Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 152

The Closeness between Grandparents and Grandchildren and Its Impact on Grandparents’

Well-being

Dissertation

Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy

in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University

By

Seojin Won

Graduate Program in Social Work

The Ohio State University

2009

Dissertation Committee:

Virginia Richardson, Advisor

Mo-Yee Lee

Holly Dabelko-Schoeny
Copyright by

Seojin Won

2009
Abstract

Grandparents are considered a resource in the family because they provide

alternative child care support and serve as role models for grandchildren. Although social

workers have increasingly examined the needs of custodial grandparents who face many

stressors, the profession lacks knowledge about functions of noncustodial grandparents,

who are important supports for families; this role of grandparents is especially crucial as

the number of multigenerational households grow. In addition, as maternal employment

rates rise, mothers increasingly seek alternative caregivers for their children. They often

choose grandparents as alternative caregivers, especially when children are young. While

both custodial and noncustodial grandparents struggle with the burden of childcare, the

relationships among grandparents and grandchildren usually vary depending upon

individual and family characteristics.

Thus, the purpose of this study was to identify factors associated with the

closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild. Also, the researcher sought factors

affecting the association between the grandparent-grandchild relationship and the

grandparent’s well-being. This study conducted secondary data analysis using the third

wave of the National Survey of Families and Households (NSFH) dataset. The number of

grandparents in the dataset was 2,541. Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) was applied

to address the relationship among variables.

ii
Findings indicated that older grandparents tended to interact less with their

grandchildren than younger grandparents. In addition, as grandparents spent more nights

with their grandchild without the parent of the child, the well-being of the grandparents

tended to decrease. On the other hand, frequency of communication, frequency of

meeting, and the grandparent’s self-rating of closeness with his or her grandchild were

positively related to the well-being of grandparents. Specifically, grandparents who

interacted with their grandchild more frequently had a higher level of well-being than

those who had less contact with their grandchild. Thus, it is assumed that the

grandparents’ well-being increased up to the point where grandparenting became a

burden for them. In this study, spending nights with the grandchild without the parent of

the child seems to be the point where the grandparents start feeling burdened. This

finding was consistent regardless of grandparents’ gender, ethnicity, and the presence or

absence of legal custody. This study suggested that social support should target both

custodial and noncustodial grandparents because noncustodial grandparents who put

great effort into child care feel the same kind of stress as custodial grandparents. In

addition, cultural diversity should be considered in work with grandparent caregivers

because various factors are associated with the well-being of the grandparents.

iii
Dedication

Dedicated to my parents

iv
Acknowledgements

Many faculty members, friends, and family have supported me in completing this

dissertation. Without their support and encouragement, I never would have finished this

dissertation process.

First, I would like to acknowledge my committee members. Their knowledge and

experience helped me to think more critically and to become a better scholar. I would

especially like to express sincere gratitude to my advisor, Dr. Virginia Richardson, for

her endless encouragement and consistent support of this dissertation. She has always

been my biggest supporter throughout my academic career at The Ohio State University.

She spent many hours reading drafts of this dissertation and proposal for feedback. I also

appreciate all the support, encouragement, and advice of Dr. Mo-Yee Lee. She helped me

establish a more thorough theoretical framework for this dissertation. I also would like to

thank Dr. Holly Dabelko-Schoeny for her continued support and feedback throughout the

process.

I wish to thank Jennifer Nakayama for her help and support. She helped me to

successfully process all the administrative work since I entered into the doctoral program.

And, she was an invaluable resource from whom I could get answers for issues relating to

my studies in the doctoral program.

v
I am grateful to my colleagues, Wonik Lee, Se Kyung Moon, Jinhyun Kim, and

Jina Han, who provided advice and feedback on statistical analyses whenever I asked.

Also, they continually shared information on research workshops, statistical techniques,

and references so I could be more knowledgeable of various research methods. Their

valuable input made this dissertation more comprehensive. In addition, their emotional

support was immensely helpful for me to complete the degree.

I want to thank my friend, Ohyon Paek, for everything she has done for me so far.

She has been my best friend, my sister, and my supporter. She has stimulated me to keep

working hard and to be productive.

Lastly, I am grateful to my mother and father for their endless love and support. I

cannot express how thankful I am. I would never be able to continue my studies at the

master’s and doctoral levels without their trust and dedication. They have guided me to

be a good researcher.

vi
Vita

September 1978 ….. Born, Seoul, Republic of Korea

2002 …………..….. B.A. Social Welfare, Seoul National University, Republic of Korea

2004 …………….... M.S. Social Work, Columbia University

2005 ~ 2008 ……… Graduate Administrative, Research, & Teaching Associate,

The Ohio State University

Publications

Won, S. (2008). Factors influencing the relationship between grandparents and

grandchildren: A literature review. Perspectives on Social Work, 7 (1), 21-24.

Fields of Study

Major Field: Social Work

Minor Field: Research Methods in Human Resource Development

Graduate Interdisciplinary Specialization in Aging

vii
Table of Contents

Abstract ............................................................................................................................... ii

Dedication .......................................................................................................................... iv

Acknowledgements..............................................................................................................v

Vita.................................................................................................................................... vii

List of Tables .......................................................................................................................x

List of Figures ................................................................................................................... xii

Chapter 1: Introduction ........................................................................................................1

Chapter 2: Literature Review...............................................................................................5

Definition of Grandparents...........................................................................................5

Factors Affecting the Grandparent-grandchild Relationship.......................................6

Consequences of Grandparenting...............................................................................15

Chapter 3: Theoretical Frameworks...................................................................................20

Life Course Perspective .............................................................................................20

Bowen Family Systems Theory.................................................................................27

Life Course Perspective and Bowen Family Systems Theory ..................................37

Rationale.....................................................................................................................42

Research Questions....................................................................................................44

viii
Chapter 4: Research Methods ............................................................................................46

Overview of the Research Design .............................................................................46

Dataset .......................................................................................................................46

Sample .......................................................................................................................49

Constructs and Instruments .......................................................................................50

Statistical Analysis ....................................................................................................58

Chapter 5: Research Findings ............................................................................................62

Initial Analyses...........................................................................................................62

Hypotheses Testing with Structural Equation Modeling ...........................................71

Chapter 6: Discussion .......................................................................................................97

Limitations of the Study.............................................................................................97

Summary of the Study .............................................................................................100

Implications for Social Work ...................................................................................108

Implications for Future Study ..................................................................................111

References .......................................................................................................................114

Appendix A: Descriptions of Constructs ........................................................................125

Appendix B: Covariance Matrixes Used for Hypotheses Testing ...................................133

ix
List of Tables

Table 1.1. Child Care Arrangement among Working Mothers with Children under 5

Years Old in 2002 ........................................................................................................2

Table 1.2. Labor Force Participation among Mothers with Children ..................................2

Table 5.1. Closeness between the Grandparent and the Grandchild..................................63

Table 5.2. Grandparent’s Well-being.................................................................................64

Table 5.3. Cultural Attitude ..............................................................................................65

Table 5.4. Grandparent-parent Support (During last month) ............................................66

Table 5.5. Grandparent’s Functional Capacity: Physical and/or Mental Limits ...............67

Table 5.6. Number of Grandchildren.................................................................................68

Table 5.7. Age of Grandparents and Grandchildren .........................................................69

Table 5.8. Demographics of Grandparents .......................................................................70

Table 5.9. Structural Model Result ...................................................................................73

Table 5.10. Measurement Model Result (H3) ..................................................................79

Table 5.11. Measurement Model Result (H4) ..................................................................83

Table 5.12. Measurement Model Result (H5) ..................................................................87

Table 5.13. Measurement Model Result (H6) ..................................................................93

Table B.1. Covariance Matrix Used for Hypothesis Test 1 ............................................134

Table B.2. Covariance Matrix Used for Hypothesis Test 2 ............................................134


x
Table B.3. Covariance Matrix Used for Hypothesis Test 3 ............................................135

Table B.4.1. Covariance Matrix Used for Hypothesis Test 4 (Grandmothers)...............136

Table B.4.2. Covariance Matrix Used for Hypothesis Test 4 (Grandfathers).................136

Table B.5.1. Covariance Matrix Used for Hypothesis Test 5 (Caucasian American

Grandparents) ...........................................................................................................137

Table B.5.2. Covariance Matrix Used for Hypothesis Test 5 (Non-Caucasian American

Grandparents) ...........................................................................................................137

Table B.6.1. Covariance Matrix Used for Hypothesis Test 6 (Custodial Grandparents).138

Table B.6.2. Covariance Matrix Used for Hypothesis Test 6 (Noncustodial Grandparents)

..................................................................................................................................138

xi
List of Figures

Figure 5.1. Impact of Number of Grandchildren, Grandparent’s Age, Age of the

Youngest Grandchild, and Age of the Oldest Grandchild on the Nights Spent with

Grandchild and the Closeness between the Grandparent and the Grandchild............75

Figure 5.2. Impact of Nights Spent with Grandchild and the Closeness between the

Grandparent and the Grandchild on the Grandparent’s Well-being ..........................77

Figure 5.3. Effects of Cultural Attitude, Grandparent-parent Support, and Grandparent’s

Functional Capacity on the Relationship of the Nights Spent with Grandchild, the

Closeness between the Grandparent and the Grandchild, and the Grandparent’s Well-

being ...........................................................................................................................81

Figure 5.4. Effects of Cultural Attitude, Grandparent-parent Support, and Grandparent’s

Functional Capacity on the Relationship of the Nights Spent with Grandchild, the

Closeness between the Grandparent and the Grandchild, and the Grandparent’s Well-

being: Grandmothers ..................................................................................................84

Figure 5.5. Effects of Cultural Attitude, Grandparent-parent Support, and Grandparent’s

Functional Capacity on the Relationship of the Nights Spent with Grandchild, the

Closeness between the Grandparent and the Grandchild, and the Grandparent’s Well-

being: Grandfathers ....................................................................................................85

xii
Figure 5.6. Effects of Cultural Attitude, Grandparent-parent Support, and Grandparent’s

Functional Capacity on the Relationship of the Nights Spent with Grandchild, the

Closeness between the Grandparent and the Grandchild, and the Grandparent’s Well-

being: Caucasian American Grandparents .................................................................90

Figure 5.7. Effects of Cultural Attitude, Grandparent-parent Support, and Grandparent’s

Functional Capacity on the Relationship of the Nights Spent with Grandchild, the

Closeness between the Grandparent and the Grandchild, and the Grandparent’s Well-

being: Non-Caucasian American Grandparents ........................................................91

Figure 5.8. Effects of Cultural Attitude, Grandparent-parent Support, and Grandparent’s

Functional Capacity on the Relationship of the Nights Spent with Grandchild, the

Closeness between the Grandparent and the Grandchild, and the Grandparent’s Well-

being: Custodial Grandparents ...................................................................................95

Figure 5.9. Effects of Cultural Attitude, Grandparent-parent Support, and Grandparent’s

Functional Capacity on the Relationship of the Nights Spent with Grandchild, the

Closeness between the Grandparent and the Grandchild, and the Grandparent’s Well-

being: Noncustodial Grandparents .............................................................................96

Figure 6.1. Key Concepts of Life Course Perspective and Variables in This Study ......102

Figure 6.2. Key Concepts of Bowen Family Systems Theory and Variables in This Study.

..................................................................................................................................104

xiii
Chapter 1: Introduction

The growing senior population and increasing maternal employment rate in the

U.S. has resulted in more senior citizens becoming caregivers for their grandchildren.

The proportion of seniors in the U.S. population had gradually increased from 6.8% in

1940 to 11.3% in 1980, and to 12.4% in 2000 (U.S. Census Bureau, 2006). The recent

annual population estimates indicated that the proportion of senior population remained

almost same compared to the proportion in 2000 as it was 12.53% in 2007 (U.S. Census

Bureau, 2007). As the senior population grows, many older adults are becoming

grandparent caregivers (Hooyman & Kiyak, 2005). According to an American

Association of Retired People (AARP) study (2006a), approximately 15% of participants

reported that they were caregivers of their grandchildren while the parents were at work.

The AARP also identified that 24% of those who take care of their grandchildren while

the parents are at work also provide childcare when the parents are not at work. Similarly,

Table 1.1. shows that 28.3% of mothers chose children’s grandparents as caregivers in

2002, followed by the father (24.7%), multiple arrangements (22.4%), or day care centers

(20.6%). When children are young, more mothers tend to choose grandparents as

childcare providers (U.S. Census Bureau, 2005).

1
Grand- Day care Sibling / Family Multiple
Father
parent center other relative daycare arrangement

28.3 24.7 20.6 11.0 10.4 22.4


Table 1.1. Child Care Arrangement among Working Mothers with Children under 5
Years Old in 2002

Besides the fact of senior population growth, the increase in labor force

participation among mothers may affect the tendency of older adults to become childcare

providers. According to the U.S. Census Bureau (2009), 61.8% of mothers who were 16

years old and over were in the workforce in 2007. More specifically, Table 1.2. shows

changes of labor force participation among mothers with children.

Age of Children 1975 1990 2000 2005 2007

Under 3 years 32.7 55.5 59.0 57.3 58.9


3 to 5 years 42.2 64.1 68.4 64.8 65.6

6 to 13 years 51.8 73.0 75.8 73.2 74.5


Table 1.2. Labor Force Participation among Mothers with Children

As described in Table 1.2., 32.7% of mothers with children under three years old

were in the workforce in 1975, and the percentage peaked to 59% in 2000. After 2000,

the labor force participation rate among mothers with children under 3 years old had

2
remained almost the same. Labor force participation among mothers with older children

was much higher. This tendency may affect the living arrangements of older adults as

well. According to the U.S. Census Bureau (2003), 3.6% of people who were 30 years

old or older lived with their grandchildren. Among those grandparents living with their

grandchildren, 2.4 million (42%) grandparents were responsible for meeting the basic

needs of their grandchildren. Moreover, 39% of them had cared for their grandchildren

for more than 5 years. The results also indicated that 62.7% of grandparents who were

responsible for their grandchildren were female. In regards to the age of grandparents,

35.1% of custodial grandparents were in their 50’s, 29.2% were in their 40’s and 21.0%

were in their 60’s. Based on the census results, the majority of grandparents serving as

the primary caregivers for their grandchildren were grandmothers between 40 and 69

years old. The American Association of Retired People (AARP) (2006b) also stated,

based on the 2006 census results, that more than 4.5 million children might currently

reside in households headed by grandparents. This means that one out of twelve children

is living in a grandparent-headed household.

Presser (1989) stated that grandmothers and mothers in the study tended to

provide childcare in turns by rearranging their work hours. One-third of employed

grandmothers worked completely different hours from the child’s mother in order to care

for the child. In addition, one-fifth of grandmothers had only a little overlap of working

hours with the mother’s working hours. Wheelock and Jones (2002) stated that full-time

working parents in the United Kingdom tended to utilize both formal and informal child

care. For instance, 22% of parents who used regular informal child care also used regular

3
formal child care. In addition, 18% of parents who used regular formal child care also

used informal child care occasionally.

As grandparents support their adult child by serving as a third party in the family

system, their well-being may be influenced by their relationship with the members of

their adult child’s family. Although grandparent caregivers often report physical, mental,

and financial difficulties, the negative consequences of grandparenting may be reduced

through the grandparent-grandchild relationship (Hayslip Jr. & Kaminski, 2005). In this

study, the researcher focused on the closeness between grandparents and grandchildren

and its effect on the grandparents’ well-being. In addition, differences between male

grandparents and female grandparents in describing the relationship between the

grandparent’s well-being and the grandparent-grandchild relationship were identified.

Likewise, the differences among ethnic groups and the differences between custodial

grandparents’ and noncustodial grandparents’ relationships with their grandchildren were

addressed.

4
Chapter 2: Literature Review

Definition of Grandparents

There are different types of grandparents based on the degree of responsibility

they assume in caring for their grandchild. First of all, ‘custodial grandparents’ are those

who have legal custody of their grandchildren. In other words, they are responsible for

daily life and the decision-making process. In addition, they are able to receive child care

benefits on behalf of their grandchildren. ‘Noncustodial grandparents’ are the opposite of

custodial grandparents, in that they may take care of their grandchild, but they do not

have legal custody (Pruchno & McKenney, 2000). Hence, non-custodial grandparents

may be either ‘living-with-grandparents’ or ‘daycare grandparents’. Kleiner, Hertzog, and

Targ (1998) stated that ‘living-with-grandparents’ are those who provide daily care for

their grandchildren but do not have legal custody. Approximately 70% of the

grandparents in their study lived with their grandchild and the parent of the child.

‘Daycare grandparents’ are those who help the child’s parents to fulfill their own needs.

This population is close to the societal definition of grandparent since the children return

to their parents at the end of the day. Traditional grandparents are similar to the daycare

grandparents whose grandchildren are in the care and custody of their own parents, yet

the grandparents sometimes help the parents care for the children.

5
Factors Affecting the Grandparent-grandchild Relationship

The family is increasingly considered to be a social system that is affected by, and

that affects, both internal and external factors (Ashford, LeCroy, & Lortie, 2001).

Grandparents are often assumed to be either formal or informal childcare providers for

their grandchildren. Internal factors affecting interactions between a grandparent and a

grandchild include gender, the developmental stage of the grandchild, the relationship

between the grandparent and the parent, and the relationship between the parent and the

child. External factors may include the culture of the ethnic group and social support for

grandparenting.

Gender

Gender influences the relationship between grandparents and grandchildren.

Women are often perceived as the kin-keepers who maintain relationships with other

family members (Lemme, 2002). Hence, grandmothers tend to have more positive

grandparent identities than grandfathers (Reitzes & Mutran, 2004). Reitzes and Mutran

assumed that the reason for women’s more positive identities is the traditional gender

norms for women to be caregivers. Strom and Strom (1997) identified perceptions that

grandparents, parents, and grandchildren in Caucasian American, African American, and

Hispanic American families had of each other. In this study, they discovered that

Caucasian American grandmothers were seen as more satisfied, more successful, and

better adjusted to their role compared to grandfathers.

6
Maccoby (2003) addressed the effect of gender on interactions between the parent

and the child. Maccoby indicated that parents tended to spend more time and have a more

intimate relationship with the same-sex child. Similarly, in a study by Crouter, McHale,

and Bartko (1993), mothers were equally involved with daughters and sons, while fathers

were more involved with their sons. Although it is not clear if the differences in

involvement with daughters and sons can be applied to the grandparent-grandchild

interaction, the power structure within the family may support Maccoby’s findings. In

other words, female family members are likely to create stronger relationships with their

family members; however, female members have less power than male members in the

family (Barber & Haddock, 2003). Interestingly, even male children tend to have more

perceived power than the mother, grandmother, and sister. Additionally, daughters are

perceived to have the least power in the family, while fathers are perceived to have the

most. Therefore, it may be possible that the power difference may result in a loss of

intimacy between fathers and daughters. On the contrary, when mothers or grandmothers

take care of a male child, it may be difficult to supervise him due to the lack of perceived

power of female caregivers.

Rosenblatt (1994) stated that, in the family, the mother-daughter relationship has

more boundary permeability than any other relationship because women are more willing

to express their feelings to others, and are more open to outside influence. Hence, Lemme

(2002) identified the mother-daughter bond as the strongest over time and as having the

highest quality, compared to the father-daughter or the mother-son relationship.

Therefore, kin interaction normally follows the maternal side rather than the paternal side

7
of the family. In Kemp’s qualitative study (2007), the maternal grandmother was

perceived as a key person in obtaining social and emotional support. Likewise, in the

United Kingdom, maternal grandmothers have been identified as the most popular choice

for childcare among working mothers, followed by maternal grandfathers, paternal

grandmothers, and paternal grandfathers (Wheelock & Jones, 2002; Euler, Hoier, &

Rohde, 2001).

Although grandmothers provide much more childcare than grandfathers,

grandfathers seem to rely more on social support than grandmothers, who tend to utilize

family resources. This may be due to the fact that noncustodial grandfathers have less

interaction with family members and informal resources than noncustodial grandmothers.

According to Kolomer and McCallion (2005), custodial grandfathers had less symptoms

of depression than custodial grandmothers because custodial grandfathers have more

support systems, such as a spouse, owning a home, and working outside of the home.

Also, the study of Hayslip Jr., Henderson, and Shore (2003) identified that daycare

grandfathers are more likely to be involved in school, jobs, and finances while daycare

grandmothers’ relationships with their grandchildren are more grounded in emotion.

While daycare grandfathers have more social support, Bullock (2005) addressed daycare

grandfathers’ mental status, claiming that 21 daycare grandfathers out of 26 have

experienced powerlessness during grandparenting. Bullock claimed that daycare

grandfathers express powerlessness because they lack of control over their grandchildren.

This feeling of powerlessness is unique, in that it appears only in grandfathers.

8
While researchers suggest that there are differences between men and women in

terms of interacting with others, Hahlweg (2004) pointed out that the results of

longitudinal studies indicate no statistical gender differences. Hatch (2000) also stated

that gender is a much more complicated factor because it actively interacts with other

social entities. For instance, experience of social events, socioeconomic status, age, or

ethnicity may contribute to interactions among women and men. In other words,

tendencies of women and men in similar age groups and socioeconomic statuses may be

more similar than women among different age groups. In addition, cultural aspects of

gender influence the grandparent-grandchild interaction. Sandel, Cho, Miller, and Wang

(2006) mentioned that Taiwanese granddaughters tend to have less satisfaction and

closeness with their grandparents than do grandsons. They assumed that it may be a result

of the traditional Taiwanese cultural preference for sons. Another reason may be that girls

are more likely to be dissatisfied with the relationship if it is not as close as expected,

because girls are more emotionally sensitive. Therefore, it is unclear whether differences

between grandfathers’ and grandmothers’ relationships with grandsons and

granddaughters result from gender or combinations of other societal factors.

Developmental stage of the child

The developmental stage of the child is another factor that affects the interaction

between grandparents and grandchildren. Hareven (2000) mentioned that average

grandparents one century ago only knew their first grandchild; however, today some of

them see middle-aged grandchildren and reach great-grandparenthood. With this change

9
in situation has come a realization that the age of the grandchild may affect the

interaction between the first and the third generations of the family. Younger

grandparents tend to have closer relationships with grandchildren than older grandparents

(Lin & Harwood, 2003). In addition, younger grandparents tend to be more involved in

the grandparent-grandchild relationship. Younger grandparents may be able to be more

involved in caring for grandchildren because they are relatively physically healthy, and

their adult child has a young child who needs an intense level of supervision. In contrast,

as grandchildren grow older, grandparents tend to spend less time with them because

children may not need all-day supervision anymore and/or grandparents are losing their

physical health. Lemme (2002) stated that young grandchildren tend to be closer to their

grandparents than teenage grandchildren. Similarly, Strom and Strom (1997) found that

African American and Caucasian American grandparents with younger grandchildren

were perceived as more satisfied with their roles as grandparents than those with older

grandchildren.

Grandparent-parent relationship

Relationships between grandparents and parents of the child are another factor

influencing the grandparent-grandchild relationship. Welsh and Stewart (1995)

mentioned that positive relationships between adult parents and their parents positively

affect children’s well-being. However, if grandparents and parents experience some kind

of emotional cut-off, the grandparent-grandchild interaction may also be limited. The

experience of being a grandparent is related to the experience of being a parent (Reitzes

10
& Mutran, 2004). In general, older parents tend to have less ambivalence toward children

who are successful. Hence, older parents are more likely to feel ambivalence toward

unmarried adult children, those who have less education, and those to whom they provide

financial support (Peters, Hooker, & Zvonkovic, 2006). In this regard, grandparents feel

more stress in their relationships with adult children and their spouses who give custody

of the grandchildren to the grandparents (Musil & Standing, 2005). Custodial

grandparents experience stress because adult children let grandparents take care of

grandchildren and are not responsible for childcare. Compared to custodial grandparents,

noncustodial grandparents are more likely to express concerns related to their husbands

rather than adult children, whether those grandparents are married or not.

Parent-child relationship

Similarly, the parent-child interaction may also affect the grandparent-grandchild

relationship. However, there has not been much research found to identify the effect of

the parent-child interaction. Goodman (2007) found that the grandparent-grandchild

relationship may be positive if the parent and the child are linked together. Goodman

referred to this pattern as the ‘child-linked family’. In this family type, the grandmother

has relatively low depression and high life satisfaction, and the grandchild also has a low

level of behavior problems, even though the grandmother-parent relationship is not linked.

On the other hand, in the ‘isolated-child family’, where the grandmother is linked to both

the grandchild and the parent of the child and there is no linkage between the parent and

the child, the child has the second-highest level of behavior problems after those in the

11
‘not-bonded family’. In this relationship, the grandchild may be hurt by poor

relationships with their parents even though the grandmother tries to take over the

parent’s role. Grandmothers in this family pattern also have a higher level of depression

and a lower level of life satisfaction than those in the ‘child-linked family’.

Culture

The grandparent-grandchild interaction is immensely dependent upon the ways

each culture perceives family and the role of grandparents. While Caucasian American

culture constitutes the majority in the U.S., African American families, Hispanic

American families, and Asian American families are the major three ethnic groups that

follow Caucasian American (Julian, McKenry, & McKelvey, 1994). Julian, McKenry,

and McKelvey indicated that Caucasian American families emphasize individual

differences and emotionally-detached relationships. In Caucasian American culture,

people are basically equal regardless of age. Thus, Caucasian American people seek to be

independent from others, and this tendency continues to the senescence period (Lemme,

2002). Hence, older adults may live close to their adult children and interact with them

frequently but may not live with them.

Compared to Caucasian American families that emphasize individual relations

with others, Asian American, African American, and Hispanic American people tend to

pursue collateral relations with people (Gladding, 2002). Minuchin, Lee, and Simon

(2006) also stated that extended family networks are important for African Americans.

The extensive kin network of African American families provides both economic and

12
emotional support to their members (Becvar & Becvar, 2006). In the U.S., African

Americans have not historically had external resources; therefore, they are more likely to

help other family members than Caucasian American. Hence, African American older

adults are more likely to take grandchildren, nieces, or nephews into their home (Mitchell

& Register, 1984). According to Gladding, African American families have more open

roles, so they are less likely to stereotype based on age or gender. Hence, grandparents

tend to have a close relationship with their children and grandchildren. Also, they tend to

be less reluctant to become primary caregivers for their grandchildren. Hence, African

American grandparents tend not to consider grandparenting stressful, compared to

Caucasian Americans (Taylor, Washington, Artinian, & Lichtenberg, 2007). On the other

hand, African Americans feel responsibility to take care of their kin because this kin

network helps family members deal with poverty and racism.

Becvar and Becvar (2006) described the Latino family’s characteristics as

“dominance by gender, dominance by age, support systems by mutual assistance among

family member, and the priority of family needs over the needs of individuals” (p.121).

This means men and older people have more power than women and younger people. In

traditional Latino families, the father has the authority in major family decision-making,

while the mother usually plays a traditional role as the wife. Also, the younger generation

has respect for the decisions made by the members of the older generation. Latino culture

also has collective characteristics since family is a priority. However, Becvar and Becvar

found that due to the variety within Latino culture, these characteristics may be difficult

to generalize.

13
According to Julian, McKenry, and McKelvey (1994), Asian culture also

emphasizes extended family function and respect toward elders, even though this varies

among ethnic groups. In general, the paternal side of Asian family relationships tend to

be stronger than the maternal side, which is due to male preference and Confucian ideas

(Lin & Harwood, 2003). For example, the majority of research participants in Lin and

Harwood’s study in Taiwan identified their lineage along the paternal side. Indian

families have similar characteristics as well. Raina (1989) stated that the father has

authority over the family, especially the son, in a classic Hindu family. Children are

required to obey the father and elders in the family; however, Raina mentioned that this

classic authority pattern is changing as children seek freedom from traditional customs.

The change occurring in India may also happen in other Asian cultures as individualism

becomes a more dominant ideology. For instance, Ikeda and Hatta (2001) described

characteristics of Japanese families, specifying that the family has no clear boundaries

between the mother and the child. The father tends to be isolated in the family system

because the expectation for men is to devote themselves to their business rather than to

build relationships with their families. However, Ikeda and Hatta also mentioned that

family structures in Japan are changing such that the father is more involved in the family

and shares relatively the same burden of childcare as the mother. The changes of family

relationships and interaction patterns may influence the relationship between

grandparents and grandchildren as well.

14
Social support system

The social support system is another external factor influencing the grandparent-

grandchild interaction. The social support system may function as a third party in the

triangular relationship because both custodial grandparents and daycare grandparents

report physical, mental, and financial difficulties while caring for their grandchildren.

Custodial grandparents take over the custody of their grandchild because the parent of the

grandchild is not able to provide childcare. Hence, these grandparents are likely to sustain

a two-way relationship because the majority of them rarely receive support from a

grandchild’s parent. Therefore, the support from other family members or the community

may become a third party and complete the triangle. The social services available for

grandparents raising their grandchildren include childcare support, medical services,

counseling services, and financial support (Sands & Goldberg-Glen, 2000). Gandparents

raising their grandchildren have expressed particular need for recreational activities for

the children, health services for both the grandchild and the grandparent, and parent

training (Landry-Meyer, 1999).

Consequences of Grandparenting

The well-being of grandparents who spend time with their grandchild may

depend on their circumstances. For instance, the grandparents’ health condition,

frequency of contact with their grandchild, and closeness with their grandchild may

influence their well-being, in terms of degree of depression and life satisfaction. There

are previous studies that illustrate custodial grandparents’ health conditions compared to

15
non-custodial grandparents. According to Hayslip Jr. and Kaminski (2005), custodial

grandparents may have poorer health conditions than non-custodial grandparents:

custodial grandparents tend to experience more illnesses, such as depression and

insomnia. Hayslip Jr. and Kaminski also stated that difficulty of performing daily

activities appears more frequently among grandparents raising grandchildren than those

who do not care for their grandchildren. Robinson and Wilks (2006) mentioned that one-

third of participants in a study by Minkler, Roe, and Price (1992) expressed that their

health condition declined after becoming custodial grandparents. While custodial

grandmothers in the study stated that they limit their activities with grandchildren due to

their health, and they also have limited time for themselves, they still say that they are

willing to take care of their grandchildren if the children’s parents are not able to do so

(Musil, Warner, Zauszniewski, Jeanblanc, & Kercher, 2006).

Aside from worsening physical health conditions, daycare grandparents tend to

experience mental health issues while they are serving as caregivers for their

grandchildren. In Ross and Aday’s study (2006), 94% of their research participants,

African American grandparents or great-grandparents, had significant levels of stress

while parenting their grandchildren. McGowen and Ladd (2006) conducted an on-line

survey of custodial grandmothers, living-with-grandmothers, and nonresident

grandmothers to identify their experience in raising grandchildren. Among these three

groups, the custodial grandmother group had a significantly lower score in life

satisfaction and success as grandparents. It is assumed that the lower score of the

16
custodial grandmothers may result from their increased responsibility as a caregiver,

compared to those in the other two groups.

Although grandparents may feel they have less time to be with their age group

after becoming a childcare provider for their grandchild, there are rewards for

grandparents. One of the benefits is financial rewards from the adult child (Presser, 1989).

Presser found that 31.1% of daycare grandmothers received cash payments for providing

childcare, and 11.7% receive non-cash payments, including meals and transportation. In a

study by Wheelock and Jones (2002) conducted in the United Kingdom, one-third of

parents gave in-kind benefits to daycare grandparents even though they rarely gave cash

benefits. In addition to the financial rewards, Wheelock and Jones also found that

grandparenting gave special moments to grandfathers who had, for the first time, the

experience of being deeply involved in childcare. Goodfellow and Laverty (2003)

identified in their qualitative study with Australian daycare grandparents that

grandparents considered caring for their grandchildren to be building kin relationships.

Daycare grandparents think the rewarding side of grandparenting is in interacting closely

with their grandchildren. This is similar to the thoughts of custodial grandparents

revealed in the Silversteins’ study (2007). Emotional satisfaction also comes from the

feeling of taking on a social career. Even though daycare grandparents do not receive

financial rewards, they often consider childcare an occupation that positively impacts the

family and society (Wheelock & Jones).

On the other hand, childcare is stressful for both custodial and noncustodial

grandparents. Goodfellow and Laverty (2003) found that caring for grandchildren was a

17
physically and emotionally exhausting job for both custodial and noncustodial

grandparents. Both custodial and noncustodial grandparents expressed the physical

challenge of grandparenting, such as playing with grandchildren and providing rides. In

addition to the physical difficulty, they experience emotional challenges such as

disempowerment and family obligation. McGowen, Ladd, and Strom (2006) stated that

living-with-grandparents have a lower level of life satisfaction than not-living-with-

grandparents, even though the living-with-grandparents have higher life satisfaction

scores than custodial grandparents. Lack of free time is another factor that brings stress to

custodial and noncustodial grandparents. Goodfellow and Laverty found that even though

both custodial and noncustodial grandparents wanted to spend time with their peers, it

was difficult to go out with a two- or three-year-old grandchild.

For parents and grandchildren, on the other hand, grandparents become a great

support system as they enter into the triangle relationship. Specifically, parents of the

child may benefit most since the grandparents’ main role is to help care for the child.

Informal child care from grandparents reduces the financial burden for the parent (Presser,

1989: Wheelock & Jones, 2002). In addition, it also reduces the social costs for providing

formal childcare or organizing volunteer childcare providers. Besides the financial

benefits, parents prefer grandparents as caregivers because they have the same family

values (Wheelock & Jones, 2002). For instance, grandparents are able to educate

grandchildren within the culture of the family, especially when the grandchild is raised in

a different country from where the grandparents and parents were raised.

18
Besides the benefit to the parent, grandchildren also benefit from the triangular

relationship because they have more time under the adult’s guidance. Silverstein and

Ruiz (2006) identified grandparents as contributing to reducing intergenerational

transmission of the mother’s depression to the children, although they do not influence

reduction in the grandchildren’s depressive symptoms. Grandparents tended to affect

grandchildren’s well-being indirectly and in the long term. In addition, researchers found

that, compared to daycare or other relative caregivers, a grandparent caregiver reduced

the risk of child injury almost by half (Science Daily, 2008).

19
Chapter 3: Theoretical Frameworks

Life Course Perspective

Key concepts

A life course perspective perceives development as a lifelong process (Hooyman

& Kiyak, 2005). Compared to Erikson’s developmental stages, which assume that the

individual’s life stage is set up from birth, the life course perspective refers to an

individual’s life span as a consequence of social and environmental factors, such as

gender, income, and social class. In other words, the life course perspective admits the

diversity of the individual’s roles, which change throughout the individual’s life span.

Hence, the life course perspective accepts multiple social roles throughout the life span

(Macmillan & Copher, 2005). Because it accepts the heterogeneity of an individual’s life

pattern, grandparenting is also considered as a part of the individual’s life span. However,

it assumes that grandparents’ earlier experience as parents affects their grandparenting

skills (Dolbin-MacNab, 2006). Moreover, the life course perspective describes different

life patterns among individuals with different backgrounds. Thus, the life course

perspective may be helpful to address how socioeconomic status affects the use of

support systems and the well-being of grandparents raising their grandchildren.

The life course perspective defines three types of time: individual, historical, and

generational. First of all, individual time refers to chronological time, which focuses on

the periods of individuals’ lives (Clausen, 1986). Therefore, individual

20
time describes culture-appropriate roles, obligations, and privileges for individuals within

society as they go through childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. Because individual

time recognizes appropriate norms within society, Clausen also called this social time.

Hence, the laws of society dictate a person’s rights and duties based on the person’s age.

For instance, a six-year-old child in the United States is expected to attend school. As he

approaches 16, the child will have a right to drive but does not obtain the right to drink

until the age of 21. Similarly to the child in the U.S., a child in Korea begins schooling at

the age of 6. However, the child in Korea will have the right to drive at the age of 18 and

the right to drink at the age of 19. While social time mostly follows chronological age, it

is not fixed to it (Clausen, 1986). There are cases where informal expectations, rather

than formal regulations, decide the age-appropriate norms. For example, some students

get into the workforce right after they finish high school, while others continue to remain

at school for undergraduate and graduate study at college. Another example is the

marriage age: some couples get married in their early 20s, while others get married in

their late 30’s.

Secondly, the cohort effect is related to another type of time, historical time, as

well. Historical time emphasizes societal changes over history and how societal changes

affect individuals (Price, McKenry, & Murphy, 2000). In other words, historical events,

such as war, economic changes, technological development, and political events

significantly impact individuals and families. Currently, developments in technologies

have brought a new method of communication between individuals. Teenagers

communicate with their friends using text messages, e-mails, and on-line messengers,

21
while some adults still have a hard time using a computer. Thus, interaction within a

family is often influenced by how well family members are able to use the Internet and

other technologies. For instance, before advancements in technologies, when a child was

out of the country for military service, it would take weeks to exchange letters. But these

days, a child in the service is able to chat online with family members at his or her

convenience.

Lastly, generational time refers to the positions of individuals within their family

and their roles and identities related to their positions (Price, McKenry, & Murphy, 2000).

It is somewhat similar to individual time, which defines individual roles according to age.

However, generational time focuses more on the role within family. Hence, a 46-year-old

grandmother and a 98-year-old grandmother are in different age groups, yet they are both

grandmothers. Generational time has changed as life expectancy has increased.

Nowadays, people can play the role of child for approximately 50 years and maintain

grandparent-grandchild relationships for 30 to 40 years (Hagestad, 2003). Hagestad stated

that only 3% of women over 60 years old had at least one living parent in the 1800s.

Comparatively, more than 60% of people in this age group had at least one living parent

in 1980. Since it has become common for two or more generations to share lifetimes

much longer than in the past, unexpected off-time, such as early parental death, may

result in personal crisis because the individuals are unprepared and have limited support

from peers.

22
Strengths of the life course perspective

The life course perspective enables one to see individuals both from their

chronological time perspectives and family development perspectives (Price, McKenry,

& Murphy, 2000; Hareven, 1994). Hence, it is possible to understand roles, rights, and

responsibilities of individuals from the perspective of both the individual and the family.

In other words, the life course perspective perceives the individual’s life transition as

relating to family transitions (Hareven, 1994). That is, changes in the individual’s life

results in changes in the family structure. Hence, not only has the role of grandparents

caring for their grandchildren changed from the grandparent to the caregiver, but also the

family structure has changed from the parent-headed family to the grandparent-headed

family.

In general, Erikson’s developmental stage theory treats all individuals in the

same way, assuming that people in a certain stage should have the same role and same

tasks; however, the life course perspective allows individuals to set their own roles and

tasks (Hooyman & Kiyak, 2005). According to Erikson’s theory, parenting is the main

task for middle adulthood. Although grandparents raising grandchildren are relatively

young, and some may be still in middle adulthood, a second round of caring may not be

their expected task, based on Erikson’s theory (Anderson, Carter, & Lowe, 1999).

Consequently, grandparents who have to deal with two life tasks at the same time –

parenting grandchildren and dealing with their own life tasks – may fail to establish a

plan for their future. Hence, maintaining two roles at the same time may result in role

23
confusion and caregiving stress. Even though Erikson’s theory explains why

grandparents raising grandchildren suffer from role confusion and isolation from their

peers, it does not identify whether grandparent caregivers are in middle or late adulthood.

As only individuals with children are able to complete the sixth and seventh stage,

grandparent caregivers do not seem to belong to any stage.

In contrast, the life course perspective accepts both 40-year-old individuals and

80-year-old individuals as grandparents as long as their position in the family is that of

the grandparent. In other words, the life course perspective describes different life

patterns among individuals with different backgrounds (Price, McKenry, & Murphy,

2000). Hence, the life course perspective accepts multiple social roles throughout the

lifespan (Macmillan & Copher, 2005). Because it accepts heterogeneity of an individual’s

life pattern, it also considers grandparenting as a part of the individual’s life span

(Dolbin-MacNab, 2006). Consequently, it may be helpful to address how the

socioeconomic status affects the use of support systems and the well-being of the

grandparent.

In addition, the life course perspective focuses not only on families with married

parents but also other types of families, such as divorced families, remarried families, and

families in poverty. The divorced family life cycle starts from the decision to divorce and

goes through the separation period and the final divorce phase (Price, McKenry, &

Murphy, 2000). In contrast, the remarried family life cycle has three phases: recovery

from loss and entering a new relationship, planning a new marriage, and family

reconstruction. In addition, the life course perspective admits different family cycles

24
according to the family’s financial situation. For instance, families in poverty tend to

have less calendar time because they spend more time overcoming economic disparity;

thus, it is harder for individuals in these families to accomplish each stage’s task due to

the shorter calendar. Recently-begun studies on these family formations may differentiate

the life course perspective from stage theories, which do not include the diversity of the

type of family.

Weaknesses of the life course perspective

The life course perspective has limitations in explaining individuals within the

family. First of all, it inadequately accounts for stress occurred from role overload and

taking a role off-time. For instance, it may not be able to identify the cause of stress that

custodial grandparents often experience in terms of role confusion and role overload. The

life course perspective is similar to role theory in that it allows individuals to take

multiple roles and responsibilities at the same time. In addition, both life course

perspective and role theory explain role changes and role loss throughout the life span.

For instance, in role theory, the role is formulized based on the discussion of the

socialization and adjustment of the individuals (Biddle, 1979). In other words, role theory

is similar to the life course perspective in that both emphasize family interactions. Hence,

from initial socialization in the family, role theory perceives that individuals learn role

behaviors that are appropriate for their social class, ethnic group, and other social

positions they occupy in life (Collier & Callero, 2005).

25
Compared to the life course perspective, however, role theory describes role

confusion and role overload of individuals. As the child develops his or her roles through

role playing and role taking, adults also form roles (Hooyman & Kiyak, 2005). On the

other hand, people may be confronted with role conflict during the process of role

formation. This means that individuals are sometimes required to perform two or more

tasks even though they are not able to do all of them. Or, people may sometimes suffer

from role overload when they are asked to do too much. Because the life course

perspective permits individuals to perform multiple roles at the same time, it is also

possible for grandparents to obtain a new role as a caregiver for their grandchildren when

the birth parent of these children is working during the day. In this situation, the life

course perspective may recommend that the grandparents and the parent should maintain

a balance in their roles of parenting; however, it may not describe what would happen

when they fail to keep the balance.

The life course perspective and role theory have different perspectives on the

timing of when an individual takes on a particular role. In the life course perspective,

generational time allows individuals to take on the same roles at a different individual

time. Role theory also describes individuals who assume a role at an off time. However,

the life course perspective does not explain the possible consequences of taking a role at

an off time, while role theory considers this an ambiguity of formatting culturally

appropriate roles (Landry-Meyer & Newman, 2004). For instance, grandparent roles are

sometimes ambiguous, in that appropriate roles as grandparents differ from family to

26
family. In addition, those who become a grandparent in their 30’s may have different

perspectives on caring for their grandchildren from 60- to 70-year-old grandparents.

Another limitation of the life course perspective is that it is a perspective, not a

theory. It means that the life course perspective has not been verified for diverse ethnic

groups and diverse cultures. The life course perspective is primarily based on white male

individuals. Because each culture has different perceptions of time, individuals from

different cultures may have different individual, historical, and generational time. In

addition, comparisons among people who live in the same location but have different

cultural backgrounds may also need to be identified in terms of their life course experiences.

It means there is heterogeneity within the same cohort. Different life courses are possible

among members of the same cohort even after they experienced the same historical event.

Also, due to the limitation of a life course ‘perspective’, it may be less able to predict

various social phenomena. Hence, researchers need to collect data from those who have

diverse backgrounds over time in order to make a better theoretical framework.

Bowen Family Systems Theory

Key concepts

Bowen family systems theory defines family as a system that gives and takes

energy from each of its members. In other words, systems continuously move between

change and stability. An individual in the family may sometimes get into an unstable

situation when tension exists within family. If so, change occurs until the situation

becomes more stable. Then, stability will be maintained unless it falls into another

27
unstable position that will need feedback to help it move back to a stable status (Becvar

& Becvar, 2009). Bowen family systems theory has eight key concepts: (1)

differentiation of self, (2) the triangle, (3) nuclear family emotional process, (4) family

projection process, (5) the multigenerational transmission process, (6) sibling position, (7)

emotional cutoff, and (8) emotional processes in society (Papero, 1990; Nichols &

Schwartz, 2001).

Differentiation of self in Bowen family systems theory focuses on the individual

(Papero, 1990). Nichols and Schwartz (2001) mentioned that differentiation refers to the

ability to act flexibly and wisely even in an anxious situation. Hence, differentiated

people tend to possess strong emotions but they are able to resist the pull of emotional

reactions. Conversely, undifferentiated people are likely to react emotionally toward

others. Papero defined emotional as “forces or pressures deeply rooted in each individual

and between the individual and his/her environment” (p. 45). In this sense, the emotional

system refers to “the reactivity of the individual to its environment” (p. 45). Papero

mentioned that the basic level of differentiation is usually established in early life,

although it may be modified in later life as well. However, once the basic level of

differentiation is established, it rarely changes throughout the life.

The triangle relationship can be observed when anxiety within the system is too

intense (Papero, 1990; Prest & Protinsky, 1993). The anxiety in the family system

evolves when the interpersonal needs of the family members are challenged. In Bowen

family systems theory, when a twosome relationship encounters anxious situations, a

third person may be able to join the twosome interaction and make a threesome

28
relationship (Ackerman, 1984). Ackerman assumed that there is no simple one-on-one

interaction among human beings. Rather, a third thing or individual constantly interrupts

an anxious twosome relationship in order to stabilize the relationship. Papero also stated

that the triangle is so natural in everyday life that people are often unaware of it. Griff

(1999) addressed an example of the triangle in which the grandparents’ participation in

the family intervention positively influenced the treatment of the grandchildren. However,

the third person in the triangle is not necessarily a part of the family. For example, in a

single parent family with a child, although a grandparent may be involved in an anxious

situation with the parent, a child’s teacher or a friend of the parent may become the third

person. In order to function positively, Ackerman stated that each relationship should

maintain certain distinctions from the others, and each person in a relationship should

have direct interactions with the other. In addition, the distance between persons should

be at an equilibrium, which means it should not be too close or too far away. On the other

hand, Bowen stated that the anxiety of the third party would increase as he or she became

more involved in the triangulation (Miller, Anderson, & Keala, 2004). When the anxiety

evolves in the marital relationship, a child often becomes the third party. Bowen was

concerned that the third party child would have emotional distress and negative social

outcomes.

The nuclear family emotional process is related to the level of a common self,

“we-ness” between couples (Papero, 1990, p. 51). However, the emotional process in the

nuclear family is often related to the extended family system because each partner applies

mechanisms that he or she used in relationships with his or her parents (Titelman, 1998).

29
The outcome of their efforts to develop the common self is a result of four mechanisms in

the nuclear family: emotional distance, marital conflict, transmission of the problem to a

child, and dysfunction in a spouse (Papero, 1990; Nichols & Schwartz, 2001). These four

mechanisms may be used in a combination of two or more at the same time. Emotional

distance may mean either external or internal distance. Couples with external distance

have less time together because they are physically apart from each other. In another

sense, internal distance often occurs without the members of a relationship being aware

of it. Papero stated that couples experiencing internal distance gradually talk less and less

with one another and finally have nothing to share with each other. Marital conflict is one

of the well-known symptoms of anxiety in a family. It is connected to emotional distance

because couples with conflict are likely to be more distant. Subsequently, couples with

great internal or external distance are likely to have marital conflict. When couples have

marital conflict, it affects their children as well because all children are likely to be

involved in the parents’ emotional process. Children may be involved in anxiety

situations as a third person to reduce anxiety between parents. Similarly, according to

Spira and Wall (2006), anxiety between grandparents and parents may influence

adolescent children as well. Lastly, dysfunction in a spouse refers to the adaptivity of

partners. In a marital relationship, couples compromise with each other in order to avoid

a conflict situation. In this relationship, each person has roughly the same amount of

responsibilities. However, if one person is under-functioning and the other person is over-

functioning, the family may experience conflict when an anxious situation arises.

30
According to Papero (1990), the family projection process relates to the

relationship between a mother and a child. In other words, the family projection process

focuses more on the mother than the father, which works under the assumption that the

mother’s emotional sensitivity to a child is greater than that of her spouse. The role of the

husband is to support the wife’s interaction with the child, while the father is equally

involved in the attachment process with the child. Hence, Bowen family systems theory

assumes the mother to be a primary person for the child. Thus, the father’s functioning

impacts the relationship of the mother and the child, not the father and the child. If the

father does not exist, the intensity between mother and child increases, whereas in a

family with the father’s support, the relationship between mother and child is more stable.

The multigenerational transmission process is similar to the family projection

process in terms of describing the effect of parents on their children (Papero, 1990).

However, the multigenerational transmission process focuses more on the effect of

parent-child involvement on the child’s development. In other words, the focused child to

whom the parent pays more attention is likely to be more attached to the parents, and

tends to be more vulnerable to emotional intensity. On the other hand, the less-focused

child is more differentiated from the parents, so the child is likely to learn more from the

parents’ strengths. Bowen proposed that people marry partners who possess a similar

level of differentiation. Therefore, focused children are more likely to seek partners with

similar or less differentiation, whereas less focused children tend to marry partners with

more differentiation. Consequently, segregation between less-differentiated children and

more-differentiated children become distinct over the generations.

31
The sibling position describes characteristics of each child based on his or her

position within the family (Papero, 1990). In other words, Bowen assumed that the level

of complementary relationships is dependent upon rank and sex conflict. Bowen

perceived that children’s personalities are developed based on their position in the family

(Nichols & Schwartz, 2001). Hence, the youngest son with sisters tends to show different

characteristics from the youngest son with brothers only. Nichols and Schwartz said that

firstborn children tend to apply power and authority to defend their status from other

siblings. In contrast, later-born children are more likely to be open to exploring because

this inquisitiveness helps them to discover a new niche.

Emotional cutoff occurs when people attempt to control their emotional

attachment to their parents and other individuals (Papero, 1990). In other words, it refers

to the way people control anxiety between generations (Nichols & Schwartz, 2001).

Papero stated that individuals can maintain emotional distance using either psychological

distance or physical distance. If an individual uses intra-psychic mechanisms for

emotional cutoff, he or she can be isolated from a family member even though he or she

lives with them. On the other hand, with physical distance, an individual can control the

frequency of his or her contact with parents. For instance, people may control emotional

attachment by reducing the number of phone calls or visits. Or, people simply live away

from their family to create emotional cutoff using physical distance.

Lastly, emotional processes in society are extended versions of emotional

processes in the family (Papero, 1990). Individuals’ anxieties tend to enhance functioning

as two people add a third person to their group. Similarly, in terms of emotional

32
processes in society, as the level of anxiety increases, society tends to seek equilibrium

by composing more stable groups. This often happens where one entity is added as a

mediator to the conflict of two social groups.

Strengths of Bowen family systems theory

Anderson, Carter, and Lowe (1999) mentioned that systems theory permits the

observer to investigate different levels within the same phenomenon at the same time and

to analyze the whole as well as the parts. Although it may make analysis more complex,

it is likely to be more accurate and closer to real life. Therefore, based on systems theory,

social workers try to observe not only clients’ personal characteristics but also their

environment, such as family, friends, religious groups, and communities. By assessing

clients’ environments, it is possible to more thoroughly analyze both their problems and

their strengths that may lead to possible solutions. Systems theory treats the client’s

environment as containing influential factors that affect assessing, and intervening in, the

client’s problem. Systems theory’s influence on the social work field is evident in the

current common practice of drawing genograms and eco-maps for assessment, and

adopting the ‘bio-psycho-social’ perspective for intervention.

Compared to the life course perspective, family systems theory focuses more on

family relationships than on examining individual family members (Fingerman &

Bermann, 2000). As family systems theory studies both micro and macro perspectives,

such as communication, transactions, and anxiety in the family, it is a better tool for

understanding behaviors of each family member than the life course perspective.

33
Specifically, because Bowen family systems theory emphasizes the triangle relationship,

it is able to show a solution for role overload. The life course perspective has a weakness

in explaining role overload; however, Bowen family systems theory provides a triangle as

a solution when family conflict occurs. For example, if a family member’s role overload

is the reason for anxiousness within the family, the family may be able to reduce the

person’s overload by adding a new person who is able to take part of the load.

Moreover, Bowen family systems theory is good for describing the relationship

among grandparents, parents, and grandchildren because it emphasizes the influence of

the extended family system. Although Bowen named the nuclear family’s emotional

system as one of the major concepts of family systems theory, the family as system refers

to at least three generations (Titelman, 1998). The nuclear family consists of the mother,

the father, and the child. In addition to that, the father’s family of origin constitutes

another emotional system, and the mother’s family of origin yet another. Hence, Nichols

and Schwartz (2001) stated that Bowenian therapy begins with collecting descriptions

and histories of existing problems by assessing the extended family systems. Bowenian

therapists assess whether the problem is related to events in the extended family. Today,

the majority of families are nuclear families that only include parents and their children.

However, the extended family influences members of independent nuclear families in

many ways. For instance, grandparents are often involved in the families of their adult

children in order to support the family. Specifically, custodial grandparents have been

involved in parenting their grandchildren when parents are not able to provide care for

their children. The triangle may exist in families with non-custodial grandparents as well.

34
Noncustodial grandparents may sometimes become part-time caregivers while parents are

at work. Hence, Bowen family systems theory is particularly strong in illustrating the

interactions of grandparents, parents, and grandchildren as extended family systems.

Weaknesses of Bowen family systems theory

Systems theory is designed to understand the process of change and stability;

changes happen constantly within and between the systems. Gerson (1996) stated that

system changes that are designed to address one problem may accelerate another problem.

Bowen stated that the anxiety of a third party entering in an anxious twosome relationship

would increase although the anxiety among the other two parties would decrease once the

third party joined (Becvar & Becvar, 2009). For instance, if a grandmother becomes a

primary caregiver for the grandchild due to a parent’s drug problems, the child may

obtain consistent care from the grandmother. Although grandparenting may benefit the

child, it may result in negative consequences for the grandmother who provides care for a

second time around. While Bowen stated that the anxiety of the third person, after being a

moderator of twosome relationship, increased, researchers have had mixed results on

correlations between the anxiety of the third party and triangulation. In other words, some

findings supported Bowen’s assumption that being in this kind of triangulated

relationship resulted in a negative attitude toward family. On the other hand, other

researchers stated that the impact of triangulation and emotional distress were not

significantly related to each other (Miller, Anderson, & Keala, 2004). In this aspect,

therapists are constantly challenged to determine whether the client’s system needs to be

35
changed or not. If they decide to make changes, the concern becomes which system,

among the various systems that exist in the client’s situation, should be utilized, because

systems interact with each other both positively and negatively.

Another weakness of Bowen family systems theory is that it focuses too much on

family conflict and anxious situations as causes of people’s mental health problems.

Bowen’s theory was originally derived from family therapy, especially while he was

working with people with mental health problems such as schizophrenia. Bowen stated

that a high level of anxiety in the family may cause mental health issues for an individual

in the family (Becvar & Becvar, 2009). However, it may be rare that the family has no

conflict at all. Also, it is true that the family is a fundamental system that strongly affects

people’s physical and mental conditions. On the other hand, as the family is one of many

systems, there are factors besides family conflict that influence people’s psychological

dysfunction.

Lastly, gender bias exists in Bowen family systems theory. When explaining the

emotional process of the family, Bowen focused on the interaction between mother and

child. Although the mother has traditionally been a primary caregiver for children, this

situation is currently changing as more women enter the workforce. Bowen family

systems theory is still based on traditional mother-father roles, in which the mother stays

at home caring for the children while the father works outside of the home. Even though

Bowen family systems theory covers extended family and blended family structures, its

basic assumption of gender roles may be a weakness when it comes to explaining diverse

types of families.

36
Life Course Perspective and Bowen Family Systems Theory

Heterogeneities

The main differences between the life course perspective and Bowen family

systems theory are the considerations of diversity and time. Regarding diversity, the life

course perspective assumes that the individual’s life course varies according to cohort,

gender, and race (George & Gold, 1991). George and Gold also stated that scholars have

found that the life course is “less predictable and more heterogeneous than was initially

imagined” (p.71). In the life course perspective, there is variability across cohorts, and

even among the same cohort, based on family characteristics. Hence, rather than

analyzing each individual’s life course with established norms, the life course perspective

tends to recognize the diversity of each person’s life course. Thus, in the life course

perspective, taking roles off time is considered as a pattern of the life course, not as a

problematic situation.

Although Bowen family systems theory also perceives that each family has

unique subsystems in the family, it tends to find norms and similarities within family

systems. Bowen family systems theory assumes that the anxiety in the relationship of two

people may be mediated by the third party (Miller, Anderson, & Keala, 2004). However,

Bowen assumes the distress of the third person may be increased as he or she plays a

mediator. This assumption is applied to all families, whether or not they have different

cultural backgrounds. Therefore, when anxiety exists in the marital relationship, a child

often takes a third party role to reduce anxiety. In addition, a single parent with a child

37
may need a third person, such as a grandparent or an adult sibling, to avoid emotional

distress. On the other hand, the life course perspective focuses on the changing roles of

the parent and the child in a given time period. Hence, the triangle relationship may not

be necessary because each individual has his or her own shifting roles.

In addition to the diversity issue, the consideration of time is different between

two theories. As its name represents, the life course perspective is based on the

individual’s life course. Therefore, life course analysis needs a dynamic and longitudinal

perspective (George & Gold, 1991). This aspect may be both a strength and a weakness

of the life course perspective. It may be a strong theoretical framework because the life

course perspective includes changes in people’s life from birth to death within a certain

society. However, it is hard to collect such data to analyze trajectories; therefore, George

and Gold mentioned that most of the life course perspective research examines transitions

rather than trajectories. Also, research on trajectories is mainly based on simulated

cohorts rather than actual longitudinal data.

Bowen family systems theory, however, focuses more on the current system of

the family rather than the pathways of people’s lives. Hence, compared to the life course

perspective practitioners, Bowenian therapists are more likely to assess the current

problem within the family and its effects on family members. They also examine family

history in order to identify the problems existing in the family; however, Bowen family

systems theory tends to focus more on the system of the family itself rather than the

changes within the family pattern or family history.

38
Reciprocities

Both the life course perspective and Bowen family systems theory emphasize

interactions within the family. Macmillan and Copher (2005) stated that role pathways in

the life course perspective illustrate the cause and the effect of interdependent family

roles. Putney and Bengtson (2005) mentioned that the life course perspective concerns

the interdependence of lives in the family where intergenerational transmissions occur. In

the same sense, family is the main place where people learn different types of roles and

how people assume and change roles. Therefore, a role change of a person in the family

affects the roles of other people in the family. For example, when parents are not

available, the eldest sibling often becomes a parental child who is responsible for caring

for younger siblings. The eldest sibling may prepare meals, help with homework, or

support the family financially by working. Another example may be a custodial

grandparent who takes a parental role for the grandchild when the parent is not able to

take care of the child. The life course perspective perceives grandparenting as acceptable

when the family needs it. Hence, taking on either full-time or part-time caregiving roles

as a grandparent is considered one of the life courses.

Similarly, Bowen family systems theory also concerns interactions among family

members. As in the life course perspective, Bowen family systems theory accepts the

grandparent entering into the relationship of the parent and the child. Bowen family

systems theory perceives grandparents as a positive resource for stable family

interactions. To identify internal interactions in the family, Bowenian family therapists

often apply a genogram. Nichols and Schwartz (2001) stated that genograms are

39
“schematic diagrams listing family members and their relationships to one another” (p.

85). The genogram includes ages, marital status, deaths in the family, and gender of each

family member. It is a static dyad because it includes the conditions of the relationships

among listed people, such as conflicts, strong bonds, cutoff, or triangles. Bowen family

systems theory assumes that family interactions include both the cause and the solution of

the client’s problem. Hence, by assessing interactions between family members,

therapists may illustrate supportive factors and risk factors within the family.

Although the life course perspective considers internal family interactions as

support for family members, kin interactions, in some cases, are considered more

ambivalent than non-family-ties (Peters, Hooker, & Zvonkovic, 2006). In addition,

generational ties are more ambivalent than horizontal sibling-ties or friend-ties. Moreover,

the life course perspective holds that historical events and cohort effects function not to

eliminate this ambivalence, but to set it at the societal level. Hence, older parents may

have both positive and negative feelings when they interact with their adult children. In

the qualitative study of Peters, Hooker and Zvonkovic, older parents generally wished for

more frequent contact with their adult children; however, they were concerned about

future requests for help from their children as well. Similarly, adult children expressed

their ambivalence toward their older parents. Although they were willing to help their

parents, they were concerned about potential caregiving. Bowen family systems theory

has similar perceptions to those of the life course perspective. Bowenian therapists focus

on family interaction because it often results in negative consequences, such as mental

illness of family members.

40
Besides internal interactions, Macmillan and Copher (2005) stated that family

roles involve complex interdependent relationships with extra-family roles in the life

course perspective. In other words, individual time is immensely affected by generational

time and historical time. Social events that occurred at a certain time influence people’s

roles in the family. For example, when economic crisis occurred in Korea a decade ago,

employment became unstable. Hence, women who had stayed at home started to come to

the workforce to support the family finances. When both parents entered the workforce,

parenting roles in the family changed as well. In the past, women were mainly

responsible for caring for the child; however, as women have acquired worker roles,

grandparents have often taken parent roles while parents are at work. Putney and

Bengtson (2005) described the extra-family effects that the mother’s entry into the

workforce may enhance, such as the family’s economic wealth; but, it also may result in

changes in the family routine or the balance of the mother’s work with housework.

Bowen family systems theory also acknowledges the interdependency between

individuals and their environment. An ecomap is applied to address the interactions that

occur with external factors. While a genogram illustrates interactions in the family, the

ecomap describes interactions with social institutions outside of the family, such as

community, religious groups, schools, workplace, and so on. The ecomap is helpful if one

of the extra-family factors causes an individual’s mental health problem. In addition, it

aids in assessing whether any of the extra-family factors might help to address the

individual’s problem.

41
Rationale

It is necessary to focus on the relationship between grandparents and

grandchildren and its relation to the grandparents’ well-being within the context of the

family relationship. Previous studies described in the literature review section have

examined gender, age, ethnicity, and/or legal guardianship of grandparents and its impact

on the grandparents’ well-being. Although these characteristics certainly impact the

grandparent’s well-being, it is also necessary to view the grandparent’s well-being within

the context of the family system. In other words, addressing the grandparent-grandchild

relationship is important because the grandparent, grandchild, and parent often make the

triangle of the family system. This triangle may influence both positively and negatively

the grandparent’s well-being as grandparents become a third party of the system. Hence,

this study focused on this grandparent-grandchild relationship and its impact on the

grandparent’s well-being. Then, this study compared the interaction between the

grandparent’s well-being and the closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild

based on the grandparent’s gender, ethnicity, and legal custody.

The closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild may depend on various

factors beyond the grandparent-grandchild relationship. In addition, the grandparent’s

well-being may be related to other factors besides the relationship between the

grandparent and the grandchild. Hence, it is necessary to examine factors associated with

the grandparent-grandchild relationship and the grandparents’ well-being. Depending on

these factors, some grandparents may feel reluctant to take care of the grandchild, while

others welcome the responsibility. For example, according to Gladding (2002),

42
multigenerational families are more common in non-Caucasian American culture;

therefore, non-Caucasian American grandparents may accept a grandparent-caregiver

role more easily than Caucasian American grandparents. Also, grandparents whose

relationship with their adult child is troubled tend to be more reluctant to take care of

their grandchildren than those who have a good relationship with their adult child (Welsh

& Stewart, 1995). Thus, in this study, three factors – grandparent’s functional capacity,

grandparent-parent support, and cultural attitude – were included to address whether they

affect the interaction between the grandparent’s well-being and the grandparent-

grandchild relationship. By analyzing these factors, it can be determined what kinds of

grandparents have a closer relationship with their grandchild; who has a higher level of

well-being; and how a grandparent’s functional capacity affects the grandparent’s well-

being.

Lastly, it is possible to investigate how Bowen family systems theory and the life

course perspective can be applied to the grandparent’s well-being and the closeness

between the grandparent and the grandchild. Bowen family systems theory emphasizes

the necessity of the triangular relationship to maintain a stable condition. Parents

typically utilize a formal public day care center to meet their childcare needs; however, in

some cases, a grandparent may become the third part of the triangle by providing

informal private care. Grandparents have several strengths as caregivers. They are

flexible, so parents do not have to pick up their children at certain time. Also,

grandparents are able to provide one-on-one guidance to the child that is not available at

a childcare center. Moreover, grandparents are able to teach family values to their

43
grandchildren. Thus, both parents and children may have more stable relationships as

grandparents become a part of the family system. However, it may be necessary to

address the changes in the grandparents’ well-being as they spend time with their

grandchildren. Bowen family system theory addresses the third person’s anxiety after he

or she becomes part of the triangulated relationship. Thus, the amount of time spent with

grandchildren and emotional ties with grandchildren may influence the grandparent’s

well-being.

According to the life course perspective, a person is able to function in more than

two roles at the same time, and the roles are changeable as the family structure changes.

Unlike custodial grandparents, noncustodial grandparents have two roles, grandparent

and caregiver, at the same time. Grandparents are able to continue to contribute to society

by providing childcare services to their adult children and grandchildren. At the same

time, they maintain the role of a traditional grandparent since they do not have legal

custody of the grandchild. In addition, the grandparent-grandchild relationship may also

be changeable according to the age of both grandparents and grandchildren. Differences

in family interaction between younger grandparents and older grandparents may affect

the closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild as well.

Research Questions

1) Do the age of both grandparents and grandchildren and the number of grandchildren

in the family affect the number of nights spent with grandchild and the closeness

between the grandparent and the grandchild?

44
2) Are the number of nights spent with grandchild and the closeness between the

grandparent and the grandchild associated with the grandparent’s well-being?

3) Do the grandparent’s functional capacity, grandparent-parent support, and cultural

attitude affect the grandparent-grandchild relationship and the grandparent’s well-

being?

4) Is the grandparent’s well-being different according to whether the grandparent is

male or female?

5) Is the grandparent’s well-being different according to whether the grandparent is

Caucasian American or non-Caucasian American?

6) Is the grandparent’s well-being different according to whether the grandparent is

custodial or noncustodial?

45
Chapter 4: Research Methods

Overview of the Research Design

This study is a secondary data analysis using the third wave of the National

Survey of Families and Households (NSFH) dataset. Of the main respondents of the

dataset, only those who had at least one grandchild in the family were included in the

study. The researcher analyzed the study population using the Structural Equation

Modeling (SEM) method. Latent variables included in this study were: (1) number of

nights spent with the grandchild; (2) closeness between the grandparent and the

grandchild; (3) cultural attitude; (4) grandparent-parent support; (5) grandparent’s

functional capacity; and (6) grandparent’s well-being. In addition, variables measuring

both grandparents’ and grandchildren’s age and the number of grandchildren in the

family were included. Three multigroup comparisons were also implemented to identify

whether grandfathers and grandmothers, Caucasian grandparents and non-Caucasian

grandparents, and custodial grandparents and noncustodial grandparents had different

patterns in the SEM model.

Dataset

The data set analyzed in this study was from the National Survey of Families and

Households (NSFH). The NSFH was a national representative panel study conducted by

the University of Wisconsin Survey Center for professors James Sweet and Larry

46
Bumpass of the Department of Sociology at the University of Wisconsin-Madison. The

initial interview with main respondents was conducted between 1987 and 1988, and wave

two was conducted from 1992 to 1994. Data for wave three, used in this study, were

collected between 2001 and 2003 via telephone survey, as were the original two waves.

The average number of call attempts for those who completed the survey was twelve, and

seventeen for those who did not complete the survey. Approximately 50% of those who

completed finished their survey in 5 call attempts. The final sample size in wave three

was 9,230, which was equivalent to 72% of those located (Sweet & Bumpass, 2002). The

sample included main respondents, spouses, and main respondents’ children.

Sampling methods used in the NSFH was a national multistage area probability

sampling that contained approximately 17,000 housing units from 100 sampling areas

across the nation. For the oversample, the researchers doubled the number of households

selected in the 100 sampling areas. Main respondents were originally selected from the

national sample of 13,007 people, including a main cross section of 9,637 households

with an over sampling of African Americans, Puerto Ricans, Mexican Americans, single

parent families, families with step children, cohabiting couples, and recently married

persons. Then, one person per household was randomly selected as the main respondent

(Sweet, Bumpass, & Call, 1988). The main respondent should be 19 years old or older to

be eligible to interview. However, those who were currently married were considered as

the main respondent although they might be under 19. In addition, if there was no family

member over 19, people under 19 years old were considered as eligible main respondents.

Children were classified from child 1 to child 7 according to their relationship with the

47
main respondent and living arrangements. In addition, 924 proxy interviews from a

family member of the main respondent were conducted in the wave three when the main

respondent was deceased or too ill to interview.

A primary reason for using this dataset for this study is that the NSFH has

variables the researcher would like to analyze. First of all, there are three generation

variables. When the main respondents are set to the first generation, not only does the

NSFH include variables of the first generation, but it also incorporates the variables of

the main respondents’ children and grandchildren. The dataset collects a particularly wide

range of information on the child of the main respondent. This information will allow the

researcher to identify the relationship between the main respondents, the first generation

and the child, and the second generation, as well as general characteristics of the second

generation. Thus, the NSFH is the most appropriate tool to facilitate the main goal of this

study, which is to examine the relationship among three generations in relation to the first

generation’s well-being.

Secondly, the NSFH enables the researcher to analyze the impact of the closeness

between the grandparent and the grandchild on the grandparents’ well-being. It has

variables measuring the closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild. In

addition, the dataset has several well-being measures, including depression, happiness,

and life satisfaction, which the researcher wants to measure. Hence, this dataset allows

the researcher to determine how the closeness of the grandparent and the grandchild

influences the well-being of the grandparent.

48
Another benefit of the NSFH is that it includes a large national sample across the

nation. More than 7,000 people participated in the research, and more than 25% of

respondents had at least one grandchild. Therefore, it is possible to include variety of

grandparents who are from various age groups and diverse cultural backgrounds.

A limitation of using the NSFH dataset is that there are few variables measuring

grandchildren’s general characteristics. The researcher regrets the lack of grandchildren’s

information, such as exact age and gender of each grandchild. In addition, it would be

beneficial if there were a variable addressing whether the grandchild is from the

daughters’ family or from the sons’ family. Moreover, the dataset has only two variables

regarding grandchildren: the age of the youngest grandchild and the age of the oldest

grandchild. Thus, this dataset only allows a limited analysis of grandchildren’s

characteristics.

Sample

The sample in this study was selected from the NSFH wave three dataset. While

the total sample size of the wave three was 9,230, the sample in this study was limited to

the main respondents. In other words, data from the main respondents’ spouses, partners,

sons, and daughters were excluded in this study. Among the 7,277 main interview

respondents, the final sample used in this study consisted of those who answered that

they had at least one grandchild. The variable, RH9, asked how many grandchildren the

respondent had. The researcher filtered out cases that responded to the RH9 with

‘Refused to answer’, ‘Don’t know’, or ‘None’. After the researcher filtered out

49
nongrandparent cases and missing cases, the sample size decreased to 2,541. At the time

of data collection (2003), the mean age of grandparents was approximately 56 years old.

In addition, the youngest grandparent was 36 years old, and the oldest was 105 years old.

It was unnecessary to consider sample weights in this study because the sample

from wave three was no longer a national probability sample. In the wave three, only a

subset of the original sample was interviewed. The subset consisted of main respondents

who were 45 years old and older by January 1, 2001 with no focal children; respondents

who had a focal child aged between 18 years old and 33 years old; spouses or partners of

main respondents in the wave one; and respondents’ eligible focal children who were

between 18 and 33 years old in the wave three. The focal child was randomly selected

among those who were between 13 and 33 years old at wave three no matter whether or

not they were interviewed at wave two.

Constructs and Instruments

Based on the research questions stated in the previous section, six research

hypotheses emerged:

1) The age of both grandparents and grandchildren and the number of grandchildren in

the family affect the number of nights spent with grandchild and the closeness

between the grandparent and the grandchild.

2) The number of nights spent with grandchild and the closeness between the

grandparent and the grandchild are associated with the grandparent’s well-being.

50
3) The grandparent’s functional capacity, grandparent-parent support, and cultural

attitude affect the relationship between the grandparent-grandchild relationship and

the grandparent’s well-being.

4) The grandparent’s well-being is different according to whether the grandparent is

male or female.

5) The grandparent’s well-being is different according to whether the grandparent is

Caucasian American or non-Caucasian American.

6) The grandparent’s well-being is different according to whether the grandparent is

custodial or noncustodial.

Measures of constructs

Six latent variables were included in the study: (1) number of nights spent with

grandchild; (2) closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild; (3) grandparent’s

wellbeing, (4) cultural attitude; (5) grandparent-parent support; and (6) grandparent’s

functional capacity.

Two latent independent variables were separated after the initial analysis, which

indicated that the number of nights spent with the grandchild and the other three observed

variables contradicted each other in terms of describing the relationship with the

grandparent’s well-being. In other words, the number of nights spent with the grandchild

tended to negatively affect the grandparent’s well-being; however, the other three

variables were positively related to the grandparent’s well-being. In addition, a reliability

check also supported two latent independent variables. Cronbach’s alpha was only .055

51
when calculated including all four observed variables as one latent independent variable.

However, when the number of nights spent with the grandchild was separated from the

other three variables, the alpha level increased to .697. Among the four variables, nights

spent with grandchild, frequency of communication, and frequency of meeting were

intended to quantitatively measure the closeness of the grandparent and the grandchild.

On the other hand, the grandparent’s self-rating of closeness was intended to measure the

grandparent-grandchild relationship from the stance of psychological distance.

Among several well-being measures, four instruments were included to address

the grandparent’s well-being. Depression and life satisfaction scales have traditionally

been popular methods of measuring older adults’ well-being in researches. In this study,

however, two measurements, happiness and psychological well-being, were included as

well as depression and life satisfaction because the correlation results of four scales were

all over .40. In addition, the researcher assumed that four different instruments would

analyze the well-being of the grandparent from a more diverse aspect than using fewer

instruments.

Three indicators of cultural attitude used in this study were from a set of cultural

attitude scales. Among them, the researcher chose five variables that were related to

explain the three generational relationships represented in this research. Among those

five, three indicators were ultimately included to measure cultural attitude based on a

reliability test. The functional capacity variable and the grandparent-parent support

variable were from a total set of variables describing two characteristics.

52
Observed variables

Several observed variables were used in this study. For instance, three variables,

the age of the grandparent, the age of the grandchild, and the number of grandchildren in

the family, were included for testing hypothesis one. The age of the grandchild is

measured through two variables, the age of the oldest grandchild and the age of the

youngest grandchild. In addition, for multiple group comparison, the gender of

grandparents, ethnicity of grandparents, and legal custody of grandparents were included.

Although gender and legal custody of grandparents were from the wave three data, the

ethnicity variable was from the wave one dataset. Thus, the researcher merged the wave

three dataset with the wave one dataset in order to include the respondents’ ethnic

backgrounds.

Latent independent variables: Nights spent with grandchild and closeness between the

grandparent and the grandchild

The main latent independent variables were nights spent with grandchild (X1) and

the closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild. The X1 variable, number of

nights the grandparent spent with their grandchild without the parent of the child, was

only one indicator of the latent variable, nights spent with grandchild. X1 was measured

into a continuous variable that ranged from 0 to 365 days. Another latent independent

variable, the closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild, was measured

according to three indicators: (1) frequency of meeting with grandchild (X2), (2)

frequency of communicating with grandchild (X3), and (3) grandparent’s self rating of

53
closeness with grandchild (X4). X2 and X3 were measured into ordinal level data,

whereas X4 was a continuous variable that ranged from 0 (not at all close) to 10

(extremely close). The Cronbach’s alpha test of reliability among X2, X3, and X4

was .697.

Latent dependent variable: Grandparent’s well-being

To measure the main latent dependent variable, grandparent’s well-being, four

observed variables – happiness (Y1), quality of life (Y2), depression (Y3), and

psychological well-being (Y4) – were included as indicators. Cronbach’s alpha of

reliability among Y1, Y2, Y3, and Y4 was .589. Two instruments, happiness and quality

of life, were from the Institute for Social Research at University of Michigan. The

variable RT201 (Y1), measuring how happy the respondents were, ranged from 1 (very

unhappy) to 7 (very happy). It was measured through global positive affect measurement

from the quality of life survey from the Institute for Social Research at the University of

Michigan.

Eleven variables, RT202A~RT202K and quality of life measure (Y2), were also

from the quality of life surveys at the University of Michigan. The measurement scale

was ordinal, ranging from 1 (very dissatisfied) to 7 (very satisfied). While the

measurement originally consisted of 11 items, the researcher excluded one item (RT202K)

for this study. The excluded item was intended to measure how satisfied the respondents

were with their present job. However, the majority of the sample in this study did not

answer this question, whether they worked or not. Thus, it was difficult to identify the

54
missing cases on the RT202K because the respondents did not work at the time of data

collection, the respondents were reluctant to answer the question, or their answer was

randomly missing. Hence, the item RT202K, was excluded from this study. The

researcher conducted the internal consistency reliability test using Cronbach’s alpha; the

alpha value did not change much after excluding RT202K. It was .836 with all eleven

items and .827 after excluding RT202K. In this study, the total summated scores of the 10

items were used to measure the quality of life, Y2. Therefore, the higher the Y2 score, the

more satisfied the respondent is with their life.

Depression, Y3, was measured through a modified version of the Center for

Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D). The measure asked how many days

during the past week the respondent felt depressed for 12 occasions. While the CES-D

was originally coded from 0 (0 days) to 7 (7 days a week), the researcher recoded it from

0 (depressed 7 days a week) to 7 (0 days). Thus, as the Y3 score gets higher, it means the

respondent is less depressed. The CES-D is applied to adults in general, compared to the

Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS), which is designed especially for older adults. Since

the age of this study’s population varies from mid-30s to late-90s, the CES-D may be

appropriate to interpret their level of depression. The original version of the CES-D has

20 self-administered items with an average internal consistency reliability of .92. Test-

retest reliability ranges from .51 to .67 in short intervals and .41 to .54 in longer intervals.

Concurrent validity is the highest with the Hamilton rating scale (.50s~.80s) and varies

from 0.30s to 0.80s with other depression scales (Burns, Lawlor, & Craig, 2004).

Beekman, Deeg, Van Limbeek, Braam, De Vries, and Van Tilburg (1997) examined the

55
criterion validity of the CES-D with an older population, between 55 and 85 years old.

When the cut-off score was set at 16, sensitivity was 100 % and specificity was 87.6 %.

As the cut-off score rose, the sensitivity tended to be lower, yet the specificity became

higher. The modified version, used in NSFH, consisted of 12 self-administered items.

Ross and Mirowsky (1988) implemented the 12-item CES-D in their study, “Child care

and emotional adjustment to wives’ employment,” and the internal consistency was .85

for wives and .81 for husbands. The researcher also tested the internal consistency using

Cronbach’s alpha for this study, and it was .90.

Psychological well-being (Y4) was measured along Carol Ryff’s well-being

scales. The scale consisted of eighteen variables, RT217A through RT217H and RT218A

through RT218J. The original scale ranged from 1 (strongly agree) to 5 (strongly

disagree); however, the researcher recoded it to 1 (strongly disagree) through 5 (strongly

agree) so that as the score gets higher, the respondent’s well-being level gets higher.

According to Ryff (1989), the original instrument consisted of 32 items, 16 positive

statements and 16 negative statements. Its rating scale was a six-point scale, ranging from

strongly agree to strongly disagree. The scale is divided into six sections – (1) self

acceptance, (2) positive relations with others, (3) autonomy, (4) environmental mastery,

(5) purpose in life, and (6) personal growth. The internal consistency reliability of the 20-

item scale varied from .86 to .93 depending upon each section. In addition, the test-retest

reliability of the 20-item scale over a 6-week period ranged from .81 to .88. To measure

validity, the researcher compared this scale with previous measures of both positive and

negative functioning, such as life satisfaction, self esteem, and depression. The results

56
indicated that all the positive functioning items were positively correlated with previous

positive measures, which ranged from .25 to .73. Similarly, correlations with the negative

functioning items were negative, and ranged from -.30 to -.60. The reliability of the 18-

item scale used in NSFH was evaluated again by Ryff in the 1990s. Ryff and Keyes

(1995) stated that the internal consistency of the 18-item scale ranged from .33 to .56.

While the reliability score that Ryff and Keyes yielded was not high, the researcher’s

own reliability score for this study using Cronbach’s alpha was .70.

Latent variable: Cultural attitude

In addition, three observed variables measured the latent variable, cultural attitude.

The three items measuring cultural attitude are from a set of family related attitude items

developed by a team at the University of Wisconsin. The variables asked whether the

respondent thought elderly parents should live with children (C1), whether the respondent

thought children should help older parents with money (C2), and whether the respondent

thought adult children with problems should live with a parent (C3). The respondents’

answers were coded into the five level ordinal data that ranged from strongly disagree to

strongly agree. The Cronbach’s alpha of the reliability among C1, C2, and C3 was .515.

Although the reliability increased to .582 if the C3 was deleted, the researcher decided to

include C3 because the Cronbach’s alpha would not increase enough to reach above .70.

Latent variable: Functional capacity

57
The grandparent’s functional capacity was measured with the total score of

limitations on daily living. It was a set of items asking whether the respondent had

physical and mental limits for daily living. The items consisted of seven questionnaires

that were measured into three ordinal levels: (1) do not limit at all, (2) limit a little, and (3)

limit a lot. The researcher used the total score of the seven items so that the latent

variable, the grandparent’s functional capacity, has only one observed variable.

Latent variable: Grandparent-parent support

The part of the survey that addresses the grandparent-parent support consists of

six dichotomous questions asking whether the grandparent and the parent give and

receive support from each other during a month. Three questions addressed whether the

grandparent provided support to his or her adult child, and another three questions asked

whether the grandparent received support from his or her adult child. The researcher also

combined the six variables into one indicator to the latent variable, grandparent-parent

support, as the indirect measure of the grandparent-parent relationship.

Statistical Analysis

Descriptive statistics of all the variables were run to address sample

characteristics. In detail, descriptions of the four observed variables consisting of two

latent independent variables, the nights spent with grandchild and the closeness between

the grandparent and the grandchild, were analyzed. Also, the range and the mean of the

four observed variables measuring the grandparent’s well-being were presented.

58
Descriptive statistics of each observed variable for measuring cultural attitude,

grandparent-parent support, and grandparent’s functional capacity were also presented. In

addition, the number of grandchildren in the family, the age of both the grandparents and

the grandchild, and the grandparent’s gender, ethnicity, and the presence or absence of

legal custody were also measured in order to identify general characteristics of the study

sample.

This analysis enabled the researcher to identify general characteristics of

grandparents, grandchildren, and families in the sample. In addition, it quantified the

amount of time grandparents usually spend with their grandchildren. By analyzing

correlations between the age of the grandparents, the age of the grandchildren, and the

number of grandchildren, along with variables measuring the closeness between

grandparents and grandchildren, it was possible to figure out which grandparents are

more likely to have a close relationship with their grandchildren.

To test the six hypotheses, Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) was used.

LISREL 8.8 was used for the SEM analysis, while descriptive statistics were analyzed

with SPSS 17.0. Model one was used to test hypothesis one. Hence, four independent

variables and two dependent variables were included in model one. The four independent

variables were (1) the number of grandchildren in family; (2) the grandparent’s age; (3)

the youngest grandchild’s age; and (4) the oldest grandparent’s age. The number of nights

spent with the grandchild and the closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild

were included as the dependent variables.

59
Model two is related to hypothesis test two, which is intended to measure two

grandparent-grandchild interaction variables and their impact on the grandparent’s well-

being. Thus, three latent variables, (1) nights spent with grandchild; (2) closeness

between the grandparent and the grandchild; and (3) grandparent’s well-being, were

included in the model two.

Model three had three more variables added to the three variables included in the

model two. The researcher intended to measure the effect of cultural attitude and the

grandparent-parent support on the relationships among the number of nights spent with

the grandchild, the closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild, and the

grandparent’s well-being. In addition, the grandparent’s functional capacity was included

in the model three in order to address its impact on the grandparent’s well-being.

Three multigroup comparison models were applied in this study to test hypotheses

four through six. The variables included in three multigroup models were the same as

those used in the model three. However, model four focused on gender difference: the

researcher tested whether grandmothers and grandfathers had statistically significantly

different coefficients for the path among the number of nights spent with the grandchild,

the closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild, and the grandparent’s well-

being. Model five tested the impact of ethnic differences in the model. The ethnicity

variable was dichotomized to Caucasian American grandparents and non-Caucasian

American grandparents. The last multigroup model comparison, model six, measured

differences between custodial grandparents and noncustodial grandparents in terms of the

paths among the number of nights spent with grandchild, the closeness between the

60
grandparent and the grandchild, and the grandparent’s well-being. As supplements to the

multigroup comparison, T-test results were also presented in order to address differences

of gender, ethnicity, and custody of the grandparents.

61
Chapter 5: Research Findings

Initial Analyses

Sample demographics are shown in Table 5.1. through Table 5.8. below. As

described in Table 5.1., grandparents spent nights with their grandchildren for about three

weeks on average. However, the number of nights spent with the grandchild varied

depending upon the grandparents’ characteristics; thus, some grandparents actually lived

with their grandchild while others had not spent a night with their grandchildren for a

year. Grandparents tended to meet or communicate with their grandchildren quite

frequently; 33.1% of grandparents answered that they meet with their grandchild more

than once a week, followed by several times a year (20.9%), 1 to 3 times per month

(19.7%), and about once a week (17.2%). Approximately 25% of grandparents reported

communicating with their grandchildren more than once a week, and 21.5% of

grandparents answered that they communicated with their grandchild more than once a

month. Lastly, grandparents tended to think that they were very close to their

grandchildren: more than 50% of grandparents in this study thought that they were

extremely close to their grandchild, and more than 30% of the grandparent respondents

answered that they were relatively close to their grandchild.

62
Frequency %
Nights spent with grandchild last year (X1)
Mean 22.05 days
Median 2 days
Minimum 0 day
Maximum 365 days
Frequency of meeting grandchild per year (X2)
Not at all 80 3.2
About once a year 138 5.5
Several times a year 520 20.9
1 to 3 times a month 490 19.7
About once a week 429 17.2
More than once a week 826 33.1
Grandchild lives with grandparent 10 0.4
Frequency of communication with grandchild per year (X3)
Not at all 196 8.8
About once a year 68 3.0
Several times a year 446 19.9
1 to 3 times a month 482 21.5
About once a week 468 20.9
More than once a week 572 25.6
Grandchild lives with grandparent 6 0.3
Closeness with grandchild (X4)
Not at all 30 1.2
1-5 231 9.3
6-9 879 35.3
Extremely close (10) 1350 54.2
Table 5.1. Closeness between the Grandparent and the Grandchild

Table 5.2. shows grandparents’ well-being scores. Happiness scores ranged from

1 to 7, where 7 is very happy. Hence, grandparents in the sample tended to feel that they
63
are happy about their overall life. Quality of life scores ranged from 7, very dissatisfied to

all 10 items, to 70, very satisfied to all 10 items. As the mean score of the quality of life

measure is 55.26, the respondents in this study were relatively satisfied their overall life.

Depression was measured with scores from 0 to 84. 0 means the respondent felt

depressed on every item seven days a week, whereas 84 means the respondent never felt

depression on any item. While depression score ranges were wider compared to other

well-being measures, the grandparents in the sample tended not to be depressed. Lastly,

psychological well-being scores range from 0 to 90, where 90 means the respondent’s

psychological well-being is very good. Compared to happiness, quality of life, and

depression scores, grandparents’ psychological well-being seems to be lower. However,

the overall psychological well-being of the grandparents was not low, either.

Mean Std.Dev Minimum Maximum


Happiness (Y1) 5.69 1.25 1 7
Quality of life (Y2) 55.79 9.11 10 70
Depression (Y3) 70.38 15.68 0 84
Psychological well-being (Y4) 66.80 5.89 39 86
Table 5.2. Grandparent’s Well-being

Grandparents’ cultural attitudes toward family related issues are shown in Table

5.3. Interestingly, more than half of the grandparents answered that elder parents should

live with their adult children. Also, the majority of the grandparents expected financial

64
support from their child as they got older. In addition, more grandparents thought adult

children with problems should live with parents. Grandparents in this study tended to

emphasize family interactions and open exchanges of support among family members.

Frequency %
Elder parents should live with (adult) kids (C1)
Strongly disagree 40 1.6
Disagree 621 25.0
Neither agree nor disagree 502 20.2
Agree 1,215 49.0
Strongly agree 102 4.1
Child should help older parent with money (C2)
Strongly disagree 16 0.6
Disagree 355 14.2
Neither agree nor disagree 358 14.3
Agree 1,641 65.7
Strongly agree 128 5.1
Adult kids with problems can live with parent (C3)
Strongly disagree 54 2.2
Disagree 727 29.1
Neither agree nor disagree 437 17.5
Agree 1,227 49.1
Strongly agree 56 2.2
Table 5.3. Cultural Attitude

Table 5.4. shows how much grandparents gave and took support from their adult

children during the last month. The percentage was 80.3% of the grandparents who
65
answered that they provided emotional support to their adult child. In addition,

grandparents tended to help their adult children with childcare. In other words, 30% of

the grandparents provided childcare support for their adult child while he or she worked.

Also, almost 40% of the grandparents provided childcare support while their adult child

was not working. The grandparents tended to provide more help than they received from

their adult child. For instance, less than 30% of the grandparents received help with

shopping or transportation from their adult child during a month. Also, more than 70% of

the grandparents tended to do their house work by themselves without help from their

child.

(Yes) Frequency %
Grandparent gave emotional support to adult child
(GPPS1) 2,033 80.3

Grandparent helped adult child with childcare while


762 30.0
working (GPPS2)
Grandparent helped adult child with childcare while not
979 38.6
working (GPPS3)
Grandparent received help with shopping, running
696 27.4
errands or transportation (GPPS4)
Grandparent received help with housework, yard work,
669 26.4
car repairs, or other work around the house (GPPS5)
Grandparent received emotional support from adult
1,432 56.5
children (GPPS6)
Table 5.4. Grandparent-parent Support (During last month)

66
Grandparents did, however, tend to receive more emotional support from their

adult child. In addition, it may be important to point that it may not be the same

population who provided child care support to their adult child with those who received

help with shopping or transportation.

Grandparent’s functional capacity is shown in Table 5.5. Most of the grandparents

in the study did not have physical and/or mental limitations to perform daily living skills.

On the other hand, it is assumed that due to the age, more than 40% of grandparents felt

that doing heavy work was difficult for them.

(Don’t limit at all) Frequency %


For personal needs, such as bathing, dressing 2,327 91.7
To move about inside the house 2,224 87.6
For day to day tasks 2,004 79.1
For climbing stairs 1,845 72.8
To walk six blocks 1,780 70.5
To do heavy work 1,369 54.5
To work 1,474 76.4
Table 5.5. Grandparent’s Functional Capacity: Physical and/or mental limits

Table 5.6. shows how many grandchildren grandparents had in their family.

Grandparents in this study had average five grandchildren in their family. While most

grandparents had less than 10 grandchildren, 13 grandparents had more than 30

grandchildren in their family.

67
Mean 5.06
Median 4
Minimum 1
Maximum 30
Frequency %
1~9 2,228 87.7
10~19 270 10.6
20~29 30 1.2
30 or more 13 0.5

Table 5.6. Number of Grandchildren

The ages of the grandparent and the grandchild are shown in Table 5.7.

Grandparents’ age varied from the 30’s to the 90’s. Grandparents in their 50’s were the

majority population in this study. Then, grandparents between 60 and 69 years old

followed, with 27%. While most of the grandparents were 50 years old and over, there

were 15 grandparents who were less than 40 years old.

Compared to the grandparent’s age, the ages of the grandchildren were limited to

the youngest and the oldest grandchildren. While the majority of the grandparents

answered their youngest grandchild was under 5 years old, more than 6% of the

grandparents had youngest grandchildren over 20 years old.

68
Frequency %
Age of the grandparent
Under 39 15 .6
40~49 281 11.1
50~59 814 32.1
60~69 685 27.0
70~79 453 17.9
80~89 258 10.2
90 or older 31 1.2
Age of the youngest grandchild
Under 1 year old 503 20.0
1~5 1159 46.0
6~12 493 19.6
13~19 204 8.1
20~29 117 4.6
30~39 40 1.6
40 or older 5 .2
Age of the oldest grandchild
Under 1 year old 82 3.3
1~5 460 18.2
6~12 712 28.2
13~19 498 19.7
20~29 505 20.0
30~39 212 8.4
40 or older 54 2.1
Table 5.7. Age of Grandparents and Grandchildren

For the multiple group comparison, three variables, grandparent’s gender,

grandparent’s ethnicity, and legal custody of their grandchild, were included. Gender of

69
the grandparents was drawn from the roster one data of wave three. As shown in Table

5.8., there were more grandmothers than grandfathers in the sample. The data of

grandparents’ ethnicity were from wave one; therefore, sample size was down to 1,660

after the researcher excluded missing values. Among 1,660 grandparents, more than 80%

were Caucasian American. In addition, approximately 14% of the grandparent

respondents had ever had primary responsibility for their grandchild at the time of data

collection.

Frequency %
Gender
Male 833 32.8
Female 1,708 67.2
Ethnicity of grandparents
Caucasian 1,351 81.4
Non Caucasian 309 18.6
African American 239 14.4
Hispanic 62 3.7
American Indian 5 .3
Asian 3 .2
Ever had primary responsibility for grandchild
Yes 366 14.4
No 2,171 85.6
Table 5.8. Demographics of Grandparents

70
Hypotheses Testing with Structural Equation Modeling

The researcher assessed the model fit between the hypothesized model and the

sample data for all models included in this study. The model fit was measured through

Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR), Goodness of Fit Index (GFI), and

Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) because chi square statistics are

influenced by large sample size. The SRMR values below .05, GFI values above .90, and

RMSEA values below .05 are often considered as an acceptable fit (Schumacker &

Lomax, 2004). Most of the models tested in this study were within these criteria. To

examine relations among latent variables and between latent variables and observed

variables, the standardized path coefficients of each model were also assessed.

Statistically significant paths (p<.05) are indicated as solid lines, whereas nonsignificant

paths are shown as dotted lines in Figures 5.1. through 5.9.

Throughout the models tested in this study, two measures of the grandparent-

grandchild relationship consistently had inverse relationships to the grandparents’ well-

being. In other words, the path from the nights spent with grandchild to the grandparent’s

well-being was negative. However, the closeness between the grandparent and the

grandchild positively influenced the grandparent’s well-being. Thus, it is assumed that

the closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild may affect grandparents

positively up to a certain point, after which it becomes a burden for grandparents.

Grandparents may feel positively about their close relationship with their grandchild. As

a result, the grandparents’ well-being tends to increase when they feel that they have a

close relationship with their grandchild. However, when grandparents frequently stay

71
overnight with their grandchild without the parent of the child, grandparents seem to feel

burdened, which leads to a decrease in their well-being.

In general, cultural attitude and grandparent-parent support also affected the

grandparents’ well-being negatively across all the models. For instance, those who

actively gave and took support with their adult child had lower well-being scores than

those who rarely exchanged support with their adult child. In addition, the closeness

between the grandparent and the grandchild had a positive impact on the cultural attitude

throughout every model. In contrast, the relationship between the nights spent with the

grandchild and cultural attitude was either positive or negative depending upon the

sample characteristics.

Hypothesis Test 1

Hypothesis one was to test if the age of both grandparents and grandchildren and

the number of grandchildren in the family affect the number of nights spent with

grandchild and the closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild. To test this

hypothesis, a multiple indicators and multiple causes (MIMIC) model with two latent

variables and four observed variables was applied to the model one. Total cases included

in the model were 1,822 after excluding missing values on each variable. In Figure 5.1.,

‘No.GC’ indicates the number of grandchild the grandparent had in family. ‘GP Age’

refers to the grandparent’s age. ‘GCY’ means the age of the youngest grandchild and

‘GCO’ means the age of the oldest grandchild. The model fit was within the acceptable

72
range. Chi square was 20.55 with a degree of freedom of 8, which resulted in a p-value of

less than .05.

Standardized
Paths
Estimates
Number of grandchild in family (No.GC) ->Nights spent with GC .09
Age of the grandparent (GP Age) -> Nights spent with GC -.15
Age of the youngest grandchild (GCY) -> Nights spent with GC .03 (NS)
Age of the oldest grandchild (GCO) -> Nights spent with GC -.03 (NS)
Number of grandchild in family -> Closeness between GP & GC .12
Age of the grandparent -> Closeness between GP & GC -.17
Age of the youngest grandchild -> Closeness between GP & GC -.08
Age of the oldest grandchild -> Closeness between GP & GC .03 (NS)
Goodness of Fit Indexes
SRMR .014
GFI 1.000
RMSEA .029
Chi square (df=8) 20.55
Note. NS: Not Significant (p=.05)
Table 5.9. Structural Model Result

In terms of path coefficients, all paths were significant, except for the three paths

shown in Table 5.9. The number of grandchildren the grandparent had in the family and

the grandparent’s age were significantly related to the number of nights spent with the

grandchild. That is, the more grandchildren a respondent had, the more likely that

grandparent was to stay overnight with those grandchildren. In addition, the older the

grandparent, the less likely they were to stay overnight with their grandchild. The number

73
of grandchildren the grandparent had in the family, the grandparent’s age, and the age of

the youngest grandchild were statistically related to the closeness between the

grandparent and the grandchild. In other words, the more grandchildren a respondent had,

the more likely the grandparent was to be close with their grandchild. Moreover, when

both grandparents and grandchildren were younger, they were more likely to be close to

each other than older grandparents with their grandchildren. The age of the oldest

grandchild was not significantly related to either nights spent with the grandchild or

closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild.

74
75
Hypothesis Test 2

Hypothesis two was to determine whether the number of nights spent with

grandchild and the closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild are associated

with the grandparents’ well-being. To test hypothesis two, three latent variables and eight

indicators were included in the model two. One of the latent independent variables was

the nights spent with grandchild. Another independent variable was the closeness

between the grandparent and the grandchild. The dependent latent variable in the model

was the grandparents’ well-being. The sample size used in the model was 1,434 after the

researcher excluded missing values on each indicator. The model fit was within the

acceptable range, except for RMSEA. SRMR value was .037, GFI value was .99, and

RMSEA value was .054. Chi square value was 81.79 with a degree of freedom of 16;

therefore, the p-value was below .05.

Standardized estimates of path coefficients are shown in Figure 5.2. All paths

were significant at the alpha level of .05. The more frequently grandparents stayed

overnight with their grandchild, the lower their well-being was. On the other hand, the

closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild was positively related to the

grandparents’ well-being.

76
77
Hypothesis Test 3

Hypothesis three was “The grandparent’s functional capacity, grandparent-parent

support, and cultural attitude affect the grandparent-grandchild relationship and the

grandparents’ well-being.”

To test hypothesis three, six latent variables were included in the model. As in

model two, the independent variables were the number of nights spent with the

grandchild and the closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild. The dependent

variable, grandparents’ well-being, also remained the same as in model two. Two latent

variables, cultural attitude and grandparent-parent support, were included in the model

three to test mediating effects. Lastly, the grandparent’s functional capacity was included

as a control variable to the grandparents’ well-being. The total sample size in this model

was 1,396. All of the path coefficients shown in Table 5.10. were significant at the alpha

level of .05. The model fit was within an acceptable range, as is shown in Table 5.10.

However, the chi square value was significant, possibly due to the large sample size.

78
Factor Loadings Standardized Estimates
Nights spent with grandchild
X1 1.00
Closeness between GP & GC
X2 .76
X3 .64
X4 .64
Grandparent’s well-being
Y1 .65
Y2 .93
Y3 .77
Y4 .55
Cultural attitude
C1 .76
C2 .56
C3 .29
Grandparent-parent support
GPPS 1.00
Grandparent’s functional capacity
FC 1.00
Goodness of Fit Indexes
SRMR .040
GFI .980
RMSEA .044
Chi square (df=56) 205.51
Table 5.10. Measurement Model Result (H3)

Figure 5.3. indicates that the number of nights spent with the grandchild was not

statistically related to cultural attitude or grandparent-parent support. However, compared

to model two, cultural attitude and grandparent-parent support decreased the negative

relationship between the number of nights spent with the grandchild and the grandparents’

well-being. In addition, the two mediating factors increased the positive relationship

between the closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild and the grandparents’

79
well-being. Although the closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild was

highly positively correlated with grandparent-parent support, grandparent-parent support

negatively affected the grandparents’ well-being. Similarly, a positive relationship was

illustrated between the closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild and cultural

attitude; however, the relation between cultural attitude and the grandparents’ well-being

was negative. The same patterns appeared in the relationship between the number of

nights spent with grandchild, cultural attitude, grandparent-parent support, and

grandparents’ well-being, although the paths from the number of nights spent with the

grandchild to cultural attitude and grandparent-parent support were not statistically

significant.

80
81
Hypothesis Test 4

Hypothesis test four addresses whether the grandparents’ well-being is different

according to whether the grandparent is male or female. The same six latent variables

were included in the test of the fourth hypothesis as were in hypothesis test three.

However, grandparents’ gender variable was included in this test to address gender

differences in the model. The total sample size in this model was 1,396: 870

grandmothers and 526 grandfathers. All the path coefficients shown below in Table 5.11.

were significant at the alpha level of .05. Also, both the grandmothers’ model fit and the

grandfathers’ model fit were within an acceptable range, as is shown in Table 5.11.

However, similarly to the model three, the chi square value was significant.

As described in Figure 5.4., in the grandmothers’ model, paths from the number

of nights spent with grandchild to cultural attitude and the grandparent-parent support

were not statistically significant. The grandfathers’ SEM model shown in Figure 5.5. had

four paths that were not statistically significant at the alpha level of .05. Hence, cultural

attitude did not affect the relationship between the closeness between the grandparent and

the grandchild or the grandparents’ well-being among grandfathers, while it did influence

the relationship among grandmothers.

The grandmothers’ well-being was more negatively influenced by the number of

nights spent with the grandchild. In addition, the impact of the closeness between the

grandparent and the grandchild on the grandparents’ well-being was lower among

grandmothers than among grandfathers. In addition, grandparents’ functional capacity

had a slightly stronger impact on grandmothers’ well-being than on grandfathers’ well-

82
being. The grandparent-parent support had a negative influence on the well-being of both

grandfathers and grandmothers.

Standardized Estimates
Factor Loadings
Grandmothers Grandfathers
Nights spent with grandchild
X1 1.00 1.00
Closeness between GP & GC
X2 .76 .74
X3 .61 .66
X4 .60 .67
Grandparent’s well-being
Y1 .67 .60
Y2 .92 .98
Y3 .79 .68
Y4 .58 .48
Cultural attitude
C1 .79 .75
C2 .63 .45
C3 .27 .33
Grandparent-parent support
GPPS 1.00 .27
Grandparent’s functional capacity
FC 1.00 1.00
Goodness of Fit Indexes
SRMR .043 .041
GFI .980 .970
RMSEA .042 .034
Chi square (df=56) 141.23 90.82
Table 5.11. Measurement Model Result (H4)

83
84
85
The T-test results for testing differences of well-being scores between

grandfathers and grandmothers were as follows: happiness (Y1) was t (1,889)=1.881, p

>.05; the quality of life (Y2) was t (1,889)=1.566, p >.05; depression (Y3) was t

(1,889)=7.521, p<.001; and psychological well-being (Y4) was t (1,889)=0.962, p >.05.

Depression was the only category that was statistically significant across gender,

indicating that grandmothers were more depressed than grandfathers.

Hypothesis Test 5

Hypothesis test five was to measure if the grandparents’ well-being is different

according to whether the grandparent is Caucasian American or non-Caucasian American.

Hypothesis test five also had used the same six latent variables as hypothesis tests three

and four. In order to identify ethnic differences, 717 Caucasian American grandparents

and 165 non-Caucasian American grandparents were included in the models. The total

sample size was decreased from 1,396 to 882 due to the missing values on the ethnicity

variable drawn from wave 1 data.

All the path coefficients shown below in Table 5.12. were significant at the alpha

level of .05. Caucasian American grandparents and non-Caucasian American

grandparents had similar patterns of factor loadings. However, Caucasian American

grandparents had the highest factor loading on frequency of meeting, X2, whereas non-

Caucasian American grandparents had the highest factor loading on frequency of

communication, X3. Similarly, in terms of the grandparents’ well-being measure, quality

86
of life, Y2, had the highest factor loading among Caucasian American grandparents,

while depression, Y3, had the highest factor loading among non-Caucasian American

grandparents.

Standardized Estimates
Factor Loadings
Caucasian GP Non-Caucasian GP
Nights spent with grandchild
X1 1.00 1.00
Closeness between GP & GC
X2 .76 .67
X3 .64 .74
X4 .69 .52
Grandparent’s well-being
Y1 .68 .35
Y2 .95 .53
Y3 .80 .87
Y4 .57 .57
Cultural attitude
C1 .78 1.00
C2 .52 .25
C3 .32 .24
Grandparent-parent support
GPPS 1.00 1.00
Grandparent’s functional capacity
FC 1.00 1.00
Goodness of Fit Indexes
SRMR .048 .056
GFI .970 .940
RMSEA .045 .024
135.99 63.68
Chi square
(df=56) (df=58)
Table 5.12. Measurement Model Result (H5)

87
The model fits for both the Caucasian American and the non-Caucasian American

grandparents’ models that are described in Table 5.12. Although GFI and RMSEA values

for both models were in an acceptable range, the SRMR value of the non-Caucasian

American grandparents’ models was higher than .05. The SRMR value did not decrease

to the acceptable level after model modifications. On the other hand, the chi square value

of the non-Caucasian American grandparents’ model was not significant (p=.28). The

degrees of freedom of each model differed because the Caucasian American grandparents’

model had two more measurement error covariances than the non-Caucasian American

grandparents’ model.

Group comparison results between Caucasian American grandparents and non-

Caucasian American grandparents are shown above in Figure 5.6. and Figure 5.7. The

Caucasian American grandparents’ model had significant paths between the nights spent

with the grandchild and the grandparents’ well-being, and between the closeness between

the grandparent and the grandchild and the grandparents’ well-being. On the other hand,

two paths were not significant in the non-Caucasian American grandparents’ model. The

Caucasian American grandparents’ model demonstrated the same patterns among the

number of nights spent with the grandchild, the closeness between the grandparent and

the grandchild, and the grandparents’ well-being as those in the model three. In other

words, their well-being tended to decrease as they spent more nights with their grandchild.

However, the closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild positively influenced

the grandparents’ level of well-being. In addition, the grandparent-parent support had a

positive relationship with the closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild,

88
while the grandparents’ well-being tended to decrease as they exchanged support and

help more frequently with their adult child. The closeness between the grandparent and

the grandchild was positively related to the cultural attitude as well, although cultural

attitude was not statistically significantly related to the grandparents’ well-being. Cultural

attitude did not affect the relationship between the number of nights spent with the

grandchild and the grandparents’ well-being.

Compared to the Caucasian American grandparents’ model, the non-Caucasian

American grandparents’ model was limited in its ability to explain the relationships

among the six latent variables, possibly due to the small sample size. Although most

paths were not significant, non-Caucasian American grandparents tended to be influenced

more from a functional capacity regarding their level of well-being. In addition, the

closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild was less likely to relate to the

grandparents’ well-being compared to the path in the Caucasian American grandparents’

model. All other paths were not significant.

The T-test results are as follows: happiness (Y1) was t (1,219)=-0.561, p >.05; the

quality of life (Y2) was t (1,219)=2.294, p=.022; depression (Y3) was t (1,219)=5.674,

p<.001; and psychological well-being (Y4) was t (1,219)=-0.692, p>.05. Non-Caucasian

American grandparents had higher scores on happiness and psychological well-being,

although the results were not significant. In contrast, Caucasian American grandparents

scored higher on quality of life and level of depression, and the results were statistically

significant.

89
90
91
Hypothesis Test 6

Lastly, hypothesis test six was to identify whether grandparents’ well-being is

different according to whether the grandparent is custodial or noncustodial. This testing

was related to the legal custody of grandparents. The same six latent variables were

included in models three, four, and five. The total sample size in hypothesis test six was

1,396. Among 1,396 grandparents, noncustodial grandparents were 1,198 and custodial

grandparents were 198.

The standardized path coefficient results and the model fit were shown above in

Table 5.13. Except for the path of the three cultural attitude variables in the custodial

grandparents’ model, all other paths were statistically significant at the alpha level of .05.

Similar patterns of factor loading were shown in the noncustodial grandparents’ model

and the custodial grandparent’s model. In other words, frequency of meeting with

grandchild, X2, was the highest factor loading in both noncustodial and custodial models.

However, Y2, quality of life, had the highest factor loadings in the noncustodial

grandparents’ model while Y3, depression, was the highest in the custodial grandparents’

model. The model fit of noncustodial grandparents was within an acceptable range;

however, the p-value of the chi square was below .05. The model of the custodial

grandparents had .067 for its SRMR and a significant chi square value, while GFI and

RMSEA values were within the acceptable range.

92
Standardized Estimates
Factor Loadings
Custodial GP Noncustodial GP
Nights spent with grandchild
X1 1.00 1.00
Closeness between GP & GC
X2 .78 .75
X3 .63 .63
X4 .46 .66
Grandparent’s well-being
Y1 .56 .64
Y2 .59 .71
Y3 .84 .69
Y4 .52 .59
Cultural attitude
C1 .64 (NS) .77
C2 .76 (NS) .55
C3 .15 (NS) .32
Grandparent-parent support
GPPS 1.00 1.00
Grandparent’s functional capacity
FC 1.00 1.00
Goodness of Fit Indexes
SRMR .067 .040
GFI .940 .970
RMSEA .050 .046
Chi square (df=57) 91.98 201.49
Note. NS: Not Significant (p=.05)
Table 5.13. Measurement Model Result (H6)

Group comparison between custodial grandparents and noncustodial grandparents

is shown in Figure 5.8. and Figure 5.9. It was consistent in the noncustodial grandparents’

models that the path from the number of nights spent with the grandchild to the

grandparents’ well-being was negative, whereas the path from the closeness between the

grandparent and the grandchild to the grandparents’ well-being was positive. However, in
93
the custodial grandparents’ model, both paths had negative impacts on the grandparents’

well-being, although these paths were not significant. The path from the closeness

between the grandparent and the grandchild to the grandparents’ well-being was

statistically significant in the noncustodial grandparents’ model; however, it was not

significant in the custodial grandparents’ model.

The T-test results are as follows: happiness (Y1) was t (1,887)=-2.743, p=.006;

quality of life (Y2) was t (1,887)=-4.265, p<.001; depression (Y3) was t (1,887)=-4.922,

p<.001; and psychological well-being (Y4) was t (1,887)=-2.335, p=.020. Because

noncustodial grandparents tended to score higher on all four measurements of well-being,

it is assumed that the well-being of non-custodial grandparents was higher than custodial

grandparents.

94
95
96
Chapter 6: Discussion

Limitations of the Study

In this study, there are limitations regarding variables not included in the analysis.

The first limitation is the lack of information regarding grandchildren. As mentioned

earlier in the method section, the dataset used in this study does not include each

grandchild’s age. Thus, the researcher used the age of the oldest grandchild and the age

of the youngest grandchild to determine the influence of age on the closeness between the

grandparent and the grandchild. The theoretical framework of this study is based on a life

course perspective and Bowen family systems theory. The life course perspective

emphasizes changes in individuals’ roles in the family as they age. In addition, the life

course perspective addresses how individual time, generational time, and historical time

influence individuals’ family life. In this respect, the age of the grandchild may be a

crucial factor in analyzing the relationship between the grandparent and the grandchild.

Although the age of the youngest grandchild may be related to the age of the oldest

grandchild and the age of the grandparent, a more accurate analysis would result if the

age of each grandchild were included in the SEM model.

Another limitation is that there were no variables available for analyzing the

relationship between the grandchild and the parent of the child. As this study’s theoretical

framework is based on Bowen family systems theory, the main purpose of the study was

to examine whether the triangular relationship among the grandparent, parent, and

97
grandchild influences the well-being of the grandparent. Although the study’s focus is on

the relationship between the grandparent and the grandchild, two other aspects (parent-

grandchild relationship and grandparent-parent relationship) are important factors that

comprise the triangular relationship between the grandparent, parent, and grandchild.

However, variables measuring the parent-grandchild relationship were not available,

although there were variables available to determine the grandparent-grandchild

relationship and the grandparent-parent support. Therefore, it was not possible to address

how the parent-grandchild relationship affects the grandparent-grandchild relationship

and the well-being of the grandparent.

In addition, it was not possible to include the status of the grandparent-parent

relationship and the parent-grandchild relationship in the analysis. In other words, Bowen

family systems theory assumes that the triangular relationship decreases the anxiety of

the relationship between any two of the constituent elements (grandparent, parent, and

grandchild) by seeking to establish a sort of balance. Hence, tension or anxiety in the

grandparent-parent relationship may affect the grandparent-grandchild relationship as

well. Specifically, the frequency of communication and/or meetings between the

grandparent and the grandchild may be closely related to the relationship between the

grandparent and the parent. For instance, the parent whose relationship with his or her

older parent is not close may be less likely to bring his or her child to that older

grandparent than those whose relationship with their older parent is good. The status of

the triangular relationship among the grandparent, parent, and grandchild is dependent

upon whether the status of each relationship is positive or negative. On the other hand, in

98
this study, the researcher was not able to assess whether each relationship was positive or

negative. Hence, it was not possible to ascertain why some grandparents rarely give

support to and receive support from their adult child, while others do frequently.

Not including the physical location of the grandparents is another limitation of

this study. In other words, the frequency of communication and/or meetings with the

grandchild may be closely related to the physical distance between the grandparent and

the grandchild. It is easily assumed that the grandparent who lives next to the grandchild

may have more chances to meet his or her grandchild than those who live in other states.

While physical distance often influences emotional distance between people, the

variables examining such aspects were not available in the dataset used in this study.

Therefore, it would be better to examine the differences in the grandparent-grandchild

relationship between grandparents who live close to their grandchild and those who live

farther away.

The lack of ethnic diversity in the subjects of this study is also one of its

limitations. In the sample, non-Caucasian American grandparents were mostly African

American. Among 309 non-Caucasian American grandparents, 239 (77%) were African

American grandparents. However, there were only five American Indian grandparents

and three Asian American grandparents. Hence, the comparison between Caucasian

American grandparents and non-Caucasian American grandparents was limited, as those

non-Caucasian American grandparents included did not have a large enough non-African

American grandparent population.

99
Lastly, the dataset is missing many values for variables that asked about economic

status. Previous studies have shown that grandparents’ well-being depends on their

economic status. The dataset has variables such as hours worked per week, gross income,

whether the respondents worked full-time or part-time and so on. Although these

variables included a sufficient number of cases, after filtering out those cases that

included at least one grandchild in the family, the number of cases became too small to

include in the SEM model.

Summary of the Study

Six hypotheses were tested in this study. The first hypothesis was to address the

relationship between grandparents’ and grandchildren’s life courses and their closeness to

each other. While the grandparent’s age affected two closeness latent variables (the

number of nights spent with grandchild and the closeness between the grandparent and

the grandchild), the age of the grandchildren was not significant to the two direct

measures of closeness. The number of grandchildren in the family was statistically

significant to the closeness variables. Two indicators out of four significantly affected the

two closeness latent variables, the number of nights spent with grandchild and the

closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild. The age of the youngest

grandchild was significantly, but inversely related to the closeness between the

grandparent and the grandchild; however, it did not affect the number of nights spent

with the grandchild. Thus, the grandchild is less likely to be close with the grandparent as

he or she gets older.

100
Although the life course perspective assumes that individuals tend to have a

different family interaction based on their life course, as shown in Figure 6.1., this study

only confirmed that the grandparents’ life course influenced the closeness with their

grandchildren. There are three life courses – the grandparent’s life course, the parent’s

life course, and the grandchild’s life course. Each individual’s life course, which inter-

correlates with every other family member’s life course, makes a whole family life course

that is unique to each family. For instance, the life course of the 19-year-old parent who

has a 2-year-old child may be totally different from that of 35-year-old parent who also

has a 2-year-old child. These two different parent life courses would result in different

life courses for their children, even though the two children are in the same generation. In

the same way, life courses of the grandparents of these two children would be different.

As described above, life courses of grandparents, parents, and grandchildren

interact with each other; however, in this study, grandparents’ life courses and their effect

on the grandparent-grandchild relationship were addressed. While the grandparent’s age

was highly correlated with the age of the oldest grandchild, the oldest grandchild’s age

was not statistically related to the two closeness latent variables. Also, younger

grandparents tended to have a closer relationship with their grandchild compared to older

grandparents. Thus, the first hypothesis was partially confirmed by this study.

101
Individual time
Generational time

Age of
grandparent

Age of IV 1: Nights spent with


youngest GC grandchild

Age of oldest IV 2: Closeness between


GC
grandparent &
grandchild

Number of
grandchildren

Family characteristics DV: Grandparent’s


well-being

Figure 6.1. Key Concepts of Life Course Perspective and Variables in This Study

The second hypothesis was fully confirmed by this study, in that the number of

nights spent with the grandchild and the closeness between the grandparent and the

grandchild were significantly associated with the grandparent’s well-being. In addition,

indicators of the three latent variables were also statistically significant. Thus,

grandparents who spent more nights with their grandchild tended to have lower well-

being compared to those who spent fewer nights with their grandchild. On the other hand,

grandparents who had more frequent communication and meetings with their grandchild

102
tended to have higher well-being compared to those who had less contact with their

grandchild.

Hypothesis three was to test whether the grandparent’s functional capacity,

grandparent-parent support, and cultural attitude affected the grandparent-grandchild

relationship and the grandparent’s well-being. This hypothesis was confirmed by the

study: the grandparent’s functional capacity, grandparent-parent support, and cultural

attitude indirectly increased the positive score of the closeness between the grandparent

and the grandchild. Similarly, these factors indirectly decreased the negative score

between number of nights spent with the grandchild and the grandparent’s well-being.

However, direct effects of cultural attitude and the grandparent-parent support on the

grandparent’s well-being were both negative.

As hypotheses three through six included the grandparent-parent support variable,

it was possible to address whether the triangular relationship in Bowen family systems

theory might be applied to this study, as shown in Figure 6.2. Bowen family systems

theory assumes that when a third person is added to an anxious relationship between two

people, the stress of that third person increases as the anxiety between two people

decreases. The result of this study partially supports the first assumption – the stress of

the third person (grandparents) increases. After testing hypothesis three, it was confirmed

that as grandparents and parents exchanged support more frequently, the grandparent’s

well-being decreased. In addition, as grandparents spent more nights with the grandchild,

the grandparents’ well-being decreased. On the other hand, a higher score for the

closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild, measuring the frequency of

103
communication and meetings between the grandparent and the grandchild, resulted in a

higher score for the grandparent’s well-being. This pattern was consistent when testing

hypotheses four, five, and six. This means that the grandparent may not feel stressed until

he or she needs to exert great effort for the grandchild.

In addition, it was confirmed that positive grandparent-parent support resulted in a

higher level of the grandparent’s well-being. However, it was not possible to address

whether the anxiety between the parent and the grandchild decreases as the grandparent

spends more nights and/or communicates more frequently with the grandchild. This kind

of analysis would be feasible if there were variables measuring the anxiety between

family members and/or the parent-grandchild relationship.

Grandchild Parent

IV 1: Nights spent with


grandchild Grandparent-parent
support
IV 2: Closeness between
grandparent &
grandchild
Grandparent

DV: Grandparent’s
well-being

Figure 6.2. Key Concepts of Bowen Family Systems Theory and Variables in This Study
104
The fourth hypothesis was to examine the difference between grandmothers and

grandfathers. As described in the results section, patterns of the paths among the

grandparents’ well-being, the number of nights spent with the grandchild, and the

closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild were similar between

grandmothers and grandfathers. On the other hand, the invariance test using chi square

demonstrated that grandmothers and grandfathers were heterogeneous in their

characteristics; thus, it was impossible to further analyze differences in the variances and

associations among the latent variables.

The fifth hypothesis, which was to address differences between Caucasian

American grandparents and non-Caucasian American grandparents, was partially

confirmed by this study. Although Caucasian American grandparents and non-Caucasian

American grandparents were not invariant, the patterns of the major paths were similar.

On the other hand, the SEM model used in this study was limited in its ability to explain

results for non-Caucasian American grandparents. The majority of the paths were not

significant and the model fit was marginal.

Lastly, hypothesis six was to determine if the grandparent’s well-being is different

according to whether the grandparent is custodial or noncustodial. The hypothesis was

partially confirmed through the SEM analysis. The variable, the nights spent with

grandchild, was less negatively correlated with the grandparent’s well-being among

noncustodial grandparents. Similarly, the closeness between the grandparent and the

grandchild was positively related to the grandparents’ well-being among noncustodial

grandparents, although it had a negative impact on the grandparents’ well-being for

105
custodial grandparents. Even though the relations among these variables were not

significant in the custodial grandparents’ model, it was assumed that staying overnight

with grandchildren was more burdensome for custodial grandparents because of their

legal responsibility for their grandchild. In addition, the impact of frequency of meeting

and communication with grandchildren on the grandparents’ well-being differed among

custodial and noncustodial grandparents; the impact was primarily among the

noncustodial grandparents. Grandparents with legal custody presumably feel burdened by

this obligation, but not necessarily by frequency of interaction, which was more

important in the model testing noncustodial grandparents. On the other hand, the amount

of variance explained in the model focusing on custodial grandparents was limited.

Most paths were not significant and the model fit among the custodial grandparents’

model was unacceptable.

This study yielded both similarities with and differences from previous research.

First, this study was consistent with previous research (e.g. Lin & Harwood, 2003;

Lemme, 2002; Strom & Strom, 1997) regarding the grandparent’s age and the

relationship between the grandparent and the grandchild. That is, it was confirmed that

the older the grandparents, the less contact they have with their grandchildren. Older

grandparents were less likely to spend nights with their grandchild than were younger

grandparents. In addition, older grandparents tended to communicate less with their

grandchild than did younger grandparents.

In addition, this study demonstrated that custodial grandparents and noncustodial

grandparents differed in well-being. Based on the results from the t-test results, custodial

106
grandparents demonstrated lower well-being than noncustodial grandparents. In addition,

path significances among the number of nights spent with the grandchild, the closeness

between the grandparent and the grandchild, and the grandparent’s well-being also

differed depending on whether the grandparent was custodial or noncustodial. Although

the custodial grandparents’ SEM model had less significant paths than the noncustodial

grandparents’ model, both spending nights with the grandchild and the closeness between

the grandparent and the grandchild tended to be burdensome for custodial grandparents.

In addition, this study’s results were consistent with the previous research (McGowen,

Ladd, & Strom, 2006) that indicates that spending nights with the grandchild is stressful

whether the grandparents are custodial or noncustodial. McGowen, Ladd, and Strom

found that grandparents’ life satisfaction was lower when grandchildren were living with

them than among grandparents who did not have grandchildren living with them.

Nevertheless, compared to custodial grandparents, noncustodial grandparents with

children living with them still had higher life satisfaction.

The major uniqueness of this study is its analysis of the closeness between the

grandparent and the grandchild and its effect on the grandparent’s well-being. Previous

research has focused mainly grandparents’ well-being comparing them in several respects,

including gender differences, ethnic differences, and differences between custodial

grandparents and noncustodial grandparents. Additionally, some studies have examined

the interactions between and among grandparents, parents, and grandchildren. However,

in this study, the researcher combined these two strands of research. The study’s focus

was on identifying the relationships between the grandparent’s well-being and the family

107
interactions using three latent variables – the number of nights spent with grandchild, the

closeness between the grandparent and the grandchild, and the grandparent-parent

support.

In addition, this study applied SEM analysis, although regression techniques have

been popular methods used to analyze these relationships. In previous studies, logistic

regression and t-test were applied to compare differences among two or more groups. For

instance, the researchers compared two or more populations, such as grandfathers and

grandmothers, Caucasian American, African American, Hispanic American populations,

and/or custodial and noncustodial grandparents (e.g. Kolomer & McCallion, 2005;

Reitzes & Mutran, 2004; Vandell, McCartney, Owen, Booth, & Clarke-Stewart, 2003). In

this study, however, the researcher included the factors related to the well-being of the

grandparents in the SEM analysis in order to explain the relationships among these

multiple factors. Thus, it was possible to test diverse models to find the best fit to explain

relationships among variables. In addition, it became possible to explain relationships

among independent variables and control variables as well as the relationship between

independent and dependent variables.

Implications for Social Work

Grandparents come into the parent-grandchild relationship for various reasons.

For instance, they sometimes become a third person in the parent-grandchild relationship

in order to reduce anxiety between the adult child and the grandchild. Some grandparents

also enter into the parent-grandchild relation just as a normal way of family interaction.

108
Thus, a grandparents’ level of stress may be depending upon the fact whether they are

with their grandchild by choice or not and how much time and energy they devote to

grandparenting. Regardless of the grandparents’ gender, ethnicity, or legal custody,

spending nights with the grandchild without the parent of the child worsened

grandparents’ well-being in this study. Goodfellow and Laverty (2003) also stated that

taking care of grandchildren was a physically and emotionally exhausting job for both

custodial and noncustodial grandparents.

Thus, it is necessary to consider providing support for grandparents who spend

significant amounts of time for grandparenting, whether the grandparents are custodial or

noncustodial. For example, support group, respite care, empowerment training,

counseling, and financial support programs are available for custodial grandparents in

many states. On the other hand, noncustodial grandparent caregivers who feel just as

stressed as custodial grandparents rarely receive formal support. The state of Georgia

provides child care subsidy as financial incentives to eligible grandparents who are over

60 years old. Cuyahoga County in Ohio provides cash assistance and day care vouchers

for low-income grandparents if they meet the eligibility guidelines. However, support for

the noncustodial grandparents is limited and it is mostly for low-income families.

Therefore, policy advocacy is necessary for more noncustodial grandparents who are

taking care of their grandchild to be eligible for formal financial support.

Ohio’s Grandparents Raising Grandchildren’s Task Force (1999) stated that

among available services for grandparent caregivers, counseling, tutoring, respite services,

legal information, stress reduction programs, and information and referral services are the

109
most needed services. Especially, as a specialized service for grandparent caregivers,

family counseling may be necessary because grandparents get into intergenerational

family relationships. They may newly define their relationship with their adult child and

grandchild. Custodial grandparents may need to accept their role as a legal guardian to

their grandchild. There may be resistance from the grandchild and/or from themselves

regarding becoming a grandparent-caregiver. Noncustodial grandparents taking care of

their grandchild may need to re-define their role in family. There is no clear definition for

noncustodial grandparents of their tasks. Their tasks may be changeable according to the

schedule of their adult child. Hence, the ambiguity of the noncustodial grandparents’

roles may result in family conflict, especially between the grandparent and the adult child.

Thus, Bowen’s family intervention may be helpful for grandparent caregivers to reduce

anxiety and intergenerational tensions. Bowen family intervention focuses on a third

person who feels stress after involved in the triangle family relationship. Hence,

grandparents raising grandchildren may get benefits through Bowen family counseling as

to seek stress reduction strategies and set their roles in the family.

In addition, it may be necessary to connect off-line resources to the on-line

support for grandparents raising their grandchildren. Although the AARP website offers

information for grandparent caregivers, much information is still off-line. Grandparents

often go to a local service center to join a grandparent support group or receive training.

On the other hand, those who take care of young children may not join programs that are

organized according to a fixed timetable. Additionally, some grandparents may have free

time in the evening. Hence, linking off-line resources with the on-line community may be

110
beneficial for those who need to seek assistance at any time. Moreover, the on-line

community may be able for grandparent caregivers to make a social network with their

grandchild, adult child, and other grandparent caregivers.

Another important factor to consider when working with grandparents and

grandchildren is cultural diversity. As indicated previously, there are various factors that

affect the well-being of the grandparents. Each culture, each generation, and each family

may have different views about family ties; thus, social work institutions may continue to

educate students about cultural diversity and respect cultural differences. In addition,

social workers should be culturally aware when working with grandparents taking care of

their grandchildren.

Implications for Future Research

For future studies of this topic, it will be necessary to include factors not included

in this study, such as the grandchild’s age, family conflict, the grandparent’s physical

distance from the grandchild, and the grandparent’s economic status, to identify the

diverse factors affecting the association between the grandparent-grandchild relationship

and the well-being of the grandparent. In addition, it may be beneficial if future studies

are able to include more non-Caucasian American grandparents in the sample. In this

study, the ratio of Caucasian to non-Caucasian American grandparents was about 8:2.

Although the national proportion of Caucasian versus non-Caucasian Americans is about

the same, it was regrettable that this study only included five American Indian and three

Asian American grandparents.

111
Different types of SEM models may be necessary to address ethnic variations and

differences related to the legal custody of the grandparents. The SEM model used in this

study did not explain the characteristics of non-Caucasian American grandparents and

custodial grandparents as much as it did the Caucasian and non-custodial grandparents.

The non-Caucasian American grandparents’ SEM model and the custodial grandparents’

SEM model contained many non-significant paths. Also, the model fit of both models

was lower than other models tested in this research. Thus, it is assumed that the two

populations may have other factors, not included in this research, that affected

grandparent-grandchild relationship and the grandparents’ well-being.

Research on diverse cultures is necessary to better serve grandparents taking care

of grandchildren. These research findings indicated that the relationship between the

nights spent with the grandchild and the grandparent’s well-being decreased after adding

three variables: cultural attitude, grandparent-parent support, and grandparent’s

functional capacity. Moreover, the relationship between the grandparent-grandchild

relationship and the grandparent’s well-being strengthened after adding the above three

variables. In this research, it was found that cultural attitude, grandparent-parent

relationship, and grandparent’s functional capacity moderated the negative relationship

and strengthened the positive relationship. On the other hand, in future research, it may

be necessary to find out what characteristics of each variable affected the result that the

negative relationship was weakened and the positive relationship was strengthened.

Grandparents might perceive grandparenting differently according to their cultural

background; for instance, whether they are from a culture that emphasizes individual

112
freedom or family ties. The model used in this study poorly explains non-Caucasian

American grandparents although it successfully represents Caucasian American

grandparents. This may due to the complex factors associated with non-Caucasian culture.

Among Asian cultures, for instance, the nature of Chinese culture and Japanese culture is

totally different. Hispanic culture, which is comprised of diverse ethnic groups, also

differs from many Caucasian American norms relating to grandparenting. Thus, social

workers must carefully assess these cultural differences and tailor services based on these

ethnic variations.

Lastly, if multilevel modeling were applied to this subject, it would be possible to

analyze the changes in the grandparent’s well-being and the grandparent-grandchild

interaction as grandparents get older. Moreover, it would be possible to identify how

generational time, an important construct underlying the life course perspective, affects

the triangular relationship among three generations. In this study, the ages of both

grandparents and grandchildren were included to identify its effects on the closeness

between the grandparent and the grandchild and the number of nights spent with the

grandchild. The age variable was not, however, included for testing hypotheses three to

six. Since the age of both grandparents and grandchildren may affect the relationship

between the grandparent-grandchild relationship and the grandparent’s well-being,

researchers should include age as a control variable in future studies.

113
References

AARP. (2006a). Grandparenting. Retrieved October 14, 2006 from the World Wide Web:

http://www.aarp.org/families/grandparents/.

AARP. (2006b). The grandparent study 2002 report. Retrieved September 30, 2006 from

the World Wide Web:

http://www.aarp.org/research/family/grandparenting/aresearch-import-488.html

Ackerman, N. J. (1984). A theory of family systems. New York: Gardner Press, Inc.

Anderson, R. E., Carter, I. & Lowe, G. R. (1999). Human behavior in the social

environment: A social systems approach (5th edition). Hawthorne, N.Y.: Aldine

De Gruyter.

Ashford, J. B., LeCroy, C.W., & Lortie, K. L. (2001). Human behavior in the social

environment: A multidimensional perspective (2nd ed.). Pacific Grove, CA:

Brooks/Cole.

Barber, C. E., & Haddock, S. A. (2003). Self-perceptions of comparative power and

worth in three generational families. Contemporary Family Therapy, 25(2), 229-

245.

Becvar, D. S., & Becvar, R. J. (2006). Family therapy: A systematic integration (6th ed.).

Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

Becvar, D. S., & Becvar, R. J. (2009). Family therapy: A systematic integration (7th ed.).

Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

114
Beekman, A. T. F., Deeg, D. J. H., Van Limbeek, J., Braam, A. W., De Vries, M. Z., &

Van Tilburg, W. (1997). Criterion validity of the center for epidemiologic studies

depression scale (CES-D): Results from a community-based sample of older

subjects in the netherlands. Psychological Medicine, 27(1), 231-235.

Biddle, B. J. (1979). Role theory: Expectations, identities, and behaviors. New York:

Academic Press.

Bullock, K. (2005). Grandfathers and their impact of raising grandchildren. Journal of

Sociology and Social Welfare, 32(1), 43-59.

Burns, A. S., Lawlor, B. A., & Craig, S. (2004). Assessment scales in old age psychiatry.

London: Taylor & Francis.

Clausen, J. A. (1986). The life course: A sociological perspective. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:

Prentice Hall.

Collier, P. J., & Callero, P. J. (2005). Role theory and social cognition: Learning to think

like a recycler. Self and Identity, 4 (1), 45-58.

Crouter, A. C., McHale, S. M., & Bartko, W. T. (1993). Gender as an organizing feature

in parent-child relationships. In R. A. Pierce & M. A. Black (Eds.), Life-span

development: A diversity reader (pp. 3-17). Dubuque, Iowa: Kendall/Hunt

Publishing Company.

Dolbin-MacNab, M. L. (2006). Just like raising your own? Grandmothers' perceptions of

parenting a second time around. Family Relations, 55(5), 564-575.

Euler, H. A., Hoier, S., & Rohde, P. A. (2001). Relationship-specific closeness of

intergenerational family ties: Findings from evolutionary psychology and

115
implications for models of cultural transmission. Journal of Cross-Cultural

Psychology, 32(2), 147-158.

Fingerman, K. L., & Bermann, E. (2000). Applications of family systems theory to the

study of adulthood. [Electronic version]. International Journal of Aging and

Human Development, 51(1), 5-29.

George, L. K., & Gold, D. T. (1991). Life course perspectives on intergenerational and

generational connections. Marriage & Family Review, 16(1-2), 67-88.

Gerson, M. (1996). The embedded self: A psychoanalytic guide to family therapy.

Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press.

Gladding, S. T. (2002). Family therapy: History, theory, and practice (3rd ed.). Upper

Saddle River, NJ: Merrill Prentice Hall.

Goodfellow, J., & Laverty, J. (2003). Grandparents supporting working families. Family

Matters, 66, 14-19.

Goodman, C. C. (2007). Intergenerational triads in skipped-generation grandfamilies.

International Journal of Aging & Human Development, 65(3), 231-258.

Griff, M. D. (1999). Intergenerational play therapy: The influence of grandparents in

family systems. Child and Youth Services, 20(1/2), 63-76.

Hagestad, G.O. (2003). Interdependent lives and relationships in changing times: A life

course view of families and aging. In R.A. Settersten Jr. (Eds.), Invitation to the

life course: Toward new understandings of later life (pp. 135-160). Amityville,

NY: Baywood Publishing Company, Inc.

116
Hahlweg, K. (2004). Strengthening partnerships and families. In P. L. Chase-Lansdale, K.

Kiernan, & R. J. Friedman, R. J. (Eds.), Human development across lives and

generations: The potential for change (pp.204-235). New York: Cambridge

University Press.

Hareven, T. K. (1994). Aging and generational relations: A historical and life course

perspective. Annual Review of Sociology, 20(1), 437-461.

Hareven, T. K. (2000). Families, history, and social change: Life course and cross-

cultural perspectives. Boulder, Colorado: Westview.

Hatch, L. R. (2000). Beyond gender differences: Adaptation to aging in life course

perspective. Amityville, NY: Baywood Publishing Company, Inc.

Hayslip Jr. B., Henderson, C. E., & Shore, R. J. (2003). The structure of grandparental

role meaning. Journal of Adult Development, 10(1), 1-11.

Hayslip Jr., B., & Kaminski, P. L. (2005). Grandparents raising their grandchildren: A

review of the literature and suggestions for practice. The Gerontologist, 45(2),

262-269.

Hooyman, N. R., & Kiyak, H. A. (2005). Social Gerontology: A Multidisciplinary

Perspective. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Ikeda, K., & Hatta, T. (2001). Perceptions of family structures by Japanese students. In

T.E. Gehring, M. Debry, & P.K. Smith, P. K (Eds.), The family system test FAST:

Theory and application (pp. 179-193). Philadelphia, PA: Taylor&Francis Inc.

117
Julian, T.W., McKenry, P.C., & McKelvey, M.W. (1994). Cultural variations in

parenting: Perceptions of Caucasian, African-American, Hispanic, and Asian-

American parents. Family Relations, 43(1), 30-37.

Kemp, C. L. (2007). Grandparent-grandchild ties: Reflections on continuity and change

across three generations. Journal of Family Issues, 28(7), 855-881.

Kleiner, H .S., Hertzog, J., & Targ, D. B. (1998). Grandparents acting as parents.

Retrieved March 3, 2008 from the World Wide Web:

http://www.uwex.edu/ces/gprg/article.html

Kolomer, S. R., & McCallion, P. (2005). Depression and caregiver mastery in

grandfathers caring for their grandchildren. International Journal of Aging and

Human Development, 60(4), 283-294.

Landry-Meyer, L. (1999). Research into action: Recommended intervention strategies for

grandparent caregivers. Family Relations, 48(4), 381-389.

Landry-Meyer, L., & Newman, B. M. (2004). An exploration of the grandparent

caregiver role. Journal of Family Issues, 25(8), 1005-1025.

Lemme, B. H. (2002). Development in adulthood (3rd ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

Lin, M., & Harwood, J. (2003). Accommodation predictors of grandparent-grandchildren

relational solidarity in Taiwan. [Electronic version]. Journal of Social and

Personal Relationships, 20(4), 537-563.

Maccoby, E. E. (2003). The gender of child and parent as factors in family dynamics. In

A. C. Crouter & A. Booth (Eds.), Children’s influence on family dynamics: The

118
neglected side of family relationships (pp. 191-206). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence

Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.

Macmillan, R., & Copher, R. (2005). Families in the life course: Interdependency of roles,

role configurations, and pathways. Journal of Marriage and Family, 67(4), 858-

879.

McGowen, M. R., Ladd, L., & Strom, R. D. (2006). On-line assessment of grandmother

experience in raising grandchildren. Educational Gerontology, 32, 669-684.

Minuchin, S., Lee, W., & Simon, G. M. (2006). Mastering family therapy: Journeys of

growth and transformation (2nd ed.). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Miller, R. B., Anderson, S., & Keala, D. K. (2004). Is Bowen theory valid? A review of

basic research. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 30 (4), 453-366.

Mitchell, J., & Register, J. C. (1984). An exploration of family interaction with the

elderly by race, socioeconomic status, and residence. The Gerontologist, 24(1),

48-54.

Musil, C. M., & Standing, T. (2005). Grandmothers’ diaries: A glimpse at daily lives.

International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 60(4), 317-329.

Musil, C. M., Warner, C. B., Zauszniewski, J. A., Jeanblanc, A. B., & Kercher, K. (2006).

Grandmothers, caregiving, and family functioning. Journal of Gerontology,

61B(2), S89-S98.

Nichols, M. P., & Schwartz, R. C. (2001). The essentials of family therapy. Boston: Allyn

and Bacon.

119
Ohio’s Grandparents Raising Grandchildren’s Task Force. (1999). Grandparents raising

grandchildren: A report to the Ohio General Assembly on the problems and

concerns of Ohioans raising their children’s children. Retrieved November 17,

2009 from the World Wide Web:

http://aging.ohio.gov/resources/publications/grandparents.pdf

Papero, D. V. (1990). Bowen family systems theory. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

Peters, C. L., Hooker, K., & Zvonkovic, A. M. (2006). Older parents' perceptions of

ambivalence in relationships with their children. Family Relations, 55(5), 539-551.

Presser, H. B. (1989). Some economic complexities of child care provided by

grandmothers. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 51(3), 581-591.

Prest, L. A., & Protinsky, H. (1993). Family systems theory: A unifying framework for

codependence. The American Journal of Family Therapy, 21(4), 352-360.

Price, S. J., McKenry, P. C., & Murphy, M. J. (2000). In S. J. Price, P. C. McKenry, & M.

J. Murphy (Eds.), Families across time: A life course perspective (pp. 2-23). LA,

CA: Roxbury Publishing Company.

Pruchno, R., & McKenney, D. (2000). Living with grandchildren: The effects of

custodial and co-resident households on the mental health of grandmothers.

Journal of Mental Health and Aging, 6, 269-289.

Putney, N. M., & Bengtson, V. L. (2005). Family relations in changing times: A

longitudinal study of five cohorts of women. International Journal of Sociology

and Social Policy, 25(3), 92-119.

120
Raina, J.L. (1989). Structural and functional changes in the joint family system: A study

based on D.C.M. workers. New Delhi: Concept Publication Co.

Reitzes, D. C., & Mutran, E. J. (2004). Grandparent identity, intergenerational family

identity, and well-being. Journal of Gerontology, 59B(4), S213-S219.

Robinson, M. M., & Wilks, S. E. (2006). "Older but not wiser": What custodial

grandparents want to tell social workers about raising grandchildren. Social work,

33(2), 164-177.

Rosenblatt, P. C. (1994). Metaphors of family systems theory. New York: The Guilford

Press.

Ross, C. E., & Mirowsky, J. (1988). Child care and emotional adjustment to wives'

employment. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 29(2), 127-138.

Ross, M. E. T., & Aday, L. A. (2006). Stress and coping in African American

grandparents who are raising their grandchildren. Journal of Family Issues, 27(7),

912-932.

Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of

psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(6),

1069-1081.

Ryff, C. D., & Keyes, C. L. M. (1995). The structure of psychological well-being

revisited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(4), 719-727.

Sandel, T. L., Cho, G., E., Miller, P. J., & Wang, S. (2006). What it means to be a

grandmother: A cross-cultural study of Taiwanese and Euro-American

grandmothers' beliefs. Journal of Family Communication, 6(4), 255-278.

121
Sands, R. G., & Goldberg-Glen, R. S. (2000). Factors associated with stress among

grandparents raising their grandchildren. Family Relations, 49(1), 97-105.

Schumacker, R. E., & Lomax, R. G. (2004). A beginner’s guide to structural equation

modeling (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Science Daily. (2008, November 4). Grandmothers as caregivers can cut risk of

childhood injury in half. Science Daily, pp. 1, 2.

Silverstein, M. (2007). Benefits of grandparents raising grandchildren. Journal of

Intergenerational Relationships, 5(3), 131-134.

Silverstein, M., & Ruiz, S. (2006). Breaking the chain: How grandparents moderate the

transmission of maternal depression to their grandchildren. Family Relations,

55(5), 601-612.

Spira, M., & Wall, J. (2006). Issues in multigenerational families: Adolescents’

perceptions of grandparents’ declining health. Child and Adolescent Social Work

Journal, 23(4), 390-406.

Strom, R. D., & Strom, S. K. (1997). Building a theory of grandparent development.

International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 45(4), 255-286.

Sweet, J., Bumpass, L., & Call, V. (1988). The design and content of the National Survey

of Families and Households (NSFH Working Paper No.1). Madison, WI:

University of Wisconsin-Madison, Center for Demography and Ecology.

Sweet, J., & Bumpass, L. (2002). The National Survey of Families and Households -

Waves 1, 2, and 3: Data Description and Documentation. Madison, WI:

University of Wisconsin-Madison, Center for Demography and Ecology.

122
Taylor, J. Y., Washington, O. G. M., Artinian, N. T., & Lichtenberg, P. (2007). Parental

stress among African American parents and grandparents. Issues in Mental Health

and Nursing, 28(4), 373-387.

Titleman, P. (1998). Family systems assessment based on Bowen theory. In P. Titelman

(Ed.), Clinical applications of Bowen family systems theory (pp. 51-68). New

York: The Haworth Press.

U.S. Census Bureau. (2003). Grandparents living with grandchildren: 2000. Retrieved

September 30, 2006 from the World Wide Web:

http://www.census.gov/prod/2003pubs/c2kbr-31.pdf

U.S. Census Bureau. (2005). Who’s minding the kids? Child care arrangements: Winter

2002. Retrieved October 2, 2006 from the World Wide Web:

http://www.census.gov/prod/2005pubs/p70-101.pdf

U.S. Census Bureau. (2006). 65+ in the United States: 2005. Retrieved September 30,

2006 from the World Wide Web: http://www.census.gov/prod/2006pubs/p23-

209.pdf

U.S. Census Bureau. (2007). Annual Estimates of the Population by Sex and Five-Year

Age Groups for the United States: April 1, 2000 to July 1, 2007 (NC-EST2007-

01). Retrieved March 14, 2009 from the World Wide Web:

http://www.census.gov/popest/national/asrh/NC-EST2007-sa.html

U.S. Census Bureau. (2009). The 2009 Statistical Abstract. Retrieved May 16, 2009 from

the World Wide Web: http://www.census.gov/compendia/statab/

123
Vandell, D. L., McCartney, K., Owen, M. T., Booth, C., & Clarke-Stewart, A. (2003).

Variations in child care by grandparents during the first three years. Journal of

Marriage and Family, 65, 375-381.

Welsh, W.M., & Stewart, A.J. (1995). Relationships between women and their parents:

implications for midlife well-being. Psychology and Aging, 10(2), 181-190.

Wheelock, J., & Jones, K. (2002). 'Grandparents are the next best thing': Informal

childcare for working parents in urban Britain. Journal of Social Policy, 31(3),

441-464.

124
Appendix A: Descriptions of Constructs

125
Nights spent with GC

1) X1: During the last 12 months, about how many nights altogether, if any, did

(your grandchild/any of your grandchildren) stay overnight at your house without

his or her parents? (RH12)

a. Scale – Continuous variable

Closeness between GP & GC

1) X2: During last 12 months, about how often did you see your grandchild/any of

your grandchildren? (RH13)

a. Scale - 1: Not at all, 2: About once a year, 3: Several times a year, 4: 1 to 3

time a month, 5: About once a week, 6: More than once a week, 7:

Grandchild lives with me

2) X3: During last 12months, about how often did you talk on the telephone or

receive a letter or e-mail from your grandchild/any of your grandchildren? (RH14)

a. Scale - 1: Not at all, 2: About once a year, 3: Several times a year, 4: 1 to 3

time a month, 5: About once a week, 6: More than once a week, 7:

Grandchild lives with me

3) X4: On a scale is from 0 to 10, where 0 is “not at all close” and 10 is "extremely

close," how would you describe your relationship with your grandchild/any of

your grandchildren? (RH15)

a. Scale - 0: Not at all ~ 10: Extremely close

Grandparent’s well-being

1) Y1: Happiness (RT201) - How would you say things are these days?

126
a. Scale – 1 : Very unhappy ~ 7: Very happy

2) Y2: Quality of life (RT202A~RT202K)

a. Scale - 1: Very dissatisfied ~ 7: Very satisfied

b. Questions - How satisfied are you with…

i. Your home? (RT202A)

ii. Your neighborhood? (RT202B)

iii. Your city or town? (RT202C)

iv. Your financial situation? (RT202D)

v. The amount of leisure time that you have? (RT202E)

vi. Your health? (RT202F)

vii. Your physical appearance? (RT202G)

viii. Your friendships? (RT202H)

ix. Your sex life? (RT202I)

x. Your family life? (RT202J)

xi. Your present job? (RT202K) – Excluded from the analysis

3) Y3: Depression (RT206A~RT206L)

a. Scale – 0: 7 days ~ 6: 1 day, 7: None

b. Questions – On how many days during the past week did you…

i. Feel bothered by things that usually don’t bother you? (RT206A)

ii. Not feel like eating or your appetite was poor? (RT206B)

iii. Feel that you could not shake off the blues even with help from

your family or friends? (RT206C)

127
iv. Have trouble keeping your mind on what you were doing?

(RT206D)

v. Feel depressed? (RT206E)

vi. Feel that everything you did was an effort? (RT206F)

vii. Feel fearful? (RT206G)

viii. Sleep restlessly? (RT206H)

ix. Talk less than usual? (RT206I)

x. Feel lonely? (RT206J)

xi. Feel sad? (RT206K)

xii. Feel you could not get going? (RT206L)

4) Y4: Psychological well-being (RT217A~RT217H & RT218A~RT218J)

a. Scale - 1: Strongly disagree ~ 5: Strongly agree

b. Statements

i. I like most parts of my personality (RT217A)

ii. I think it is important to have new experience that challenge how

you think about yourself and the world (RT217B)

iii. The demands of everyday life often get me down (RT217C)

iv. Maintaining close relationships has been difficult and frustrating

for me (RT217D)

v. I tend to be influenced by people with strong opinions (RT217E)

vi. I have confidence in my own opinions, even if they are different

from the way most other people think (RT217F)

128
vii. I live life one day at a time and don’t really think about the future

(RT217G)

viii. I have not experienced many warm and trusting relationships with

others (RT217H)

ix. When I look at the story of my life, I am pleased about how things

have turned out (RT218A)

x. In many ways, I feel disappointed about my achievements in life

(RT218B)

xi. People would describe me as a giving person, willing to share my

time with others (RT218C)

xii. I gave up trying to make big improvements or change in my life a

long time ago (RT218D)

xiii. In general, I feel I am in charge of the situation in which I live

(RT218E)

xiv. I sometimes feel as if I have done all there is to do in life (RT218F)

xv. I am quite good at managing the many responsibilities of my daily

life (RT218G)

xvi. Some people wonder aimlessly through life, but I am not one of

them (RT218H)

xvii. For me, life has been a continuous process of learning, changing,

and growth (RT218I)

129
xviii. I judge myself but what I think is important, not by the values of

what others think is important (RT218J)

Cultural attitude

1) C1: Elder parents should live with kids (RT1805C)

a. 0: Strongly disagree ~ 4: Strongly agree

2) C2: Child should help older parent with money (RT1805F)

a. 0: Strongly disagree ~ 4: Strongly agree

3) C3: Adult kids with problems can live with parent (RT1809C)

a. 0: Strongly disagree ~ 4: Strongly agree

Grandparent-parent support

1) GPPS1: Gave emotional support to child during the last month (RG5)

a. Scale – 0: No, 1: Yes

2) GPPS2:During the last month, helped children with childcare while they are

working (RG7)

a. Scale – 0: No, 1: Yes

3) GPPS3:During the last month, helped children with childcare while they are not

working (RG9)

a. Scale – 0: No, 1: Yes

4) GPPS4:Receive help from adult children with shopping, running errands, or

transportation during the last month (RG12)

a. Scale – 0: No, 1: Yes

130
5) GPPS5:Received help from adult children with housework, yard work, car repairs,

or other work around the house during the last month (RG14)

a. Scale – 0: No, 1: Yes

6) GPPS6:Received emotional support from children during the last month (RG16)

a. Scale – 0: No, 1: Yes

Grandparent’s functional capacity

1) Physical/mental limits (ADL)

a. Scale – 1: Limit a lot, 2: Limit a little, 3: Do not limit at all

b. Questionnaires – Physical/mental limits…

i. For personal needs, such as bathing, dressing (RT205A)

ii. To move about inside the house (RT205B)

iii. For day to day tasks (RT205C)

iv. For climbing stairs (RT205D)

v. To walk six blocks (RT205E)

vi. To do heavy work (RT205F)

vii. To work (RT205G)

Other observed variables influencing the closeness between the grandparent and the

grandchild

1) GP Age: Age of grandparent (dobm & doby)

a. Scale – Continuous variable

2) Age of grandchild

a. GCY: Age of youngest grandchild (RH10) – Continuous variable

131
b. GCO: Age of oldest grandchild (RH11) – Continuous variable

3) No. GC: Number of grandchildren (RH9)

a. Scale – Continuous variable

Categorical variables for multiple group comparison

1) Grandparent’s gender (sex_a from Roster 1 data)

a. Scale – 1: Male, 2: Female

2) Grandparent’s ethnicity (recoded from the variable M484 from Wave 1 data)

a. Scale – 1: Caucasian American, 2: Non Caucasian American

3) Ever had primary responsibility for grandchild (RH16)

a. Scale – 1: Yes, 2: No

132
Appendix B: Covariance Matrixes Used for Hypotheses Testing

133
No. GC GP Age GCY GCO X1 X2 X3 X4
21.308
17.669 108.365
-.189 11.413 10.905
24.607 56.839 8.893 69.634
3.010 -60.698 -4.715 -23.479 2142.693
.525 -1.839 -.655 -.375 16.163 1.956
.245 -1.554 -.213 -.072 13.605 1.022 2.321
.276 -.609 -.262 .400 13.239 1.328 1.319 4.131
Table B.1. Covariance Matrix Used for Hypothesis Test 1

X1 X2 X3 X4 Y1 Y2 Y3 Y4
2204.993
16.215 1.949
13.562 1.007 2.346
15.208 1.389 1.348 4.181
-3.680 -.006 .028 .313 1.521
-47.906 -.446 .120 2.721 7.012 84.627
-72.059 -1.291 -.695 -.544 9.209 67.427 246.400
-12.579 -.093 .422 .846 2.797 24.184 38.857 36.476
Table B.2. Covariance Matrix Used for Hypothesis Test 2

134
X1 X2 X3 X4 Y1 Y2 Y3 Y4 C1 C2 C3 GPPS FC
2215.040
16.103 1.960
13.450 1.012 2.355
15.472 1.407 1.368 4.246
-3.380 .004 .037 .310 1.517
-49.642 -.501 .124 2.616 7.083 85.408
-68.059 -1.283 -.644 -.641 9.335 68.526 250.437
-11.096 -.083 .412 .784 2.786 24.441 39.224 36.126
2.108 .104 .097 .146 -.008 -.346 -.800 .250 .871
.516 .051 .060 .094 .015 .098 -.510 .429 .327 .661
2.746 .094 .112 .100 -.053 -.443 -.504 -.013 .192 .126 .889
14.147 .875 .766 .894 -.135 -2.091 -4.178 .056 .171 .129 .148 2.546
-3.298 -.137 .023 .068 .790 7.968 16.018 4.324 .191 .193 .055 -.345 7.375
Table B.3. Covariance Matrix Used for Hypothesis Test 3

135
X1 X2 X3 X4 Y1 Y2 Y3 Y4 C1 C2 C3 GPPS FC
2569.547
17.168 1.781
13.920 .913 2.319
14.472 1.100 1.081 3.267
-3.475 -.001 .044 .287 1.649
-53.329 -.513 .056 2.502 7.911 94.842
-79.105 -1.303 -.785 .112 11.283 82.594 296.521
-16.677 -.035 .328 .798 3.203 27.198 49.023 39.696
3.483 .144 .124 .150 -.019 -.665 -1.351 .235 .861
1.041 .079 .100 .103 .011 .154 -.672 .482 .363 .627
3.804 .141 .151 .089 -.078 -.613 -.782 -.101 .179 .135 .906
14.216 .769 .644 .622 -.145 -2.135 -4.004 -.107 .233 .119 .200 2.432
1.139 -.004 .116 .259 .921 8.380 18.545 5.051 .182 .210 -.018 -.256 8.197
Table B.4.1. Covariance Matrix Used for Hypothesis Test 4 (Grandmothers)

X1 X2 X3 X4 Y1 Y2 Y3 Y4 C1 C2 C3 GPPS FC
1607.474
12.848 2.169
10.740 1.060 2.266
14.015 1.726 1.598 5.478
-2.555 .054 .077 .435 1.282
-38.713 -.182 .621 3.427 5.592 68.986
-26.779 .159 1.401 1.020 5.504 40.816 153.230
.811 .001 .762 1.100 2.030 19.393 20.588 29.996
-.133 .041 .054 .145 .009 .175 .082 .273 .888
-.623 -.013 -.028 .044 .029 .059 .006 .372 .270 .714
1.258 .032 .069 .149 -.017 -.214 -.284 .104 .212 .114 .861
10.647 .844 .703 .916 -.025 -1.350 -1.315 .695 .071 .110 .098 2.274
-8.202 -.208 .060 .060 .509 6.819 9.602 2.863 .203 .192 .150 -.159 5.790
Table B.4.2. Covariance Matrix Used for Hypothesis Test 4 (Grandfathers)

136
X1 X2 X3 X4 Y1 Y2 Y3 Y4 C1 C2 C3 GPPS FC
1412.925
14.551 2.012
12.971 1.038 2.411
15.064 1.605 1.575 4.693
-2.377 .026 .081 .368 1.425
-46.532 -.572 .163 3.220 7.111 83.703
-72.632 -1.225 -.727 1.006 9.507 67.556 223.974
-17.876 -.252 .235 .508 2.962 25.697 40.421 35.228
.978 .127 .061 .127 .024 -.017 -.673 .366 .870
.244 .071 .015 .061 .049 .468 .215 .485 .313 .666
3.347 .126 .081 .065 -.072 -.515 -.772 .260 .214 .127 .858
9.082 .807 .642 .864 -.209 -2.823 -4.595 -.255 .159 .136 .155 2.387
-2.730 -.065 .101 .287 .859 8.765 14.591 4.294 .318 .300 .126 -.275 7.734
Table B.5.1. Covariance Matrix Used for Hypothesis Test 5 (Caucasian American
Grandparents)

X1 X2 X3 X4 Y1 Y2 Y3 Y4 C1 C2 C3 GPPS FC
5013.658
23.759 1.861
19.918 1.039 2.482
16.901 .738 1.027 2.491
-6.916 -.059 .135 .132 1.627
-89.750 -.784 .931 1.387 6.976 101.136
-46.861 -1.517 1.698 -2.739 7.304 92.711 364.672
-1.119 -.756 .486 -.074 2.166 20.701 58.806 40.748
-1.652 .104 .101 .007 -.076 -.457 -.428 -.185 .798
.847 .041 .108 .023 -.084 -.613 -.646 1.025 .157 .487
-1.140 .047 .101 .088 -.071 -.060 .175 -.240 .211 -.010 .971
30.079 .732 .896 .422 -.209 -.965 -3.890 .223 .207 .064 .274 2.733
9.157 .000 .315 -.509 .510 6.287 30.937 8.138 .126 .099 .290 -.575 10.929
Table B.5.2. Covariance Matrix Used for Hypothesis Test 5 (Non-Caucasian American
Grandparents)

137
X1 X2 X3 X4 Y1 Y2 Y3 Y4 C1 C2 C3 GPPS FC
9607.934
35.954 1.721
19.861 .920 2.124
22.827 .666 .779 2.306
-3.457 -.061 .115 .174 2.018
-181.295 -2.203 -.811 1.315 10.059 116.307
-161.880 -1.673 -.249 -2.047 12.228 90.763 325.349
18.303 -.267 .927 .264 2.823 27.305 50.841 42.348
5.560 .074 -.032 .002 -.032 .337 -1.903 .455 .850
5.024 .047 .064 -.012 .130 .778 .149 .558 .342 .579
2.435 .124 .135 .098 .004 -.528 -1.427 -.760 .070 .091 .926
27.533 .782 .667 .395 -.300 -5.341 -6.730 -.095 .182 .088 .265 2.792
31.285 .385 .441 .759 1.348 10.278 28.556 6.228 .153 .364 -.206 -.424 11.081
Table B.6.1. Covariance Matrix Used for Hypothesis Test 6 (Custodial Grandparents)

X1 X2 X3 X4 Y1 Y2 Y3 Y4 C1 C2 C3 GPPS FC
808.360
10.488 1.972
9.708 .995 2.357
10.689 1.486 1.415 4.501
-1.238 .041 .054 .373 1.412
-10.127 -.001 .529 3.166 6.400 78.725
-17.209 -.783 -.210 .253 8.473 61.616 231.760
-8.494 .039 .432 1.009 2.700 23.295 35.968 34.843
1.028 .103 .111 .161 .002 -.411 -.524 .237 .874
-.564 .047 .054 .105 .000 .018 -.557 .421 .324 .674
2.579 .087 .105 .096 -.059 -.409 -.312 .118 .211 .132 .884
9.214 .857 .745 .926 -.077 -1.303 -3.254 .188 .161 .131 .125 2.469
-4.361 -.165 .019 .041 .647 7.163 13.111 3.834 .210 .173 .103 -.266 6.660
Table B.6.2. Covariance Matrix Used for Hypothesis Test 6 (Noncustodial Grandparents)

138

You might also like