Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 17

Predictors of Ethical Code

Use and Ethical Tolerance Neal M. Ashkanasy*


Sarah Falkus
in the Public Sector Victor J. Callan

ABSTRACT. This paper reports the results of a different mechanisms underlie each of the criterion
survey of ethical attitudes, values, and propensities in variables. Use of ethical codes was determined
public sector employees in Australia. It was expected primarily on the basis of a perception that others use
that demographic variables, personal values, and con- the code, while ethical tolerance was determined by
textual variables at the individual level, and group- personal values. At an applied level, the research high-
and organisational-level values would predict use of lights the need for orgasnizations to establish a critical
formal codes of ethics and ethical tolerance (tolerance mass of code users, so that this operates as a norma-
of unethical behaviour). Useable data were received tive influence on others in the organization.
from 500 respondents selected at random across public
sector organisations in a single Australian state. Results
supported the study hypotheses, but indicated that Introduction

Dr. Neal M. Ashkanasy, is a Reader in Organizational The 1980s have been characterized as the
Behaviour in the Graduate School of Management, The “decade of excess” (Sykes, 1994). Paradoxically,
University of Queensland. He has a Ph.D. in one result of this has been a resurgence of interest
Psychology from the same university, and has research in ethics in both the public and private sectors,
interests in leadership, organizational culture, ethics and and in both academic and business research
emotions. He is on the Editorial Board of the Academy (Lozano, 1996). Recent figures reveal that some
of Management Journal, and has published in journals 90% of large U.S. based companies have since
such as the Journal of Personality and Social
taken initiatives to install formal codes of ethics,
Psychology and Organisational Behaviour and
Human Decision Processes. of which a third were established between 1985
Sarah Falkus has a Masters degree in Organizational and 1990 (Murphy, 1995). In Australia, a similar
Psychology from the School of Psychology at the pattern of accountability has emerged. Since the
University of Queensland. At the time this paper was stock market crash of 1987, there has been a
written, she was a Research Assistant in the Graduate Royal Commission in each Australian state, and
School of Management, The University of Queensland. an Australian survey over a range of professions
Her primary areas of interest are organizational culture and industries found that almost three quarters
and leadership. She currently works as an Editorial had a code of ethics in place (Milton-Smith,
Assistant with Blackwell Publishers in the U.K. 1995).
Victor J. Callan is Professor of Management and Head of Recent calls for business accountability have
the Graduate School of Management at The University prompted an increased emphasis on business
of Queensland. He has a Ph.D. in Psychology from
ethics internationally. Indeed, research by Carlson
the Australian National University. Professor Callan is
an organizational researcher and management consultant. and Kacmar (1997) and Zahra and LaTour (1987)
As a researcher he has published internationally in the indicates that there is a sound basis for believing
areas of organizational communication and change that ethics is of critical importance to the func-
including 8 books and over 100 international research tioning of a company, beyond a mere reaction
articles and book chapters in some of the world’s leading to past corruption. In this respect, Zahra and
management journals, and edited volumes. LaTour found that corporate social responsibility

Journal of Business Ethics 25: 237–253, 2000.


© 2000 Kluwer Academic Publishers. Printed in the Netherlands.
238 Neal M. Ashkanasy et al.

is associated with organizational effectiveness, of work place ethical norms (Delaney and
while the ethical environment of an organization Stockwell, 1992).
is linked to a stronger culture, more commit- The use of formal codes of conduct is dis-
ment, and lower turnover among employees cussed in the literature as a potential predictor
(Carlson and Kacmar, 1997). of ethical behavior (Akaah and Riordan, 1989;
The importance of ethical values for the Public Hunt, Chonko and Wilcox, 1984). Researchers
Sector, over and above private corporations and have been unable to conclude, however, whether
professions, is identified as “the position of trust, the use of a code necessarily predicts ethical
power and privilege which public servants hold, intentions and behaviors. Akaah and Riordan
and the resulting obligation not to breach that (1989) found that the existence of a formal code
trust and not to misuse their power or privilege” of ethics did not significantly relate to people’s
(MAB and MIAC, 1996, p. 2). In Australia, the ethical judgments. Similarly, Hunt et al. (1984)
resurgence of interest in Public Sector ethics has found that the existence of a corporate code of
been attributed to: emerging evidence of uneth- ethics made no difference in respondents’ per-
ical conduct, the questioning of the distinction ceptions of ethical problems in their organiza-
between policy making and administration, and tions. The problem is that these studies have
the increasing acceptance that non-elected offi- failed to clarify the degree to which such findings
cials exercise power and that it is difficult for the arise from factors other than code salience. For
public to ascertain whether that power is being example, in cross-sectional research, variation in
properly used (EARC, 1992). knowledge and understanding of the content of
the relevant codes can mask the effect of the
codes on decision making. Variation in the
The use of formal codes of conduct calibre of the corporate codes being sampled can
have a similar effect (Morris, Marks, Allen and
The majority of research to date has relied on Peery, 1996). Finally, informal codes and norms
ethical values or behavioral intentions as the can also function alongside formal codes to
criterion variable, and has dealt with individual influence ethical decision making (Hunt and
ethics, resting upon the premise that the ethics Vitell, 1993; Zey-Ferrell, Weaver and Ferrell,
of individuals aggregate to influence the ethics of 1979).
the organization (Carlson and Kacmar, 1997). In There is also another, more positive, aspect of
this study we describe this variable as ethical ethical code usage. This is the extent to which
tolerance. The present study follows this pattern, codes indicate an organization’s commitment to
but also examines the less well established crite- ethics. Writers such as Kitson (1996), and Soutar,
rion, use of a firm’s formal code of conduct by McNeil and Molster (1995) have observed that,
individuals, as a dependent variable. Further, this while the existence of a code may not necessarily
study includes variables at different levels of translate into behavior, codes at least indicate this
organizational analysis. sort of commitment. For example, Hunt and his
Milton-Smith (1995) has shown that organi- colleagues (1984), in a study of marketing
zations attempt to meet ethical concerns through researchers, found that companies which
introduction of formal codes. In this respect, reported few ethical problems were those where
formal codes of ethics remain the most obvious, formal norms were upheld. Further, informal
and the most applied way for organizations to norms which deviated from standards of profes-
attempt to encourage ethical behavior. Despite sional conduct were not tolerated by top man-
this, and while our knowledge of moral reasoning agement. Morris and his group (1996) also
has increased with the past thirty years’ of ethics discuss the role of formal codes of conduct as an
research, such codes remain largely unaddressed indicator of organizational commitment to ethics.
in the ethics literature. One aim of the present Finally, there is a small body of research which
study is to extend beyond theories of moral rea- has examined the determinants of formal code
soning and values, and to address directly the role use. Pearson, Crosbey, and Shim (1997) studied
Predictors of Ethical Propensity 239

whether information systems professionals adhere human moral reasoning, but does have the poten-
to organization or professional code criteria. tial to indicate ethical behavior within the limits
They found that adherence was stronger for that the code sets. Such a focus also reflects a
codes relevant to specific organization-focused theme apparent in recent business ethics research
criteria, rather than the more cosmopolitan, that there is a need to turn from personal respon-
career-focused guidelines. The relative impor- sibility to corporate responsibility (see Lozano,
tance of criteria differed by age, gender, level of 1996).
importance, whether individuals perceived a Another main aim of the present study, there-
formal code of ethics as necessary, and whether fore, is to compare the pattern of relationships
they belonged to an organization which consid- obtained when predicting the use of a specific
ered itself a leader in its industry. formal code of conduct with the more tradi-
In summary, ethical code usage represents a tional, generalizable, ethical behavior which we
potentially important, but little investigated term ethical tolerance. The specific hypotheses
aspect of organizational behavior. In the fol- in the forthcoming sections outline the similar-
lowing section, the relationship between use of ities and differences we expected to be evident
codes and ethical behavior and attitudes is dis- between the two criteria. The variables included
cussed further. in the present study were similar to those used
in past research to predict ethical behavior from
the perspective of moral reasoning. Individual,
Relationship between codes and ethical behavior group, organizational, and contextual variables
were included to test their relative importance.
Kitson (1996) has pointed out that there is The following provides a brief outline of the
ambiguity surrounding the relationship between predictive role of each group of variables.
code use (i.e. as measured by the existence of a
code of ethics) and ethical behavior. This ambi-
guity may, however, derive in part from the Individual level
measure used to ascertain the existence of a
formal code, rather than whether such a code is Individual-level variables include both demo-
utilized in the organization. The present study graphic variables, (e.g., gender, managerial level
seeks to rectify this by going beyond the mere and job tenure) and personal ethical values.
existence of a formal code to a measure of orga- Gender is a demographic variable which has
nizational code use which is based on clear and received considerable attention (see Mason and
specific guidelines. In an organization where the Mudrack, 1996). There are, however, two com-
formal code of conduct has been carefully peting theoretical positions in this regard: gender
designed to reflect and to clearly outline desired socialization theory and occupational socialization
ethical behavior in a given work context, it theory. Gender socialization theory posits that
follows that adherence to such a code would at men and women are socialized to think and
least be likely to create an environment for behave in different ways, and that this leads to
ethical behavior (at least within the confines differences in ethical values which persist
established by the code; see Lozano, 1996). between the genders beyond work experiences.
It is notable that much of the previous research The proponents of occupational socialization
is predicated on the idea that ethical behavior is theory argue, on the other hand, that similar
determined by personal values and moral rea- employment experiences create similarities in
soning. Thus, each individual’s struggle between ethical values in men and women.
deontological and teleological concerns is an Empirical studies have yielded conflicting
indicator of more durable, generalizable moral results, supporting one or other of these theories,
philosophies (DeConnick and Lewis, 1997). On and the relationship between gender and ethics
the other hand, we argue that adherence to a is still unclear. While researchers such as Ruegger
given code of conduct need not generalize to and King (1992), Khazanchi (1995), and
240 Neal M. Ashkanasy et al.

Schminke and Ambrose (1997) have reported authority and power were negatively correlated
gender differences in ethical values, others (e.g. with cronyism, but unrelated to other ethical
Hodgkinson, 1971; Kidwell, Stevens and Bethke, criteria.
1987) have found similarities between males and In summary, research has indicated, albeit
female ethical propensities. Still other researchers sometimes equivocally, that gender, managerial
have reported mixed results. For example, Callan level, job tenure, and role responsibility are pre-
(1992) found gender differences correlated with dictors of ethical behavior. In the context of the
one of the six ethical dimensions used and no present study, these findings lead to two
significant differences on the other five (see also hypotheses:
McNichols and Zimmerer, 1985; Stead, Worrell,
Spalding and Stead, 1987). In general, empirical H1: Individual variables (gender, managerial
evidence has shown that the relationship between level, job tenure, role responsibility) will
gender and ethics is slight at best (see McCuddy be significant predictors of the use of
and Peery, 1996). formal codes of conduct.
Another individual variable which has received
attention is managerial level. Despite occasional H2: Individual variables (gender, managerial
anomalous findings (e.g. Akaah, 1996), manage- level, job tenure, role responsibility) will
rial level has generally been found to be a robust be significant predictors of ethical toler-
predictor in ethics research, with those at higher ance.
levels reporting more ethical behavior and values.
Callan (1992) found that managerial level can Researchers have looked at individual-level
predict perceptions of ethical problems (see also variables apart from demographic information.
Hunt et al., 1984), and Akaah and Riordan For example, McKenna (1996) posed that ethical
(1989) found it can predict ethical judgments. behaviors may vary because of differing levels of
A third variable of interest is job tenure. Kelly, awareness of ethics. A manager’s actions may lead
Ferrell and Skinner (1990) found that older pro- to consequences which are widely considered as
fessionals and those holding their jobs for ten amoral, not because of wrongdoing but because
years or longer tended to give themselves higher of this absence of ethical awareness. Along similar
ethical ratings. Morris and his colleagues (1996) lines, Morris and colleagues (1996) concluded
outline alternative explanations for this finding that it may be that the more knowledge people
(along with similar results they found with have about ethical standards, the more their
respect to age). It is possible that the deeper and behavior is affected according to perceived moral
broader exposure to professional practice that standards.
comes with job experience induces a belief in The role of personal values is emphasized also
returning, when in doubt, to the basics of one’s in the conceptual framework of Ferrell and
profession, such as those articulated in codes of Gresham (1985), as well as Hunt and Vitell
ethics. Thus, according to Morris et al. (1996), (1993). In these theories, strong moral values are
older more experienced professionals may tend proposed to instil more ethical functioning;
to think and behave according to formal codes, values that weigh personal advancement may do
while younger, less experienced professionals the opposite. Personal values, ethical principles
adhere more to the informal norms of the pro- and ethical norms are a part of an individual’s
fession. ethical philosophy (Stead, Worrell and Stead,
The present study includes gender, managerial 1990; Windsor and Ashkanasy, 1995) and thus
level and job tenure as predictor variables. A influence individual behavioral decisions (Hogan,
fourth variable, role responsibility, is also included 1973).
in this set of individual variables. Role respon- In this present study we examine personal
sibility reflects characteristics of a person’s job values towards ethics in terms of leniency (the
which should be related to managerial level. For degree to which things are not perceived to be
instance, Callan (1992) found that indices of either right or wrong, but can be viewed with
Predictors of Ethical Propensity 241

some leniency) and triviality (the perception that work. The existence of conflict between personal
some wrongdoings are not important, and are ethics and perceived company interests is likely
even trivial in some situations). These variables to be a source of ethical dilemma or ethical
capture ideas of idealism and relativism, two ambivalence ( Jansen and Von Glinow, 1985). In
dimensions by which Forsyth (1980) proposes the face of competing demands between these
individuals will differ in their behavioral mani- obligations, ethics may be considered as the
festation of ethical philosophies. Idealism is the individual’s attempt to “do the right thing”
degree to which a person believes that ethical (Froelich and Kottke, 1991). Based on the
behavior always results in good outcomes (akin findings of Froelich et al. (1991) we hypothesize
to low leniency). Relativism is the degree to that the level of conflict between personal values
which a person believes that ethics are situational and organisational demands will predict ethical
(that is, triviality, see Stead et al., 1990). From predilications as measured by either the use of
this research, another two hypotheses are formal codes or ethical tolerance.
proposed. In addition to conflict, two other contextual
variables shall be considered. These measures are
H3: Personal values, in the form of perceived the perceived relevance of a formal code in
leniency and triviality accorded to some general for an individual’s work, as well as the
unethical behavior, will be significant individual’s perceived need for the formal code
predictors of the use of formal codes of of conduct. If a code is deemed irrelevant or not
conduct. necessary for one’s work, use of such formal
codes will not be an accurate measure of ethical
H4: Personal values, in the form of perceived behavior for that sphere of work. Thus, the
leniency and triviality accorded to some pattern of relationships between individual con-
unethical behavior, will be significant textual variables and use of formal codes will be
predictors of ethical tolerance. different to the pattern between contextual
variables and the non-workplace specific measure
It is further hypothesized that, of ethical tolerance. We propose nonetheless that,
of the three contextual variables, only conflict
H5: Personal values will predict ethical toler- will be a significant predictor of ethical tolerance.
ance more strongly than they will predict This proposition is based on the notion that,
the use of formal codes of ethics. although an organisation may establish a context
for ethical and unethical behaviour, the existence
of conflict in that context will lead to a mitiga-
Context tion of other factors such as those related to
formal codes. This leads to two hypotheses:
Other variables to consider are those that focus
on the contexts of ethical and unethical behav- H6: Contextual variables (perceived conflict
iour. As Kahn (1990) suggests, ethics research between personal values and work
needs to consider not only the person’s internal demands, perceived relevance of a formal
awareness of ethical principles, but also the code of conduct, and perceived need for
organisational contexts of thought and work a formal code of conduct) will be signif-
demands. That is, there is a need to assess the icant predictors of the use of formal
situational constraints which affect the way an codes of ethics.
individual’s demographic history and personal
values predict ethical behavior. H7: Of the contextual variables (perceived
In the present study, the first context consid- conflict between personal values and
ered is conflict. That is the level of conflict work demands, perceived relevance of a
between an individual’s personal ethical beliefs formal code of conduct, and perceived
and the ethical values demanded for successful need for a formal code of conduct), only
242 Neal M. Ashkanasy et al.

conflict will be significantly related to organization’s stakeholders (ethical ambivalence).


ethical tolerance. In this respect, researchers such as Hegarty and
Sims (1978), Stead et al. (1990), and Trevino
(1986) have stressed the effect of reinforcement
Group level and reward structures on the ethical behavior of
employees. Chen, Sawyers, and Williams (1997)
The work group represents another level of argue further that it is the ethical attributes of a
analysis which needs to be considered in business corporate culture that help employees to see
ethics research. Models of ethical behavior have ethical dilemmas and to maintain an ethical point
incorporated group variables through the propo- of view, rather than the individual attributes of
sition that, if interaction with peers is more sig- the employees.
nificant that interaction with management, then The second perspective is based on the notion
such a peer group will have more influence on of ethical climate, and has received much atten-
both an individual’s ethical decision making tion since the development of a measure by
(Ferrell and Gresham, 1985; Ferrell, Gresham and Victor and Cullen (1990). Organizational ethical
Fraedrich, 1989), and the use of informal norms climate is a subset of organizational climate,
for use of codes among peers (see Hunt and embodying normative values and beliefs
Vitell, 1986; Zey-Ferrell et al., 1979). Thus, we involving moral rightness and wrongness shared
hypothesize that: by the employees of the organization (Weber,
1995). The ethical climate survey distinguishes
H8: The extent to which an individual’s work different climates on two dimensions. The first
group uses the code of conduct will dimension differentiates maximization of one’s
predict the use of formal codes by that own self-interests, maximizing joint interest, or
individual. adherence to universal principles (Victor and
Cullen, 1990). The second dimension represents
H9: The extent to which an individual’s work level of ethical concern: individual, local or
group uses the code of conduct will cosmopolitan. Several recent business ethics
predict the ethical tolerance of that indi- studies have utilized this measure and validated
vidual. the concepts of ethical climates and sub-climates
(e.g. Cullen, Victor and Bronson, 1993;
Deshpande, 1996; Weber, 1995).
Organizational level The present study is not a study of ethical
climate per se, and does not utilize the ethical
At the organizational level, two schools of climate survey. Elements of the ethicality of the
thought exist about the potential determinants of organization and its reward systems are included
ethical behavior. The first is the role of general as a measure, however. As such, we expected to
cultural norms, the second is the role of “ethical find that:
climate” (Victor and Cullen, 1990).
First, shared cultural norms and values in an H10: The ethicality of the organization’s
organization appear to predict ethical behavior reward system will be a significant pre-
(Ferrell et al., 1989). Jansen and Von Glinow dictor of use of formal code of conduct.
(1985) argue that there is a need for the organi-
zational culture practised in an organization to H11: The ethicality of the organization’s
match the values espoused by organizational reward system will be significant a pre-
members. It is especially important that the dictor of ethical tolerance.
reward system of the organization does not
conflict with espoused values in the formal code In summary, demographics, personal values,
of conduct or informal norms or, indeed, with context, group and organization variables, which
personal ethical values and judgments of the have been found in the literature to be predic-
Predictors of Ethical Propensity 243

tive of ethical behavior, are used in the present TABLE I


study to test two operationalizations of ethical Descriptive statistics
behavior propensity: use of formal code and
ethical tolerance. Variable Mean SD N

01. Role responsibility 2.55 1.02 476


02. Job tenure 2.31 0.84 458
Method 03. Gender 1.57 0.50 499
04. Level 1.43 0.50 496
Sample 05. Lenient 1.67 0.78 493
06. Trivial 1.95 0.65 490
The data for this analysis were collected from a 07. Perceived need 2.65 0.58 490
large-scale ethics survey carried out in public 08. Perceived relevance 3.19 1.05 494
sector organizations in one Australian state. A 09. Conflict 2.59 0.80 486
total of 1200 questionnaires were mailed to 10. Use of obligation –
public sector employees selected on a fully Diligence 4.23 0.81 498
random basis in organisations throughout the 11. Use of obligation –
public sector. Five hundred and twelve ques- economy and
efficiency 3.90 1.04 497
tionnaires were returned, representing a return
12. Use of obligation –
rate of just under 43%. Twelve of the question- integrity 3.91 0.88 496
naires were unusable, and were discarded. The 13. Use of obligation –
demographic composition of the sample is given respect for law 4.19 0.74 492
in Table I. 14. Use of obligation –
respect for persons 4.19 0.82 498
15. Others’ use of code 2.74 1.27 480
Instrument 16. Organization’s ethics 3.10 0.92 485
17. Use of code 3.18 1.36 488
The questionnaire used in the present study was 18. Ethical tolerance 1.35 0.45 480
developed jointly by the participating public
sector organizations and the authors.1 The scales
for the questionnaires were the result of focus ranged from 6.37 to 1.74. Factor loadings were
groups and prior research in the participating in a range from 0.30 to 0.61. Alpha reliability
organizations. The adopted questionnaire con- coefficients for the seven scales, however, were
sisted six sections, comprising 56 questions. moderate, ranging from 0.60 to 0.69. In this
Many of the questions included multiple items, respect, Nunnally (1978) has noted that an alpha
however, so that the total number of items was of 0.5 or 0.6 is acceptable at the early stages of
83. Each item was responded to on a 5-point research, especially when the number of scale
Likert-type scale, where 1 = “Strongly disagree” items is small. Further, items were deliberately
and 5 = “Strongly agree”. selected in the present study to provide an appro-
An principal axis exploratory factor analysis priate balance between homogeneity and vari-
(using Varimax rotation) was used to determine ability within scales (Boyle, 1991).
the factor structure among the items representing
the individual and organizational-level variables. Demographic measures. Three demographic vari-
Factors were selected on the basis of a scree ables and one job characteristic were incorpo-
criterion and interpretability. Resulting from this rated into the study: gender, years in the
analysis, 29 items with factor loadings of 0.3 and organization, managerial level, and role respon-
above (Tabachnick and Fidell, 1996) were sibility. Women were coded as 1 and men were
retained in a 7-factor structure, which explained coded as 2. Years in the organization were cate-
52% of the total variance. None of the retained gorized into three groups: 0–5 years, 6–10 years
items cross-loaded on other factors. Eigen values and 11+ years. Managerial level was a dichoto-
244 Neal M. Ashkanasy et al.

mous variable, distinguishing between middle to ethical code in place in the organization under
top management (coded as 2) and non-manage- study. These variables were not included in the
rial positions, such as administrative, clerical, pro- exploratory factor analysis because they are
fessional, technical roles (coded as 1). Role qualitatively different from the others. The first
responsibility was taken as the mean value of group-level measure consisted of only one item,
seven items which addressed specific areas of asking whether “other people in my work area
responsibility held by the employee. For example often use this Code of Conduct”. The other
granting a licence, payment or benefit. High measures addressed the account taken in practice
scores indicated higher realms of responsibility. by work group members of each of five obliga-
tions included in the formal code. The measures
Personal ethical values and attitudes. The exploratory were represented in the questionnaire by the four
factor analysis of the original nine items indicated specific elements of each ethical obligation
two ethical value factors, labelled triviality (six included in the code. The final scores on each
items, alpha = 0.69) and leniency (three items, obligation were the mean responses on the four
alpha = 0.65). An example of a triviality item is elements. The five obligations comprise: (1) dili-
“Avoiding procedures is sometimes justifiable to gence, (2) economy/efficiency, (3) integrity,
get past bureaucratic red tape”. High scores on (4) respect for law, and (5) respect for persons.
this factor indicated that the respondent per-
ceived that some wrongdoing is trivial and Organizational practices. The organizational
justifiable. An example of a lenience item is “If variable included in the study, organizational
something is done for the right reasons, it cannot ethical practices, was confirmed by the
be called wrongdoing”. High scores for leniency exploratory factor analysis. It comprised of five
indicated that the respondent believed that only items, coded such that high scores represent
illegal, repeatable or actions done for the wrong ethical practices (alpha = 0.60). These items
reasons are of ethical concern. addressed issues such as sexual discrimination
(unethical behavior) and achievement by merit
Context variables. Context variables were (ethical behavior). An example is “The reward
included in the study to take into account the system in the public sector does not encourage
perceived relevancy and need for adherence to ethical behavior” (reverse coded).
ethics given the work context of respondents. Dependent variables: These comprised two
The factor analysis of the original 10 items measures: (1) use of formal code of conduct and
revealed that there were three dimensions within (2) ethical tolerance. Use of code was measured
this category: conflict, perceived relevance of the by the single item “I use the Code of Conduct
code, and perceived need for a formal code. written for my employing authority”. High
Three items, for example: “It is difficult to scores on this item indicate a propensity to use
achieve fairness in all official dealings with the the code of ethics. Ethical tolerance was con-
public and other departmental employees”, were firmed from the exploratory factor analysis, and
labelled Conflict (alpha = 0.60). Two items were included six “scenario” questions, where respon-
retained in the Relevance scale (alpha = 0.61). dents were asked to rate their agreement that the
An example is: “Ethical issues often arise as a behavior presented in each scenario was ethical.
significant consideration in my job”. Six items An example is: “A public sector employee gives
were retained in the Need scale (alpha = 0.69); personal information about the agency’s clients
for example: “If a Code of Conduct were avail- to her spouse who works in an insurance
able, I believe that I would not need to consult company”. The final measure of ethical tolerance
it” (reverse coded). was the mean response on these items. High
scores on this scale indicate a propensity to
Group-level variables. Group-level variables tapped tolerate unethical practices. Alpha was 0.68.
the normative practices in each individual’s work
group or department with regard to the formal
Predictors of Ethical Propensity 245

Procedure tions were negatively skewed. Even a moderate


transformation technique (e.g. square-root)
Twelve hundred questionnaires were mailed to a rendered these measures positively skewed,
random selection of employees in the public however, so the untransformed scores were
sector in one State of Australia. The organiza- retained. Multivariate outliers, when removed
tions included public service, local government, from the analyses, did not affect the results and
government instrumentalities, and educational so were retained.
institutions. Respondents were told that the The primary method of analysis used in this
questionnaire had been authorized by their chief study was hierarchical regression. The order of
executive, and would be completely confidential. entry of variables progressed from individual
They were told further that “The survey is not variables (demographics, values) to organization-
asking you about your personal moral standards. level variables. Individual demographic variables
It is concerned with the relationship between the were entered first and personal values second, to
rules, practices, and traditions which apply in control for individual-level differences. Context
your workplace.” A postage paid return envelope variables were entered next, again because these
was provided with each questionnaire. variables need to be controlled for before group
and organization effects can be explained. The
organizational-level variable was entered last.
Results This procedure was on the basis of evidence
(Hegarty and Sims, 1978; Stead et al., 1987;
Data analysis in this study was performed using Trevino, 1986) that organizational-level variables
SPSS computer package. Mean responses and are likely to influence ethical decisions made by
standard deviations for each measure involved in employees, over and above the effect of indi-
the study are presented in Table I. Inter- vidual- or group-level variables.
correlations between the variables are detailed in Results of the hierarchical regressions are
Table II. presented in Table III. The beta weights listed
The results shown in Table I indicate that in the table are from the final model, when all
items were answered by between 458 and 499 variables are entered.
respondents. Mean values (on a 1–5 scale) were
in the range 1.35 (Ethical tolerance) to 4.23
(Diligence obligation). Mean scores are within Hypothesis tests
the expected ranges for each variable, with low
scores on “unethical” variables such as leniency, Use of formal code of ethics. Hypotheses 1, 3, 6,
triviality, and tolerance; and high scores on 8, and 10 relate to respondents’ use of the code
ethical obligations. Scores on perceived need, of ethics. It was expected, respectively, that
relevance, and conflict were mid-range. individual demographic variables, personal values,
Interestingly, rating of the organization’s ethics contextual variables, and group- and organiza-
was equivocal (Mean = 3.10), suggesting that tional-level variables predict use of the codes.
respondents perceived themselves as behaving From Table II, it can be seen that use of codes
more ethically than mandated by organizational was significantly related to all independent vari-
reward systems. Yet mean scores on others’ use ables with the exception of gender, leniency, and
of formal codes (2.74) and respondents’ own use conflict.
of the code (3.18) were mediocre, suggesting that The hierarchical regression results shown in
code use is not necessary for a perception of eth- Table III, however, give a better idea of the
icality (eg. use of industry rather than organiza- relative contribution of each variable. In partic-
tion code). ular, this table shows the contribution of each
All measures were checked for normality using group of variables when the previously entered
the skew parameter. The five measures relating variables are taken into account. These results
to work group members’ specific ethic obliga- indicate support for Hypotheses 1, 3, 6, and 8,
246
TABLE II
Intercorrelations between variables

Variable ––1 ––2 ––3 ––4 ––5 ––6 ––7 ––8 ––9 –10 –11 –12 –13 –14 –15 –16 –17

01. Role ––
02. Tenure –0.11* ––
03. Gender –0.16** –0.16** ––

Neal M. Ashkanasy et al.


04. Level –0.66** –0.08 –0.18** ––
05. Lenient –0.19** –0.04 –0.02 –0.14** ––
06. Trivial –0.25** –0.01 –0.04 –0.15** –0.38** ––
07. Need –0.09 –0.01 –0.02 –0.07 –0.15** –0.17** ––
08. Relevant –0.28** –0.01 –0.04 –0.18** –0.06 –0.13** –0.12** ––
09. Conflict –0.22** –0.02 –0.07 –0.18** –0.24** –0.22** –0.23** –0.08 ––
10. Ob. Dil. –0.21** –0.10* –0.02 –0.08 –0.05 –0.29** –0.17** –0.09* –0.19** –
11. Ob. Eco. –0.25** –0.07 –0.04 –0.16** –0.10* –0.33** –0.17** –0.08 –0.20** –0.65** ––
12. Ob. Int. –0.24** –0.04 –0.08 –0.19** –0.11* –0.38** –0.21** –013** –0.12** –0.57** –0.50** ––
13. Ob. Law. –0.26** –0.08 –0.09* –0.19** –0.16** –0.32** –0.13** –0.15** –0.12** –0.37** –0.33** –0.53** ––
14. Ob. Pers. –0.21** –0.12** –0.04 –0.09 –0.08 –0.34** –0.17** –0.11* –0.12** –0.65** –0.56** –0.62** –0.48** ––
15. Others –0.01 –0.05 –0.06 –0.05 –0.09* –0.16** –0.33** –0.17** –0.01 –0.26** –0.17** –0.26** –0.18** –0.29** ––
16. Orgn. –0.11* –0.03 –0.08 –0.09* –0.02 –0.28** –0.17** –0.06 –0.18** –0.37** –0.37** –0.27** –0.19** –0.37** –0.15** ––
17. Use –0.13** –0.09* –0.08 –0.18** –0.02 –0.18** –0.30** –0.14** –0.08 –0.19** –0.19** –0.20** –0.14** –0.21** –0.71** –0.11* ––
18. Toler. –0.20** –0.10* –0.03 –0.20** –0.38** –0.41** –0.06 –0.08 –0.20** –0.16** –0.19** –0.28** –0.26** –0.22** –0.09 –0.05 –0.14**

* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.


Predictors of Ethical Propensity 247

but not Hypothesis 10. Thus, each set of pre- significant predictors. As for use of formal code,
dictors added significant variance to the equation, the organization’s ethics was not a significant pre-
except the organization’s ethics. It appears that, dictor. Thus Hypothesis 11 was not supported.
in the present study, that the organizational-level Hypothesis 7, that conflict would be the only
variable was subsumed in the individual and variable out of the context values to be related
group-level effects. The hierarchical regression to ethical tolerance, was supported. From Table
also shows the specific contribution of each II, conflict can be seen to be the only contex-
variable when the others are included. In this tual variable significantly related to ethical toler-
respect, managerial level, perceived need for a ance. Conflict also had a significant beta-weight
code, economy, and others’ use of the code were and a higher beta weight in the analysis pre-
the significant predictors at each of the steps in dicting ethical tolerance than the analysis pre-
the model. An interesting result was that per- dicting use of formal code. This result is
ceived need for a code was negatively related to notwithstanding the finding that the introduction
use of the code. The higher the perceived need of context variables in Step 3 of the hierarchical
for a code, the lower the reported use of the regression did not add significantly to the
code. This result, however, is explainable in terms equation. Thus, while conflict was the only
of respondents’ perception of the relevance of the contributing contextual variable, our results
code. The perceived need for code was negatively suggest that this effect is subordinate to the effects
related to perceived relevance and positively of demographic variables and personal values.
related to code use. Thus, the less relevant a code, Hypothesis 5, that personal values will be
the greater the perceived need for a code stronger predictors of ethical tolerance than use
(improved), and the less use of the existing code. of formal codes, was supported in terms of both
the zero-order correlations and the hierarchical
Ethical tolerance. Hypotheses 2, 4, 9, and 11 relate regression analysis.
to respondents’ ethical tolerance. It was expected, Overall, the results provide support for the
respectively, that individual demographic vari- hypothesized effects with the exception of the
ables, personal values, contextual variables, and role of organizational-level ethical values. It
group- and organizational-level variables predict appears that, within the context of the present
ethical tolerance. From Table II, it can be seen study, individual and group-level variables are the
that the pattern of supporting relationships is not main predictors of use of formal codes and
as clear as for use of codes. Significantly related ethical tolerance. For both analyses, however, it
variables were role, tenure, and level (Hypothesis is clear that there are dominant predictors: others’
2); perceptions of lenience and triviality use of code for the prediction of use of formal
(Hypothesis 4); and conflict and ethical obliga- code (accounting for a unique variance of 34%)
tions (Hypothesis 7). Support for Hypotheses 11 and the two personal values measures, lenience
was not forthcoming in these results. Finally, it and triviality, for the prediction of ethical toler-
is noted that there was a small but significant ance (together accounting for 9% of unique
correlation between use of formal codes and variance).
ethical tolerance, as would be expected.
Results of the hierarchical regression (Table
III) show that demographics and personal Discussion
values were significant predictors, supporting
Hypotheses 2 and 4. Variables entered in Step 4 The aim of the present study was to examine
also produced a small but significant increase in potential predictors of use of a formal code of
R squared, although none of the beta-weights ethics in public sector organizations and to
in this group were significant. This finding thus compare the pattern of relationships on this
provides only limited support for Hypothesis 9. variable with ethical tolerance, the more estab-
Within these regressions, the values of leniency lished criterion. Results revealed that demo-
and triviality and perception of conflict were the graphics, personal values, and contextual and
248
TABLE III
Hierarchical multiple regression predicting use of code and ethical tolerance

Predictors Criterion variables

Use of code Ethical Tolerance

AdjR2 F df ∆R2 F change β AdjR2 F df ∆R2 F change β

Step 1 0.05 05.94*** 04,376 0.06 06.79*** 04,364


Level 0.1300 –0.1000
Job tenure 0.0500 –0.0600

Neal M. Ashkanasy et al.


Gender 0.0400 –0.0400
Role responsibility –0.00000 0.030
Step 2 0.07 05.73*** 06,374 0.03 05.07*** 0.25 21.21*** 06,362 0.19 46.64***
Lenient –0.05000 –0.21**
Trivial –0.04000 00.27**
Step 3 0.15 08.49*** 09,371 0.09 12.90*** 0.25 14.55*** 09,359 0.01 1.1800
Perceived need –0.08*00 –0.0600
Percieved relevance –0.00000 0.000
Conflict –0.05000 0.11*
Step 4 0.50 25.67*** 15,365 0.34 43.18*** 0.26 09.81*** 15,353 0.03 2.24*0
Use of Ob. Diligence –0.07000 0.020
Use of Ob. Economy 0.10*0 –0.0100
Use of Ob. Integrity –0.03000 –0.0900
Use of Ob. Law –0.05000 –0.0700
Use of Ob. Persons 0.0000 –0.0600
Others’ Use of Code 00.66*** –0.0600
Step 5 0.50 24.19*** 16,364 0.00 0.2500 0.27 09.36*** 16,352 0.00 2.1000
Organization’s ethics –0.02000 0.080

* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.


Predictors of Ethical Propensity 249

group-level variables were predictors of both On the other hand, workplace-specific measures,
dependent variables, but that the pattern of such as colleagues’ use of formal codes, is most
prediction varied for each. Results also revealed predictive of use of formal code in the specific
that the organization’s reward structure relating work environment.
to ethics was not a significant predictor for either Our results also indicate that gender does not
use of code or ethical tolerance, especially when play an important role in determining ethical
individual and group-level variables are propensities. As such, the present study supports
accounted for. the occupational socialization theory over gender
Use of formal codes was chosen as a depen- socialization theory (see Mason and Mudrack,
dent variable because codes are now widespread 1996). Note, however, that gender was positively
(Milton-Smith, 1995), and it is widely perceived correlated with organizational level (Table II)
that codes of ethics are necessary precursors for which, in turn, was found to be a determinant
ethical behavior in organizations. In the present of ethical outcomes. Thus, there is a possibility
study, we contrast predictors of the use of codes that gender effects may have been masked by
with a measure of ethical tolerance. The emphasis hierarchical level.
of this discussion therefore is on the compara- An interesting finding of this study is that
tive use of codes as a dependent variable along- propensity to use formal codes was predicted by
side the more established criterion: ethical the use of codes by others in the organization,
tolerance. The practical implications of the rather than application of the specific require-
findings will then be canvassed. The paper con- ments within the code. This suggests that the
cludes with a discussion of some of the method- concept of a formal ethical code is not simply
ological limitations of the study. an aggregation of specific ethical requirements.
The outstanding finding of the present study Rather, it may be that ethical attitudes are holistic
is that different mechanisms operate in the pre- in nature, and that an ethical moral climate
diction of the two measures of ethical propen- derives more from a state of mind than a set of
sity. The organizations which were included in carefully crafted and detailed codes.
the present study shared an established and well A further consideration is the question of
specified formal code of ethics. Our results show, work-group culture versus organization-wide
however, that the prime predictor of use of this culture. Trice and Beyer (1993) argue that
code was the extent to which others in the indi- organizational culture cannot be considered to
vidual’s work group used the code. On the other remain constant across a whole organization or
hand, ethical tolerance was predominantly pre- employment sector. In the present study,
dicted by the personal values which individuals although organization-wide ethical values were
hold regarding the leniency with which they not a predictor of use of codes or ethical toler-
view unethical behavior, and the extent to which ance, others’ use of codes was a critical predictor
they regard such behaviors as trivial. These of code usage. This suggests that ethical climate
findings are consistent with earlier studies (e.g. may be determined more at the group level than
Kitson, 1996; Soutar et al., 1995) which have by organization-wide practices and reward
shown that formal codes of ethics do not neces- systems. Alternatively, it is possible that respon-
sarily translate into ethical behavior and attitudes. dents may have assessed others’ use of formal
The important implication of the present codes in a wider context than just an immediate
research, however, is not so much that use of work group. If this is so, then the non-significant
codes and ethical tolerance are unrelated (in fact, results for organizational-level variables would
there was a small but significant correlation have to be reassessed. Clearly, further research
between them), but that different processes will be needed to clarify this issue.
appear to underlie each criterion. Thus, personal Finally, our results are consistent with the
ethical values, which are not situation-specific, earlier research which has shown that ethical
are most predictive of generalized personal ethical behavior endorsement increases with level in the
tolerance (i.e. similar to Hunt and Vitell, 1993). organizational hierarchy (Callan, 1992). In this
250 Neal M. Ashkanasy et al.

respect, managers lead the way as role models in and conclusions. The first of these is that the data
ethical behavior (Hunt et al., 1984). Since it were collected using a self-administered survey
appears that use of a formal code of ethics is questionnaire. Thus the data are likely to be
determined by perceptions that others’ use the affected by social desirability, or a tendency to
code, it would seem to follow that senior supply “correct” answers instead of a true
managers have a pivotal role to play in promul- reflection of the respondents beliefs, values, and
gation of such codes. practices. In terms of the objectives of the present
study, however, where the focus is on relation-
ships between variables rather than absolute
Practical implications scores, this may not be a problem. A second
limitation of this study was that it was conducted
The main practical implication of the present only in the Public Sector of one State in
study is that managers have a special responsi- Australia. Additional research is required to
bility to encourage their employees to use formal determine if these results will apply in other
codes of ethics as a means to engender a more sectors, states or countries. Of more importance,
positive ethical climate in their organizations. however, is the issue of common method covari-
The dominant predictor of use of a formal code ance. Since hierarchical regression was used to
to emerge from this study was others’ use of the test the study hypotheses, on the other hand, this
code. The prime importance of this predictor problem is minimized (Williams and Brown,
indicates that organizations need to work to 1994).
establish a “critical mass” of code users, so that
others in the organization adopt this as a nor-
mative influence. Conclusions
At the same time, managers need to realize
that ethical behavior involves much more than Models of ethical decision-making have been a
use of codes of ethics. The present study reveals focus in business ethics research for over thirty
that ethical tolerance is determined primarily by years. The present study involved a survey of
personal values, and is only marginally deter- ethical attitudes, beliefs, and practices within
mined by use of formal codes. In this respect, public sector organizations, and has largely con-
managers need to establish a climate where the firmed the earlier research in respect of the con-
dominant values support ethical behavior and tributions of demographic variables, personal
propensities as a holistic concept, rather than as values, contextual variables, and group-level
a set of prescribed ethical obligations. variables. The major contribution of the present
Finally, it should be noted that an essential study, however, is to reveal that factors under-
assumption underlying the present study is that lying the use of formal codes of ethics differ from
the code of ethics is of an adequate quality. those underlying ethical tolerance, which is more
Research has shown that codes need to be traditionally studied. Results also highlight some
specific enough to be meaningful and pertinent of the inherent practical implications of ethical
to particular work units, must clearly state basic code usage. Further study needs to be conducted
principles and expectations, must be realistically to encourage adherence to codes that exist in
based on potential dilemmas, must be commu- organizations, and so to encourage further
nicated, and must be enforced (Stead et al., adoption and refinement of such codes in more
1990). organizations. The present study suggests that
encouraging the use of a code in an organiza-
tion is a self-perpetuating activity – the more
Limitations people who use the code in a given organization,
the more likely it is that other individuals will
The present research is subject to limitations also use the code.
which must be considered in assessing the results
Predictors of Ethical Propensity 251

Notes Considerations and Ethical Climate on Sales


Managers Intentions to Reward or Punish Sales
* Dr. Neal M. Ashkanasy, Graduate School of Force Behavior’, Journal of Business Ethics 16,
Management; Sarah Falkus, School of Psychology; 497–506.
Professor Victor J. Callan, Graduate School of Delaney, J. and D. Stockwell: 1992, ‘Do Company
Management. Ethics Training Programs Make a Difference? An
This ethics study is part of an ongoing study Empirical Analysis’, Journal of Business Ethics 11,
coordinated by the Graduate School of Management, 719–727.
University of Queensland, in collaboration with the Deshpande, S. P.: 1996, ‘Ethical Climate and the Link
Office of the Public Service. We would like to between Success and Ethical Behavior: An
acknowledge, in particular, the assistance of Howard Empirical Investigation of a Non-Profit Organiza-
Whitton for his role in this project, and Lyndelle tion’, Journal of Business Ethics 15, 315–320.
Broadfoot for editorial assistance. EARC: 1992, ‘Report on The Review of Codes of
Correspondence concerning this articles should be Conduct for Public Officials’, Electoral &
addressed to Dr. Neal M. Ashkanasy, at the Graduate Administrative Review Commission (May).
School of Management, University of Queensland, Ferrell, O. C. and L. G. Gresham: 1985, ‘A Contin-
Brisbane, 4072. Electronic mail may be sent to gency Framework for Understanding Ethical
n.ashkanasy@gsm.uq.edu.au. Decision Making in Marketing’, Journal of
1
A copy of the questionnaire used in this study is Marketing 49, 87–96.
available on request from the first author. Ferrell, O. C., L. G. Gresham and J. Fraedrich: 1989,
‘A Synthesis of Ethical Decision Models for
Marketing’, Journal of Macromarketing 11, 55–64.
Forsyth, D. R.: 1980, ‘A Taxonomy of Ethical
References Ideologies’, Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology 39, 175–184.
Akaah, I. P. and E. A. Riordan: 1989, ‘Judgments of Froelich, K. S. and J. L. Kottke: 1991, ‘Measuring
Marketing Professionals about Ethical Issues in Individual Beliefs about Organizational Ethics’,
Marketing Research’, Journal of Marketing Research Educational and Psychological Measurement 51,
26, 112–120. 377–383.
Akaah, J. P.: 1996, ‘The Influence of Organizational Hegarty, W. H. and H. P. Sims: 1978, ‘Some
Rank on Marketing Professionals Ethical Judg- Determinants of Unethical Decision Behavior. An
ments’, Journal of Business Ethics 15, 605–613. Experiment’, Journal of Applied Psychology 63,
Boyle, G. J.: 1991, ‘Does Item Homogeneity Indicate 45–457.
External Consistency or Item Redundancy in Hodgkinson, C.: 1971, ‘Organizational Influences on
Psychometric Scales?’, Personality and Individual Value Systems’, Educational Administration Quarterly
Differences 12, 291–294. 7, 46–55.
Callan, V. J.: 1992, ‘Predicting Ethical Values and Hogan, R.: 1973, ‘Moral Conduct and Moral
Training Needs in Ethics’, Journal of Business Ethics Character: A Psychological Perspective’, Psycho-
11, 761–769. logical Bulletin 79, 217–232.
Carlson, D. S. and M. K. Kacmar: 1997, ‘Perceptions Hunt, S. D., L. B. Chonko and J. B. Wilcox: 1984,
of Ethics across Situations: A View through Three ‘Ethical Problems of Marketing Researchers’,
Different Lenses’, Journal of Business Ethics 16, Journal of Marketing Research 22, 309–324.
147–160. Hunt, S. D. and S. J. Vitell: 1986, ‘A General Theory
Chen, A. Y. S., R. B. Sawyers and P. F. Williams: of Marketing Ethics’, Journal of Macromarketing 6,
1997, ‘Reinforcing Ethical Decision Making 5–16.
through Corporate Culture’, Journal of Business Hunt, S. D. and S. J. Vitell: 1993, ‘A General theory
Ethics 16, 855–865. of Marketing Ethics: A Retrospective and
Cullen, J. B., B. J. Victor and J. W. Bronson: 1993, Revision’, in N. C. Smith and J. A. Quelch (eds.),
‘The Ethical Climate Questionnaire: An Assess- Ethics in Marketing (Irwin Inc., Howewood, IL), pp.
ment of its Development and Validity’, Psychological 775–784.
Reports 73, 667–674. Jansen, E. and M. A. Von Glinow: 1985, ‘Ethical
DeConnick, J. B. and W. F. Lewis: 1997, ‘The Ambivalence and Organizational Reward System’,
Influence of Deontological and Teleological Academy of Management Review 10, 814–822.
252 Neal M. Ashkanasy et al.

Kahn, W. A.: 1990, ‘Toward an Agenda for Business Technology Requires Greater Attention to Ethical
Ethics Research’, Academy of Management Review Issues’, Communication of the ACM 40, 94–100.
15, 311–328. Ruegger, D. and E. W. King: 1992, ‘A Study of the
Kelly, S. W., O. C. Ferrell and S. K. Skinner: 1990, Effect of Age and Gender upon Student Business
‘Ethical Behavior among Marketing Professionals’, Ethics’, Journal of Business Ethics 11, 179–186.
Journal of Business Ethics 9, 681–668. Schminke, M. and M. L. Ambrose: 1997,
Khazanchi, D.: 1995, ‘Unethical Behavior in ‘Asymmetric Perceptions of Ethical Frameworks of
Information Systems: The gender factor’, Journal of Men and Women in Business and Non-Business
Business Ethics 14, 741–749. Settings’, Journal of Business Ethics 16, 719–729.
Kidwell, J. M., R. E. Stevens and A. L. Bethke: 1987, Soutar, G. N., M. McNeil and C. Molster: 1995, ‘A
‘Differences in Ethical Perceptions between Male Management Perspective on Business Ethics’,
and Female Managers’, Journal of Business Ethics 6, Journal of Business Ethics 14, 603–611.
489–493. Stead, W. E., D. L. Worrell, J. B. Spalding and J. G.
Kitson, A.: 1996, ‘Taking the Pulse: Ethics and the Stead: 1987, ‘Unethical Decisions: Socially Learned
British Cooperative Bank’, Journal of Business Ethics Behaviors’, Journal of Social Behavior and Personality
15, 1021–1031. 2, 105–115.
Lozano, J. P.: 1996, ‘Ethics and Management: A Stead, W. E., D. L. Worrell, and J. G. Stead: 1990,
Controversial Issue’, Journal of Business Ethics 15, ‘An Integrative Model for Understanding and
227–236. Managing Ethical Behavior in Business Organiza-
MAB and MIAC: (1996), ‘Ethical Standards and tions’, Journal of Business Ethics 9, 233–242.
Values in the Australian Public Service’, Sykes, T.: 1994, The Bold Riders: Behind Australia’s
Management Advisory Board and Management Corporate Collapses (Allen and Unwin, St Leonards,
Improvement Advisory Committee 19 (May). Australia).
Mason, E. S. and P. E. Mudrack: 1996, ‘Gender and Tabachnick, B. G. and L. S. Fidell: 1996, Using
Ethical Orientation: A Test of Gender and Multivariate Statistics, 3rd ed. (Harper Collins, New
Occupational Socialization Theories’, Journal of York).
Business Ethics 15, 599–604. Trevino, L. K.: 1986, ‘Ethical Decision Making in
McCuddy, M. K. and B. L. Peery: 1996, ‘Selected Organizations: A Person-situation Interactionalist
Individual Differences and Collegians Ethical Model,’, Academy of Management Review 11,
Beliefs’, Journal of Business Ethics 15, 261–272. 601–617.
McKenna, R. J.: 1996, ‘Explaining Amoral Decision Trice, H. M. and J. M. Beyer: 1993, The Cultures of
Making: An External View of Human Disaster’, Work Organization (Prentice Hall, Englewood
Journal of Business Ethics 15, 681–694. Cliffs, NJ).
McNichols, C. W. and T. W. Zimmerer: 1985, Victor, B. and J. B. Cullen: 1990, ‘A Theory and
‘Situational Ethics: An Empirical Study of Measure of Ethical Climate in Organizations’, in
Differentiators of Student Attitudes’, Journal of W. C. Frederick and L. E. Preston (eds.), Business
Business Ethics 4, 175–180. Ethics: Research Issues and Empirical Studies ( JAI
Milton-Smith, J.: 1995, ‘Ethics as Excellence: A Press, Greenwich, CT), pp. 77–97.
Strategic Management Perspective’, Journal of Weber, J.: 1995, ‘Influences upon Organizational
Business Ethics 14, 683–693. Ethical Subclimates: A Multi-departmental Analysis
Morris, M. H., A. S. Marks, J. A. Allen and N. S. of a Single Firm’, Organization Science 6, 509–
Peery: 1996, ‘Modeling Ethical Attitudes and 523.
Behaviors under Conditions of Environmental Williams, L. J. and B. K. Brown: 1994, ‘Method
Turbulence: The Case of South Africa’, Journal of Variance in Organizational Behavior and Human
Business Ethics 15, 1119–1130. Resources Research: Effects on Correlations, Path
Murphy, P. E.: 1995, ‘Corporate Ethics Statements: Coefficients, and Hypothesis Testing’, Organiza-
Current Status and Future Prospects’, Journal of tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes 57,
Business Ethics 14, 727–740. 185–209.
Nunnally, J. C.: 1978, Psychometric Theory (McGraw Windsor, C. A. and N. M. Ashkanasy: 1995, ‘The
Hill, New York). Effect of Client Management Bargaining Power,
Pearson, J. M., L. Crosby and J. P. Shim: 1997, Moral Reasoning Development, and Belief in a Just
‘Measuring the Importance of Ethical Behavior World on Auditor Independence’, Accounting,
Criteria: Increased Reliance of Information Organizations and Society 20, 701–720.
Predictors of Ethical Propensity 253

Zahra, S. A. and M. S. LaTour: 1987, ‘Corporate Graduate School of Management,


Social Responsibility and Organizational Effective- University of Queensland,
ness: A Multivariate Approach’, Journal of Business 4072 Brisbane,
Ethics 6, 459–467. Queensland,
Zey-Ferrell, M., K. M. Weaver and O. C. Ferrell: Australia.
1979, ‘Predicting Unethical Behavior among
E-mail: n.ashkanasy@gsm.uq.edu.au.
Marketing Practitioner’, Human Relations 32,
557–561.

You might also like