Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

Somatosensory and Motor Research, December 2012; 29(4): 131–143

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Biomechanical and neurophysiological mechanisms related to postural


control and efficiency of movement: A review

ANDREIA S. P. SOUSA1,2, AUGUSTA SILVA1, & JOÃO MANUEL R. S. TAVARES3


1
Escola Superior da Tecnologia de Saúde do Instituto Politécnico do Porto, Área Cientı́fica de Fisioterapia, Centro de
Estudos de Movimento e Actividade Humana, Vila Nova de Gaia, Portugal, 2Faculdade de Engenharia, Universidade
do Porto, Porto, Portugal, and 3Instituto de Engenharia Mecânica e Gestão Industrial, Departamento de Engenharia
Mecânica, Faculdade de Engenharia, Universidade do Porto, Porto, Portugal

(Received 27 July 2012; accepted 15 August 2012)

Abstract
Understanding postural control requires considering various mechanisms underlying a person’s ability to stand, to walk, and
to interact with the environment safely and efficiently. The purpose of this paper is to summarize the functional relation
between biomechanical and neurophysiological perspectives related to postural control in both standing and walking based
on movement efficiency. Evidence related to the biomechanical and neurophysiological mechanisms is explored as well as
the role of proprioceptive input on postural and movement control.

Keywords: Postural stability, proprioceptive input, compensatory postural adjustments, standing, walking, gait

Introduction maintain a steady, erect stance; remodeling of stance


in preparation for a voluntary movement; shaping of
Postural control has been defined as the control of
the body for display purposes, as in dance; mainte-
the body’s position in space for the purposes of
nance of balance, as on the gymnast’s beam; or
balance and orientation (Massion 1998; Shumway-
conservation of energy in a demanding task (Kandel
Cook and Woolacott 2000, 2007; Horak 2006).
et al. 2000; Shumway-Cook and Woolacott 2007).
Postural orientation involves the active control of Biomechanically, a postural control position is
body alignment and tonus in relation to gravity, base achieved when the CoM is within the base of support
of support, environment, and internal references and is aligned with the center of pressure (CoP)
(Winter et al. 1990; Massion 1998; Kandel et al. (Winter 1995). Any external or internal perturbation
2000; Lundy-Ekman 2002; Horak 2006; Raine et al. that changes the projection of the CoM to the limits
2009). Postural equilibrium involves the coordina- of the base of support, and the alignment between
tion of sensorimotor strategies to stabilize the body’s CoM and CoP, may lead to postural challenge. The
center of mass (CoM) during both self-initiated and ability to maintain the body’s CoM within a specific
externally triggered disturbances in postural stability boundary is dictated by the efficiency of the individ-
(Horak 2006). Postural stability has been defined as ual’s balance mechanisms (Raine et al. 2009) related
the ability to control the CoM in relation to the base to anticipatory postural adjustments (APA), trig-
of support (Shumway-Cook and Woolacott 2000, gered by feedforward mechanisms prior to the
2007). The weight of each component, that is, of perturbation (Belen’kii et al. 1967; Massion 1992;
orientation and stability, varies according to the task Aruin and Latash 1995; Schepens and Drew 2004;
and the environment. Indeed, the postural control Li and Aruin 2007), as well as to compensatory
system adjusts its goal under different circumstances, postural adjustments (CPA) that are initiated by
such as longitudinal alignment of the whole body to sensory feedback signals (Park et al. 2004;

Correspondence: J. M. R. S. Tavares, Instituto de Engenharia Mecânica e Gestão Industrial, Departamento de Engenharia Mecânica, Faculdade de
Engenharia, Universidade do Porto, Rua Dr Roberto Frias, s/n, 4200-465 Porto, Portugal. E-mail: tavares@fe.up.pt
ISSN 0899–0220 print/ISSN 1369–1651 online ß 2012 Informa Healthcare Ltd.
DOI: 10.3109/08990220.2012.725680
132 A. S. P. Sousa et al.

Alexandrov et al. 2005). The process of generation of 1996; Mazzaro et al. 2005; Segers 2006;
APA is likely to be affected by expected magnitude Arechavaleta et al. 2008). The CPG designs spinal
(Aruin and Latash 1996; Bouisset et al. 2000) and networks that can generate patterns of rhythmic
direction (Aruin and Almeida 1997; Santos and activity in the absence of external feedback or
Aruin 2008) of the perturbation, voluntary action supraspinal control. However, these spinal networks
associated with the perturbation (Arruin 2003; are modulated by peripheral input and supraspinal
Shiratori and Aruin 2007), postural task and body control (Armstrong 1986; Rossignol et al. 2006).
configuration (van der Fits et al. 1998; Arruin 2003). The present study aims to review the biomechan-
In conditions of high instability demands, the central ical and neurophysiological mechanisms related to
nervous system (CNS) may suppress APA as a postural control in both standing and walking based
protection against their possible destabilizing effects on movement efficiency. In the following sections,
(Arruin et al. 1998). In fact, a relation between APA the neural mechanisms, the role of afferent informa-
and CPA has been demonstrated (Santos et al. tion, and biomechanical aspects will be considered to
2009), suggesting the existence of an optimal utili- upright standing and human gait.
zation of APA in postural control. The CPA response
depends not only on the APA, but also on the
direction and magnitude of the perturbation, the Postural and movement control
base of support dimension (Horak and Nashner Neural mechanisms
1986; Henry et al. 1998; Dimitrova et al. 2004; Jones
et al. 2008), and on the involvement in a secondary Upright standing. The upright stance of the human
task (Bateni et al. 2004). is an unstable position (Peterka and Loughlin 2004).
The main sensory systems involved in postural Postural sway reflects noise and regulatory activity of
control are proprioception, the vestibular system and the several control loops involved in maintenance of
vision, and their afferent pathways within the CNS balance, which requires that the CoM never deviates
(Day and Cole 2002; Shumway-Cook and Woolacott beyond the support area. The control of the appro-
2007). Afferent and efferent pathways involve the priate level of neuromuscular activity to produce
spinal cord, the brain stem, the cerebellum, the rapid postural control strategies involves medial
midbrain, and the sensorimotor cortex. All of these descending systems (Raine et al. 2009). The role of
contribute to the development of an internal repre- these systems is fundamental to the organization of
sentation of body posture that is continuously postural tone appropriately according to environ-
updated based on multisensory feedback and is ment demands, gravity, and base of support. The
used to forward commands to control body position vestibular system action is related to postural tone
in space (Massion 1994; Mergner and Rosemeier adjustments to body weight support (Matsuyama and
1998). This provides a basis for all interactions Drew 2000). This system plays a major role in the
involving perception and action with respect to the antigravity function (Latash 1998; Siegel and Sapru
external world and is likely to be partly genetically 2011) as it is responsible, through the lateral
determined and partially acquired through ongoing vestibulospinal tract, for the activation of ipsilateral
experiential learning. It is therefore, adaptable and extensor motor neurons and their associated gamma
vulnerable, is dependent on the ongoing information motor neurons (Rothwell 1994; Latash 1998; Siegel
that it receives (Meadows and Williams 2009), and is and Sapru 2011). The reticular formation has an
related to human movement variability, allowing for important role on APA production (Schepens and
adjustable functional behavior (Van Emmerik and Drew 2004) as it receives afferent input from all the
Van Wegen 2000). sensory systems and also from the pre-motor cortex
The neural process involved in stability organiza- and supplementary motor area (Brodal 1981;
tion and body orientation in space is necessary Rothwell 1994; Kiernan 2005). The possible role of
practically for all dynamic motor actions (Massion the cortex in postural control has been discussed and
1998). Specifically, the control of balance during gait relevance has been given to the role of pre-motor
and while changing from one posture to another cortex (Massion 1992) and supplementary motor
requires a complex control of a moving body CoM area in the control of APA production (Jacobs et al.
that is not within the base of foot support (Winter 2009).
et al. 1993). In fact, human gait is influenced by a
multifactorial interaction that results from neural and
mechanical organization, including musculoskeletal Human gait. Appropriate mechanisms for control-
dynamics, a central pattern generator (CPG), based ling muscle tone are essential to maintain stable
on a genetically determined spinal circuit, and postural and locomotor synergies in bipedal gait
peripheral and supraspinal inputs (McCollum et al. performance. The dependency between postural
1995; Borghese et al. 1996; Horak and Macpherson control and movement may be justified by the
Postural control and efficiency of movement 133

connection between the cortex and the reticular motor learning (Wichmann and DeLong 1996;
formation. Actually, muscle tone and the locomotor Mackay-Lyons 2002). The midbrain locomotor
system can be controlled, in parallel, by a combined region activates the ‘‘muscle tone excitatory system’’
input to the brain stem of net inhibition from the and ‘‘rhythm generating system’’ (Takakusaki et al.
basal ganglia and net excitation from the motor 2004). Although not being relevant in gait, the motor
cortex (Takakusaki et al. 2004). Specifically, an cortex is involved in the modification of CPG activity
important neuronal circuit that allows the coexis- in unstable surfaces or when gait needs a visual
tence of postural adjustments and execution of orientation. The degree of supraspinal and spinal
movement is the cortico-ponto-cerebellar pathway, influences in movement generation is determined by
which allows the connection of the cortex with the context (Mackay-Lyons 2002). The main structures
nucleus of the brain stem and cerebellum (Ito 2006). involved in postural control in both standing and
With this circuit, the postural control can be orga- walking are represented in Figure 1.
nized ipsilaterally to the activated side with respect to
the control of movement in the contralateral side.
The role of afferent information
This relationship between movement and postural
control through the activation of ventro-medial and Upright standing. The ability to reweight sensory
dorso-lateral systems, as well as the importance of information depending on the context is important to
the coactivation mechanism between the two lower maintain stability when a person moves from one
limbs (Dietz et al. 2002) to keep the body CoM over context to another (Peterka 2002). For instance,
the feet (Dietz et al. 1989, 1992), justify the study of while vestibular information may not be a large
mechanisms that occur in both sides of the body in contributor for the control of upright stance (Winter
relation to a unilateral movement like gait initiation et al. 1998) and for triggering or coordinating muscle
as well as in relation to the gait cycle. activation patterns associated with ankle strategy
Basic structures involved in the control of locomo- (Horak et al. 1990), it is likely to play a crucial role
tion and postural muscle tone are located in the during moments of increased postural instability
midbrain (Takakusaki et al. 2004). Some circum- (Fitzpatrick et al. 1994). As in normal conditions,
scribed regions have been identified as relevant in proprioceptive information assumes more relevance
activating and controlling the intensity of spinal than other sources; in this paper focus has been given
locomotor CPG operation, maintaining equilibrium to the role of proprioceptive information, Figure 2.
during locomotion, adapting limb movement to It is well known that the mechanoreceptors (i.e.,
external conditions, and coordinating locomotion to specialized sensorial receptors responsible for trans-
other motor acts (Armstrong 1986; Jordan 1986; duction of mechanic events into neural signs (Grigg
Orlovsky 1991). Among the main supraspinal centers 1994)) accounting for proprioceptive information are
involved are the sensorimotor cortex and the supple- primarily founded on muscles, tendons, ligaments,
mentary motor area (Mackay-Lyons 2002; Miyai et al. and capsule (Johansson et al. 1991; Jami 1992;
2002; Kapreli et al. 2006), the cerebellum, the basal Hogersvorst and Brand 1998). Receptors located in
ganglia (Garcia-Rill 1986; Mackay-Lyons 2002), the the deeper skin tissue and fascia are traditionally
midbrain locomotor region (Kandel et al. 2000; associated with touch receptors, being categorized as
Mileykovskiy et al. 2000), and the spinal cord (Dietz additional sources (Macefield et al. 1990; Grigg
et al. 1992). The sensorimotor cortex is involved in 1994; Edin and Johansson 1995). Support has been
the preparation for and execution of movement given to the role of the Golgi tendon organs in
(Nelson 1996). The cerebellum receives copies of providing afferent input from ‘‘gravity-dependent’’
CPG output to motoneurons via ventral spinocer- receptors required to indicate the projection of the
ebellar tract and spinoreticularcerebellar pathways, as body’s CoM within the base of support (Dietz et al.
well as information about the activity of the peripheral 1992; Dietz 1996, 1998; Dietz and Colombo 1996).
motor apparatus via the dorsal spinocerebellar tract In addition, the small magnitudes of sway observed
(Orlovsky 1991). Based on these, they influence during quiet standing may be enough to alter muscle
motoneurons indirectly via vestibulospinal, rubrosp- lengths, resulting in changes of Ia afferent input onto
inal, reticulospinal, and corticospinal pathways the motoneuron pool of the lower limbs. Recent
(Orlovsky 1991). The cerebellum’s main role may studies by Loram et al. (2005b) have suggested this
be the timing of muscle activation, ‘‘fine-tuning’’ the possibility, whereby muscle length changes in the
output by adapting each step (Lansner and Ekeberg gastrocnemius and soleus muscles during quiet
1994). Nevertheless, both the cerebellum and the standing have been detected within the range at
basal ganglia seem to play an important role in timing which muscle spindles are sensitive to movement
of sequential muscle activation, with the basal ganglia (Proske et al. 2000). Support has been given to the
operating at the level of planning, initiation, execu- role of medium latency responses from group II
tion, and termination of motor programs as well as during standing (Schieppati et al. 1995; Corna et al.
134 A. S. P. Sousa et al.

Figure 1. A conceptual schematic diagram illustrating the main structures involved in postural control in both standing and
walking.

1996; Nardone et al. 1996). Indeed, there is evidence been considered the source of muscle proprioceptive
that muscle spindle type II fibers play a more relevant information signaling changes in body position
role than group Ia fibers in the control of bipedal (Fitzpatrick et al. 1992b; Loram and Lakie 2002a;
stance (Marchand-Pauvert et al. 2005) as only Loram et al. 2005b). These muscles act predomi-
medium latency responses have a stabilizing effect nantly as active agonists and, because the foot is
during perturbations of stance, and also because constrained on the ground, they prevent forward
these fibers are more influenced by the ‘‘postural set’’ toppling of the body, whose center of gravity is
(Nardone et al. 1990). Findings obtained by maintained in front of the ankle joint (Fitzpatrick
Nardone et al. (1996), demonstrate the existence of et al. 1992a; Maki and Ostrovski 1993; Loram and
crossed neural pathways fed by these fibers, which Lakie 2002a; Lakie et al. 2003; Loram et al. 2005a).
explains the bilateral electromyographic responses to The problem with muscle spindles as position
unilateral perturbations during standing. This find- sensors is that they are able to generate impulses in
ing is supported by Dietz (1996), as this author response to muscle length changes as well as from
argues that a complex bilateral coordination of leg fusimotor activity (Proske 2006). According to Di
muscle activation (mediated by a spinal mechanism Giulio et al. (2009), the best proprioceptive infor-
(Dietz and Berger 1984)) is needed for upright mation may come from un-modulated muscles
postural control during locomotion. crossing the joint in parallel with the active agonist.
All the receptors mentioned above, and the corre- In fact, earlier studies stated that, depending upon
sponding afferent input, may allow a modulation of the stance conditions, muscle stretch does not
postural activity in relation to muscle length and necessarily result in a compensatory stretch reflex
tension variation, but only a combination of afferent response but instead results in an antagonistic
inputs can provide the necessary information to muscle activation (Hansen et al. 1988; Gollhofer
control body equilibrium (Dietz 1996). The role of et al. 1989). Based on this, it has been argued that
proprioceptive information from ankle muscles has reciprocal patterns of muscle activation are typically
been highlighted in various studies (Fitzpatrick et al. involved in postural control (Latash 1993; Di Giulio
1992a, 1994; Gatev et al. 1999; Loram et al. 2005a). et al. 2009). Neurophysiologically, reciprocal inhibi-
Some authors go further, arguing that normal sub- tion is mediated, at least in part, by a dysinaptic
jects can stand in a stable manner when receptors of circuit in the spinal cord that is subject to several
the ankle muscles are the only source of information supraspinal as well as segmental modulator mecha-
about postural sway (Fitzpatrick et al. 1992a, 1994). nisms (Jankowska 1992) and varies according to the
The soleus and the gastrocnemius have traditionally way in which antagonist muscles are activated
Postural control and efficiency of movement 135

Figure 2. Representation of the most important proprioceptive receptors and afferences and their role in standing and stance
phase of gait (a). There are also represented important networks related to proprioceptive information (b). Dotted lines in (b)
represent efferent pathways and solid lines represent afferent pathways.

(Lavoie et al. 1997). Synergies between antagonist detail in the temporal pattern of the muscle activation
muscles include simple patterns of reciprocal activa- sequence (Pearson 1993; Ivanenko et al. 2006), (2)
tion, co-contractions, and complex triphasic activa- the reinforcement of ongoing motor activity, parti-
tion patterns (Lavoie et al. 1997). There is evidence cularly that involving load-bearing muscles, such as
that the strength of dysinaptic inhibition is reduced the extensor muscles during the stance phase of gait
during co-contraction of antagonist muscles com- (Pearson 1993; Stephens and Yang 1996; Sinkjær
pared with reciprocal activation (Nielsen and et al. 2000), and (3) the control of transition from
Kagamihara 1992). Another source of proprioceptive one phase of movement to another (Pearson 1993;
information may come from the cutaneous afferents Lacquaniti et al. 1999). Swing is initiated when the
of the feet as there is a large distribution of cutaneous leg is extended (stretching the flexor muscles) and
receptors at various locations on the sole of the foot unloaded (reduced force in extensor muscles sensed
(Kennedy and Inglis 2002). It has been suggested by the Golgi tendon organ of the extensor muscles)
that this source of proprioceptive information con- (Zehr and Duysens 2004). Consequently, gait cycles
tributes to both the coding and spatial representation depend on the afferent input from peripheral recep-
of body posture during standing (Roll et al. 2002), tors as the muscle force production at a given level of
and that the architecture and physiology of the foot motor unit recruitment can change according to
appear to contribute to the task of bipedal postural length (velocity) and tension variations (Frigo et al.
control with great sensitivity (Wright et al. 2012). 1996).
The monosynaptic excitation of spinal motoneu-
rons from the large diameter group Ia afferent fibers
Human gait. During gait, afferent feedback adapts related to a short latency response (Matthews 1991)
dynamically, through a reciprocal relationship, the has been demonstrated when an expected stretch of
response of the CPG to environmental requirements the ankle extensors is imposed during gait (Yang
and assumes multiple roles in regulating the produc- et al. 1991; Sinkjaer et al. 1996). In addition, a phasic
tion of motor patterns, such as: (1) the production of modulation of Ia input has also been demonstrated
136 A. S. P. Sousa et al.

by changes in the magnitude of H-reflex over the the body, causing it to accelerate further away from
course of the gait cycle, with the greatest attenuation the upright position. Corrective torque must be
occurring during flexion (Yang and Whelan 1993; generated to counter the destabilizing torque due to
Schneider et al. 2000). This modulation is consistent gravity. This process of continuous small body
with the fact that the maximum soleus length occurs deviations countered by corrective torques creates a
during the foot off, when maximum plantar flexion of pattern known as spontaneous sway. The mecha-
the foot occurs, which is coincident with its maxi- nisms underlying spontaneous sway are not fully
mum force production (Orendurff et al. 2005). The understood, and controversy remains regarding the
modulation of the H-reflex is a reflection of: (1) the organization of sensory and motor systems contrib-
background excitability of the motoneuron pool, (2) uting to the spontaneous sway. Numerous authors
the modulation associated with the activation of the have suggested that active feedback control mecha-
antagonist muscle, and (3) presynaptic inhibition of nisms contribute to the maintenance of upright
the primary afferents (Yang and Whelan 1993) that stance (Johanson and Magnusson 1991; Peterka
seems to be related partially to Ia afferents from the and Benolken 1995; Fitzpatrick et al. 1996; van der
hip and knee extensor muscles (Brooke et al. 1997). Kooij et al. 2001; Peterka and Loughlin 2004).
Medium latency response from group II has been Recent studies have shown that a model based
demonstrated during gait (Dietz et al. 1985) and primarily on a feedback mechanism with 150–
some authors argue that this group is more important 200 ms of delay can account for postural control
to feedback in the stance phase than group Ia during a broad variety of perturbations (Peterka and
(Sinkjær et al. 2000; Grey et al. 2001, 2002; Benolken 1995; Peterka 2002; Peterka and Loughlin
Nielsen and Sinkjaer 2002). Earlier studies have 2004) and can yield a spontaneous sway pattern that
suggested that strong central effects of group II resembles normal (Peterka 2000) or pathological
muscle afferents are mediated via a complex neural spontaneous sway (Parkinson’s disease (Maurer et al.
pathway influenced by supraspinal input and periph- 2003)). However, the relevance of feedback mecha-
eral input during walking (Dietz et al. 1987; Yang nisms for postural control is still debated. Some
et al. 1991). Specifically, there is evidence for the role authors concluded from their experiments that cor-
of vestibulo- and reticulospinal pathways (Davies and rective torque originating from feedback control is
Edgley 1994) that supports the hypothesis that the insufficient for stabilizing the body (Fitzpatrick et al.
facilitation of the relevant lumbar propriospinal 1996). Others suggested additional sources for cor-
neurons by descending tract neurons would be rective torque, like prediction (Morasso et al. 1999;
stronger over group II during maintenance of posture van der Kooij et al. 2001), or have proposed more
than during voluntary contractions (Marchand- complex concepts (Collins and De Luca 1993;
Pauvert et al. 2005). In addition, the role of group Baratto et al. 2002; Loram and Lakie 2002b).
Ib load-sensitive afferences related to a medium Postural sway has been viewed as a result of a
latency response has been reported to contribute to correlated random-walk process (Collins and De
the regulation stance phase of gait (Stephens and Luca 1993), a result of computational noise (Kiemel
Yang 1999) associated with a disynaptic Ib reflex et al. 2002), and/or a moving reference point
reversal (Stephens and Yang 1996). Findings (Zatsiorsky and Duarte 1999). The possible impor-
reported in Grey et al. (2007) suggest that tendon tance of the postural sway as a reflection of a
organ feedback via an excitatory group Ib pathway hypothetical search process within the system of
contributes to the late stance phase enhancement of postural stabilization has been emphasized (Riley
the soleus muscle activity. The combination of the et al. 1997; Mochizuki et al. 2006).
different afferent inputs plays an important role on From a functional point of view, the control of
gait dynamics related to the ipsilateral limb but also human upright posture stability is commonly viewed
on the contralateral limb, as it has been demon- as a continuous stabilization process of a multilink
strated that unilateral leg displacement during gait inverted pendulum, where the main controlled
evokes a bilateral response pattern, with a similar parameter is the CoM position within the limits of
onset on both sides (Dietz and Berger 1984). From a the supporting base (Maurer and Peterka 2005).
functional point of view, this interlimb coordination This aspect has been described as biomechanical
is necessary to keep the body’s CoM over the feet constraints that determine patterns of postural coor-
(Dietz 1996). dination (Buchanan and Horak 2003). In stance, the
limits of stability, that is, the area over which
individuals can move their CoM and maintain
Biomechanical aspects
equilibrium without changing the base of support,
Upright standing. Upright stance is associated with is shaped like a cone (McCollum and Leen 1989).
small deviations from an upright body position, Thus, equilibrium is not a particular position but a
which results in a gravity-induced torque acting on space determined by the size of the support base and
Postural control and efficiency of movement 137

the limitations on joint range, muscle strength, and activity, muscle torque, ground reaction forces,
sensory information available to detect limits. The kinematics, and metabolic energy costs have been
CNS has an internal representation of this cone of assessed and quantified. This data set requires an
stability that it uses to determine how to move to interpretation and organization of the fundamental
maintain equilibrium (Horak 2006). Gatev et al. principles that elucidate the mechanisms of gait.
(1999) reported a signiEcant correlation between Several models have been suggested to describe
spontaneous body sway and the activity of the human gait mechanisms (Saunders et al. 1953;
gastrocnemius muscle. They also found that gastroc- Cavagna and Margaria 1966; Cavagna and Kaneko
nemius activity preceded temporally CoM displace- 1977; Waters and Mulroy 1999; Donelan et al.
ment, suggesting a central program of control of the 2002b; Kuo et al. 2005, 2007). The six determinants
ankle joint stiffness working to predict the loading of gait theory (Saunders et al. 1953), based on the
pattern. More recent studies proposed that the actual premise that vertical and horizontal CoM displace-
postural control system during quiet standing adopts ments are energetically costly, propose a set of
a control strategy that relies notably on velocity kinematic features that help to reduce CoM dis-
information of CoM and that such a controller can placement. However, there is evidence that some
modulate muscle activity in an anticipatory manner determinants have a non-significant role on the CoM
without using feedforward mechanisms (Masani vertical displacement and that there is higher meta-
et al. 2003). According to this view, velocity feedback bolic expenditure when subjects voluntary reduce
can play a signiEcant role in anticipating body vertical displacement of CoM (for review, see Kuo
position changes because it carries information et al. 2007). The inverted pendulum model proposes
about the subsequent state of the body, that is, a that most of the work during gait is performed by a
change in CoM velocity indicates the direction and passive mechanism of exchange of gravitational
intensity with which the current CoM displacement potential and kinetic energies (60–70%) (Cavagna
will be changed in the following time instant. It has et al. 1977; Griffin et al. 2003). However, this model
been hypothesized that the integration of proprio- cannot reproduce the existence of two peaks in the
ceptive and plantar cutaneous sensations would play vertical ground reaction force (Pandy 2003; Zajac
a signiEcant role in the velocity feedback mechanism et al. 2003) and does not account for the costs which
(Masani et al. 2003). Another biomechanical con- are not considered responsible for work, like isomet-
straint is related to frequency of postural sway ric force stabilization and body weight support (Kuo
(Nashner et al. 1989), as when postural sway is et al. 2005). The difference in the percentage of
lower than 0.5 Hz, the body can be compared to a energy recovery in relation to an ideal inverted
simple inverted pendulum (McCollum and Leen pendulum has been related mostly to the double
1989), and when it is higher than this value, the body support phase (McGeer 1990). Indeed, a low
can be compared to a double inverted pendulum percentage of energy recovery has been demon-
with the fulcrum at the hip level (Yang et al. 1990). strated in the double support phase (Geyer et al.
2006) related to the interruption of the energy-
conserving motion of single support by an inelastic
Human gait. The coordination between posture collision of the swing leg with the ground, leading to
and movement involves the dynamic control of the changes in velocities of the legs and the CoM (Kuo
CoM in the base of support (Stapley et al. 1999). et al. 2007). This energy loss can be reduced by 75%
Consequently, to access the simplified concept of through the application of a propulsion impulse in
locomotion it is necessary to consider the behavior of the trailing leg immediately before collision of the
the CoM during gait cycles. The trajectory described leading leg (Kuo 2002). Simulations suggest that
by the CoM in the plan of progression is a sinusoidal the ankle plantar flexor (soleus, gastrocnemius) and
curve that moves vertically twice during one cycle the uni- and biarticular hip extensors (gluteus max-
and laterally in the horizontal plan, and that is similar imus, hamstrings) dominate work output over the
in form to that found in the vertical displacement gait cycle (Neptune et al. 2004). These muscles,
(Norkin and Levangie 1992; Gard et al. 2004). Peak- being active in the late stance and in the beginning of
to-peak amplitude is described as being about stance, are therefore restoring energy to the body
4–5 cm for adults at freely chosen speed and has near double support (Zajac et al. 2003).
been used to estimate exchanges of mechanical Ankle plantar flexors are the primary contributors
energy, efficiency, work, and to describe the sym- for forward progression and vertical support (Kepple
metry as an indicator of the quality of gait (for more et al. 1997), before midstance, they hinder progres-
information, see Gard et al. 2004). sion (Neptune et al. 2001) and during midstance,
The human gait results from a complex interaction they maintain body support and the forward motion
of muscle forces, joint movements, and neural of the trunk and leg, which is consistent with
commands. Variables, including electromyographic inverted-pendulum-like ballistic walking as the
138 A. S. P. Sousa et al.

synergy of these muscles in this subphase occurs with can store and recover energy in the passive elastic
minimal metabolic energy expenditure, as expected tissues in the tendon and muscles. However, this last
in ballistic-like walking (Zajac et al. 2003). energy-conserving mechanism is quite small in
Biarticular hip extensors generate forward accelera- walking (Winter and Eng 1995). The CNS has
tion during the first half of stance, while uniarticular learnt how to create motor patterns to conserve
quadriceps muscles and the uniarticular hip exten- much of the energy that was generated earlier in the
sors decelerate the body mass center and provide gait cycle. It has been estimated that of the total
body support (Neptune et al. 2004; Liu et al. 2006). energy changes of all body segments over the gait
The biarticular quadriceps muscle is a significant cycle only 33% are caused by active muscle gener-
contributor to forward progression in late stance ation and absorption, while 67% are due to the
(Neptune et al. 2004). passive transfers between segments (Pierrynowski
According to Donelan et al. (2004), lateral stabi- et al. 1980). Considering this, it is important to
lization exacts a modest metabolic cost as walking quantify the movement also on the criterion of
requires active lateral stabilization. It has been efficiency (Fetters and Holt 1990; Sparrow and
demonstrated that the gluteus medius, although Newell 1998).
acting primarily outside the sagittal plane in walking, In biomechanical and physiological research, effi-
contributes to support and slows progression (less ciency of movement is normally defined as the ratio
than the other muscles) in the first half of stance and of the mechanical work performed and the metabolic
contributes to support in the second half (Liu et al. cost of performing the work (Stainsby et al. 1980).
2006). Additionally, it has been demonstrated that Typically, the efficiency is calculated as
the body lateral motion is partially stabilized via
medio-lateral foot placement (Kuo 1999; Donelan Mechanical work
Efficiency ð%Þ ¼  100:
et al. 2004). Energy work
Energy expenditure during walking can be char-
Movement efficiency acterized through mechanical energy estimations
(Cavagna et al. 1963; Willems et al. 1995; Saibene
The relationship between muscle activity and whole
and Minetti 2003) or metabolic energy measure-
body mechanics is too variable and complex to allow
ments (Waters and Mulroy 1999). Mechanical
direct control of the latter without an intermediate
energy is generally estimated by one of three
kinematic representation (Lacquaniti et al. 1999).
approaches: (1) analysis of energy changes of the
There is evidence that supports the idea that global
CoM in relation to the surroundings (external work)
kinematic gait is controlled (Ivanenko et al. 2004).
Kinematics is relatively invariant in various modes of and of the body segments regarding the CoM
locomotion, while the electromyographic activity (internal work) (Cavagna et al. 1963; Cavagna and
patterns to produce the required kinematic patterns Margaria 1966; Willems et al. 1995); (2) analysis of
can vary considerably (Grasso et al. 1998; Ivanenko the energy changes of moving body segments (sum of
et al. 2004). These findings suggest that neural segmental energies); or (3) measurement of muscle
circuitry can somehow specify limb kinematics, and power around the joints (net joint work) (Winter
the appropriate muscle synergies would be deter- 2005). In all mechanical energy estimations, the
mined in a subordinate and flexible manner to adapt actual amount of work performed is underestimated,
to the current mechanical constraints (Lacquaniti as additional metabolic work resulting from isometric
et al. 1999, 2002). The basic biomechanical control muscle contractions or antagonist co-contractions is
signal may exert its action through an appropriate not taken into account (Fetters and Holt 1990;
model of inverse dynamics and feedback device that Winter 2005). This problem is overcome when
determines the muscle torque necessary to achieve assessing metabolic energy, that is, by measuring
kinematic patterns (Ivanenko et al. 2004). The oxygen consumption during walking (Fetters and
significance of muscle redundancy would then be Holt 1990; Vandewalle 2004). The relation between
to allow the same movement to be carried out by metabolic cost and the mechanical work performed
means of different combinations of muscle activity by stance limb muscles to lift and accelerate the CoM
under dissimilar environmental circumstances, for during walking has been already demonstrated
instance, to cope with fatigue or changes in load (Donelan et al. 2001, 2002a) and has been consid-
(Lacquaniti et al. 1999). ered a valid predictor of walking performance
The major function of muscles in gait is to (Anderson and Pandy 2001). Metabolic energy
generate and absorb energy; such function is largely expenditure can be accessed through indirect calo-
ignored in neurophysiological research (Winter and rimetry, where oxygen consumption and/or carbon
Eng 1995). The body has the capacity of transferring dioxide production is measured and converted into
energy between segments across the joint centers and energy expenditure using formulae (Garby and
Postural control and efficiency of movement 139

Astrup 1987; Cunningham 1990) which have been triggered by standard motor action. Electroencephalogr Clin
reported as a valid method (Levine 2005). Neurophysiol 101(6):497–503.
Aruin A, Almeida G. 1997. A coactivation strategy in anticipatory
Mechanical and metabolic energy analyses allow postural adjustment in persons with Down syndrome. Motor
monitoring how the CNS takes advantage of energy- Control (2):178–197.
conserving mechanisms in order to achieve a more Baratto L, Morasso PG, Re C, Spada G. 2002. A new look at
efficient movement. posturographic analysis in the clinical context: Sway-density
versus other parameterization techniques. Motor Control
6(3):246–270.
Concluding remarks Bateni H, Zecevi A, McIlroy W, Maki B. 2004. Resolving conflicts
in task demands during balance recovery: Does holding an
Postural control has been vastly explored in the object inhibit compensatory grasping? Exp Brain Res
scientific community. However, the complexity of 157(1):49–58.
the interrelations between neural and mechanical Belen’kii V, Gurfinkel’ V, Pal’tsev Y. 1967. Elements of control of
voluntary movements. Biofizika 10:135–141.
aspects and the environment leads to the need to Borghese N, Bianchi L, Lacquaniti F. 1996. Kinematic
study postural control in a holistic way. In addition, determinants of human locomotion. J Physiol 494(3):863–869.
the study of postural control needs to reflect the Bouisset S, Richardson J, Zattara M. 2000. Are amplitude and
dynamic interrelation of the different components of duration of anticipatory postural adjustments identically scaled
human movement on the basis of movement effi- to focal movement parameters in humans? Neurosci Lett
278(3):153–156.
ciency. The adaptability, vulnerability, and continu-
Brodal A. 1981. Neurological anatomy in relation to clinical
ous dependency of afferent information on the medicine, 3rd ed. New York: Oxford University Press.
postural control system turn this area into a focus Brooke JD, Cheng J, Collins DF, McIlroy WE, Misiaszek JE,
of clinical interest. Considering that the postural Staines WR. 1997. Sensori-sensory afferent conditioning with
control system has the capacity of reorganization for leg movement: Gain control in spinal reflex and ascending
higher movement performance, it is important to paths. Prog Neurobiol 51(4):393–421.
Buchanan J, Horak F. 2003. Voluntary control of postural
understand in detail the static and dynamic postural equilibrium patterns. Behav Brain Sci 143:121–140.
control mechanisms and strategies and how these Cavagna G, Saibene F, Margaria R. 1963. External work in
mechanisms influence other systems and are influ- walking. J Appl Physiol 18:1–9.
enced by changes in afferent and efferent Cavagna G, Margaria R. 1966. Mechanics of walking. J Appl
information. Physiol 21(1):271–278.
Cavagna G, Kaneko M. 1977. Mechanical work and efficiency in
level walking and running. J Physiol 268(2):467–481.
Cavagna GA, Heglund NC, Taylor CR. 1977. Mechanical work in
Acknowledgement terrestrial locomotion: Two basic mechanisms for minimizing
energy expenditure. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol
Declaration of interest: The authors report no 233(5):R243–R261.
conflicts of interest. Collins J, De Luca C. 1993. Open-loop and closed-loop control of
posture: A random-walk analysis of center-of-pressure trajec-
tories. Exp Brain Res 95(2):308–318.
Corna S, Galante M, Grasso M, Nardone A, Schieppati M. 1996.
References Unilateral displacement of lower limb evokes bilateral EMG
responses in leg and foot muscles in standing humans. Exp
Alexandrov A, Frolov A, Horak F, Carlson-Kuhta P, Park S. Brain Res 109(1):83–91.
2005. Feedback equilibrium control during human standing. Cunningham J. 1990. Calculation of energy expenditure from
Biol Cybern 93:309–322.
indirect calorimetry: Assessment of the Weir equation.
Anderson FC, Pandy MG. 2001. Dynamic optimization of human
Nutrition 6(3):222–223.
walking. J Biomech Eng 123(5):381–390.
Davies HE, Edgley SA. 1994. Inputs to group II-activated
Arechavaleta GL, Laumond J-P, Hicheur H, Berthoz A. 2008. An
midlumbar interneurones from descending motor pathways in
optimality principle governing human walking. IEEE Trans
the cat. J Physiol 479(Pt 3):463–473.
Robotics 24(1):5–14.
Day B, Cole J. 2002. Vestibular-evoked postural responses in the
Armstrong M. 1986. Supraspinal contributions to the initiation
absence of somatosensory information. Brain
and control of locomotion on the cat. Prog Neurobiol
125(9):2081–2088.
26:273–361.
Di Giulio I, Maganaris C, Baltzopoulos V, Loram I. 2009. The
Arruin A, Forrest W, Latash M. 1998. Anticipatory postural
proprioceptive and agonist roles of gastrocnemius, soleus and
adjustments in conditions of postural instability.
tibialis anterior muscles in maintaining human upright posture.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 109(4):350–359.
J Physiol 587(10):2399–2416.
Arruin A. 2003. The effect of changes in the body configuration
Dietz V, Berger W. 1984. Interlimb coordination of posture in
on anticipatory postural adjustments. Motor Control 7(3):
264–277. patients with spastic paresis. Brain 107(3):965–978.
Aruin A, Latash M. 1995. The role of motor action in Dietz V, Quintern J, Berger W. 1985. Afferent control of human
anticipatory postural adjustments studied with self-induced stance and gait: Evidence for blocking of group I afferents
and externally triggered perturbations. Exp Brain Res during gait. Exp Brain Res 61(1):153–163.
106:291–300. Dietz V, Quintern J, Sillem M. 1987. Stumbling reactions in man:
Aruin A, Latash M. 1996. Anticipatory postural adjustments Significance of proprioceptive and pre-programmed mechan-
during self-initiated perturbations of different magnitude isms. J Physiol 386(1):149–163.
140 A. S. P. Sousa et al.

Dietz V, Horstmann GA, Berger W. 1989. Interlimb coordination Geyer H, Seyfarth A, Blickhan R. 2006. Compliant leg behaviour
of leg-muscle activation during perturbation of stance in explains basic dynamics of walking and running. Proc R Soc B
humans. J Neurophysiol 62(3):680–693. Biol Sci 273(1603):2861–2867.
Dietz V, Gollhofer A, Kleiber M, Trippel M. 1992. Regulation of Gollhofer A, Horstmann GA, Berger W, Dietz V. 1989.
bipedal stance: Dependency on ‘‘load’’ receptors. Exp Brain Compensation of translational and rotational perturbations in
Res 89(1):229–231. human posture: Stabilization of the centre of gravity. Neurosci
Dietz V. 1996. Interaction between central programs and afferent Lett 105(1–2):73–78.
input in the control of posture and locomotion. J Biomech Grasso R, Bianchi L, Lacquaniti F. 1998. Motor patterns for
29(7):841–844. human gait: Backward versus forward locomotion. J
Dietz V, Colombo G. 1996. Effects of body immersion on postural Neurophysiol 80(4):1868–1885.
adjustments to voluntary arm movements in humans: Role of Grey MJ, Ladouceur M, Andersen JB, Nielsen JB, Sinkjær T.
load receptor input. J Physiol 497(Pt 3):849–856. 2001. Group II muscle afferents probably contribute to the
Dietz V. 1998. Evidence for a load receptor contribution to the medium latency soleus stretch reflex during walking in humans.
control of posture and locomotion. Neurosci Biobehav Rev J Physiol 534(3):925–933.
22(4):495–499. Grey MJ, Van Doornik J, Sinkjær T. 2002. Plantar flexor stretch
Dietz V, Muller R, Colombo G. 2002. Locomotor activity in reflex responses to whole body loading/unloading during
spinal man: Significance of afferent input from joint and load human walking. Eur J Neurosci 16(10):2001–2007.
receptors. Brain 125:2626–2634. Grey MJ, Nielsen JB, Mazzaro N, Sinkjaer T. 2007. Positive force
Dimitrova D, Horak F, Nutt J. 2004. Postural muscle responses to feedback in human walking. J Physiol 581(1):99–105.
multidirectional translations in patients with Parkinson’s Griffin T, Roberts T, Kam R. 2003. Metabolic of generation
disease. J Neurophysiol 91(1):489–501. muscular force in human walking: Insights from load-carrying
Donelan J, Kram R, Kuo A. 2001. Mechanical and metabolic and speed experiments. J Appl Physiol 95(1):172–183.
determinants of the preferred step width in human walking. Grigg P. 1994. Peripheral neural mechanisms in proprioception.
Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 268:1985–1992. J Sports Rehabil 3:2–17.
Donelan J, Kram R, Kuo A. 2002a. Mechanical work for step-to- Hansen PD, Woollacott MH, Debu B. 1988. Postural
step transition is a major determinant of the metabolic cost. responses to changing task conditions. Exp Brain Res 73(3):
J Exp Biol 205:3717–3727. 627–636.
Donelan J, Kram R, Kuo A. 2002b. A simultaneous positive and Henry S, Fung J, Horak F. 1998. EMG responses to maintain
negative external mechanical work in human walking. stance during multidirectional surface translations.
J Biomech 35(1):117–124. J Neurophysiol 80(4):1939–1950.
Donelan J, Shipman D, Kram R, Kuo A. 2004. Mechanical and Hogersvorst T, Brand R. 1998. Mechanoreceptors in joint
metabolic requirements for active lateral stabilization in human function. J Bone Joint Surg 80:1365–1378.
walking. J Biomech 37:827–835. Horak B, Macpherson M. 1996. Postural orientation and
Edin B, Johansson N. 1995. Skin strain patterns provide equilibrium. In: Rowell L Shepherd J, editors. Handbook of
kinaesthetic information to the human central nervous system. physiology. Exercise: Regulation and integration of multiple
J Physiol 487:243–251. systems. Washington, DC: American Physiology Society.
Fetters L, Holt K. 1990. Efficiency of human movement:
pp 255–292.
Biomechanical and metabolic aspects. Pediatr Phys Ther
Horak F, Nashner L. 1986. Central programming of postural
2:55–59.
movements: Adaptation to altered support-surface configura-
Fitzpatrick R, Gorman R, Burke D, Gandevia S. 1992a. Postural
tions. J Neurophysiol 55(6):1369–1381.
proprioceptive reflexes in standing human subjects: Bandwidth
Horak F, Nashner L, Diener H. 1990. Postural strategies
of response and transmission characteristics. J Physiol
associated with somatosensory and vestibular loss. Exp Brain
458(1):69–83.
Res 82(1):167–177.
Fitzpatrick R, Taylor J, McCloskey D. 1992b. Ankle stiffness of
Horak F. 2006. Mechanistic and physiological aspects. Postural
standing humans in response to imperceptible perturbation:
orientation and equilibrium: What do we need to know about
Reflex and task-dependent components. J Physiol 454:533–547.
neural control of balance to prevent falls? Age Ageing
Fitzpatrick R, Douglas K, McCloskey D. 1994. Stable human
35(S2):ii7–ii11.
standing with lower-limb muscle afferents providing the only
Ito M. 2006. Cerebellar circuitry as a neuronal machine. Prog
sensory input. J Physiol 2536:395–403.
Neurobiol 78(3–5):272–303.
Fitzpatrick R, Burke D, Gandevia SC. 1996. Loop gain of reflexes
Ivanenko Y, Poppele R, Macellari V, Lacquaniti F. 2004. Five
controlling human standing measured with the use of postural
basic muscle activation patterns account for muscle activity
and vestibular disturbances. J Neurophysiol 76(6):3994–4008.
during human locomotion. J Physiol 556(1):267–282.
Frigo C, Nielsen J, Crenna P. 1996. Modelling the triceps surae
Ivanenko YP, Poppele RE, Lacquaniti F. 2006. Spinal cord maps
muscle–tendon complex for the estimation of length changes
of spatiotemporal alpha-motoneuron activation in humans
during walking. J Electromyogr Kinesiol 6(3):191–203.
Garby L, Astrup A. 1987. The relationship between the walking at different speeds. J Neurophysiol 95(2):602–618.
Jacobs JV, Lou JS, Kraakevik JA, Horak FB. 2009. The
respiratory quotient and the energy equivalent of oxygen
during simultaneous glucose and lipid oxidation and lipogen- supplementary motor area contributes to the timing of the
esis. Acta Physiol Scand 129:443–444. anticipatory postural adjustment during step initiation in
Garcia-Rill E. 1986. The basal ganglia and the locomotor regions. participants with and without Parkinson’s disease.
Brain Res Rev 11(1):47–63. Neuroscience 164(2):877–885.
Gard S, Miff S, Kuo A. 2004. A comparison of kinematic and Jami L. 1992. Golgi tendon organs in mammalian skeletal muscle:
kinetic methods for computing the vertical motion of the Functional properties and central actions. Physiol Rev
body central mass during walking. Hum Movement Sci 72:623–666.
22:597–610. Jankowska E. 1992. Interneuronal relay in spinal pathways from
Gatev P, Thomas S, Kepple T, Hallet M. 1999. Feedforward proprioceptors. Prog Neurobiol 38(4):335–378.
ankle strategy of balance during quiet stance in adults. J Physiol Johanson R, Magnusson M. 1991. Human postural dynamics.
514(3):915–928. Crit Rev Biomed Eng 18(6):413–437.
Postural control and efficiency of movement 141

Johansson H, Sjolander P, Sojka P. 1991. A sensory role for the paradoxical changes in muscle length be produced? J Physiol
crusiate ligaments. Clin Orthop 80:1365–1378. 564(1):283–293.
Jones S, Henry S, Raasch C, Hitt J, Bunn J. 2008. Responses to Loram I, Maganaris C, Lakie M. 2005b. Human postural sway
multi-directional surface translations involve redistribution of results from frequent, ballistic bias impulses by soleus and
proximal versus distal strategies to maintain upright posture. gastrocnemius. J Physiol 564(1):295–311.
Exp Brain Res 187(3):407–417. Lundy-Ekman L. 2002. Neuroscience: Fundamentals for rehabi-
Jordan M. 1986. Initiation of locomotion in mammals. Ann N Y litation, 2nd ed. Philadelphia, PA: Saunders.
Acad Sci 860:83–93. Macefield G, Gandevia S, Burke D. 1990. Perceptual responses to
Kandel E, Schwartz J, Jessell T. 2000. Principals of neural science, microstimulation of single afferents innervating joints, muscles
4th ed. New York: McGraw-Hill. and skin of the human hand. J Physiol 429:113–129.
Kapreli E, Athanasopoulos S, Papathanasiou M, Van Hecke P, Mackay-Lyons M. 2002. Central pattern generation of locomo-
Strimpakos N, Gouliamos A, Peeters R, Sunaert S. 2006. tion: A review of the evidence. Phys Ther 82:69–83.
Lateralization of brain activity during lower limb joints move- Maki B, Ostrovski G. 1993. Scaling of postural responses to
ment. An fMRI study. NeuroImage 32(4):1709–1721. transient and continuous perturbations. Gait Posture
Kennedy P, Inglis J. 2002. Distribution and behaviour of glabrous 1(2):93–104.
cutaneous receptors in the human foot sole. J Physiol Marchand-Pauvert V, Nicolas G, Marque P, Iglesias C, Pierrot-
538(3):995–1002. Deseilligny E. 2005. Increase in group II excitation from ankle
Kepple T, Siegel K, Stanhope S. 1997. Relative contributions of muscles to thigh motoneurones during human standing. J
the lower extremity joint moments to forward progression and Physiol 566(1):257–271.
support during stance. Gait Posture 6:1–8. Masani K, Popovic MR, Nakazawa K, Kouzaki M, Nozaki D.
Kiemel T, Oie KS, Jeka JJ. 2002. Multisensory fusion and the 2003. Importance of body sway velocity information in
stochastic structure of postural sway. Biol Cybern controlling ankle extensor activities during quiet stance. J
87(4):262–277. Neurophysiol 90(6):3774–3782.
Kiernan J. 2005. The human nervous system. An anatomical Massion J. 1992. Movement, posture and equilibrium: Interaction
viewpoint, 8th ed. London: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. and coordination. Prog Neurobiol 38:35–56.
Kuo A. 1999. Stabilization of lateral motion in passive dynamic Massion J. 1994. Postural control system. Curr Opin
walking. Int J Robotic Res 18(9):917–930. Neurophysiol 4:877–887.
Kuo A. 2002. Energetics of actively powered locomotion using the Massion J. 1998. Postural control systems in developmental
simplest walking model. J Biomech Eng 124(1):113–120. perspective. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 4:465–472.
Kuo A, Donelan M, Ruina A. 2005. Energetic consequences of Matsuyama K, Drew T. 2000. Vestibulospinal and reticulospinal
walking like an inverted pendulum: Step to step transitions. neuronal activity during locomotion in the intact cat. II.
Exerc Sport Sci Rev 33(2):88–97. Walking on an inclined plane. J Appl Physiol 84:2257–2276.
Kuo A, Donelan M, Ruina A. 2007. The six determinants of gait Matthews PBC. 1991. The human stretch reflex and the motor
in the inverted pendulum analogy: A dynamic walking cortex. Trends Neurosci 14(3):87–91.
perspective. Hum Movement Sci 26(4):617–656. Maurer C, Mergner T, Xie J, Faist M, Pollak P, Lücking CH.
Lacquaniti F, Grasso R, Zago M. 1999. Motor patterns in 2003. Effect of chronic bilateral subthalamic nucleus (STN)
walking. News Physiol Sci 14(4):168–174. stimulation on postural control in Parkinson’s disease. Brain
Lacquaniti F, Ivanenko Y, Zago M. 2002. Kinematic control of 126(5):1146–1163.
walking. Arch Ital Biol 140(4):263–272. Maurer C, Peterka RJ. 2005. A new interpretation of spontaneous
Lakie M, Caplan N, Loram I. 2003. Human balancing of an sway measures based on a simple model of human postural
inverted pendulum with a compliant linkage: Neural control by
control. J Neurophysiol 93(1):189–200.
anticipatory intermittent bias. J Physiol 551(1):357–370.
Mazzaro M, Sznaier M, Camps O. 2005. A model (in)validation
Lansner A, Ekeberg Ö. 1994. Neuronal network models of motor
approach to gait classification. Transactions on Pattern Analysis
generation and control. Curr Opin Neurobiol 4(6):903–908.
and Machine Intelligence 27(11):1820–1825.
Latash M. 1993. Control of human movement Champaign, IL:
McCollum G, Leen T. 1989. Form and exploration of mechanical
Human Kinetics.
stability limits in erect stance. J Mot Behav 21(3):225–244.
Latash M. 1998. Neurophysiological basis of movement
McCollum G, Holroyd C, Castelfranco M. 1995. Forms of early
Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
walking. J Theor Biol 176:373–390.
Lavoie B, Devanne H, Capaday C. 1997. Differential control of
McGeer T. 1990. Passive dynamic walking. Int J Robotic Res
reciprocal inhibition during walking versus postural and
9:62–82.
voluntary motor tasks in humans. J Neurophysiol
Meadows L, Williams J. 2009. An understanding of functional
78(1):429–438.
movement as a basis for clinical reasoning. In: Raine S,
Levine J. 2005. Measurement of energy expenditure. Public
Meadows L, Lynch-Ellerington M, editors. Bobath concept:
Health Nutr 8(7A):1123–1132.
Theory and clinical practice in neurological rehabilitation.
Li X, Aruin A. 2007. The effect of short-term changes in the body
Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.
mass on anticipatory postural adjustments. Exp Brain Res
Mergner T, Rosemeier T. 1998. Interaction of vestibular,
181:333–346.
somatosensory and visual signals for postural control under
Liu M, Anderson F, Pandy M, Delp S. 2006. Muscles that
terrestrial and microgravity conditions: A conceptual model.
support the body also modulate forward progression during
walking. J Biomech 39(14):2623–2630. Brain Res Rev 28(1–2):118–135.
Loram I, Lakie M. 2002a. Direct measurement of human ankle Mileykovskiy BY, Kiyashchenko LI, Kodama T, Lai Y-Y,
stiffness during quiet standing: The intrinsic mechanical Siegel JM. 2000. Activation of pontine and medullary motor
stiffness is insufficient for stability. J Physiol 545(3):1041–1053. inhibitory regions reduces discharge in neurons located in the
Loram I, Lakie M. 2002b. Human balancing of an inverted locus coeruleus and the anatomical equivalent of the midbrain
pendulum: Position control by small, ballistic-like, throw and locomotor region. J Neurosci 20(22):8551–8558.
catch movements. J Physiol 540(3):1111–1124. Miyai I, Yagura H, Oda I, Konishi I, Eda H, Suzuki T, Kubota K.
Loram I, Maganaris C, Lakie M. 2005a. Active, non-spring-like 2002. Premotor cortex is involved in restoration of gait in
muscle movements in human postural sway: How might stroke. Ann Neurol 52(2):188–194.
142 A. S. P. Sousa et al.

Mochizuki L, Duarte M, Amadio A, Zatsiorsky V, Latash M. Raine S, Meadows L, Lynch-Ellerington M. 2009. Bobath
2006. Changes in postural sway and its fractions in conditions concept. Theory and clinical practice in neurological rehabilita-
of postural instability. J Appl Biomech 22:51–60. tion. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.
Morasso PG, Baratto L, Capra R, Spada G. 1999. Internal Riley MA, Wong S, Mitra S, Turvey MT. 1997. Common effects
models in the control of posture. Neural Networks 12(7– of touch and vision on postural parameters. Exp Brain Res
8):1173–1180. 117(1):165–170.
Nardone A, Giordano A, Corrá T, Schieppati M. 1990. Responses Roll R, Kavounoudias A, Roll J-P. 2002. Cutaneous afferents from
of leg muscles in humans displaced while standing. Effects of human plantar sole contribute to body posture awareness.
types of perturbation and of postural set. Brain 113(1):65–84. NeuroReport 13(15):1957–1961.
Nardone A, Grasso M, Giordano A, Schieppati M. 1996. Rossignol S, Dubuc R, Gossard P. 2006. Dynamic sensorimotor
Different effect of height on latency of leg and foot short- and interactions in locomotion. Physiol Rev 86:89–154.
medium-latency EMG responses to perturbation of stance in Rothwell J. 1994. Control of human voluntary movement, 2nd ed.
humans. Neurosci Lett 206(2–3):89–92. London: Chapman & Hall.
Nashner L, Shupert C, Horak F, Black F. 1989. Chapter 33 Saibene F, Minetti A. 2003. Biomechanical and physiological
Organization of posture controls: an analysis of sensory and aspects of legged locomotion in humans. Eur J Appl Physiol
mechanical constraints. In Allum JHS & Hulliger M, editors. 88(4):297–316.
Progress in Brain Research. Vol. 80. Elsevier. pp 411–418. Santos M, Aruin A. 2008. Role of lateral muscles and body
Nelson RJ. 1996. Interactions between motor commands and orientation in feedforward postural control. Exp Brain Res
somatic perception in sensorimotor cortex. Curr Opin 184(4):547–559.
Neurobiol 6(6):801–810. Santos M, Kanekar N, Aruin A. 2009. The role of anticipatory
Neptune R, Kautz A, Zajac E. 2001. Contributions of the postural adjustments in compensatory control of posture: 1.
individual ankle flexors to support, forward progression and Electromyographic analysis. J Electromyogr Kinesiol
swing initiation during normal walking. J Biomech 20(3):388–397.
34(11):1387–1398. Saunders M, Inman T, Heberhart D. 1953. The major determi-
Neptune R, Kautz S, Zajac F. 2004. Muscle force redistributes nants in normal and pathological gait. J Bone Joint Surg
segmental power for body progression during walking. Gait 53(3):543–558.
Posture 19(2):194–205. Schepens B, Drew T. 2004. Independent and convergent signals
Nielsen J, Kagamihara Y. 1992. The regulation of disynaptic from the pontomedullary reticular formation contribute to the
reciprocal Ia inhibition during co-contraction of antagonistic control of posture and movement during reaching in the cat.
muscles in man. J Physiol 456:373–391. J Neurophysiol 92:2217–2238.
Nielsen JB, Sinkjaer T. 2002. Afferent feedback in the control of Schieppati M, Nardone A, Siliotto R, Grasso M. 1995. Early and
human gait. J Electromyogr Kinesiol 12(3):213–217. late stretch responses of human foot muscles induced by
Norkin C, Levangie K. 1992. Joint structure and function. A perturbation of stance. Exp Brain Res 105(3):411–422.
comprehensive analysis, 2nd ed. Philadelphia, PA: Davis. Schneider C, Lavoie BA, Capaday C. 2000. On the origin of the
Orendurff MS, Segal AD, Aiona MD, Dorociak RD. 2005. soleus H-reflex modulation pattern during human walking and
Triceps surae force, length and velocity during walking. Gait its task-dependent differences. J Neurophysiol
83(5):2881–2890.
Posture 21(2):157–163.
Segers V. 2006. A biomechanical analysis of the realization of
Orlovsky N. 1991. Cerebellum and locomotion.
actual human transition. Ghent: Ghent University.
In: Shimamura M, Griller S, Edgerton V, editors.
Shiratori T, Aruin A. 2007. Modulation of anticipatory postural
Neurobiologic basis of human locomotion. Tokyo: Scientific
adjustments associated with unloading perturbation: Effect of
Society Press. pp 187–199.
characteristics of a motor action. Exp Brain Res
Pandy MG. 2003. Simple and complex models for studying
178(2):206–215.
muscle function in walking. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci
Shumway-Cook A, Woolacott M. 2000. Attentional demands and
358(1437):1501–1509.
postural control: The effect of sensory context. J Gerontol
Park S, Horak F, Kuo A. 2004. Postural feedback responses scale
55A:M10–M16.
with biomechanical constraints in human standing. Exp Brain
Shumway-Cook A, Woolacott M. 2007. Motor control:
Res 154:417–427.
Translating research into clinical practice, 3rd ed. Lippincott:
Pearson K. 1993. Common principles of motor control in
Williams & Wilkins.
vertebrates and invertebrates. Ann Rev Neurosci 16:265–297.
Siegel A, Sapru H. 2011. Essential neuroscience, 2nd ed.
Peterka R. 2002. Sensorimotor integration in human postural
Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
control. J Neurophysiol 88:1097–1118.
Sinkjaer T, Andersen JB, Larsen B. 1996. Soleus stretch reflex
Peterka R, Loughlin P. 2004. Dynamic regulation of sensorimotor
modulation during gait in humans. J Neurophysiol
integration in human postural control. J Neurophysiol
76(2):1112–1120.
91:410–423.
Sinkjær T, Andersen JB, Ladouceur M, Christensen LOD,
Peterka RJ, Benolken MS. 1995. Role of somatosensory and
Nielsen JB. 2000. Major role for sensory feedback in soleus
vestibular cues in attenuating visually induced human postural
EMG activity in the stance phase of walking in man. J Physiol
sway. Exp Brain Res 105(1):101–110. 523(3):817–827.
Peterka RJ. 2000. Postural control model interpretation of Sparrow W, Newell. 1998. Metabolic energy expenditure and the
stabilogram diffusion analysis. Biol Cybernet 82(4):335–343. regulation of movement economy. Psychon Bull Rev
Pierrynowski MR, Winter DA, Norman RW. 1980. Transfers of 5(2):173–196.
mechanical energy within the total body and mechanical Stainsby W, Gladden L, Barclay J, Wilson B. 1980. Exercise
efficiency during treadmill walking. Ergonomics efficiency: Validity of base-line subtractions. J Appl Physiol
23(2):147–156. 48:518–522.
Proske U, Wise A, Gregory J. 2000. The role of muscle receptors Stapley P, Pozzo T, Cheron G, Grishin A. 1999. Does the
in the detection of movements. Prog Neurobiol 60(1):85–96. coordination between posture and movement during human
Proske U. 2006. Kinesthesia: The role of muscle receptors. whole-body reaching ensure center of mass stabilization. Exp
Muscle Nerve 34(5):545–558. Brain Res 129:134–146.
Postural control and efficiency of movement 143

Stephens MJ, Yang JF. 1996. Short latency, non-reciprocal group Winter D. 1995. Human balance and posture control during
I inhibition is reduced during the stance phase of walking in standing and walking. Gait Posture 3:193–214.
humans. Brain Res 743(1–2):24–31. Winter D. 2005. Biomechanics and motor control of human
Stephens MJ, Yang JF. 1999. Loading during the stance phase of movement. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.
walking in humans increases the extensor EMG amplitude but Winter DA, MacKinnon CD, Ruder GK, Wieman C. 1993.
does not change the duration of the step cycle. Exp Brain Res Chapter 32 An integrated EMG/biomechanical model of upper
124(3):363–370. body balance and posture during human gait. In: Allum JHJ,
Takakusaki K, Saitoh K, Harada H, Kashiwayanagi M. 2004. Role Allum-Mecklenburg DJ, Harris FP, Probst R, editors. Progress
of basal ganglia–brainstem pathways in the control of motor in brain research., Vol. 97. Oxford: Elsevier. pp 359–367.
behaviors. Neurosci Res 50(2):137–151. Winter DA, Eng P. 1995. Kinetics: Our window into the goals and
van der Fits I, Klip A, van Eykern L, Hadders-Algra M. 1998. strategies of the central nervous system. Behav Brain Res
Postural adjustments accompanying fast pointing movements in 67(2):111–120.
standing, sitting and lying adults. Exp Brain Res Winter DA, Patla A, Prince F, Ishac M, Gielo-Perczak K. 1998.
120(2):202–216. Stiffness control of balance in quiet standing. Journal of
van der Kooij H, Jacobs R, Koopman B, van der Helm F. 2001. Neurophysiology 80:1211–1221.
An adaptive model of sensory integration in a dynamic Wright W, Ivanenko Y, Gurfinkel V. 2012. Foot anatomy
environment applied to human stance control. Biol Cybern specialization for postural sensation and control. J
84(2):103–115. Neurophysiol 107(5):1513–1521.
Van, Emmerik R, Van, Wegen E. 2000. On variability and stability Yang J, Winter D, Wells R. 1990. Postural dynamics in standing
in human movement. J Appl Biomech 16:394–406. human. Biol Cybern 62(4):309–320.
Vandewalle H. 2004. Consommation d’oxygène et consommation Yang JF, Stein RB, James KB. 1991. Contribution of peripheral
maximale d’oxygène: Intérêts et limites de leur mesure. Ann afferents to the activation of the soleus muscle during walking in
Réadapt Méd Phys 47(6):243–257. humans. Exp Brain Res 87(3):679–687.
Yang JF, Whelan PJ. 1993. Neural mechanisms that contribute to
Waters L, Mulroy S. 1999. The energy expenditure of normal and
cyclical modulation of the soleus H-reflex in walking in humans.
pathological gait: Relation to mechanical energy cost. J
Exp Brain Res 95(3):547–556.
Neurophysiol 9(3):207–231.
Zajac F, Neptune R, Kautz S. 2003. Biomechanics and muscle
Wichmann T, DeLong MR. 1996. Functional and pathophysio-
coordination of muscle walking Part II: Lessons from dynamic
logical models of the basal ganglia. Curr Opin Neurobiol
simulations and clinical implications. Gait Posture 17(1):1–17.
6(6):751–758.
Zatsiorsky V, Duarte M. 1999. Instant equilibrium point and its
Willems A, Cavagna A, Heglund C. 1995. External, internal and
migration in standing tasks: Rambling and trembling compo-
total work in human locomotion. J Exp Biol 198:379–383.
nents of the stabilogram. Motor Control 3(1):28–38.
Winter D, Patia A, Frank J. 1990. Assessment of balance control
Zehr EP, Duysens J. 2004. Regulation of arm and leg movement
in humans. Med Prog Technol 16:31–51.
during human locomotion. The Neuroscientist 10(4):347–361.
Copyright of Somatosensory & Motor Research is the property of Taylor & Francis Ltd and its content may not
be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written
permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

You might also like