. Research in Education Scries
role of the integrative motive on students' participation in No. 7, Ontario Institute for Studies in Education, Toronto,
the French classroom. Canadian Modern Language Review, 38, Ontario.
625^7. OXFORD, R. L. (1990a). Language learning strategies: what every
GORDON, M. E. (1980). Attitudes and motivation in second language teacher should know. New York: Newbury House.
achievement: a study of primary school students learning English OXFORD,
in R. L. (1990fe). Styles, strategies, and aptitude:
Belize, Central America. Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, connections for language learning. In T. S. Parry & C. W.
University of Toronto. Stansfield (eds.), Language aptitude reconsidered, 67-125.
HoRwrrz, E. K. (1986). Preliminary evidence for the reliability Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice Hall Regents.
and validity of a foreign language anxiety scale. TESOL OXFORD, R. L. & CROOKALL, D. (1989). Research on language
Quarterly, 20, 559-62. learning strategies: methods, findings, and instructional issues.
HoRwrrz, E. K., HORWITZ, M. B. & COPE, J. (1986). Foreign Modern Language Journal, 73, 404-19.
language classroom anxiety. Modern Language Journal, 70, OXFORD, R. L. & NYIKOS, M. (1989). Variables affecting choice
125-32. of language learning strategies by university students. Modern
HORWITZ, E. K. & YOUNG, D.J. (1991). Language anxiety. Language Journal, 75, 292-300.
Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice Hall. PAK, A. W.-P., DION, K. L. & DION, K. K. (1985). Correlates
JORESKOG, K. G. & SORBOM, D. (1984). LISREL VI: Analysis of of self-confidence with English among Chinese students in
linear structural relationships by the method of maximum likelihood. Toronto. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 17, 369-78.
Mooresville, Indiana: Scientific Software Inc. RAM AGE, K. (1990). Motivational factors and persistence in
KLEINMANN, H. H. (1977). Avoidance behavior in adult second foreign language study. Language Learning, 40, 189-219.
language acquisition. Language Learning, 27, 93-107. ROGER, D., BULL, P. & FLETCHER, R. (1981). The construction
KRAEMER, R. (1990). Social psychological factors related to the study and validation of a questionnaire for measuring attitudes
of Arabic among Israeli Jewish high school students. Unpublished towards learning foreign languages. Educational Review, 33,
Doctoral dissertation, Tel-Aviv University. 223-30.
LABRIE, N. & CLEMENT, R. (1986). Ethnolinguistic vitality, self- ROST, M. & Ross, S. (1991). Learner use of strategies in
confidence and second language proficiency: an investigation. interaction: typology and teachability. Language Learning, 41,
Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 7, 235-73.
269-82. SCHWARZER, R. (1986). Self-related cognition in anxiety and
LAINE, E. (1977). Foreign language learning motivation in Finlandmotivation. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
I. Turku: Turku University Press. SCOVEL, T. (1978). The effect of affect on foreign language
LALONDE, R. (1982). Second language acquisition: a causal analysis.learning: a review of the anxiety research. Language Learning,
Unpublished Master's thesis, University of Western Ontario, 28, 129-42.
London, Canada. SISON, M. P. A. (1991). The relationship of college student attitudes,
LALONDE, R. N. & GARDNER, R. C. (1985). On the predictive motivation, and anxiety to second language achievement: a test of
validity of the Attitude/Motivation Test Battery. Journal of Gardner's model. Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, Uni-
Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 6, 403-12. versity of Southern California.
LAMBERT, W. E. (1955). Measurement of the linguistic domi- STEINBERG, F. S. & HORWITZ, E. K. (1986). The effect of
nance of bilinguals._/oMiW of Abnormal and Social Psychology, induced anxiety on the denotative and interpretive content of
50, 197-200. second language speech. TESOL Quarterly, 20, 131-6.
LETT, J. & O'MARA, F. E. (1990). Predictors of success in an STRONG, M. H. (1984). Integrative motivation: cause or result
intensive language learning context: correlates of language of successful second-language acquisition? Language Learning,
learning at the Defense Language Institute Foreign Language 34, 1-14.
Center. In T. S. Parry & C. W. Stansfield (eds.), Language TRYLONG, V. L. (1987). Aptitude, attitudes, and anxiety: a study of
aptitude reconsidered, 222-260. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Pren- their relationships to achievement in the foreign language classroom.
tice Hall Regents. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Purdue University.
LUKMANI, Y. M. (1972). Motivation to learn and learning TUCKER, G. R., HAMAYAN, E. & GENESEE, F. H. (1976).
proficiency. Language Learning, 22, 261-73. Affective, cognitive and social factors in second-language
MACINTYRE, P. D. & GARDNER, R. C. (1989). Anxiety and acquisition. Canadian Modern Language Review, 32, 214-26.
second language learning: toward a theoretical clarification. YOUNG, D.J. (1986). The relationship between anxiety and
Language Learning, 39, 251-75. foreign language oral proficiency ratings. Foreign Language
MACINTYRE, P. D. & GARDNER, R. C. (1991). Language anxiety: Annals, 12, 439-48.
its relation to other anxieties and to processing in native and YOUNG, D.J. (1990). An investigation of students' perspectives
second languages. Language Learning, 41, 513-34. on anxiety and speaking. Foreign Language Annals, 23,
MACINTYRE, P. D. & GARDNER, R. C. (1992). The pervasive' 539-53.

11

You might also like