Persuasion. Advertisement

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/262678032

Consumers' green involvement and the persuasive effects of emotional


versus functional ads

Article  in  Journal of Business Research · September 2014


DOI: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2013.11.054

CITATIONS READS

155 2,683

3 authors:

Jörg Matthes Anke Wonneberger


University of Vienna University of Amsterdam
272 PUBLICATIONS   9,445 CITATIONS    43 PUBLICATIONS   717 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Desirée Schmuck
KU Leuven
54 PUBLICATIONS   1,155 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

The who, what and how of mediated climate controversies View project

THREATPIE: The Threats and Potentials of a Changing Political Information Environment View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Jörg Matthes on 16 December 2020.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached
copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research
and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution
and sharing with colleagues.
Other uses, including reproduction and distribution, or selling or
licensing copies, or posting to personal, institutional or third party
websites are prohibited.
In most cases authors are permitted to post their version of the
article (e.g. in Word or Tex form) to their personal website or
institutional repository. Authors requiring further information
regarding Elsevier’s archiving and manuscript policies are
encouraged to visit:
http://www.elsevier.com/authorsrights
Author's personal copy

Journal of Business Research 67 (2014) 1885–1893

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Business Research

Consumers' green involvement and the persuasive effects of emotional


versus functional ads
Jörg Matthes ⁎, Anke Wonneberger 1, Desirée Schmuck 1
Department of Communication, University of Vienna, Währingerstr. 29, 1090 Vienna, Austria

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This study compares the effects of four types of ads: a functional green ad promoting the environmental advan-
Received 7 April 2013 tages of a product, an emotional green ad using a visual representation of pleasant natural scenery, a mixed type
Received in revised form 11 September 2013 green ad using functional and emotional strategies, and a control group. Findings of an experimental study using
Accepted 29 November 2013
a representative sample of U.S. consumers suggest that both the emotional and the mixed-type ads significantly
Available online 26 December 2013
affect brand attitude, mediated by attitude toward the ad. These effects do not depend on consumers' green
Keywords:
involvement. Functional ads, in contrast, only impact brand attitudes when involvement, measured as green
Green advertising purchase behavior or green product attitudes, is high.
Green involvement © 2013 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Environmental concern
Brand attitude
Attitude toward the ad

1. Introduction a functional appeal, an emotional appeal, and a combination of the


two. Functional appeals use arguments about environmental friendly
A growing number of consumers are becoming concerned about product attributes or production processes. Emotional appeals, in
environmental degradation, waning natural resources, or threats associ- contrast, merely rely on visual representations of pleasant natural scen-
ated with climate change over the last decades (Dunlap & Scarce, 1991; ery. Combined appeals use both functional and emotional strategies in
Eurobarometer, 2011). Parallel to this trend, the use of environmental one ad.
appeals in advertising is very popular among practitioners and mar- Despite the practical relevance and widespread occurrence of func-
keters (Easterling, Kenworthy, & Nemzoff, 1996; Schmidt & Donsbach, tional, emotional and combined green ads, knowledge about their
2012). Especially in the early days of green marketing, companies distinct effects remains scarce. The literature displays three research
were quick to adopt green claims, sometimes using delusive or exag- gaps. First, although the currently available body of knowledge suggests
gerated environmental appeals resulting in “greenwashing” (Gaski & that all three types of green ads exert a significant impact on brand and
Etzel, 1986; Kangun, Carlson, & Grove, 1991). In recent years, however, advertising evaluations (Hartmann & Apaolaza-Ibáñez, 2009; Hartmann
green claims have become more substantial and less ambiguous, et al., 2005), no single study sufficiently tests their independent effects.
which “expresses a more responsible approach to green advertising” Studies either compare a control group with an emotional (or functional)
(Leonidou, Leonidou, Palihawadana, & Hultman, 2011, p. 24). None- appeal (e.g., Davis, 1994; Grimmer & Woolley, 2012; Newman, Howlett,
theless, critics on green marketing are still cautious about various Burton, Kozup, & Heintz, 2012; Schuhwerk & Lefkoff-Hagius, 1995;
forms of greenwashing and their impact on growing confusion and Searles, 2010; Spack, Board, Crighton, Kostka, & Ivory, 2012), or they
cynicism among consumers (Dahl, 2010; Peattie & Crane, 2005). use a mix of emotional and functional appeals and check whether such
Given this ambivalent nature of green advertising, it is crucial to an ad is more persuasive than a non-green ad (Hartmann & Apaolaza-
understand how consumers respond to it. In today's green advertising, Ibáñez, 2009; Hartmann et al., 2005). Second, based on dual-process
Hartmann, Ibáñez, and Sainz (2005) distinguish three major appeals: models such as the Elaboration-Likelihood Model (ELM, Petty &
Cacioppo, 1990), the effectiveness of emotional versus functional
⁎ Corresponding author. Tel.: +43 1 4277 49307; fax: +43 1 4277 49316.
appeals should depend on consumers' green involvement. However,
E-mail addresses: joerg.matthes@univie.ac.at (J. Matthes), previous research ignores the moderating role of green involvement.
anke.wonneberger@univie.ac.at (A. Wonneberger), desiree.schmuck@univie.ac.at Third, a lion's share of prior research relies on student or convenience
(D. Schmuck). samples (e.g., Grimmer & Woolley, 2012; Newman et al., 2012;
URL's: http://advertisingresearch.univie.ac.at (J. Matthes),
Hartmann et al., 2005; Searles, 2010; Schuhwerk & Lefkoff-Hagius,
http://advertisingresearch.univie.ac.at (A. Wonneberger),
http://advertisingresearch.univie.ac.at (D. Schmuck). 1995; Spack et al., 2012; but see Grimmer & Bingham, 2013; Hartmann
1
Tel.: +43 1 4277 49308; fax: +43 1 4277 49316. & Apaolaza-Ibáñez, 2009). Since green consumerism significantly varies

0148-2963/$ – see front matter © 2013 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2013.11.054
Author's personal copy

1886 J. Matthes et al. / Journal of Business Research 67 (2014) 1885–1893

by age, education, and gender (Roberts, 1996), student samples lack pop- with a representative sample of 360 participants employs three experi-
ulation validity. mental conditions: a non-environmental ad, an environmental ad pro-
The present study attempts to fill these research gaps by investigat- viding information on the environmental features of the brand, and a
ing the distinct and combined effects of emotional versus functional nature ad which combines the environmental features with a visual
green advertising appeals. Furthermore, this study systematically tests design to evoke nature experiences. Hartmann and Apaolaza-Ibáñez
the moderating roles of several interrelated green involvement dimen- (2009) find that the association of a green brand with pleasant nature
sions. These dimensions are environmental concern, green purchase imagery in addition to the presentation of information on specific envi-
intentions, and attitudes toward green products. The analysis also con- ronmental product features leads to more positive brand attitudes, as
trols for the impact of key demographic characteristics by using a compared to the effect of informational claims alone. According to the
quota sample of U.S. consumers. authors, pleasant nature imagery leads to the association of specific
emotional experiences with the brand that are to some extent compara-
2. Functional versus emotional green ads ble to those consumers' experience through contact with nature. Inter-
estingly, depictions of landscapes with lush green vegetation and clear
Although scholars and marketers agree that green positioning is an water elicit more favorable emotional responses than other landscapes
essential factor in the market success of products and services, still re- or urban scenes, regardless of the natural environment in which indi-
search devotes minimal attention to the question of how the design of vidual live (Hartmann & Apaolaza-Ibáñez, 2012).
green claims affects the processing of the message and its effectiveness The evidence from these studies suggests that all three positioning
(Hartmann & Apaolaza-Ibáñez, 2009). Positioning a brand as a “green strategies exert positive overall influences on brand attitude. So far
brand” entails an active communication of its environmentally sound however, no study compares the three positioning strategies simulta-
attributes (Hartmann et al., 2005). According to classification schemes neously. In Hartmann et al.'s (2005) first study, the combined condition
of generic positioning strategies (Aaker, 1996), brand positioning uses contains only a reduced number of informational appeals. As a conse-
functional attributes and/or by emotional benefits. A green positioning quence, the design does not allow to accurately separate the three
strategy based on functional attributes should communicate the rele- appeals. Strictly speaking, the combined condition is not the sum of
vant environmental advantages of the product compared to competing the functional and the emotional conditions. Also, the student sample
conventional products. Such advantages may include, for example, warrants replication with a broader sample. Hartmann and Apaolaza-
environmentally friendly production processes, product use, or product Ibáñez (2009), in contrast, do not use a student sample. In this study,
elimination. The aim of this positioning strategy is to build brand asso- one condition combines the information about the environmental
ciations by delivering information on environmentally relevant utilitar- features of the brand with pleasant nature scenery. However, the
ian product attributes (Hartmann et al., 2005). Studies confirm that study does not include a condition to test the sole effect of pleasant na-
environmentally sound product arguments can affect purchase inten- ture scenery on brand attitude. Therefore, a study that simultaneously
tions (Hartmann & Apaolaza-Ibáñez, 2009; see also Roberts, 1996). examines the impact of all three positioning strategies seems warrant-
Emotional brand advertising, in contrast, aims to transfer affective ed. Furthermore, since the ads employ (functional) arguments versus
responses to the brand (Edell & Burke, 1987). Searles (2010) finds (emotional) heuristic cues, environmental involvement may moderate
that verbal cues based on enthusiasm appeals reinforce an individual's the effect of these positioning strategies.
positive attitude toward efforts to protect the environment and posi-
tively influence participants' pro-environmental views. Anxiety appeals 3. Environmental involvement
induce anti-environmental attitudes. However, verbal cues are not the
only way to evoke emotional responses. Chowdhury, Olsen, and A wealth of research suggests that consumers differ in how they re-
Pracejus (2008) show that a single dominant positive image might be spond to persuasive appeals. One key variable to explain this response is
sufficient to engender significantly positive affective responses. In the consumers' level of involvement. Involvement refers to the degree to
case of green advertising, visual representation of pleasant nature scen- which a person perceives an attitude object as personally relevant
ery may have this effect, based on the principle that both contact with (Petty & Cacioppo, 1990). As the Elaboration-Likelihood Model (ELM,
nature itself and exposure to nature images generate positive emotions Petty & Cacioppo, 1990) suggests, highly involved individuals are moti-
(Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989; Stokols, 1990). vated to process information that leads to message elaboration. For
However, only a few studies deal with the question of whether these people, arguments about the attitude object should exert more
functional attributes or emotional appeals exert a stronger effect influence on their opinions than emotional appeals. In contrast, less
on brand attitudes. Among the most notable of these studies is a se- involved consumers may rely on heuristic cues such as emotions or feel-
ries of experiments by Hartmann and colleagues. Hartmann et al. ings to form their opinions.
(2005) address this question in an experimental field study with In the context of environmental advertising, high involvement con-
160 university students. The authors design a functional positioning sumers process the arguments displayed in an advertisement. The
strategy, an emotional positioning strategy, and a combined strategy evaluation of a brand thus depends on the functional green arguments.
for a hypothetical car brand. In the functional experimental condition, Low involvement consumers are not motivated to process those argu-
the ad gives detailed information on how emissions are reduced. The ments. For these people, emotional appeals such as beautiful nature
image is identical to the one of the control group and has no emotional scenery should serve as a heuristic cue guiding attitude formation. How-
connotations. The emotional positioning ad uses pleasant nature imag- ever, involvement is a rather fuzzy concept with a myriad of different
ery without cues about functional product attributes. The third condi- conceptualizations and operationalizations, not only in advertising
tion combines both strategies. This ad includes the same image as the research but also in the more general persuasion literature. Regarding
second condition, but mentions only a reduced number of environmen- green involvement, there are at least three relevant conceptualizations,
tally utilitarian attributes. A conventional ad serves as control condition. (1) environmental concern, (2) attitudes toward green products, and
The findings of this study suggest that “a combined strategy, which ap- (3) green purchase behavior. While these facets of environmental
peals to both environmental consciousness and emotional benefits, involvement are related, they can have different antecedents and out-
will yield a stronger attitudinal effect than either functional or emotional comes. Therefore, research on the moderating effect of involvement
positioning strategies on their own” (p. 21). on green advertising effectiveness should take all three facets into
In a later study, Hartmann and Apaolaza-Ibáñez (2009) find that na- account.
ture imagery may also enhance the positive influence of informational A lion's share of studies defines involved green consumers as those
appeals on consumers' attitude towards a brand. A national survey who are highly concerned about the environment (D'Souza & Taghian,
Author's personal copy

J. Matthes et al. / Journal of Business Research 67 (2014) 1885–1893 1887

2005; Mohr, Eroğlu, & Ellen, 1998; Schuhwerk & Lefkoff-Hagius, 1995). H2. Emotional ads have a positive impact on brand attitudes by shaping
The concept of environmental concern involves an awareness of environ- ad attitudes.
mental problems combined with the perceived necessity of protecting
the environment. Environmental concern plays an important role in H3. Ads that combine functional and emotional appeals have a positive
green buying decisions (Schwartz & Miller, 1991). Furthermore, impact on brand attitudes by shaping ad attitudes.
scholars treat consumers who maintain positive attitudes toward green
products as highly involved (Chan, 2001; Roberts, 1996). Attitudes Moreover, environmental involvement may moderate the effects of
toward green products relate to the advantages, favorability, or the functional, emotional, and combined ads. According to the ELM, emotion-
quality of green products (Chang, 2011). Finally, actual green purchase al appeals should yield the strongest effects for individuals low in involve-
behavior is an indicator of environmental involvement (Chang, 2011; ment. Low involved consumers may simply base their opinions on the
Mohr et al., 1998; Schlegelmilch, Bohlen, & Diamantopoulos, 1996). impression of the typical green nature scenery without asking for more
This can be actual behavior or behavioral intentions (Schwepker & substantial information such as functional arguments. People high in
Cornwell, 1991; Stone, Barnes, & Montgomery, 1995). green involvement may find such an unmeaning ad deceptive which
Knowledge about the moderating roles of several green involvement should hamper positive attitudinal effects. Functional appeals—that is,
dimensions in the context of advertising effectiveness is scarce. Most appeals that are based on substantive arguments rather than emotion-
studies reveal that environmental claims exert stronger effects for eliciting images—should have a stronger impact for people high in in-
consumers high as opposed to low in environmental involvement. volvement, because low involvement individuals will not be motivated
Grimmer and Bingham (2013), for instance, observe a relationship be- to process the functional arguments, rendering the arguments ineffective.
tween the environmental performance of a company and consumers Since both substantive arguments and heuristic cues are present,
purchase intensions, especially for consumers high in environmental consumers may use both of them, or only one of them, in forming
concern. Similarly, Grimmer and Woolley (2012) compare the effects their impressions. Therefore, a first research question asks whether
of a personal benefit green ad with those of an environmental benefit the effects of combined ads are stronger for individuals with low or
green ad, and a control group. Unlike prior research, the authors also high green involvement. In addition, this study aims to examine the
operationalize environmental affect as an indicator of environmental above assumptions for several distinct dimensions of green involve-
involvement. As a result, “participants with low environmental affect ment. Since the correlation between these dimensions is usually high,
showed higher purchase intentions for the personal benefit message a second research question asks which of these dimensions will yield
over the pure environmental benefit message” (p. 13). Conversely, the the strongest moderating impact.
effects of a pure environmental benefit message compared to the
personal benefit message are stronger for consumers with high com- H4. The effects of functional green ads will be stronger for consumers
pared to low environmental affect. In contrast to these studies, findings high in green involvement.
by Schuhwerk and Lefkoff-Hagius (1995) suggest that less involved
H5. The effects of emotional green ads will be stronger for consumers
consumers react more positively to environmental claims compared to
low in green involvement.
highly involved consumers.
Taken together, the available findings on the moderating role of RQ1. Are the effects of combined functional and emotional appeals
environmental involvement are compatible with the ELM (see stronger for individuals with low or high green involvement?
Grimmer and Woolley (2012) for an explanation of Schuhwerk and
Lefkoff-Hagius' findings). When effects of green ads are stronger RQ2. Which dimension of green involvement—environmental concern,
for high involvement individuals, scholars apply environmental attitudes toward green products, or green purchase behavior—is the
claims that contain some kind of argument (e.g., an environmental strongest moderator of green ad effects?
benefit). However, these designs do not include emotional appeals.
It follows that previous research is not sensitive to the possible vari- 4. Method
ety of green advertising appeals and green involvement dimensions.
No research systematically compares the effects of functional versus 4.1. Design, sample, and procedure
emotional green claims depending on the distinct dimensions of
consumers' environmental involvement. Based on the ELM, the pres- U.S. citizens (n = 484) participated in a 1 × 4 (green appeal: emo-
ent study theorizes that all three types of green ads—functional, tional, functional, combined, none) between subjects experiment in
emotional, and combined appeals—may yield significant effects on August/September 2012. The online experiment was part of a larger
brand attitudes. However, the pattern of effects may be completely survey (n = 1015) that also included a second experimental study.
different depending on the type of the green appeal and consumers' The company Survey Sampling International (SSI) recruited respon-
green involvement. dents from an online access panel. The company provided incentives
To test these assumptions, this study further distinguishes two for successful participation. The response rate for online data collection
outcome variables, attitude toward the ad and attitude toward the was RR1 = .62. Quota sampling yielded a high agreement with the
brand. More specifically, attitude toward the ad may mediate the ef- general U.S. population in terms of age (M = 43.9, SD = 16.3), gender
fects of ad claims on brand attitudes (see for this reasoning, Mitchell (57% female), and education (6.0% completed some high school, 69.2%
& Olson, 1981; for green ads, Schuhwerk & Lefkoff-Hagius, 1995). high school graduate, 24.8% college degree). The completion time of
Rephrased, green appeals impact brand attitudes because they the survey took is 10 min on average (median = 8 min).
shape ad liking. Since previous studies find functional and emotional This study employs four versions of a print ad for an ecological laun-
green appeals as well as combined forms to be effective generally dry detergent (see Appendix B). The experimental conditions vary
(Hartmann & Apaolaza-Ibáñez, 2009; Hartmann et al., 2005), this in their visual and textual layout. The emotional appeal condition
mechanism may apply for all three types of green appeals. Also, all (n = 133) depicts the product, brand name, and a slogan together
three types of green ads use green elements that should generally with a scenic nature image in the background. Following the studies
make them more persuasive compared to a control group. This leads by Hartmann et al. (2005), the image shows a lake surrounded by
to the following hypotheses: mountains, forests, and a waterfall. Austrian students rated this picture
as significantly positive in a pretest (n = 28). According to this pretest,
H1. Functional ads have a positive impact on brand attitudes by the picture evokes positive emotions such as happiness and satisfaction.
shaping ad attitudes. The functional condition (n = 116) shows product, brand, and slogan
Author's personal copy

1888 J. Matthes et al. / Journal of Business Research 67 (2014) 1885–1893

along with a list of six brief ecological advantages of the detergent in toward marketing is a key predictor of advertising effectiveness
front of a neutral background. The arguments are arguments used in (Mohr et al., 1998). Negative sentiment toward marketing consists
real ads for laundry detergents (see Appendix B). A second pretest of of two items (Mohr et al., 1998), (Cronbach's α = .67; M = 3.67,
n = 256 Austrian students suggests that these arguments are signifi- SD = 1.40). Fig. 1 shows the full model.
cantly perceived as “convincing”. The combined condition (n = 128)
unites the nature image and the arguments. The ad of the control condi-
4.3. Data analysis
tion presents a picture of the product and the slogan in front of a neutral
background (n = 107).
The analysis employs the Full Information Maximum Likeli-
Before presentation of the stimulus, the experimenter asked partici-
hood (FIML) method (Enders & Bandalos, 2001). Comparative Fit
pant to carefully examine the following ad. They could freely choose to
Index (CFI), Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA),
end exposure and continue with the survey via a mouse click. Plausibly,
and PCLOSE serve as the criteria to evaluate model fit. The experi-
exposure time in seconds varies significantly between the conditions;
mental groups are dummy coded and control group serves as the
F (3, 480) = 7.103, p b .001, with M = 30.96, SD = 28.03 for the
reference group (see Fig. 1). The model treats ad evaluation and
functional group, M = 32.95, SD = 49.62 for the combined group,
brand evaluation as latent variables, and the other variables as
M = 19.10, SD = 21.03 for the emotional group, and M = 18.21,
manifest. The model includes the interactive effects as multiplica-
SD = 16.10 for the control group. Exposure time is controlled in all sta-
tive terms of condition and moderator in moderated path regres-
tistical analyses.
sion analysis. This study uses continuous variable interactions as
opposed to multiple group analyses (see Irwin & McClelland,
2002).
4.2. Measures

Five seven-point semantic differential scales measure the two 5. Results


dependent variables (see Appendix A, attitude toward the ad:
Cronbach's α = .92; M = 5.28, SD = 1.31; attitude toward the The first three hypotheses aim at the main effects of functional,
brand: Cronbach's α = .96; M = 5.24, SD = 1.35). As for the inde- emotional and combined appeals. The corresponding model is a
pendent variables (1—“strongly disagree” and 7—“strongly structural equation model without the interactive effects as
agree”), the study employs three forms of green involvement depicted in Fig. 1. All standardized factor loadings on the latent
prior to the experimental stimulus. Environmental concern consists variables are above λ = .80. The fit is very good (Chi-square =
of four items based on Schuhwerk and Lefkoff-Hagius (1995), 290.97, df = 130, CFI = .98, RMSEA = .05, PCLOSE = .44). The
(Cronbach's α = .85; M = 5.24, SD = 1.19). Attitude toward emotional green ad (b = .58; p b .001) and the combined green
green products entails four items based on Chang (2011) ad (b = .63; p b .001) significantly affect attitude toward the ad
(Cronbach's α = .82; M = 4.96, SD = 1.13). Four items measure (see Table 1). The functional ad exerts a marginally significant
green purchase behavior (Kim & Choi, 2005; Cronbach's α = .89; effect on ad attitudes (b = .28; p = .07). Following expectations,
M = 4.43, SD = 1.41). The study adds two additional controls. the attitude toward the ad has a highly significant impact on the
First, perceived consumer effectiveness (PCE) is as a key variable in attitude toward the brand (b = .73; p b .001). This finding sug-
green advertising (Ellen, Wiener, & Cobb-Walgren, 1991; Kim & gests that the emotional and the combined green ads have an
Choi, 2005). PCE consists of two items (Ellen et al., 1991). These influence on brand attitudes through their effects on ad evalua-
are reverse coded to express consumer effectiveness (Cronbach's tions. This reasoning is supported by a mediation analysis.
α = .61; M = 4.51, SD = 1.48). Second, consumer sentiment The indirect effects of emotional (b = .43; p b .001) as well as

Item 1

Item 2

Functional Appeal Attitude toward


Item 3
the Ad
Item 4

Item 5
Emotional Appeal
Item 6

Item 7
Attitude toward
Combined Appeal Item 8
the Brand
Item 9

Item 10

Environmental Concern Attitude toward Green Green Purchase Behavior


Products

Fig. 1. Theoretical structural equation model.


Note. Control variables (age, gender, education, view time, perceived consumer effectiveness and negative sentiment), measurement errors, and correlations between all exogenous var-
iables were omitted from depiction for clarity reasons.
Author's personal copy

J. Matthes et al. / Journal of Business Research 67 (2014) 1885–1893 1889

Table 1
Structural equation modeling unstandardized path coefficients.

Independent variables Dependent variables

Attitude toward the ad Attitude toward the brand

b SE b SE

Age .00 .00 .00 .00


Sex −.09 .11 −.14+++ .08
Education −.07 .10 .05 .07
View time .00 .00 .00 .00
PCE .02 .04 .01 .03
NSM .08 .05 −.03 .04
Environmental concern .04 .06 −.05 .04
Attitudes toward green products .41⁎⁎⁎ .07 .21⁎⁎⁎ .05
Green purchase behavior .05 .06 −.01 .04
Functional green ad .28++ .15 .03 .11
Emotional green ad .58⁎⁎⁎ .15 .14 .11
Combined green ad .63⁎⁎⁎ .15 .24⁎ .11
Attitude toward the ad .73⁎⁎⁎ .04
Functional green ad ∗ environmental concern .15 .11 −.08 .08
Functional green ad ∗ attitudes toward green products .17+++ .10 −.01 .07
Functional green ad ∗ green purchase behavior .15+ .08 .01 .06
Emotional green ad ∗ environmental concern −.09 .10 .02 .07
Emotional green ad ∗ attitudes toward green products −.01 .09 .01⁎⁎ .07
Emotional green ad ∗ green purchase behavior −.03 .08 .00 .06
Combined green ad ∗ environmental concern .14 .09 −.02 .07
Combined green ad ∗ attitudes toward green products .07 .10 −.11 .07
Combined green ad ∗ green purchase behavior .11 .09 −.06 .06

Note. PCE = perceived consumer effectiveness. NSM = negative sentiment toward marketing.
⁎⁎⁎ p b .001.
⁎⁎ p b .01.
⁎ p b .05.
+++
p = .06.
++
p = .07.
+
p = .08.

combined (b = .46; p b .01) green ads on brand attitudes, medi- ads nor combined ads exhibit moderated effects (see Table 1). In
ated by ad attitudes, are significant. The indirect effect of func- other words, the effects of emotional and combined ads are
tional ads is also significant (b = .20; p b .05). These findings completely independent from environmental concern, attitudes
clearly support Hypotheses 2 and 3, and lend some support for toward green products, and green purchase behavior. This also
Hypothesis 1. answers research question 1.
The next step is to check whether the size of the effects differs However, the analysis reveals a marginally significant interac-
between conditions by a nested model comparison: This analysis tion between functional ad exposure and green purchase behavior
compares a model without any constraints to a model that restricts (b = .15; p = .06). The positive sign of this interaction signals that
the effects of the experimental groups to be equal. The effect of the effect of functional ad exposure increases with rising levels
the argument condition on ad evaluation is significantly different of green purchase behavior. To interpret this interaction, an addi-
(i.e., lower) from the effects of the emotional (Δχ 2 = 4.52, tional analysis probes the effect of functional appeals at low (one
p b .05) and the combined (Δχ 2 = 5.83, p b .05) conditions. The SD below the mean), moderate (at the mean) and high (one SD
latter two, however, do not significantly differ (Δχ 2 = .09, n.s.). above the mean) levels of green purchase behavior using manifest
This finding suggests that the emotional and the combined variables (i.e., not by splitting the sample, see Hayes & Matthes,
conditions yield stronger persuasive effects. However, only the 2009). Fig. 2 visualizes the results. Functional green ads (compared
combined condition shows a direct effect on brand attitudes (b = to the control group) have a significant effect for moderate
.24; p b .05). (b = .25; p b .05) and high (b = .39; p b .01) levels of green pur-
Regarding the three dimensions of green consumer involve- chase behavior. However, individuals who score low on green pur-
ment, only attitudes toward green products (b = .41; p b .001) chase behavior do not react with more favorable ad evaluations in
significantly relate to the attitude toward the ad. Attitudes toward response to the functional ad. A similar interaction pattern appears
green products also have a direct effect on brand attitudes (b =.21; for attitudes toward green products. The interaction effect is below
p b .01), and male respondents are marginally more negative in the conventional level of statistical significance (b = .17; p = .08)
their brand evaluations than females (b = − .09; p = .06). In but the pattern is precisely the same. When probed at low, moder-
total, the independent variables explain 31% of the variance of atti- ate, and high levels of green attitudes, the effects of functional ad
tude toward the ad and 71% of the variance of attitude toward the exposure are significant for respondents who report moderately
brand. positive (b = .25; p b .05) or highly positive (b = .41; p b .01)
Hypotheses 4 and 5 assume that the effects of functional versus green product attitudes. The overall indirect effect of functional
emotional green ads would depend on consumers' green involve- ad exposure on brand liking, moderated by purchase behavior (or
ment. Separate path analyses test the moderating roles of environ- product attitudes) and mediated by ad liking, is also significant
mental concern, attitudes toward green products, and green (purchase behavior: b = .11; p b .05; product attitude: b = .21;
purchase behavior. Contrary to expectations, neither emotional p b .05).
Author's personal copy

1890 J. Matthes et al. / Journal of Business Research 67 (2014) 1885–1893

greater range of consumers—the ones low as well as high in in-


volvement. Yet somehow surprisingly, concerning the effects on
ad evaluation, the combined green ad is not superior compared
to the emotional green ad. However, the combined green ad also
exerts a direct effect on brand attitudes, not mediated by ad evalu-
ation. So in total, the combined ad has the greatest impact on brand
attitudes.
Although future studies on green advertising should further test
the differential effects of various forms of green appeals, the findings
provide evidence for the universality and strength of emotional
appeals (either alone or combined with attributes). As Hartmann
and Apaolaza-Ibáñez (2009) state, most advertising scholars recom-
mend cognitive persuasion strategies in green advertising, “assum-
ing the consumer's high involvement regarding environmental
issues” (p. 719). Our findings as well as the evidence presented by
Hartmann and Apaolaza-Ibáñez (2009) suggest that emotional
green ads are more powerful than functional ones. The emotional
experience based on an exposure to a green ad comes close to the
emotions experienced in contact with real nature (Hartmann &
Apaolaza-Ibáñez, 2009). Based on evolutionary processes, beautiful
nature sceneries have a positive marker in the human information
processing system. Due to such overall affinity toward nature, ad
exposures may automatically activate virtual nature experiences.
These positively marked virtual nature experiences then associate
with the green ad. The main point is that, whether you are a green
consumer or not, emotional green ads seem to automatically
activate virtual nature experiences, which transfer to the green
Fig. 2. Attitude toward the ad (z-standardized) as a function of experimental group and ad. As a consequence, the attitude toward the brand becomes
green involvement (upper part: green purchase behavior; lower part: attitude toward positive.
green products). But why does green involvement not matter in this process? An ELM
account would suggest that emotional images are especially powerful
for individuals that are low in green involvement (Petty & Cacioppo,
1990). In pondering this paradox, one should reconsider how the
present study conceptualizes and measures green involvement.
Environmental concern does not significantly interact with function- As in almost all prior research, the constructs build on explicit
al ad exposure, although the effect points in the expected direction statements about green consumerism. Such explicit statements,
(b = .15; p = .18). These findings lend some support to Hypothesis 4. however, do not tap the positive affect that is automatically
Hypothesis 5, however, can be falsified. activated when people are confronted with nature images. In fact,
The conclusion regarding research question 2 is that green pur- psychological research suggests that there are two types of atti-
chase behavior and green product attitudes exert the strongest tudes, explicit and implicit attitudes. Explicit attitudes are those
moderating effects on the outcome variables. Those effects are that people are consciously aware of, they are deliberative, and
non-existent for environmental concern. However, this conclusion people can control them. Implicit attitudes, in contrast, are “auto-
only refers to the impact of functional green ads. For the other two matic affective reactions resulting from the particular associations
types of green ads, involvement does not matter at all. An addi- that are activated automatically when one encounters a relevant
tional analysis checks whether the effects of (functional) green stimulus” (Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006, p. 693). Translated
ads on the attitude toward the ad increase with rising viewing to green advertising, people may vary in their implicit positive
time. The idea is that effects may grow stronger with exposure attitudes toward nature. However, measures of explicit attitudes—
time to an advertisement. The test finds no evidence for such an such as environmental concern, attitudes toward green products, or
interaction (functional ads ∗ viewing time: b = .01, n.s.; emotion- green purchase behavior—do not take these implicit gut reactions
al ads ∗ viewing time: b = .00, n.s.; combined ads ∗ viewing time: into account. Consequently, (explicit) green involvement fails to
b = .00, n.s.). The same is true for the outcome variable attitude qualify the effects of emotional green ads. By contrast, one could
toward the brand (functional ads ∗ viewing time: b = .01, n.s.; emo- speculate that implicit measures may moderate the effects of emo-
tional ads ∗ viewing time: b = .00, n.s.; combined ads ∗ viewing time: tional appeals.
b = .00, n.s.). Functional green appeals do not equally affect all consumers. More
specifically, the results seem to suggest that green ads using functional
arguments are more powerful when consumers score high on green
6. Discussion purchase behavior or green product attitudes. The ELM may explain
this finding. People that like or frequently buy green products are
This article advances the literature by focusing on the interac- more motivated to process the arguments that are displayed in the ad.
tion of three types of green ads with several dimensions of green A careful processing of ad arguments, in turn, increases the liking of
consumer involvement. Findings suggest that emotional appeals the ad, which in turn, influences brand attitudes. Surprisingly however,
play a key role in shaping consumers' brand attitudes. An experi- environmental concern is not relevant in explaining how consumers
ment reveals significant main effects of the emotional green ad react to functional green ads. Environmental concern is a more general
and the combined green ad. However, only a barely significant type of green involvement that does not necessarily translate to the
main effect appears for the functional ad. According to the ELM, appreciation of green products and green buying behavior. Attitudes
combining emotional and functional cues should appeal to a toward green products and green purchase behavior signal a higher
Author's personal copy

J. Matthes et al. / Journal of Business Research 67 (2014) 1885–1893 1891

degree of green involvement compared to environmental concern. Iron- “greenwashed” consumers who believe they buy green and feel good
ically, while a large segment of the public claims to be sympathetic to about it when, in fact, their consumption behavior is not environmen-
the environment, the market success of green products remains at a tally friendly. The misuse of functional positioning strategies, in con-
comparatively low level in many Western countries. Scholars often trast, might be less effective. Since mainly highly involved consumers
refer to the attitude–behavior gap of green consumerism (Kalafatis, are reached, these might also be more likely to recognize vague or
Pollard, East, & Tsogas, 1999; Vermeir & Verbeke, 2006). Expressing en- false claims that are made.
vironmental concern is one thing; buying green is another (Kalafatis
et al., 1999; Vermeir & Verbeke, 2006). Thus, it makes sense that envi-
ronmental concern cannot explain consumers' susceptibility to green 6.3. Limitations and future research
ad appeals.
Several notable research limitations qualify these practical implica-
6.1. Alternative explanations tions. This study compares the effects of emotional and functional
claims by contrasting a visual representation of nature and the presen-
One might argue that functional ads yield no strong main effect tation of elaborated product attributes. Generalizability of the findings
because respondents do not read the arguments that are conveyed should be tested by including variations of these claim types, such as
in the ad. This explanation does not hold because viewing time is verbal emotional claims or lower levels of information. In particular, fu-
significantly higher in the argument conditions compared to the ture research needs to test whether the moderated effects of green con-
two conditions in which no arguments are present. Importantly, sumer involvement are still present for smaller amounts of green
this also concerns the combined condition. Because the ads only product information. Also the validity of green appeals should be con-
differ in the presence of arguments, and the argument conditions sidered. The specific effectiveness of functional and emotional claims
have a longer viewing time than the non-argument conditions, and underlying processes for greenwashing campaigns still remain
people must have read those arguments. Alternatively, critics understudied. Related to this, scholars often regard consumer skepti-
might say that the functional arguments are too weak. However, cism as an obstacle for green advertising and green consumption. Future
the study employs existing arguments for laundry detergents research should examine more closely how and for whom emotional
drawn from real green campaigns. The development of strong and functional claims may trigger or prevent skeptical reactions to
arguments is based on content analytical research on green ads. green ads. Although this study observes the effects of emotional ads, it
In fact, the ads do not contain vague or ambiguous claims but pre- does not measure emotional reactions such as virtual nature experi-
cise, causal statements (Leonidou et al., 2011). Pretesting also sug- ences (as do Hartmann & Apaolaza-Ibáñez, 2009). As a consequence,
gests that these arguments are strong. Another related criticism this study cannot model the processes behind the observed effects, it
might be that ad evaluation serves as a mediator. In line with can only infer them. Looking at short-term effects does not enable to de-
Mitchell and Olson (1981) and many others, this is a theoretically termine the longevity or ultimate power of green ads. Future work
sound way of modeling advertising effects. That is, consumers should therefore expand this research design by including measures of
form judgments about a brand based on their evaluation of the emotional reactions and implicit green attitudes. Finally, research de-
ad. In fact, without the mediator in the model, the basic effects of signs should incorporate intended and actual purchase behavior to as-
emotional (b = .57, p b .001) and combined appeals (b = .68, sess to what extent emotional claims can help to overcome the
p b .001) remain constant. Furthermore, the model explains a attitude–behavior gap. Such a research program could help to corrobo-
total of 71% of the variance of brand evaluations and is, thus, supe- rate the claim that emotional green appeals are universal and powerful
rior to a model that explains less variance. tools to shape brand attitudes.

6.2. Practical implications


Appendix A. Items
Our findings have considerable practical implications. As Hartmann
and Apaolaza-Ibáñez (2009) argue, time is ripe to go beyond the classic Attitude toward the ad: bad–good; unpleasant–pleasant; unfavorable–
dictum to approach green consumers with substantive arguments only. favorable; unconvincing–convincing; incredible–credible.
In fact, emotional ads can be a powerful marketing weapon to reach out Attitude toward the brand: bad–good; unattractive–attractive;
to both, green and non-green consumers. Especially the marketers often negative–positive; not likable–likable; not recommendable–
neglect non-green consumers in their hunt for green advertising effec- recommendable.
tiveness. Our study shows that emotional appeals can serve to target Environmental concern: I am concerned about the environment; The
people with no explicit attachment to environmental issues. This may condition of the environment affects the quality of my life; I am willing
help to sustain and foster environmentally friendly consumption habits. to make sacrifices to protect the environment; My actions impact the
Of course, a risk remains that some consumers may react with skep- environment.
ticism when they are confronted with emotional, non-functional ap- Attitudes toward green products: I like green products; I feel positive
peals. Previous research has yielded ambiguous results about the toward green products; Green products are good for the environment; I
role of consumer skepticism for green advertising (Mohr et al., feel proud when I buy/use green products.
1998; Shrum, McCarty, & Lowrey, 1995). In the present study, highly Green purchase behavior: I make a special effort to buy products in
involved green consumers do not devalue emotional green ads. In biodegradable packages; I would switch from my usual brands and
fact, the opposite is the case: those consumers are equally persuad- buy environmentally safe cleaning products, even if I have to give up
ed by green ads than their less involved counterparts. Still, to be on some cleaning effectiveness; I have switched products for ecological
the safe side, a combined strategy using both functional and emo- reasons; When I have a choice between two equal products, I purchase
tional appeals may be recommendable because, according to the the one less harmful to the environment.
present findings, combined appeals are at least equally effective Perceived consumer effectiveness: There is not much that any one in-
than emotional appeals. dividual can do about the environment (revers.); The conservation ef-
A downside of the effectiveness of emotional appeals is its potential forts of one person are useless as long as other people refuse to
to serve greenwashing campaigns. Findings of this study imply that the conserve (revers.)
use of emotions for greenwashing may appeal to low as well as highly Negative sentiment toward marketing: Most products I buy are
involved consumers. As a consequence, emotional appeals may create overpriced; Most products I buy wear out too quickly.
Author's personal copy

1892 J. Matthes et al. / Journal of Business Research 67 (2014) 1885–1893

Appendix B. Stimuli

The stimuli can be obtained from the first author upon request.

Control Group Functional Ad

Emotional Ad Combined Ad

References Enders, C. K., & Bandalos, D. L. (2001). The relative performance of full information max-
imum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Structural
Aaker, D. A. (1996). Building strong brands. New York, NY: Free Press. Equation Modeling, 8, 430–457.
Chan, R. Y. K. (2001). Determinants of Chinese consumers' green purchase behavior. Eurobarometer (2011). Attitudes of European citizens towards the environment:
Psychology and Marketing, 18(4), 389–413. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/mar.1013. Special Eurobarometer 365. Retrieved from. http://ec.europa.eu/environment/
Chang, C. (2011). Feeling ambivalent about going green. Journal of Advertising, 40(4), pdf/EB_summary_
19–32. EB752.pdf. Gaski, J. F., & Etzel, M. J. (1986). The index of consumer sentiment toward
Chowdhury, R. M., Olsen, G. D., & Pracejus, J. W. (2008). Affective responses to images in marketing. Journal of Marketing, 50(3), 71–81.
print advertising: Affect integration in a simultaneous presentation context. Journal of Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in eval-
Advertising, 37(3), 7–18. uation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological
D'Souza, C., & Taghian, M. (2005). Green advertising effects on attitude and choice Bulletin, 132(5), 692–731.
of advertising themes. Asia Pacific Journal of Marketing and Logistics, 17(3), Grimmer, M., & Bingham, T. (2013). Company environmental performance and consumer
51–66. purchase intentionsJournal of Business Research, 66(10), 1945–1953.
Dahl, R. (2010). Greenwashing: Do you know what you're buying? Environ Health Grimmer, M., & Woolley, M. (2012). Green marketing messages and consumers' purchase
Perspectives, 118(6), A246–A252. intentions: Promoting personal versus environmental benefits. Journal of Marketing
Davis, J. J. (1994). Consumer response to corporate environmental advertising. Journal of Communications, 1–20.
Consumer Marketing, 11, 25–37. Hartmann, P., & Apaolaza-Ibáñez, V. (2009). Green advertising revisited: Conditioning
Dunlap, R. E., & Scarce, R. (1991). Poll Trends: Environmental problems and protection. virtual nature experiences. International Journal of Advertising, 28(4), 715–739.
Public Opinion Quarterly, 55(4), 651–672. Hartmann, P., & Apaolaza-Ibáñez, V. (2012). Consumer attitude and purchase intention
Easterling, D., Kenworthy, A., & Nemzoff, R. (1996). The greening of advertising: A toward green energy brands: The roles of psychological benefits and environmental
twenty-five year look at environmental advertising. Journal of Marketing Theory and concern. Journal of Business Research, 65(9), 1254–1263.
Practice, 4(1), 20–34. Hartmann, P., Ibáñez, V. A., & Sainz, F. J. F. (2005). Green branding effects on attitude:
Edell, J. A., & Burke, M. C. (1987). The power of feelings in understanding advertising Functional versus emotional positioning strategies. Marketing Intelligence & Planning,
effects. Journal of Consumer Research, 14, 421–433. 23(1), 9–29.
Ellen, P.S., Wiener, J. L., & Cobb-Walgren, C. (1991). The role of perceived consumer effec- Hayes, A. F., & Matthes, J. (2009). Computational procedures for probing interactions in OLS
tiveness in motivating environmentally conscious behaviors. Journal of Public Policy & and logistic regression: SPSS and SAS implementations. Behavior Research Methods, 41,
Marketing, 10(2), 102–117. 924–936.
Author's personal copy

J. Matthes et al. / Journal of Business Research 67 (2014) 1885–1893 1893

Irwin, J. R., & McClelland, G. H. (2002). Negative consequences of dichotomizing continu- Schlegelmilch, B. B., Bohlen, G. M., & Diamantopoulos, A. (1996). The link between green
ous predictor variables. Journal of Marketing Research, 40, 366–371. purchasing decisions and measures of environmental consciousness. European Jour-
Kalafatis, S. P., Pollard, M., East, R., & Tsogas, M. H. (1999). Green marketing and Ajzen's nal of Marketing, 30(5), 35.
theory of planned behaviour: A cross-market examination. Journal of Consumer Schmidt, A., & Donsbach, W. (2012). „Grüne“Werbung als Instrument für
Marketing, 16(5), 441–460. „schwarze“Zahlen. Publizistik, 57(1), 75–93.
Kangun, N., Carlson, L., & Grove, S. J. (1991). Environmental advertising claims: A prelim- Schuhwerk, M. E., & Lefkoff-Hagius, R. (1995). Green or non-green? Does type of appeal
inary investigation. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 10(2), 47–58. matter when advertising a green product? Journal of Advertising, 24(2), 45–54.
Kaplan, R., & Kaplan, S. (1989). The experience of nature: A psychological perspective. : Cam- Schwartz, J., & Miller, T. (1991). The earth's best friends. American Demographics, 13(2),
bridge University Press. 26–35.
Kim, Y., & Choi, S. M. (2005). Antecedents of green purchase behavior: An examination of Schwepker, J. C. H., & Cornwell, T. B. (1991). An examination of ecologically concerned
collectivism, environmental concern, and PCE. Advances in Consumer Research, 32(1), consumers and their intention to purchase ecologically packaged products. Journal
592–599. of Public Policy & Marketing, 10(2), 77–101.
Leonidou, L. C., Leonidou, C. N., Palihawadana, D., & Hultman, M. (2011). Evaluating the Searles, K. (2010). Feeling good and doing good for the environment: The use of emotion-
green advertising practices of international firms: A trend analysis. International al appeals in pro-environmental public service announcements. Applied
Marketing Review, 28, 6–33. Environmental Education and Communication, 9(3), 173–184.
Mitchell, A. A., & Olson, J. C. (1981). Are product attribute beliefs the only mediator of ad- Shrum, L. J., McCarty, J. A., & Lowrey, T. M. (1995). Buyer characteristics of the green con-
vertising effects on brand attitude? Journal of Marketing Research (JMR), 27, 318–332. sumer and their implications for advertising strategy. Journal of Advertising, 24(2),
Mohr, L. A., Eroğlu, D., & Ellen, P. A.M. S. (1998). The development and testing of a mea- 71–82.
sure of skepticism toward environmental claims in marketers' communications. Spack, Justine A., Board, Virginia E., Crighton, Lindsay M., Kostka, Phillip M., & Ivory, James
Journal of Consumer Affairs, 32(1), 30–55. D. (2012). It's easy being green: The effects of argument and imagery on consumer
Newman, C. L., Howlett, E., Burton, S., Kozup, J. C., & Heintz, Tangari Andrea (2012). The responses to green product packaging. Environmental Communication: A Journal of Na-
influence of consumer concern about global climate change on framing effects for en- ture and Culture, 6(4), 441–458.
vironmental sustainability messages. International Journal of Advertising, 31, 511–527. Stokols, D. (1990). Instrumental and spiritual views of people–environment relations.
Peattie, K., & Crane, A. (2005). Green marketing: Legend, myth, farce or prophesy? American Psychologist, 45(5), 641.
Qualitative Market Research: An International Journal, 8(4), 357–370. Stone, G., Barnes, J. H., & Montgomery, C. (1995). ECOSCALE: A scale for the measurement
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1990). Involvement and persuasion: Tradition versus inte- of environmentally responsible consumers. Psychology & Marketing, 12(7), 595–612.
gration. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 367–374. Vermeir, I., & Verbeke, W. (2006). Sustainable food consumption: Exploring the consumer
Roberts, J. A. (1996). Green consumers in the 1990s: Profile and implications for advertis- “attitude–behavioral intention” gap. Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics,
ing. Journal of Business Research, 36(3), 217–231. 19(2), 169–194.

View publication stats

You might also like